Skip to main content
BMC Medical Genomics logoLink to BMC Medical Genomics
. 2020 Aug 26;13:121. doi: 10.1186/s12920-020-00769-y

Association of blood pressure with cognitive function at midlife: a Mendelian randomization study

Daokun Sun 1, Emy A Thomas 1, Lenore J Launer 2, Stephen Sidney 3, Kristine Yaffe 4, Myriam Fornage 1,5,
PMCID: PMC7448985  PMID: 32847530

Abstract

Background

Whether high blood pressure has a causal effect on cognitive function as early as middle age is unclear. We investigated whether high blood pressure (BP) causally impairs cognitive function at midlife using Mendelian Randomization (MR).

Methods

We applied a two-sample MR approach to investigate the causal relationship between BP and midlife cognitive performance measured by the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), and Stroop Interference test. We used a total of 109 genetic polymorphisms with established associations with BP as instrumental variables and estimated gene-cognitive function association in 1369 middle-aged adults (Mean age (SD): 50.8 (3.3), 54.0% women) from the CARDIA study.

Results

A 10 mmHg increment in genetically-predicted systolic, diastolic, or pulse pressure was associated with a 4.9 to 7.7-point lower DSST score (P = 0.002, SBP; P = 0.005, DBP and P = 0.008, PP), while a 10 mmHg increment in genetically-predicted SBP was associated with a 0.7 point lower RAVLT and a 2.3 point higher Stroop (P = 0.046 and 0.011, respectively).

Conclusions

This MR analysis shows that high BP, especially SBP, is causally associated with poorer processing speed, verbal memory, and executive function during midlife. These findings emphasize the need for further investigation of the role and mechanisms of BP dysregulation on cognitive health in middle age and perhaps, more broadly, across the lifespan.

Keywords: Mendelian randomization, Blood pressure, Cognitive disorders, Risk factors, Dementia

Background

Hypertension is one of the long-established modifiable risk factors for age-related dementia [1]. Several studies have reported that high blood pressure (BP) developed by middle age is associated with cognitive decline [2, 3] and with global and domain-specific cognitive impairment in late life [4, 5]. However, other studies have reported non-monotonic or null relationships between high BP and late-life cognitive function [6, 7]. The inconsistency of results may be due to differences in study populations, study design, and methodological aspects. Reverse causation may also play a role. Randomized clinical trials (RCTs) that examine the long-term effects of elevated blood pressure on cognitive ability would represent a gold standard but are costly and difficult to conduct since only randomization based on anti-hypertensive treatment rather than on BP level can be achieved. Early clinical trials designed to evaluate the cognitive benefits of antihypertensive treatment showed conflicting results, most of which were not in favor of beneficial effects [8]. The more recent SPRINT MIND trial reported that, in older adults at increased risk of cardiovascular disease, intensive systolic BP control reduced the risk of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as well as the combined occurrence of MCI or dementia [9]. Whether the possible benefits of BP control in older adults extend earlier in life is unknown.

We employed a two-sample MR approach to expand existing knowledge about the effect of high BP burden on cognitive health during middle age [10], a pivotal period in the life course when cognitive function begins to decline among healthy adults [11].

Methods

Study population

Participants were from the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) study, a prospective multi-center cohort study investigating the natural history and etiology of cardiovascular disease in 2637 African-Americans and 2478 European-Americans aged 18–30 years at the time of initial examination in 1985–1986 [12]. Participants were recruited from the community in Birmingham, AL; from selected census tracts in Chicago, IL and Minneapolis, MN; and from the Kaiser-Permanente health plan membership in Oakland, CA. The sample was approximately balanced with respect to race, age, sex, and education groups. Participants have completed nine sequential examinations every 2 to 5 years for 30 years. For this study, we used 1369 subjects of European ancestry who had genome-wide genotype data and phenotypic data for three cognitive tests at Year 25 examination.

Cognitive assessment

Three standardized tests in CARDIA were administered at Year 25 examination (2010–2011) to assess major aspects of cognitive function by trained and certified technicians following an established protocol as described elsewhere [13] and in the supplemental materials (Additional File 1). Briefly, the battery of tests included the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (score range: 0–15), a measure of verbal learning and memory [14]; the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) (Part D) (score range: 0–133), a measure primarily of psychomotor speed; and the Stroop Interference Test (score range: 1–160), a measure of executive function [15]. A higher score in DSST and RAVLT indicates better cognition in the corresponding domain whereas that in the Stroop Interference test reflects poor cognitive performance.

Instrumental variables and association analysis

A total of 152 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) previously identified in one of the largest GWAS of BP in individuals of European descent [16] were selected for analysis. These SNPS were selected based on their genome-wide significant association with blood pressure traits (P < 5 × 10− 8) and evidence of independent replication in multiple studies. From the summary statistics, we obtained the association estimate of each SNP with systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and pulse pressure (PP) respectively, which had been replicated among 140,886 European participants. Of these, 145 were available in CARDIA. To minimize pleiotropic effects [17], we removed 8 SNPs which overlapped or were in high linkage disequilibrium (LD) (r2 > 0.8) with SNPs influencing either known BP risk factors (body mass index (BMI), smoking, and type 2 diabetes), which can serve as confounders; or education, which may bias the gene-cognition association [18]. We also pruned SNPs that were in LD with one another at r2 greater than 0.001 within a 10 kb distance. Thus, we used 109 SNPs independently and exclusively associated with BP at genome-wide significance level as instrumental variables (IVs) (99 for SBP, 98 for DBP, and 98 for PP, Table S1). We estimated the allelic effect of each SNP on measures of cognitive performance in 1369 CARDIA participants adjusting for age, sex, and study site. We aligned the direction of SNP-cognitive function association with that in the BP GWAS.

Statistical analysis

We implemented a multivariable two-sample Mendelian Randomization (MR) approach using the TwoSampleMR R package [19] to explore the causal association of genetically-predicted BPs with DSST, RAVLT, and Stroop test’s performance. A summary of our analytical approach is shown in Figure S1 (Additional file 1). As primary analysis, we employed a standard inverse-variance weighted (IVW) method to derive the causal estimates for each variant, which were combined by random-effects meta-analysis. We carried out detection of potentially invalid or influential instruments using radial plots in RadialMR [20]. We then repeated IVW MR analyses after removing the outliers. We applied additional MR methods including the weighted median method [21], MR-Egger regression [22], and the contamination mixture MR [23] as sensitivity analyses for robust inference.

To examine the effect of potential unbalanced pleiotropy on causal estimates, we employed the MR-Egger intercept test [24], the Mendelian Randomization Pleiotropy RESidual Sum and Outlier (MRPRESSO) test [25] and constructed funnel plots [26]. We evaluated the assumption of “NO Measurement Error” (NOME) by Bowden I2 statistics. We further estimated the heterogeneity of selected IVs by Cochran’s Q test [27]. To preclude the possibility that the causal inference was driven by any individual variant, we performed a leave-one-out analysis for the IVW and MR-Egger methods.

Because of the overlap in genetic instruments for the 3 BP measures (Table S1, Additional file 1) and correlations among cognitive tests, we did not adjust for multiple testing. A P-value of association less than 0.05 is considered significant.

Results

The CARDIA participants were in their fifties (Mean [range] age: 51 [42–57] years) at Year 25. They were well educated but were slightly overweight. One-quarter of them were hypertensive, but less than one fifth were taking anti-hypertension medication. The prevalence of type 2 diabetes and current smoking were low (Table 1).

Table 1.

Characteristics of 1369 CARDIA participants with cognitive function at Year 25

Variable Value
Sample size, N 1369
Age (SD) (year) 50.8 (3.3)
Female (%) 739 (54.0)
Center (%)
 Birmingham, AL 273 (19.9)
 Chicago, IL 320 (23.4)
 Minneapolis, MN 443 (32.4)
 Oakland, CA 333 (24.3)
At least college education (%) 917 (67.0)
BMI (SD) (kg/m2) 28.3 (6.1)
Type2 Diabetes (%) 124 (9.1)
Current Smoking (%) 165 (12.1)
Systolic Blood Pressure (SD) (mmHg) 114.4 (13.7)
Diastolic Blood Pressure (SD) (mmHg) 70.8 (10.1)
Pulse Pressure (SD) (mmHg) 43.6 (7.6)
Hypertension (%) 341 (24.9)
HBP Rx use (%) 228 (16.7)
DSST (Mean (SD): Min-Max) 75.0 (14.9): 21–125
RAVLT (Mean (SD): Min-Max) 9.4 (3.1): 0–15
Stroop (Mean (SD): Min-Max) 19.3 (8.0): 4–71

Abbreviations: CARDIA, the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study; SD Standard deviation, BMI Body mass index (calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared), HBP Rx Anti-hypertensive medication, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Stroop The Stroop Interference test

The most notable association by the standard IVW method was found between midlife genetically-predicted BP level and DSST score. For each unit (mm Hg) increment in genetically-predicted BP level, there was a 0.49 to 0.67-point lower DSST score. There was no significant association between genetically-predicted BPs and performance on the RAVLT or Stroop Interference test. The direction of association was however consistent among the 3 cognitive tests (Table 2).

Table 2.

Causal association between blood pressure and midlife cognitive function inferred by inverse-variance weighted MR analysis

DSST RAVLT Stroop
SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP
IVW
Estimatea − 0.49 −0.66 − 0.52 − 0.04 − 0.04 − 0.07 0.15 0.18 0.16
95% CI −0.79, − 0.18 − 1.20, − 0.13 −0.99, − 0.04 −0.11, 0.03 − 0.16, 0.08 −0.18, 0.03 0.03, 0.32 −0.12, 0.48 − 0.11, 0.43
P-value 0.002 0.014 0.033 0.242 0.481 0.164 0.100 0.236 0.251
IVW after removing outliers
Estimatea − 0.49 −0.77 − 0.67 − 0.07 − 0.05 −0.07 0.23 0.24 0.27
95% CI −0.80, − 0.17 −1.03, − 0.23 − 1.16, − 0.18 − 0.14, 0.00 − 0.16, 0.06 −0.17, 0.03 0.05, 0.41 −0.07, 0.54 − 0.01, 0.56
P-value 0.002 0.005 0.008 0.046 0.37 0.165 0.011 0.126 0.058

Abbreviations: IVW Inverse-variance weighted, MR Mendelian randomization, SBP Systolic Blood Pressure, DBP Diastolic Blood Pressure, PP Pulse Pressure, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Stroop The Stroop Interference test

a Represents the change in cognitive test score per 1-unit (mmHg) change in BP

We conducted analyses to detect potential invalid and/or influential instruments. Radial plots indicated the presence of outlier instruments (Figure S2 in Additional file 1). After removing these outliers, the causal associations between genetically-predicted BPs and midlife cognitive function were generally strengthened (Table 2). Notably, genetically-predicted SBP was now also associated with RAVLT and Stroop performance. Each unit (mm Hg) increment in genetically-predicted SBP level was associated with a 0.07-point lower RAVLT score and a 0.23-point higher Stroop Interference test score (Table 2).

The IVW method is optimally efficient when all genetic variants are valid IVs but is biased when one or more genetic variants are invalid IVs. Therefore, we performed sensitivity analyses using additional MR methods that vary in their robustness to the presence of invalid IVs (Tables 3 and 4). The weighted median method indicated a similar or stronger magnitude of causal associations, although with larger confidence intervals, while the MR-Egger method was insufficiently powered. Estimates from the contamination mixture MR method were similar to those of the IVW (Tables 3 and 4). The Bowden I2 statistics across the three BP exposures were all high, around 0.96, indicating a high strength of IVs and a less than moderate degree of NOME assumption violation (Tables 3 and 4). The mean F-statistics, another measure of IV strength, were 29.4 for SBP, 32.8 for DBP, and 26.0 for PP, indicating that our causal estimates are not likely to be biased by weak IVs.

Table 3.

Sensitivity analyses for robust estimation of the causal association between blood pressure and midlife cognitive function before removing outliers

DSST RAVLT Stroop
SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP
Weighted Median
Estimate (SD) −0.32 (0.23) − 0.58 (0.40) − 0.30 (0.42) − 0.05 (0.05) −0.06 (0.08) 0.08 (0.09) 0.19 (0.13) 0.26 (0.24) 0.08 (0.24)
P-value 0.154 0.15 0.462 0.341 0.484 0.352 0.146 0.269 0.731
MR Egger Regression
Estimate (SD) −0.53 (0.33) − 0.82 (0.54) 0.19 (0.40) 0.05 (0.07) −0.12 (0.12) 0.01 (0.09) 0.09 (0.18) −0.12 (0.30) −0.21 (0.23)
P-value 0.106 0.133 0.788 0.487 0.318 0.91 0.922 0.702 0.731
ConMix
Estimate −0.38 −0.54 −0.44 − 0.03 −0.05 − 0.01 0.17 0.28 0.18
95% CI (−0.71, − 0.07) (−1.03, − 0.04) (−0.93, 0.01) (− 0.08, 0.04) (− 0.15, 0.07) (−0.12, 0.08) (0.00, 0.33) (−0.04, 0.56) (− 0.07, 0.45)
I2 statistic
P-value 0.97 0.97 0.96 0.97 0.97 0.96 0.97 0.97 0.96
MRPRESSO
P-value 0.83 0.49 0.61 0.06 0.09 0.11 0.51 0.49 0.36
Cochran Statistic
Q Statistics 85.93 126.23 91.71 119.19 116.26 113.72 97.76 97.69 101.15
P-value 0.803 0.648 0.633 0.072 0.089 0.118 0.488 0.461 0.366
Egger Intercept Test
Estimate (SD) 0.02 (0.14) 0.05 (0.14) −0.22 (0.11) −0.05 (0.03) 0.02 (0.03) −0.03 (0.03) 0.06 (0.08) 0.09 (0.08) 0.13 (0.06)
P-value 0.874 0.74 0.055 0.15 0.453 0.236 0.426 0.259 0.045

Abbreviations: MR Mendelian randomization, SBP Systolic Blood Pressure, DBP Diastolic Blood Pressure, PP Pulse Pressure, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Stroop The Stroop Interference test, MRPRESSO Mendelian Randomization Pleiotropy RESidual Sum and Outlier, conMIX Contamination mixture, SD Standard deviation, CI Confidence Interval

Table 4.

Sensitivity analyses for robust estimation of the causal association between blood pressure and midlife cognitive function after removing outliers

DSST RAVLT Stroop
SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP SBP DBP PP
Weighted Median
Estimate (SD)

−0.33

(0.22)

−0.86

(0.39)

− 0.31

(0.42)

−0.09

(0.05)

− 0.06

(0.09)

0.07

(0.09)

0.21

(0.13)

0.26

(0.23)

0.25

(0.23)

P-value 0.141 0.027 0.451 0.066 0.512 0.442 0.114 0.252 0.272
MR Egger Regression
Estimate (SD)

−0.66

(0.33)

−0.88

(0.55)

−0.26

(0.42)

− 0.05

(0.07)

−0.13

(0.12)

0.04

(0.09)

−0.08

(0.19)

− 0.24

(0.30)

−0.06

(0.24)

P-value 0.048 0.111 0.526 0.483 0.269 0.643 0.656 0.437 0.272
ConMix
Estimate −0.48 −0.79 −0.65 − 0.07 −0.06 − 0.07 0.2 0.25 0.28
95% CI (−0.80, − 0.15) (−1.36, − 0.24) (−1.14, −.17) (−0.13, 0.00) (− 0.19, 0.07) (− 0.17, 0.02) (0.02, 0.37) (− 0.13, 0.57) (0.00, 0.55)
I2 statistic
P-value 0.97 0.97 0.96 0.96 0.97 0.96 0.97 0.97 0.96
MRPRESSO
P-value 0.97 0.94 0.96 0.95 0.90 0.89 0.93 0.92 0.96
Cochran Statistic
Q Statistics 71.01 97.24 71.33 70.16 75.22 74.98 71.65 74.18 67.53
P-value 0.97 0.47 0.96 0.95 0.88 0.90 0.95 0.93 0.96
Egger Intercept Test
Estimate (SD)

0.09

(0.14)

0.04

(0.14)

−0.14

(0.12)

−0.01

(0.03)

0.02

(0.03)

−0.04

(0.02)

0.16

(0.08)

0.14

(0.08)

0.12

(0.07)

P-value 0.553 0.805 0.222 0.766 0.444 0.105 0.057 0.073 0.082
nSNPS 96 93 94 92 92 93 94 94 91

Abbreviations: MR Mendelian randomization, SBP Systolic Blood Pressure, DBP Diastolic Blood Pressure, PP Pulse Pressure, DSST Digit Symbol Substitution Test, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Stroop The Stroop Interference test, MRPRESSO Mendelian Randomization Pleiotropy RESidual Sum and Outlier, conMIX Contamination mixture, SD Standard deviation, CI Confidence Interval

We also conducted sensitivity analyses to investigate potential pleiotropy, which could invalidate MR assumptions and bias causal estimates. Indeed, a key assumption of MR is that the IVs have an effect on the outcome (cognition) only via the exposure (BP). Using the MR-Egger intercept test, we did not detect evidence of significant directional pleiotropy in any of the analyses performed. Consistently, the MR-PRESSO global test also did not indicate any evidence of directional pleiotropy (Tables 3 and 4). We also plotted IV strength against IV estimates in funnel plots (Figure S3) and confirmed the absence of pleiotropy. All of these plots showed a symmetrical distribution indicating that pleiotropic effects were well balanced across all the genetic variants. Cochran Q statistics also confirmed that there was no evidence of heterogeneity in causal estimates (Tables 3 and 4).

To further preclude the possibility that the causal inference was driven by any individual variant, we performed additional sensitivity analyses. Leave-one-out results confirmed that the causal estimates were not driven by the influence of any individual SNP (Figures S4 and S5). Indeed, while omitting some variants placed heavier burden on causal estimates than others, these estimates were, both, stable and consistent in direction (did not cross the 0 line).

Discussion

Mendelian randomization holds substantial promise for causal inference from observational data. This approach mimics the random allocation in RCTs by using risk factor-associated genetic variants as IVs. It is based on the principle that the distribution of those genetic variants in the population is randomly assigned at meiosis, independent of potential confounders and not susceptible to reverse causation or other biases [17]. In this study, we inferred the causal effect of high BP on midlife cognitive function through a two-sample MR method and further sensitivity analyses. We report that higher genetically-predicted BP, especially SBP, is causally associated with lower midlife processing speed and lower executive function and verbal memory but the estimated effects of genetically-predicted BP on midlife cognitive function is small.

A recent MR study from the UK Biobank and including participants aged 40 to 70 years observed no detrimental effect of genetically-predicted SBP on cognitive ability measured by a verbal-numerical reasoning test [28]. Given the differences in study design, including cognitive assessment and participant characteristics, it is possible that one does not contradict the other. Indeed, compared to our study, the UK Biobank study included a large range in population age. Including older individuals into the analysis has the potential to introduce biases owing to the effect of high blood pressure on mortality [29, 30]. Moreover, the relationship between BP and cognitive function may vary across the lifespan. For example, a MR study recently reported that higher genetically-predicted SBP significantly lowered the risk of Alzheimer’s disease [31].

The specific mechanisms that underlie the causal association between high BP and poor processing speed are not known. One possible mechanism may involve the known pathologies of hypertension-induced brain vascular injury, most notably to the cerebral small vessels. Indeed, long-term hypertension is known to cause vascular hypertrophy and microvascular remodeling, which result in regional cerebral blood flow dysfunction and lead to white matter disease and neuronal loss [32]. Indeed, previous studies have suggested that elevated BP is a major risk factor for several magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) markers of cerebral small vessel disease (SVDs) [33]. For example, elevated SBP is associated with brain atrophy, reduced gray matter volume and white matter hyperintensities (WMH) [34], while DBP is associated with brain atrophy and WMH [35]. It has been shown that the presence of SVD appears to align with a compromised cognitive profile of early-impaired processing speed [36, 37]. Elevated BP may also affect brain anatomical connectivity. Indeed, regional white matter integrity is lower among individuals with higher BP, regardless of hypertension status [38]. In a recent study of cognitively healthy older adults, the relationship between SBP and poorer processing speed appeared to be mediated by functional connectivity of the right superior temporal gyrus within the ventral attention network (VAN) [39]. This is consistent with a previous report showing that variation in structural organization within the frontoparietal system, which comprises the VAN, is associated with differences in attentional functions, including visual short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and spatial bias [40].

Whether antihypertensive treatment can reverse the pathological process of vascular damage, restore cerebral function, and improve cognitive ability remains inconclusive. On one hand, it appears that lowering BP is helpful to rehabilitate some of the vascular functions impaired by BP elevation. For instance, a previous clinical trial among subjects older than 70 suggested that right-shifted cerebral autoregulation curve induced by hypertension can be back-shifted towards normal through intensive BP lowering [41]. On the other hand, however, most clinical trials have failed to identify significant benefits of antihypertensive therapy in improving cognitive fitness [8]. The majority of these studies were carried out over a short period of time and limited to aged populations on average between 62 to 83 years of age. Elderly individuals may be exposed to the negative effects of hypertension for a longer time and may have more severe vascular injury due to hypertension than middle-aged adults, and, thus, may be more resistant to the benefits of BP lowering. Moreover, the effects of antihypertensive treatment on cognitive function or risk of dementia have been shown to differ across therapeutic classes [42], suggesting that the neuroprotective effects of antihypertensive therapy may extend beyond BP lowering.

Strengths of our study include the use of a multi-SNP genetic instrument with well-established associations with BP. The largest BP GWAS at the time of this analysis reported genetic association for 267 SNPs in total, 115 of which were newly validated [16]. We opted for the smaller set of SNPs, which were previously identified with stronger evidence of validity, although fewer IVs theoretically reduce statistical power [43]. The use of summary data for gene-BP association from a large GWAS analysis also improved power and precision of our study. However, several limitations are acknowledged. First, the genetic variants for the various BP measures almost fully overlapped, which makes it difficult to discriminate possible unique causal relationships between the specific components of BP and cognitive function. Excluding genetic variants that overlap among BP traits would likely result in a loss of strength of our MR analyses. Second, possible effects of unmeasured confounders are a recognized limitation of the MR approach. In particular, the possible confounding effects of antihypertensive medication use could not be accounted for in our analyses because the estimates of allelic effects on BP were obtained from a GWAS that corrected for medication use by adding 15 and 10 mmHg to the SBP and DBP values, respectively. If genetically predicted higher BP is also associated with a higher probability of being on antihypertensive medication as previously observed [31], the true causal association between BP and cognitive function is likely underestimated. Moreover, the GWAS summary statistics used in this analysis adjusted for BMI and, thus, can potentially yield biased estimates. However, a recent study using both simulations and empirical data from the UK Biobank showed that using adjusted and corrected GWAS in MR analysis is unlikely to make a large difference to causal estimates [44]. Third, these analyses were conducted in persons of European ancestry. Whether the results are generalizable to diverse populations remains to be examined. Expanding efforts for genetic discoveries of BP in African-Americans and Hispanics/Latinos is required to meet this need. Finally, although this study provided evidence for a causal relationship between SBP and processing speed, it does not shed light on the possible biological mechanisms involved.

Conclusion

By providing support for a causal relationship between BP and cognitive health in middle age, our MR study underscores the need for further investigations of the role and mechanisms of BP dysfunction on cognitive health across the lifespan, which may inform on early intervention and timely treatment of hypertension to maintain brain health.

Supplementary information

12920_2020_769_MOESM1_ESM.docx (1.1MB, docx)

Additional file 1: Cognitive assessment in CARDIA at Year 25 examination (2010–2011); Table S1. SNPs and SNP-exposure effects used in the MR analyses.; Figure S1. Summary of the MR approach used in the study. Figure S2. Radial plot for detection of outliers. Figure S3. Funnel plot of blood pressure effect on cognitive function. Figure S4. Leave-one-out result of IVW estimates. Figure S5. Leave-one-out result of MR-Egger estimates.

Acknowledgments

The Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults Study (CARDIA) is conducted and supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) in collaboration with the University of Alabama at Birmingham (HHSN268201300025I & HHSN268201300026I), Northwestern University (HHSN268201300027I), University of Minnesota (HHSN268201300028I), Kaiser Foundation Research Institute (HHSN268201300029I), and Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine (HHSN268200900041I). CARDIA is also partially supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging (NIA) and an intra-agency agreement between NIA and NHLBI (AG0005). Genotype data were obtained as part of the Gene Environment Association Studies (GENEVA) through grants U01-HG004729, U01-HG04424, and U01-HG004446 from the National Human Genome Research Institute. This manuscript has been reviewed by CARDIA for scientific content.

Abbreviations

BP

Blood Pressure

BMI

Body Mass Index

CARDIA

Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults

DBP

Diastolic Blood Pressure

DSST

Digit Symbol Substitution Test

GWAS

Genome-wide Association Study

IV

Instrumental Variable

IVW

Inverse-Variance Weighted

LD

Linkage Disequilibrium

MCI

Mild Cognitive Impairment

MR

Mendelian Randomization

NOME

NO Measurement Error

PP

Pulse Pressure

RAVLT

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

RCT

Randomized Controlled Trial

SBP

Systolic Blood Pressure

SD

Standard Deviation

SNP

Single Nucleotide Polymorphism

SVD

Cerebral Small Vessel Disease

VAN

Ventral Attention Network

WMH

White Matter Hyperintensities

Authors’ contributions

DS performed the statistical analyses, interpreted the data, and was a major contributor in writing the manuscript; EAT performed the statistical analyses; LJL, SS, and KY supported the collection of the phenotypic data; MF conceived and designed the study, supported the collection of the genetic data, and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health R01 HL122658 and NS087541.

Availability of data and materials

CARDIA data are available through the National Center for Biotechnology Information database of Genotypes and Phenotypes (dbGaP). More information is provided at https://www.cardia.dopm.uab.edu/study-information/genetic-data/cardia-genetic-data-in-dbgap-ncbi.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Written informed consent was obtained from participant at each examination and all study protocols were approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, the University of Alabama at Birmingham, the University of Minnesota, Northwestern University, and the Kaiser Permanente Division of Research.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s12920-020-00769-y.

References

  • 1.Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Burns A, Cohen-Mansfield J, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390(10113):2673–2734. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gottesman RF, Schneider AL, Albert M, Alonso A, Bandeen-Roche K, Coker L, Coresh J, Knopman D, Power MC, Rawlings A, et al. Midlife hypertension and 20-year cognitive change: the atherosclerosis risk in communities neurocognitive study. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(10):1218–1227. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.1646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Debette S, Seshadri S, Beiser A, Au R, Himali JJ, Palumbo C, Wolf PA, DeCarli C. Midlife vascular risk factor exposure accelerates structural brain aging and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2011;77(5):461–468. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318227b227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Elias MF, Wolf PA, D'Agostino RB, Cobb J, White LR. Untreated blood pressure level is inversely related to cognitive functioning: the Framingham study. Am J Epidemiol. 1993;138(6):353–364. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Launer LJ, Masaki K, Petrovitch H, Foley D, Havlik RJ. The association between midlife blood pressure levels and late-life cognitive function. The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Jama. 1995;274(23):1846–1851. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Bennett DA, Bienias JL, Wilson RS, Morris MC, Evans DA. Blood pressure and late-life cognitive function change: a biracial longitudinal population study. Neurology. 2004;62(11):2021–2024. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000129258.93137.4b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Taylor C, Tillin T, Chaturvedi N, Dewey M, Ferri CP, Hughes A, Prince M, Richards M, Shah A, Stewart R. Midlife hypertensive status and cognitive function 20 years later: the Southall and Brent revisited study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013;61(9):1489–1498. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Iadecola C, Yaffe K, Biller J, Bratzke LC, Faraci FM, Gorelick PB, Gulati M, Kamel H, Knopman DS, Launer LJ, et al. Impact of hypertension on cognitive function: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Hypertension. 2016;68(6):e67–e94. doi: 10.1161/HYP.0000000000000053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sprint Mind Investigators for the SPRINT Research Group. Williamson JD, Pajewski NM, Auchus AP, Bryan RN, Chelune G, Cheung AK, Cleveland ML, Coker LH, Crowe MG, et al. Effect of intensive vs standard blood pressure control on probable dementia: a randomized clinical trial. Jama. 2019;321(6):553–561. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.21442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Yaffe K, Vittinghoff E, Pletcher MJ, Hoang TD, Launer LJ, Whitmer R, Coker LH, Sidney S. Early adult to midlife cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation. 2014;129(15):1560–1567. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.113.004798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, Ferrie JE, Dugravot A. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Bmj. 2012;344:d7622. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d7622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Friedman GD, Cutter GR, Donahue RP, Hughes GH, Hulley SB, Jacobs DR, Jr, Liu K, Savage PJ. CARDIA: study design, recruitment, and some characteristics of the examined subjects. J Clin Epidemiol. 1988;41(11):1105–1116. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(88)90080-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Launer LJ, Miller ME, Williamson JD, Lazar RM, Gerstein HC, Murray AM, Sullivan M, Horowitz KR, Ding J, Marcovina S, et al. Effects of intensive glucose lowering on brain structure and function in people with type 2 diabetes (ACCORD MIND): a randomised open-label substudy. Lancet Neurol. 2011;10(11):969–977. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70188-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rosenberg SJ, Ryan JJ, Prifitera A. Rey auditory-verbal learning test performance of patients with and without memory impairment. J Clin Psychol. 1984;40(3):785–787. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198405)40:3<785::aid-jclp2270400325>3.0.co;2-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol. 1935;18(6):643. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Warren HR, Evangelou E, Cabrera CP, Gao H, Ren M, Mifsud B, Ntalla I, Surendran P, Liu C, Cook JP, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies novel blood pressure loci and offers biological insights into cardiovascular risk. Nat Genet. 2017;49(3):403–415. doi: 10.1038/ng.3768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Davey Smith G, Hemani G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum Mol Genet. 2014;23(R1):R89–R98. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddu328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Buniello A, MacArthur JAL, Cerezo M, Harris LW, Hayhurst J, Malangone C, McMahon A, Morales J, Mountjoy E, Sollis E, et al. The NHGRI-EBI GWAS catalog of published genome-wide association studies, targeted arrays and summary statistics 2019. Nucleic Acids Res. 2019;47(D1):D1005–D1012. doi: 10.1093/nar/gky1120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hemani G, Zheng J, Elsworth B, Wade KH, Haberland V, Baird D, Laurin C, Burgess S, Bowden J, Langdon R, et al. The MR-base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. Elife. 2018;7:e34408. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bowden J, Spiller W, Del Greco MF, Sheehan N, Thompson J, Minelli C, Davey Smith G. Improving the visualization, interpretation and analysis of two-sample summary data Mendelian randomization via the radial plot and radial regression. Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(6):2100. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyy265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Haycock PC, Burgess S. Consistent estimation in Mendelian randomization with some invalid instruments using a weighted median estimator. Genet Epidemiol. 2016;40(4):304–314. doi: 10.1002/gepi.21965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bowden J, Davey Smith G, Burgess S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through egger regression. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(2):512–525. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyv080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Burgess S, Foley CN, Allara E, Staley JR, Howson JMM. A robust and efficient method for Mendelian randomization with hundreds of genetic variants. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):376. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-14156-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Burgess S, Thompson SG. Interpreting findings from Mendelian randomization using the MR-egger method. Eur J Epidemiol. 2017;32(5):377–389. doi: 10.1007/s10654-017-0255-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Verbanck M, Chen CY, Neale B, Do R. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat Genet. 2018;50(5):693–698. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0099-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Burgess S, Bowden J, Dudbridge F, Thompson SG. ARXIV. eprint arXiv:1606.03729. 2016. Robust instrumental variable methods using multiple candidate instruments with application to Mendelian randomization. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Burgess S, Bowden J, Fall T, Ingelsson E, Thompson SG. Sensitivity analyses for robust causal inference from Mendelian randomization analyses with multiple genetic variants. Epidemiology. 2017;28(1):30–42. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hagenaars SP, Gale CR, Deary IJ, Harris SE. Cognitive ability and physical health: a Mendelian randomization study. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):2651. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02837-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Boef AG, le Cessie S, Dekkers OM. Mendelian randomization studies in the elderly. Epidemiology. 2015;26(2):e15–e16. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Vansteelandt S, Dukes O, Martinussen T. Survivor bias in Mendelian randomization analysis. Biostatistics. 2018;19(4):426–443. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxx050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ostergaard SD, Mukherjee S, Sharp SJ, Proitsi P, Lotta LA, Day F, Perry JR, Boehme KL, Walter S, Kauwe JS, et al. Associations between Potentially Modifiable Risk Factors and Alzheimer Disease: A Mendelian Randomization Study. PLoS Med. 2015;12(6):e1001841. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pantoni L, Simoni M. Pathophysiology of cerebral small vessels in vascular cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2003;15(Suppl 1):59–65. doi: 10.1017/S1041610203008974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Smith EE, Beaudin AE. New insights into cerebral small vessel disease and vascular cognitive impairment from MRI. Curr Opin Neurol. 2018;31(1):36–43. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Maillard P, Seshadri S, Beiser A, Himali JJ, Au R, Fletcher E, Carmichael O, Wolf PA, DeCarli C. Effects of systolic blood pressure on white-matter integrity in young adults in the Framingham heart study: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(12):1039–1047. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70241-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.McNeil CJ, Myint PK, Sandu AL, Potter JF, Staff R. Whalley LJ, Murray AD. Increased diastolic blood pressure is associated with MRI biomarkers of dementia-related brain pathology in normative ageing. Age Ageing. 2018;47(1):95–100. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afx102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Prins ND, van Dijk EJ, den Heijer T, Vermeer SE, Jolles J, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Cerebral small-vessel disease and decline in information processing speed, executive function and memory. Brain. 2005;128(Pt 9):2034–2041. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lawrence AJ, Patel B, Morris RG, MacKinnon AD, Rich PM, Barrick TR, Markus HS. Mechanisms of cognitive impairment in cerebral small vessel disease: multimodal MRI results from the St George's cognition and neuroimaging in stroke (SCANS) study. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e61014. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Salat DH, Williams VJ, Leritz EC, Schnyer DM, Rudolph JL, Lipsitz LA, McGlinchey RE, Milberg WP. Inter-individual variation in blood pressure is associated with regional white matter integrity in generally healthy older adults. NeuroImage. 2012;59(1):181–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wong NML, Ma EP, Lee TMC. The integrity of the Corpus callosum mitigates the impact of blood pressure on the ventral attention network and information processing speed in healthy adults. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:108. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chechlacz M, Gillebert CR, Vangkilde SA, Petersen A, Humphreys GW. Structural variability within Frontoparietal networks and individual differences in Attentional functions: an approach using the theory of visual attention. J Neurosci. 2015;35(30):10647–10658. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0210-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tryambake D, He J, Firbank MJ, O'Brien JT, Blamire AM, Ford GA. Intensive blood pressure lowering increases cerebral blood flow in older subjects with hypertension. Hypertension. 2013;61(6):1309–1315. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.112.200972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Levi Marpillat N, Macquin-Mavier I, Tropeano AI, Bachoud-Levi AC, Maison P. Antihypertensive classes, cognitive decline and incidence of dementia: a network meta-analysis. J Hypertens. 2013;31(6):1073–1082. doi: 10.1097/HJH.0b013e3283603f53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Pierce BL, Ahsan H, Vanderweele TJ. Power and instrument strength requirements for Mendelian randomization studies using multiple genetic variants. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(3):740–752. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyq151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Walker VM, Harrison S, Carter AR, Gill D, Tzoulaki I, Davies NM. The consequences of adjustment, correction and selection in genome-wide association studies used for two-sample Mendelian randomization. medRxiv. 2020; 2020.2007.2013.20152413. 10.1101/2020.07.13.20152413.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

12920_2020_769_MOESM1_ESM.docx (1.1MB, docx)

Additional file 1: Cognitive assessment in CARDIA at Year 25 examination (2010–2011); Table S1. SNPs and SNP-exposure effects used in the MR analyses.; Figure S1. Summary of the MR approach used in the study. Figure S2. Radial plot for detection of outliers. Figure S3. Funnel plot of blood pressure effect on cognitive function. Figure S4. Leave-one-out result of IVW estimates. Figure S5. Leave-one-out result of MR-Egger estimates.

Data Availability Statement

CARDIA data are available through the National Center for Biotechnology Information database of Genotypes and Phenotypes (dbGaP). More information is provided at https://www.cardia.dopm.uab.edu/study-information/genetic-data/cardia-genetic-data-in-dbgap-ncbi.


Articles from BMC Medical Genomics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES