Skip to main content
The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences : MJMS logoLink to The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences : MJMS
. 2020 Aug 19;27(4):85–96. doi: 10.21315/mjms2020.27.4.8

Relationship Between Passion and Courage among the Experienced Male Soccer Players

Erkut Konter 1, Yee Cheng Kueh 2, Garry Kuan 3,4,
PMCID: PMC7444832  PMID: 32863748

Abstract

Background

The nature of the relationship between passion and courage and its influence on soccer performance has yet to be revealed. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine passion attributes (i.e. harmonious and obsessive inclinations) among male soccer players in relation to the level of courageous characteristics (i.e. mastery, determination, assertiveness, venturesome and sacrificial behaviour), other demographic and player characteristics.

Methods

Data were collected from 278 male soccer players aged 13–38 years (mean [M] = 17.42 ± 4.36) with the number of competitive soccer experiences ranging from 1–28 years (M = 7.51 ± 4.23 years). Participants had at least a year of experience in the sport of soccer completed the validated passion scale, sports courage scale and demographic form.

Results

Analyses revealed that soccer players with higher levels of total courage (P < 0.001), have more experience in soccer (P = 0.011), and their soccer level being professional (P < 0.001) had a significantly higher score in harmonious passion. There was no significant difference in obsessive passion among different level of total courage (P = 0.154). However, soccer players with more experience (P = 0.011) and higher soccer level being professional (P < 0.001) demonstrated a significant higher score in obsessive passion.

Conclusion

In conclusion, soccer players with higher harmonious and obsessive passionate attributes had higher courage (except for mastery). In addition, the courageous and passionate traits of the soccer players played meaningful roles in indicating individual and performance variables.

Keywords: courage, harmonious passion, obsessive passion, performance, soccer

Introduction

Players who enjoy playing soccer are autonomously and passionately involved in the game. Many of the world’s best soccer players, for example, Diago Maradona and Cristiano Ronaldo, anecdotally refer to the joy and pleasures they attained from the sport (1). This sense of joy and pleasure could be linked with the self-determination theory by Deci and Ryan (2, 3). This theory is based on the three basic needs of human beings: autonomy (i.e. the need to have control over one’s actions), relatedness (i.e. the need to feel connected with other people) and competence (i.e. the need to have a positive effect on outcomes and surroundings) (1, 4).

Jordet (1) concluded that there is indirect evidence of self-determined involvement in the game that has been cultivated as the foundation within the players who make it into the professional ranks in soccer. Self-determined behaviour that is driven from being intrinsically involved in soccer seems to influence the performance outcome positively. Thus, the self-determined behaviour observed in the soccer players are linked with commitment and passioned.

Vallerand (5) originally developed the passion scale (PS) based on the dualistic passion model (DPM). Passion refers to strong inclination towards an activity that a person likes or loves and finds important, in which significant time and energy are invested (5). According to Vallerand (5), passion can be divided into harmonious passion (HP) and obsessive passion (OP). HP originates from an autonomous internalisation of the activity in identity and leads people to choose to be engaged in the activity that they love. It is expected that HP leads to more adaptive outcomes. Conversely, OP originates from a controlled internalisation in identity that leads people to an uncontrollable urge to engage in an activity (6). A study conducted by Vallerand et al. (6) showed that a higher level of passion led to deliberate practice and better performance. However, both types of passions seemed to be important in this context (7).

In soccer, HP can be found in players who freely choose to participate, with no sense of obligation, while OP can be found in those who have intense feelings about the sport. However, when there is a drive comes from the internal pressure, a feeling that the person ‘has to’ train, play and so forth. Both types of passions can lead to high-performance outcomes (1, 5). Vallerand (6) reported that preliminary evidence reveals that HP may positively contribute to psychological adjustment through its impact on the positive situational affect. OP, on the other hand, does not contribute to psychological adjustment and may even distract from it through right persistence in ill-advised activities, such as gambling. In sports, athletes with OP can experience a higher risk of maladaptive consequences, such as exhaustion, overtraining, and burnout (5). In sports, a number of researches had indicated relevant links between Vallerand’s passion model (including HP and OP) and diverse life activities (6); mastery goals and performance-avoidance goals (6), deliberate practice, performance attainment and subjective well-being (HP positively and OP negatively) (8), coach-athlete relationship (HP positively and OP negatively) (9), and flow (the positive relationship between the flow state and HP, and negative relationship between the flow state and OP). Furthermore, Vallerand’s passion model is also connected to intrinsic-extrinsic motivation (positively correlated with both HP and OP), amotivation (negative correlation with OP but positive correlation with HP), task and ego orientation (OP positively associated with both task orientation and ego orientation) and sports experience (less experienced athletes of 1 to 10 years, have a higher passion for sports activity compared to more experienced athletes over 11 years) (10). These findings revealed that passion matters not only in relation to intrapersonal outcomes (e.g. cognition, affect, psychological well-being, physical health and performance), but also for interpersonal, intergroup and societal consequences. These studies also address the importance of considering the nature of a situation to better predict the consequences of the types of passion in sports (6).

In sports, courage is frequently considered the principal virtue (11, 12). It is universally valued across cultures (13). Despite, there are increasing interest in understanding the concept of courage in sports (14), it has received little scientific attention (11). Thus, Konter and Ng (12) examined the initial development of specific multidimensional sports courage model leading to the development of the sports courage scale-31 (SCS-31) to measure courage (12). There are five subscales in courage, known as mastery (MT), determination (DT), assertiveness (AT), venturesome (VS) and sacrificial behaviour (SB).

Recently, Konter and Beckman’s (15) reviewed the importance of courage in sports and reported that courage is frequently considered a significant factor in sports performance. The researchers addressed courage from a self-regulation perspective. They indicated that courage in soccer should allow players to initiate actions and persistence in the pursuit of goals despite the risk. Thus, courage could act as a self-regulation process refers to the ability to face danger, difficulty, uncertainty, or pain and overcome the fear to maintain the chosen course of action.

Some soccer courage research using the SCS-31 approach indicated significant positive relationship in term of i) coping with stress, confidence and achievement motivation (16, 17); ii) commitment, peaking under pressure and imagery (18); iii) adventurous behaviour, perception of success, unaffectedness by negative referee decisions (19); iv) confidence building, unaffectedness by hostile and supportive spectators (20); v) team captains, proneness to injury, receiving more yellow and red cards, fewer chances of being selected for the national team (21); vi) higher risk-taking and injury proneness (15); and vii) problematic relationships with their coaches and managers (22).

Thus, research findings indicated that the concepts of soccer courage and soccer psychological skills seem to be related. It could be concluded that the positive relationship of better performance and courage in football is mediated by adventurous behaviour, perception of success and reduced irritability, which are components of courage (15). One of the characteristics of courageous players is that to a large degree, they do not conform but stand up for their convictions, even if that means opposition to the coach. Thus, the coach might perceive them as being quarrelsome and hard to coach. There is currently a limited understanding of the importance of the psychological aspect of courage in coaches and officials. In general, some coaches might demand that their players play it safe, stick to the game plan, tactics, and strategies, and carry out the positional demands assigned to them. This conflicts with the courageous player’s basic orientation that is driven by a high degree of autonomy. If they are forced to stick to the coach’s directions, it might interfere with their instinct to take risks and be creative in the game (15).

Courageous players are especially needed when the going gets tough. As the concept of courage suggests, more courageous footballers (with higher MT and AT) appear to be positively motivated by negative events (such as bad referee decisions or being behind at half time). Therefore, courage is needed under negative or aversive conditions to maintain or increase performance. A courageous leader on the pitch may be required to prevent the team from collapsing and turn the game around (23). However, the exact nature of the relationship between passion and courage in soccer performance is still relatively unknown. Therefore, the purpose of this study is to examine the passion possessed by male soccer players in relation to the level of courage, individual criteria (age, body mass index [BMI], years of experience, formal education and playing position), and performance variables.

Methods

Study Design, Sampling Method and Participants

Data collection was held in soccer training premises in Turkey. A cross-sectional study design with convenience sampling method was carried out among soccer players. The soccer players needed to have a minimum experience of one year in competitive soccer playing and were identified by their coaches. Then, the researchers approached the soccer players to be recruited in the study. The data collection was conducted between February and April 2019. All participants volunteered to participate in this study.

Measures

The soccer players responded to the validated PS (24) and SCS-31 (12), together with their demographic and player characteristics.

Demographic form

The demographic form, which includes individual’s characteristics (age, BMI, years of competitive experience and playing position), and participants’ performance (the number years of representation at the national team, performance response in adverse decisions, performance response against weaker and stronger opponents, and level of participation) were collected.

PS

The original PS consisted of two six-item subscales assessing HP and OP. The PS is in Turkish language and has been validated in the Turkey population (24). It is a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (do not agree at all) to 7 (completely agree). The internal consistency coefficients were 0.83 for HP and 0.78 for OP (24).

SCS-31

Konter and Ng (12) initially developed the SCS-31 using the large sample of athletes from individual and team sports (n = 768). The SCS-31 is in Turkish language and has been validated in Turkey population (12). Exploratory factor analyses (EFA) and confırmatory factor analyses (CFA) revealed a 5-factors structure of the SCS-31 that supported factorial validity and reliability of the scale. These factors were labelled as MT, DT, AT, VS and SB. EFA and CFA suggested a good fit: χ2(429) = 584.32, P < 0.01, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.93, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = 0.93, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.03, standardised root mean residual (SRMR) = 0.06. The Cronbach alpha for the subscales were: MT = 0.82, DT = 0.82, AT = 0.72, VS = 0.72 and SB = 0.61. Then, test-retest reliability of the SCS-31 also supported strong reliabity, based on the responses from 75 athletes: MT = 0.77, DT = 0.73, AT = 0.67, VS = 0.74, SB = 0.62, total SCS-31 = 0.82 (12).

Procedure

The study received approval from the first author’s institutional Human Research Ethics Committee and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Data were collected using convenience sampling with voluntary participation. Managers and coaches of the soccer clubs were contacted, and the research project explained. After the authorities granted relevant permissions, the time and place for the data collection were determined. Forms were distributed to the soccer players at their soccer training premises. Participants were encouraged to answer honestly and ensured confidentiality. The data collection took approximately 20 min to complete the set of forms.

Statistical Analysis

Data analysis was conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS version 25.0). Numerical variables were described as mean (M) ± standard deviation (SD), categorical variables were described as frequency (n) and percentage (%). The distributions of all numerical variables were assessed for normality by using histogram and Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. The numerical variables were or approximately to normal distribution; therefore, the collected data were analysed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and independent t-test. If one-way ANOVA of F-statistics test is significant, Bonferroni multiple pairs comparison test was used to local the differences. Assumption of equal variance in independent t-test and one-way ANOVA were checked by using Levene’s test.

Sample Size

Sample size calculation was conducted by using GPower 3.1.9.7. Due to the lack of published study on passion and courage in sport, especially in soccer, therefore we estimate the sample size based on an effect size suggested by a psychologist expert in soccer. As this study is relatively new in the sport of soccer, thus small to medium effect size was suggested by the expert. We believed that the difference of passions’ score between low and high levels of courage would have an effect size of 0.35. Thus, with an alpha value of 0.05, power of 0.80, the sample size needed was 260 (130 per group). After adding 10% of dropout rate, a total sample size of 289 was needed. In the present study, total participants were 278, which fulfilled the minimum sample of 260.

Results

Participants’ Demographic Information

A total of 278 male soccer players aged ranging from 13–38 years (M = 17.42) (SD = 4.36); 233 amateurs, 43 professionals and two did not state their status) with the competitive soccer experiences ranging from 1–28 years (M = 7.51) (SD = 4.23), participated in the study. Table 1 shows the descriptive characteristics of the participants in this study. The mean age of participants was 17 years old and all participants had an average of eight years of soccer experiences.

Table 1.

Descriptive characteristics of participant’s sociodemographic

Variables M (SD) n (%)
Age 17.42 (4.36) 271 (97.5)
 Unstated 7 (2.5)
Soccer experiences (year) 7.51 (4.22) 263 (94.6)
 Unstated 15 (5.4)
BMI 20.81 (2.58) 271 (97.5)
 Unstated 7 (2.5)
National team participation (years)
 Non-selected national 258 (92.8)
 Selected national 10 (3.6)
 Unstated 10 (3.6)
Level of participation
 Amateur 233 (83.8)
 Professional 43 (15.5)
 Unstated 2 (0.7)
Playing position
 Goalkeeper 33 (11.9)
 Full back 59 (21.2)
 Stopper 26 (9.4)
 Libero 10 (3.6)
 Centre midfield 40 (14.4)
 Right-left winger 51 (18.3)
 Right-left midfield 26 (9.4)
 Centre forward 30 (10.8)
 Unstated 3 (1.1)
Passion
 HP 29.42 (6.18)
 OP 23.73 (6.78)
Courage
 MT 20.87 (5.79)
  Low 146 (52.4)
  High 132 (47.5)
 DT 33.45 (5.84)
  Low 150 (54.0)
  High 128 (46.9)
 AT 24.09 (4.22)
  Low 152 (54.7)
  High 126 (45.3)
 VS 13.46 (2.74)
  Low 149 (53.6)
  High 129 (46.4)
 SB 12.94 (2.73)
  Low 152 (54.7)
  High 126 (45.3)
Total courage 104.81 (15.98)
 Low 139 (50.0)
 High 139 (50.0)

Levels of HP and Related Variables

Table 2 shows the differences levels of HP among different courage subscales’ groups (i.e. low, high). Soccer players with a higher level of MT, DT, AT, VS and total courage had significantly higher points of HP than soccer players with the lower level. Besides, there were significant differences between the levels of HP on experiences in soccer playing and soccer level. Professional soccer players have significantly higher points of HP than amateur soccer players.

Table 2.

HP in relation to related variables

Variables M (SD) MD (95% CI) Test statistics (df) P-value
MT −2.82 (−3.51, −0.62) −2.82 (276)t 0.005
 Low 28.44 (6.06)
 High 30.50 (6.15)
DT −2.76 (−4.19, −1.33) −3.80 (276)t < 0.001
 Low 28.15 (6.18)
 High 30.91 (5.86)
AT −2.26 (−3.70, −0.82) −3.08 (276)t 0.002
 Low 28.40 (5.94)
 High 30.67 (6.26)
VS −2.01 (−3.46, −0.57) −2.74 (276)t 0.007
 Low 28.49 (5.78)
 High 30.50 (6.46)
SB −1.44 (−2.89, −0.02) −1.94 (276)t 0.054
 Low 28.77 (5.92)
 High 30.21 (6.40)
Total courage −3.32 (−4.73, −1.92) −4.65 (276)t < 0.001
 Low 27.76 (6.02)
 High 31.08 (5.90)
Experience 2.08 (0.25, 3.93) 2.23 (261)t 0.026
 1–10 years 29.97 (5.61)
 > 10 years 27.87 (6.85)
Soccer level −3.17 (−5.16, −1.18) −3.14 (274)t 0.002
 Amateur 26.71 (7.24)
 Professional 29.88 (5.86)
Performance after unfavourable decision 2.86 (2, 273)f 0.059
 Better 30.50 (6.55)
 Worse 29.85 (5.28)
 No difference 28.52 (6.23)

Notes: t = t-statistic from independent t-test applied; f = F-statistic from one-way ANOVA test applied; MD = mean difference; CI = confidence interval

Levels of OP and Related Variables

Table 3 shows the differences levels of OP among different courage subscales’ groups (i.e. low, high). Soccer players with a higher level of AT and VS had significantly higher points of OP than soccer players with the lower level. Soccer players with experiences in soccer-playing (more than 10 years) and were professional players have significantly higher points of OP than those with less than 10 years of experiences and were amateur players, respectively. There was also a significant difference in OP among the three education levels. Interestingly, soccer players with secondary education background had higher points of OP than those with the university background.

Table 3.

OP in relation to related variables

Variables M (SD) MD (95% CI) Test statistics (df) P-value
MT 0.53 (−1.08, 2.13) 0.65 (276)t 0.520
 Low 23.98 (7.06)
 High 23.45 (6.46)
DT −1.04 (−2.65, 0.56) −1.28 (276)t 0.202
 Low 23.25 (6.78)
 Hgh 24.29 (6.76)
AT −2.15 (−3.74, −0.56) −2.67 (276)t 0.008
 Low 22.75 (6.24)
 High 24.90 (7.22)
VS −1.96 (−3.55, −0.37) −2.43 (276)t 0.016
 Low 22.82 (6.53)
 High 24.78 (6.93)
SB −1.60 (−3.20, 0.01) −1.97 (276)t 0.051
 Low 23.00 (6.38)
 High 24.60 (7.16)
Total courage −1.16 (−2.76, 0.44) −1.43 (276)t 0.154
 Low 23.15 (6.78)
 High 24.31 (6.75)
Experience 2.71 (0.63, 4.78) 2.57 (261)t 0.011
 1–10 years 24.50 (6.35)
 > 10 years 21.80 (7.62) < 0.001
Soccer level −4.32 (−6.49, −2.16) −3.93 (274)t
 Amateur 20.05 (5.91)
 Professional 24.38 (6.57)
Performanc after unfavourable decision 0.149 (2, 273)f 0.861
 Better 23.42 (7.37)
 Worse 24.00 (6.46)
 No difference 23.82 (6.61)

Notes: t = t-statistic from independent t-test applied; f = F-statistic from one-way ANOVA test applied; MD = mean difference; CI = confidence interval

Performance Against Stronger Opponent

Table 4 shows the mean comparison of total courage score between different levels of performance against the stronger opponents (better, worse and no difference). There was significantly different between the mean of MT among the three performance categories. Soccer players with a better performance against stronger opponents have significantly higher points of MT than soccer players with worse performance.

Table 4.

Results of courage and performance against stronger opponent

Courage variables Against stronger opponent categories M (SD) F-statistics (df) P-value
MT Performance 4.90 (2, 274) 0.008*
 1-Better 21.46 (5.47)
 2-Worse 17.31 (4.24)
 3-No difference 20.57 (6.60)
DT Performance 1.48 (2, 274) 0.230
 1-Better 33.34 (5.95)
 2-Worse 31.91 (4.52)
 3-No difference 34.14 (5.58)
AT Performance 1.33 (2, 274) 0.267
 1-Better 24.28 (3.92)
 2-Worse 22.75 (4.67)
 3-No difference 24.25 (4.29)
VS Performance 0.124 (2, 274) 0.883
 1-Better 13.50 (2.61)
 2-Worse 13.24 (2.58)
 3-No difference 13.56 (2.81)
SB Performance 0.256 (2, 274) 0.774
 1-Better 12.90 (2.65)
 2-Worse 12.83 (1.76)
 3-No difference 13.13 (2.87)
Total courage Performance 2.22 (2, 274) 0.110
 1-Better 105.48 (15.12)
 2-Worse 98.04 (11.93)
 3-No difference 105.65 (17.27)

Notes:

*

Bonferroni multiple pairs comparison: Better versus worse (P = 0.007), Better versus no difference (P = 0.701), Worse versus no difference (P = 0.061)

National Team Participation

Selected national soccer players have higher points of DT, AT, VS and total courage than nonselected national soccer players, however, the mean differences are not statistically significant (Table 5).

Table 5.

Results of courage and national team participation

Variables Groups M (SD) MD (95% CI) t-statistics (df) P-value
MT Performance −0.31 (−3.46, 4.07) 0.16 (266) 0.873
 1-Non-national 20.98 (5.84)
 2-National 20.67 (8.15)
DT Performance −3.06 (−6.81, 0.68) −1.61 (266) 0.108
 1-Non-National 33.27 (5.97)
 2-National 36.33 (3.27)
AT Performance −2.65 (−5.34, 0.04) −1.94 (266) 0.053
 1-Non-national 23.92 (4.26)
 2-National 26.57 (3.40)
VS Performance −0.15 (−1.91, 1.60) −0.17 (266) 0.863
 1-Non-national 13.45 (2.77)
 2-National 13.60 (2.62)
SB Performance 0.04 (−1.70, 1.78) 0.04 (266) 0.968
 1-Non-national 12.94 (2.67)
 2-National 12.90 (4.29)
Total courage Performance −5.53 (−15.77, 4.72) −1.06 (266) 0.289
 1-Non-national 104.55 (16.24)
 2-National 110.08 (13.15)

Notes: t-statistic from independent t-test applied; MD = mean difference; CI = confidence interval

Discussion

HP

The results indicated that soccer players with higher points of sports courage factors (MT, DT, AT, VS and total courage) had significantly higher scores of HP than soccer players with lower points of sports courage. This could be important for the development of courage in terms of HP. The soccer player without courage could not generate the desired levels of HP and performance results. Therefore, passionate play is not sufficient for successful performance outcomes. For example, Holt and Dunn (25) studied successful youth elite players at an academy level or playing for their country’s youth national team. All the players referred to their love of the game as their primary motivation. However, in another study, players of the same age who were about to be cut from their club after being labelled unsuccessful, have the same motive and the love of the game as their core motive (26). In other words, both successful and unsuccessful players claimed that they have a similar motive. Therefore, motivation or HP without courage could not generate the desired results. For example, research indicated that characteristics that differentiate those who reach the professional level from those who do not include courage (15). Researchers also indicated some other factors could be related to HP, for example, psychological skills (27), mental toughness (28), coping (29), goal-setting, leadership, responsibility and emotional regulation (30), mastery climate (31), mindfulness (32), flow (10), coach-athlete relationship (9, 33), DT, goal commitment and strive for success, high levels of effort, invest much energy, and mobilising all possible resources to get where they want to be (1). However, the results of the present research showed that soccer courage is a major factor for HP. There is not enough HP research concerning sports courage to generalise indicated results. Future research could include these variables in relation to HP. More research is needed in order to provide conclusive results.

Besides, this also seems to be the case in relation to the years of experience as a function of age, whereby soccer players who have 1–10 years of experience have significantly higher points of HP than soccer players with 10 years of experience and above. This result is supported by Kelecek (24), who stated that professional soccer players have significantly higher HP points than amateur soccer players. Perception of the reward system could influence the motivation and HP of the soccer players. As a result, performance problems could arise. Therefore, more research is needed for more conclusive results in relation to age, years of experiences and level of soccer.

Results showed that the performance of soccer players with higher HP could either be better or worse than players with no performance difference after unfavourable decisions in soccer. Therefore, performance after unfavourable decisions could be more related to other psychological factors than HP (for example, courage). In other words, having performance consistency does not mean having performance deterioration under aversive and disadvantages conditions. Factors such as, DT and goal directed behaviour (1), MT goals (5, 6), 5C traits (i.e. commitment, communication, concentration, confidence and control) (34), anxiety (35), courage (27) could be important. Besides, HP seems to be more related to the performance and accuracy of affective forecast (36) than OP (5). High intrinsic involvement in soccer seems to be linked with passionate participation (1).

Jordet (1) suggested that coaches can stimulate intrinsic, self-determined motivation and HP in their players by i) providing an autonomy-supportive environment; ii) allowing players’ motivational needs to be met in relation to autonomy, relatedness and competence; iii) taking players’ perspectives, desires, feelings and choices seriously; and iv) establishing safe and genuinely sound relationships with players by demonstrating concern for them and by being approachable and trustworthy. However, the present research gives partial support for these contentions and HP in terms of performance. Results showed that courage appears to be more related to performance than HP and OP. More research is needed for more conclusive results.

OP

The results indicated that soccer players with higher points of AT and VS have significantly higher scores of OP compared to soccer players with lower points of AT and VS. Therefore, similar comments could be made for OP as HP. Courage involves OP to the point, but it is a different concept from OP. However, we found not much research related to OP and soccer courage to generalise the results.

It appears that HP is slightly more correlated with soccer courage factors, including MT, DT, AT, VS, SB and total courage. It seems that VS and SB were more correlated with HP than OP. There is not enough research related to soccer courage, OP, and HP to make generalisations. The negative side of courage could be more related to OP, and the positive side of courage could be more related to HP as a self-regulatory and multidimensional concept (15). Extensive research is needed for more conclusive results.

Soccer players with ten years above of experience have significantly higher points of OP than soccer players with 10 years below. Similarly, professional soccer players had significantly higher points of OP than amateur soccer players. In addition, soccer players with secondary school education had significantly higher points of OP than soccer players with university education. All these results are similar to the HP results. Therefore, we can make the same or similar comments as indicated above with the HP.

Sports Courage

Soccer players with a better performance against stronger opponents have significantly higher points of MT compared to soccer players with worse performance. It appears that courage is essential to challenge against superior opponents, as shown by the results of MT. The level of soccer courage could predict performance against stronger opponents. Therefore, soccer courage education could be an essential area that sports psychologist, coaches and club managers should consider. Results indicated that selected national soccer players had generally higher points of DT, AT and total courage than nonselected national soccer players. It seems that DT and AT play major roles to be selected for the national soccer teams. To increase players DT and AT could help their soccer performance and their selection for the national teams.

Some limitations should be noted, such as self-reporting nature of the surveys during the training sessions. Thus, some athletes might over-report or under-report some results. Besides, some soccer players missed some answers, such as their age or soccer experiences. In addition, not all soccer players volunteered to participate in this study. This study was also limited to male soccer players; hence results were limited to one gender. Future study should include female soccer players to examine their sports courage and passion for sports. With that said, the present study was based on a real-world study occurring in a real-world setting.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the study revealed a number of significant differences between the passion scale scores of the soccer players and the level of courage (low and high), and their various individual and performance variables. Soccer players with higher courage had generally higher points of HP and OP. The courage and passion traits of the soccer players seemed to have an essential role in indicated individual and performance variables.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge all the participants for their commitment and cooperation in the process of data collection.

Footnotes

Ethics of Study

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Human Research Ethics Committee and with the Helsinki declaration.

Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Funds

This research was partially funded by the Research University Individual Grant from Universiti Sains Malaysia (1001.PPSP.8012250).

Authors’ Contributions

Conception and design: EK, YCK, GK

Analysis and interpretation of the data: EK, YCK, GK

Drafting of the article: EK, YCK, GK

Critical revision of the article for important intellectual content: EK, YCK, GK

Final approval of the article: EK, YCK, GK

Provision of study materials: EK, YCK, GK

Statistical expertise: EK, YCK

Obtaining of funding: EK, YCK, GK

Administrative, technical, or logistic support: EK, YCK, GK

Collection and assembly of data: EK, YCK, GK

References

  • 1.Jordet G. Psychology of elite soccer players. In: Strudwick T, editor. Soccer science. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2016. pp. 367–388. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Intrinsic motivation and self-dertermination in human behavior. New York: Plenum; 1985. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Deci EL, Ryan RM. The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-DT of behavior. Psychol Inq. 2000;11:227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pain M. Mental interventions. In: Strudwick T, editor. Soccer science. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2016. pp. 389–413. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vallerand RJ. The dualistic model of in passion in sport and exercise. In: Roberts GC, Tressure DC, editors. Advances in motivation in sport and exercise. 3rd ed. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2012. pp. 169–206. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vallerand RJ, Mageau GA, Elliot AJ, Dumais A, Demers M, Rousseau F. Passion and performance attainment in sport. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2008;9(3):373–392. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.05.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Amiot CH, Vallerand RJ, Blanchard CM. Passion and psychological adjustment: a test of the person-environment fit hypothesis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2006;32:220–229. doi: 10.1177/0146167205280250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Vallerand JR, Salvy S, Mageau AG, Elliot JA, Denis DP, Grouzet FME, et al. On the role of passion in performance. J Personal. 2007;75(3):506–533. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00447.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Lafrenière MK, Jowett S, Vallerand RJ, Donahue EG, Lormer R. Passion in sport: on the quality of the coach–athlete relationship. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2008;30(5):541–560. doi: 10.1123/jsep.30.5.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Kelecek S. Master’s diss. Health Sciences Institute, Department of Exercise and Sport Performance, Başkent Universiy; Ankara: 2013. Investigate the role of passion in predicting the flow, sport motivation and goal orientation in athletes. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Corlett J. Virtue lost: courage in sport. In: Hollowchak A, editor. Philosophy in sport. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Konter E, Ng J. Development of sport courage scale. J Hum Kinet. 2012;33:163–172. doi: 10.2478/v10078-012-0055-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dahlsgaard K, Peterson C, Seligman MEP. Shared virtue: the convergence of valued human strengths across culture and history. Rev Gen Psychol. 2005;9(3):203–213. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.203. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Pury CLS, Lopez SJ. In: Courage. 2nd ed. Lopez SJ, Snyder CR, editors. Oxford: handbook of positive psychology; New York: Oxford University Press; 2009. pp. 375–382. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Konter E, Beckmann J. Courage in football. In: Konter E, Beckmann J, Loughead MT, editors. Football psychology: from theory to practice. Abingdon: Routledge; 2019. pp. 38–49. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hidrus AB, Kuan G, Konter E, Kueh YC. Relationship between courage and coping among football player in Kelantan. 14th ISSP World Congress of Sport Psychology; 10–14 June; Sevilla, Spain. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Smith ER, Schutz WR, Smoll LF, Ptacek TJ. Development and validation of a multidimensional measure of sport-specific psychological skills: athletic coping skills ınventory-28. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1995;17(4):378–398. doi: 10.1123/jsep.17.4.379. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Konter E. Psychological skills of football players in relation to level of courage, individual and performance variables. 14th ISSP World Congress of Sport Psychology; 10–14 June; Seville, Spain. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Konter E. Courage of association football players: decision-making at risk. 14th European Congress of Sport Psychology; 14–19 July; Bern, Switzerland. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Konter E. Courage and football performance in the face of negative and positive circumstances. International Sport Sciences Research Congress-USBAK; 10–13 September; Çanakkale, Turkey. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Konter E. Courage profile of adolescent football players in relation to their selected individual variables. Third International Exercise and Sport Psychology Congress; 23–25 October; İstanbul, Turkey: Marmara Üniversitesi, Anadolu Hisarı; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Konter E. Courage profile of professional footballers in relation to selected individual, social and performance variables. National Sport Medicine Congress; 20–22 November; Swiss Otel, Ankara, Turkey. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Apitzsch E. Collective collapse in football. In: Konter E, Beckmann J, Loughead MT, editors. Football psychology: from theory to practice. Abingdon: Routledge; 2019. pp. 38–49. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kelecek S, Aşçı H. Validity and reliability of “Passion Scale” for Turkish college athletes. J Sport Sci. 2013;5(2):80–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Holt NL, Dunn JGH. Towards a grounded theory of the psychosocial competencies and environmental conditions associated with soccer success. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2004;16(3):199–219. doi: 10.1080/10413200490437949. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Holt NL, Michell T. Talent development in English professional soccer. Int J Sport Psychol. 2006;37(2):77–98. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Konter E, Beckmann J, Mallett C. Psychological skills for football players. In: Konter E, Beckmann J, Loughead MT, editors. Football psychology: from theory to practice. London: Routledge; 2019. pp. 179–197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Coulter JT, Thelwell CR. Courage in football. In: Konter E, Beckmann J, Loughead MT, editors. Football psychology: from theory to practice. London: Routledge; 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schellenberg BJI, Gaudreau P, Crocker PRE. Passion and coping: relationships with changes in burnout and goal attainment in collegiate volleyball players. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2013;35(3):270–280. doi: 10.1123/jsep.35.3.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Taylor IM, Bruner MW. The social environment and developmental experiences in elite youth soccer. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2012;13(4):390–396. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.01.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Deighan M. A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the honors program. Department of Applied Human Sciences, University of Prince Edward Island; Charlottetown, PE, Canada: 2018. Exploring the relationship of passion and goal orientation with university athletes’ university experiences. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Amemiya R, Sakairi Y. The effects of passion and mindfulness on the intrinsic motivation of Japanese athletes. Pers Individ Dif. 2019;142(1):132–138. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jowett S, Lafrenière MK, Vallerand RJ. Passion for activities and relationship quality: a dyadic approach. J Soc Pers Relat. 2012;30(6):734–749. doi: 10.1177/0265407512467748. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Steptoe K, King T, Harwood C. The consistent psycho-social development of young football players: implementing the 5C’s as a whicle for interdisciplinary cohesion. In: Konter E, Beckmann J, Loughead MT, editors. Football psychology: from theory to practice. Abingdon: Routledge; 2019. pp. 61–73. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nesti M. Performance mind-set. In: Strudwick T, editor. Soccer science. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics; 2016. pp. 415–430. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Verner-Filion J, Lafrenière MK, Vallerand RJ. On the accuracy of affective forecasting: the moderating role of passion. Pers Individ Dif. 2012;52(7):849–854. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Malaysian Journal of Medical Sciences : MJMS are provided here courtesy of School of Medical Sciences, Universiti Sains Malaysia

RESOURCES