Abstract
Although popular culture describes them as mortal enemies, more and more often, dogs and cats live under the same roof. Does this make them best friends? Can sharing the same social and physical environment make them similar? This study compares the approaches of dogs and cats living in the same household have towards humans and other pets as perceived by the owner. Questionnaires collected from 1270 people owning both dog(s) and cat(s) were analysed. Most dogs and cats living together are playful with familiar humans (76.2%) but dogs have a more sociable approach towards strangers and conspecifics than cats (P<0.001). Moreover, the percentage of dogs that have a playful relationship with the owner (84.0%) was higher than cats (49.2%; P<0.001). Dogs and cats living together eat in different places and show different mutual interactions: more dogs lick the cat (42.8%) and more cats ignore the dog (41.8%) than vice versa (P<0.001). However, most dogs and cats sleep at least occasionally (68.5%) and play together (62.4%; P<0.001). Although some body postures, such as the tail’s position, are interpreted differently by the two species, the greater proportions of dogs and cats show a relaxed response to several kinds of approaches of their roommate. Our questionnaire confirms the common beliefs about the sociability of the dog and the privacy of the cat, but this does not result in continuous internal struggles. Most cohabitations are peaceful. Moreover, it is true that they speak different languages, but they seem to understand each other well and interpret each other's approaches in the right way. Thus, aspiring owners should not blindly believe popular assumptions, but both knowledge and respect for species-specific pet behaviours are essential to establish a balance in the household.
Introduction
In the last 9.000 years, cats and dogs have been living in settlements with humans and assuming crucial roles within the communities. Dogs, the earliest to be domesticated, proved useful as guards, herding and as hunters [1]. To date, the dog is also appreciated for its ability to detect people, explosives or drugs, and to assist humans with disabilities [2–6]. The cat was domesticated later and it has played an important role in controlling rodents [1] though contemporary society especially appreciates its ease of management [7]. Both the cat and the dog are today of priceless value also as companions. They provide amusement and pleasure, they encourage social interactions, alleviate loneliness and comfort at stressful times, separations or mourning, as well as bring benefits in various pathological conditions [4, 8, 9].
For all these and other reasons, to date about one in four people owns at least a dog or a cat (data referring to America and Europe [10, 11]) and many had both species [12, 13]. A survey in the United Kingdom indicated that 7% of respondents had both species [14] while, among the participants of our previous survey in Italy, this percentage rose to 43% [12].
Owners consider their dog or cat to be family members [12, 15] but perceive differences in the personality of the two species: dogs prove more sociable and protective whilst cats more neurotic [16, 17]. Physiological and ethological features, as well as divergent evolutionary trajectories, may have modulated dogs and cats’ social behaviours [17–19]. Moreover, the sensitivity, the prejudices, and the attachment level of the observer could affect the qualitative judgment on pets’ behaviour [13, 17]. Dog adopters have higher expectations for the behaviour and the relationship with their pet compared to cat adopters [20]. Moreover, the tendency to consider their pet as “family member” is weaker in cat owners than dog owners [15].
Actually, both dogs and cats show interspecies communicative skills [21–24] and manifest attachment behaviour towards their owner [25, 26]. In addition to the olfactory sense, both dogs and cats use body postures, facial expressions and vocalizations to communicate [27–29]. Nevertheless, dog and cat’s language may be different and this could influence their mutual relationship, as well as those with humans and with other animals. Studies on interrelationships of dogs and cats report that dogs use more referential gazes towards humans compared to cats, while the latter display more aggressive or antagonizing behaviours towards dogs than vice versa [7, 13, 23]. The knowledge of the comparative aspects of expressive language could improve human-pet interactions, animal welfare, and management especially in the multiple-pets homes [13, 17]. It is also interesting to understand if and how the owner perceives these differences. As mentioned above, the perception of such differences could be influenced by popular breed stereotypes and owner’s expectations and, in turn, could also imply a different approach of the owner towards the two species. For example, the owner can expect that cats behave similarly to dogs or expect that the cats have the same body language that dogs have. It would lead to negative interpretations of unexpected (but normal) cat behaviours.
Little has been studied on the coexistence of dogs and cats under the same roof. This social phenomenon is growing [7, 13, 17] and offers interesting topics of study. For instance, this peculiar situation allows to evaluate the approach of a pet with both known and unknown animals and if pets that share the same environmental and social context have similarities in setting up their relationships. Moreover, dogs and cats are judged by the same owner, therefore, reducing the possible influences related to the demographic characteristics and the sensitivity of the owner.
Then, we investigated the intra and interspecies relationships of dogs and cats by administering a questionnaire to owners of both species. Specifically, we analysed how the owner perceives the approach of dogs and cats towards people and conspecifics as well as dog-cat interactions. We also investigated the differences in attitudes according to the familiarity that pets have with people or other animals. We hypothesized that, although we could not directly assess the behaviour of cats and dog living in the same household, we might still provide reliable and valid information regarding this behaviour by collecting the owners’ perceptions and opinions about it. Thus, this has to be regarded as a study based on owners’ reports and not on dog and cat behaviour.
Materials and methods
Participants
The questionnaire was voluntarily and anonymously completed by 1270 people owning at least one dog and one cat. This sample can be considered representative of a larger population. Data were collected over three-month period (from May to July 2014) from 1270 residents in Italy, who voluntarily and anonymously completed the questionnaire. We could not calculate the relative response rate because the questionnaire was distributed through email and social networks.
Ethical statement
All participants had to provide their written consent and they were briefly debriefed before answering the questionnaire. All of them anonymously and voluntarily agreed to participate in the study. In our study, the questionnaire did not collect any privacy-sensitive data, identifying information or health information of the participants, items were invented by the authors and did not aim at assessing respondents’ personality. Our focus was on the animals, but none of them has been used for the research. No one animal has been treated in any way or submitted to any experimental protocol. Thus, ethical approval was not required.
Materials and procedure
The study is part of a broader project, named “RandAgiamo®”, which aims to increase the adoptability of adult shelter dogs in the Umbria region of Italy (for details see [30, 31]).
The survey was carried out by using an online questionnaire (Google Form®). The link was spread via email (to authors' acquaintances) via various cat and dog breed-specific groups, public and through social media e.g. Facebook to generate a snowball sample. The questionnaire was preceded by an introduction explaining the purpose of the study, the characteristics required to participate in the survey (being 18 years or older and having owned at least a dog and a cat at the same time), and the instructions on how to fill it out. In particular, it was specified that if the participant had more than one dog/cat, the answers had to be referred to the first pet of each species acquired still living at home. The questionnaire also included a sentence with which respondents were giving their consent to the collection of data, which were collected anonymously and treated according to the Italian law on privacy.
In the present work, we analysed only a subset of this long-form questionnaire, which explored several aspects of the cohabitation between dogs and cats (for details see [17]).
The part of the questionnaire analysed in this paper concerns the dog-cat relationship and was divided into five sections, as follows:
Section A included 5 multiple choice questions on participant demographic data (age, gender, region of residence, previous pet-ownership, specific expertise on pets, number of pets owned).
Section B consisted of multiple choice questions concerning the physical characteristics (age, sex, de-sexing status, type, health status) and age of acquisition of the dog. To facilitate the comparison between dogs and cats, age and age at acquisition were categorized as shown in S1 Table.
In Section C the participants had to describe their dog’s behaviour during previous meetings with cats and vice versa (approach when the dog saw a cat, whether the dog was attacked and/or injured by a cat,). To determine the relationship with other known and unknown conspecifics, possible behavioural approaches were given in the questionnaire as follows: (a) Playful / Sociable, (b) Uninterested, (c) Aggressive, (d) Scary / Suspicious, (e) Other.
Section D-E: contained the same questions as Sections B and C, respectively, but about the cat.
Section F contained questions related to the relationship between cohabiting dogs and cats during mealtime, play-time, on returning home from the walk, while sleeping or being pampered. We distinguished the questions into those regarding the dog’s approaches towards the cat (dog-cat relationship) and the cat’s approaches towards the dog (cat-dog relationship). For interspecies interrelationship, participants could choose one or more of the following behavioural approaches: (a) licks, (b) plays, (c) ignores, (d) moves away, (e) runs away, (f) growls/hisses, (g), attacks. Finally, owners were asked to describe their dog’s behaviour in response to different postures of their cat and vice versa. For the dog’s reaction to different postures of a non-cohabiting cat, participants could choose: (a) moves away, (b) turns his head, (c) wags tail, (d) stays quiet, (e) growls (f) attacks. For the cat’s reaction to different postures of a non-cohabiting dog, the possible answers were: (a) moves away, (b) gets close amicably, (c) stays quiet, (d) hisses (e) attacks.
Statistical analysis
The data obtained from the questionnaires were entered in an Excel sheet and transferred onto the statistical program SPSS Statistics version 23 (IBM, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) for analysis. We used Microsoft Excel 2007 to build polar graphics. Descriptive statistics were used to present the demographic characteristics of participants and animals. Distributions within categorical variables were analysed using Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Tests. The goal was to assess whether, for each question, the answers occurred with equal frequencies and the null hypothesis assumed that there is no significant difference between the observed and the expected value (all categories equal). We used Pearson Chi square to compare the answers for dogs and cats. The null hypothesis was that the frequency of a response is not significantly different between dogs and cats. Moreover, when the owner had the same set of answers for the dog and cat, we considered the data as paired and assessed the agreement in owners’ perceptions about their cohabiting dog and cat using the McNemar or Bowker tests. The null hypothesis was that there is no difference in the response of the same owner between dog and cat (i.e. consistency in the answers that the same owner gave for its cat and dog). Only questionnaires with valid responses on both dog and cat questions were included in the comparison analysis.
Results
Demographic characteristics of participants, dogs and cats
Tables 1 and 2 present demographic characteristics of participants, dogs and cats (descriptive statistics). For a detailed description, please refer to Menchetti et al. [17].
Table 1. Demographic characteristics, age at the time of the acquisition, and living and sleeping habits of dogs (N = 1270) and cats (N = 1270) living in the same household.
Parameter | Dog | Cat | |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 0–6 months | 23 (1.8%) | 52 (4.1%) |
>6 months to 2 years | 264 (20.8%) | 315 (24.9%) | |
>2 to 8 years | 621 (49.0%) | 561 (44.3%) | |
>8 years | 359 (28.3%) | 338 (26.7%) | |
Sex | Male | 560 (44.3%) | 622 (49.2%) |
Female | 705 (55.7%) | 643 (50.8%) | |
Reproductive status | Entire | 545 (43.3%) | 138 (10.9%) |
Neutered | 715 (56.7%) | 1130 (89.1%) | |
Breed | Mixeda | 664 (54.6%) | 1060 (91.8%) |
Purebred | 553 (45.4%) | 95 (8.2%) | |
Age with mother | <1 week | 31 (2.4%) | 81 (6.4%) |
Until 1 month | 219 (17.2%) | 344 (27.3%) | |
Until 3 months | 572 (45.0%) | 428 (34.0%) | |
>3 months | 10 (0.8%) | 7 (0.6%) | |
Unknown | 438 (34.5%) | 398 (31.6%) | |
Age at acquisition | 1–3 months | 736 (58.7%) | 900 (72.7%) |
4 months to 1 year | 309 (24.7%) | 247 (20.0%) | |
1–8 years | 173 (13.8%) | 85 (6.9%) | |
>8 years | 35 (2.8%) | 6 (0.5%) | |
Where the pet lives | Outdoors | 496 (39.4%) | 69 (5.5%) |
Indoors | 661 (52.5%) | 588 (46.7%) | |
Outdoors and indoors | 103 (8.2%) | 603 (47.9%) | |
Where the pet sleeps | Free outdoors | 77 (6.1%) | 95 (7.5%) |
Enclosed space | 72 (5.7%) | 53 (4.2%) | |
Home area | 168 (13.3%) | 163 (12.9%) | |
Free in the home | 448 (35.4%) | 599 (47.5%) | |
Bedroom | 253 (20.0%) | 77 (6.1%) | |
On the bed | 232 (18.3%) | 201 (15.9%) | |
Other | 16 (1.3%) | 74 (5.9%) |
a including European cats.
Values are numbers and percentages of valid responses (in brackets).
Table 2. Demographic characteristics of the participants (N = 1270).
Characteristics | N° | Valid percent | |
---|---|---|---|
Sex of respondent | Men | 111 | 8.8% |
Women | 1157 | 91.2% | |
Age of respondent (class) | 18–25 years | 217 | 17.1% |
26–40 years | 559 | 44.1% | |
41–55 years | 400 | 31.5% | |
56–70 years | 93 | 7.3% | |
Region | North | 554 | 44.6% |
Centre | 477 | 38.4% | |
South | 212 | 17.1% | |
Expert | No expertise | 249 | 19.9% |
Dogs expertise | 120 | 9.6% | |
Cats expertise | 62 | 4.9% | |
Dogs/Cats expertise | 822 | 65.6% | |
N° dogs | 1 dog | 686 | 56.4% |
2–5 dogs | 493 | 40.5% | |
>5 dogs | 38 | 3.1% | |
N° cats | 1 cat | 446 | 36.9% |
2–5 cats | 592 | 48.9% | |
>5 cats | 172 | 14.2% |
Values are numbers and percentages of valid responses (in brackets).
First encounters between dogs and cats living in the same household
For many pets, the first meeting with the other species took place before they turned six months old (55.8% and 58.0% in dogs and cats, respectively), followed by more than two years (24.5% and 22.4% in dogs and cats, respectively), and between six months and two years (19.6% and 19.5% in dogs and cats, respectively; N = 1263 dogs and 1244 cats; Chi-Square Test of Independence, χ2 = 1.7, P > 0.05).
According to owners’ reports, 274/1267 dogs (21.6%) attacked a cat while 304/1260 dogs (24.1%) were attacked by a cat.
Relationship with humans of dogs and cats living in the same household
More dogs were perceived by their owners as sociable with familiar people than cats (95.6% and 79.2% for dogs and cats, respectively), while more cats were reported as disinterested (2.0% and 14.6% for dogs and cats, respectively) or fearful compared to dogs (1.5% and 3.6% for dogs and cats, respectively; N = 1260 dogs and 1258 cats; Chi-Square Test of Independence, χ2 = 163.5, P < 0.001; Fig 1A). We found agreement in the perception of owner about his/her cohabiting dog and cat in 76.9% of dyads: 76.2% of dogs and cats living together were both playful with known humans, 0.5% were disinterested, and 0.2% (2 dyads) were fearful (N = 1251; Bowker test, χ2 = 160.4, P < 0.001).
More dogs were sociable (51.4% and 29.4% for dogs and cats, respectively) or aggressive (5.9% and 0.7% for dogs and cats, respectively) towards unknown people while cats were most fearful (23.7% and 37.1% for dogs and cats, respectively) and disinterested (14.8% and 29.8% for dogs and cats, respectively; N = 1243 dogs and 1253 cats; Chi-Square Test of Independence, χ2 = 227.2, P < 0.001; Fig 1B). Only 33.2% of dogs and cats living in the same household showed the same approach towards strangers (N = 1230; Bowker test, χ2 = 225.4, P < 0.001): 17.6% were playful, 5.9% disinterested, and 9.5% were both fearful. There were playful dogs that lived with disinterested (25.2%) or fearful (37.4%) cats, but also 71 fearful dogs living with playful cats (24.6%).
Relationship with conspecifics of dogs and cats
As shown by the different shapes that the distributions take within the radar charts, dogs and cats have a different relationship with the conspecifics (Fig 2). In particular, the percentage of dogs having a playful relationship with known (84.0%; N = 1256 dogs and 1243 cats; Chi-Square Test of Independence, χ2 = 355.1, P < 0.001; Fig 2A) and unknown (35.9%; N = 1244 dogs and 1229 cats; Chi-Square Test of Independence, χ2 = 244.8, P < 0.001; Fig 2B) conspecifics was higher than cats (49.2% and 10.1% with known and unknown, respectively). More cats had a disinterested (27.3% and 22.3% with known and unknown, respectively), aggressive (6.2% and 27.0% with known and unknown, respectively), or a fearful (7.2% and 27.3% with known and unknown, respectively) approach with known and unknown conspecifics than dogs (disinterested: 11.3% and 16.9%; aggressive: 1.5% and 15.7%; fearful: 1.2% and 23.4% with known and unknown, respectively; P < 0.05; Fig 2A and 2B).
As with regards to the agreement in owner’ perception about his/her cohabiting pets, 42.7% of participants described the relationship with known conspecifics of their dog and cat as playful. However, statistics did not confirm consistency between dogs and cats of the same owner (N = 1231; Bowker test, χ2 = 324.8, P < 0.001). For example, dogs that had a playful relationship with other dogs could live with cats showing a disinterested (21.8%), aggressive (5.0%) or fearful (6.7%) approach towards other cats.
Relationships with unknown conspecifics are also diversified between dogs and cats living in the same household (N = 1212; Bowker test, χ2 = 248.0, P < 0.001) since the agreement was only found in 21.2% of participants (4.5% playful, 4.9% disinterested, 5.4% aggressive, and 6.4% fearful). 12.3% of playful dogs lived in the household with playful cats towards unknown conspecifics, while 22.4% of playful dogs lived with disinterested ones, 24.9% with aggressive and 28.5% with fearful cats towards unknown cats. We also found fearful dogs living with aggressive cats towards unknown conspecifics (26.2%).
Response of the dog at the sight of a cat and vice versa
Participants were asked to indicate their dog's reaction to the sight of a cat and their cat's reaction to the sight of a dog. When many of the dogs saw a cat, they attacked (37.2%), barked (23.8%) or wagged their tails (20.5%; P < 0.001; Table 3). Only one dog ignored them (0.1%). On the contrary, many cats at the sight of a dog, escaped (31.3%), ignored (24.5%), hissed (21.4%) or approached him amicably (13.5; P < 0.001; Table 3). Small percentages of cats observed or attacked other dogs.
Table 3. Reaction of the dog at the sight of a cat and vice versa.
Item | Number and percentage | χ2 | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
When the dog sees a cat, the dog…a | ||||
Ignores it | 1 (0.1%) | 1320.5 | <0.001 | |
Wags his tail | 260 (20.5%) | |||
Barks | 302 (23.8%) | |||
Growls | 162 (12.8%) | |||
Chases it | 11 (0.9%) | |||
Attacks | 473 (37.2%) | |||
Runs away | 46 (3.6%) | |||
Other | 0 (0.0%) | |||
When the cat sees a dog, the cat…b | ||||
Ignores it | 305 (24.5%) | 989.0 | <0.001 | |
Approaches him amicably | 168 (13.5%) | |||
Hisses | 266 (21.4%) | |||
Attacks | 21 (1.7%) | |||
Observes it | 27 (2.2%) | |||
Escapes | 390 (31.3%) | |||
Other | 68 (5.4%) |
The prevailing category on the total number of respondents is indicated in Italics (Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Tests; null hypothesis: all categories equal).
a Total number of respondents = 1270.
b Total number of respondents = 1245.
Relationship between the dog and the cat: Feeding and sleeping habits
Most owners (42.2%) declared that their dog ate in a bowl placed on the ground while their cat in a bowl positioned on the top of something (P < 0.001; Table 4); 27.2% of dogs and cats living in the same household ate in bowls positioned far from each other while 20.8% in bowls close to one another. In most of the dyads, the animal that finished the first the meal moved away or waited close to the other pet that was still eating (P < 0.001; Table 4).
Table 4. Relationship between the dog and the cat: Feeding and sleeping habits.
Item | Number and percentage | χ2 | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
The dog and the cat eat…a | ||||
In the same bowl | 49 (3.9%) | 627.2 | <0.001 | |
In bowls close to one another | 260 (20.8%) | |||
In bowls positioned far from each other | 341 (27.2%) | |||
The dog eats in a bowl placed on the ground while the cat in a bowl positioned at the top | 528 (42.2%) | |||
Other | 74 (5.9%) | |||
The animal that ends the meal first…b | ||||
Moves away | 697 (57.7%) | 1764.8 | <0.001 | |
Waits close to the animal that is still eating | 313 (25.9%) | |||
Removes the other animal and eats his meal | 88 (7.3%) | |||
Dog and cat eat together from the bowl where it remained the food | 10 (0.8%) | |||
Dog and cat eat separate | 75 (6.2%) | |||
Other | 24 (2.0%) | |||
Dog and cat sleep nearby…c | ||||
Never | 383 (31.5%) | 16.7 | < 0.01 | |
Occasionally | 703 (57.8%) | |||
Always | 130 (10.7%) |
The prevailing category on the total number of respondents is indicated in Italics (Chi-Square Goodness of Fit Tests; null hypothesis: all categories equal).
a Total number of respondents = 1252.
b Total number of respondents = 1207.
c Total number of respondents = 1249.
Most cats and dogs that live under the same roof slept together occasionally (57.8%) or always 10.7% (P < 0.01; Table 4).
Relationship between the dog and the cat living in the same household: Playing habits, interactions and mutual approaches when the dog comes back home from the walk
Most of the dogs and cats living in the same household played together (62.4%; N = 1232; Chi-Square Goodness of Fit test, χ2 = 702.2, P < 0.001): 58.1% chased each other, 40.9% fought, 43.9% of cats played with the dog’s tail, 63.8% made ambushes (more than one answer was possible).
Participants had to indicate how the dog interacted with the cat and vice versa (Fig 3). As the participants could choose more than one answer, each answer was compared between dog and cat with a Chi-Square Test of Independence (N = 1237 dogs and 1231 cats). Many more dogs licked the cat than the opposite (42.8% and 34.3% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 19.1, P <0 .001). On the contrary, more cats ignored (28.4% and 41.8% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 49.1, P < 0.001), ran away (1.5% and 5.9% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 6.8, P < 0.001) and hissed at the dog (2.3% of dogs growled and 10.2 of cats hissed; χ2 = 66.1, P<0.001) than vice versa. There was no difference between dogs and cats in the play approach (53.3% and 52.7% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 0.1, P = 0.783) and in the aggressive behaviours (3.1% and 3.9% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 1.3, P = 0.262).
Concerning the comparison of pets that live together (McNemar test; N = 1219), 24.4% of dogs and cats living in the same household licked each other (P < 0.001). Only 17.9% of dogs and cats living in the same household ignored each other (P < 0.001), 2.7% of both dogs and cats slowly moved when meeting their roommate (P < 0.05). Only 9 owners (0.7%) claimed that both their dog and cat had reciprocal aggressive vocalizations (the dog growled at the cat and the cat hissed at the dog; P < 0.001) and 6 owners (0.5%) reported that their pets attacked (P > 0.1). This last subgroup was further investigated to evaluate whether the mutual aggressive approaches reported by these 6 owners were associated with some demographic variables of the owners or of the pets, but no significant associations were found (S1 Table).
Owners were asked to indicate the mutual approach of their pets when the dog returned home from the walk (more than one answer was possible; Fig 4). Comparing the species (Chi-Square Test of Independence; N = 1210), there was a lower percentage of dogs that did not interact with the cat after the walk than vice versa (25.7% and 57.7% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 258.7, P < 0.001) but more dogs approached the back of the cat than the opposite (33.4% and 12.2% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 157.6, P < 0.001) and more dogs approached the cats wagging their tails than cats approaching the dog with the tail up (36.7% of dogs wagged their tails and 28.2% of cats approached with the tail up; χ2 = 20.2, P < 0.001). There were more cats lying down than dogs (6.2% and 13.1% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 34.0, P < 0.001). There was no prevalence of species for a nose contact (38.6% and 38.6% for dogs and cats, respectively; χ2 = 0.0, P>0.1).
Relationship between the dog and the cat: Response of the dog to the cat's approach and vice versa
Owners were asked to describe the behaviour of their dog in response to different approaches of the cohabiting cat and vice versa. Table 5 summarizes the results while numbers, percentages and statistics are reported in the supplemental material (S2 and S3 Tables). For each question, just one answer was possible. The greater proportions of dogs and cats showed a quiet attitude (“Wags tail”, “Gets close amicably” or “Stays quiet”) in response to most hypothesized approaches. We can only notice a different distribution of percentages in response to “Approaches the dog/cat bowl”, when 25.7% of the dogs growled while 20.9% of cats moved away.
Table 5. Response of the dog to the cat’s approach and vice versa: Summary of the results.
CAT / DOG APPROACH | DOG RESPONSE | CAT RESPONSE |
---|---|---|
Bends on the front limbs | ✓ or O | ✓ or O |
Approaches for a nose-to-nose greeting | ✓ | -1 |
Turns his head to one side | O | O |
Lies down beside | ✓ | ✓ |
Wags tail | O | ✓ or O |
Approaches with tail up | ✓ | O or X |
Come in the bed of dog/cat (empty) | O or X | O |
Come in the bed of dog/cat while he sleeps | O or X | O or X |
Approaches the dog/cat bowl | O or X | O or X |
Approaching while the owner is cuddling the other dog/cat | ✓, O or X | O or X |
The cat/dog is pampered by the owner | ✓, O or X | ✓ or O |
The reaction was classified according to the prevailing responses of the owners:
✓ = Amicable (Wags tail, Get close amicably).
O = Indifferent (Moves away, Turns his head, Stays quiet).
X = Aggressive (Growls, Attacks).
1 approach not considered for the dog.
The dog wagged his tail mainly when the cat bent on the front limbs, approached him for a nose-to-nose greeting or with the tail up, lay down beside him, when the cat approached while the dog was being pampered by the owner or the owner was pampering the cat. Conversely, the dog growled or attacked mainly when the cat got in his bed, approached his bowl, approached him while the owner was cuddling him or while the cat was being pampered by the owner.
The cat approached amicably especially when the dog bent on the front limbs, lay down beside him or wagged his tail. The cat hissed or attacked when the dog approached with the tail up, entered his bed while he was sleeping, approached his bowl or approached him while the owner was cuddling him.
Discussion
Dog’s and cat’s relationship with people and other pets
The first part of the questionnaire investigated the owners’ opinions on the general relationship of the dog and cat living in the same household. Our results show that most owners perceived their dogs and cats as friendly with familiar people but having a different approach to strangers. Participants reported that in their dogs a sociable behaviour prevails although they are not always friendly with cats. Indeed, dogs were perceived as playful even with strangers and unknown conspecifics, while they could wag their tails or attack when seeing a cat. Conversely, owners perceived their cat as more disinterested in social relationships. Many participants referred that their cats have a fearful approach or are indifferent towards unknown people or conspecifics and, at the sight of dogs, run away or ignore them. A disagreement in the owner’s perception of their cohabiting pets as regards their response to unfamiliar people and animals also appeared, even though the pets shared the same social environment. Thus, the owner perceived the relationship with strangers or other pets of his/her cohabiting dog and cat as to be primarily a species-specific factor, regardless of their environment/family context. These findings are in agreement with Menchetti et al. [17] and Serpell [16], who described cats as more distant and neurotic (including aggressive, shy, and independent approaches) with strangers compared to dogs. Moreover, these findings support the public’s perception of the behaviour of the two species who believes dogs to be more extroverted and cats more independent and shy [15, 17, 23].
The dog's sociability and his interspecific communication skills are widely recognized, even by the observational studies. Several researches, comparing dogs’ social skills with those of wolves or chimpanzees, have emphasized the role of domestication and selective processes on dogs’ sociability [32, 33]. Dogs were the first animals to be domesticated and have shared a common ecological niche with humans for a longer time than any other species [1]. In reality, some authors hypothesized that humans and domestic dogs experienced convergent evolution of advanced social cognition, which has led to the emergence of one single, heterospecific group [34, 35]. Conversely, other authors [36–38] emphasized the role of learning in the development of dogs’ social skills for interacting with humans. As shown for a number of mammalian taxa [39], early social experiences affect later behaviour and the quality of relationship dogs and humans share, including aggressive and fearful attitudes [36, 40, 41]. In a 1961 study, Freedman et al. [38] showed that puppies that were not exposed to humans during their period of socialization were never able to develop normal bonds with humans later on. Scott and Fuller [42] obtained similar results. To conciliate the role of phylogeny and ontogeny, Udell et al. [43] supported the “Two Stage Hypothesis”. They proposed that the domestication per se has not necessarily changed the dog’s capacity to form social relationships, but it has extended the period of socialization, giving dogs more opportunities to form successful relations compared to other species [19]. In addition, it is well recognized that dogs can use their owner as a secure base [44] and form interspecific attachment bonds even in adulthood [45, 46].
For cats, the issue of sociability is even more controversial [17, 18, 47]. Contrary to the dog, the modern domestic cat is the product of a natural selection in which humans have intervened relatively recently (i.e. within the last 200 years; [1]). Feral and free-living domestic cats subsist by hunting alone and can live in the solitary state all their life [1, 18, 47]. The close coexistence of many cats is in primis conditioned by the food and space availability and appears to be the result of human intervention, for example for breeding purposes, in multiple-cat household or in colonies of abandoned cats [1, 18, 47, 48]. Unfamiliar cats are often turned away from a group and aggressive and abnormal behaviours are often reported within multiple-cat households [18, 49]. Potter and Mills [50], using an Ainsworth Strange Situation Test, argued that cats have not been domesticated long enough to show a preference towards human interaction as a focus of safety and security in the same way that dogs do. They concluded claiming that cats are typically quite autonomous in their social relationships [50].
However, cats can engage in a variety of intraspecific interactions and co-operative relationships so that the colony looks like a complex social organization [18, 47, 48]. The amount of affiliative behaviours results to be associated with familiarity and relatedness between cats [51, 52]. The cat appears to be also fit for an interspecific social system. Cats distinguish the voice of family and non-family members [22, 25] and can use human pointing to locate hidden food in a manner similar to the dog [23]. The cat has transferred some intraspecific behaviour, such as tail up, rubbing, kneading and purring, in the relationship towards humans [18]. Other communicative signals, like the “meow” vocalization, seem mainly used by pet cats to attract human attention [18, 24, 47, 48]. Contrary to Potter and Mills’ [50] claim, Edwards et al. [25] sustained that cats can manifest attachment behaviours towards their owners. Moreover, Vitale Shreve et al [21], using a free operant preference assessment, demonstrated that cats prefer social interaction with a human rather than other stimuli such as food, toys or scent. However, Vitale Shreve et al [21] admitted that there is a large individual variation on cats’ social behaviour, probably due to a combination of factors including biological predispositions and lifetime experiences. In fact, as for the dog, the cat's social behaviour is also affected by the amount of handling received during the socialisation period [48, 53].
Can we ultimately confirm the common beliefs about the sociability of dogs and cats? Can human's approach, perhaps not lacking in prejudices, influence the perception of their behaviours? These are unanswered questions. Humans’ assumptions that dogs are more friendly and emotional while cats are more independent may lead to a qualitatively different type of relationship with these species [15, 23]. Moreover, although our survey proposed questions related to precise and real situations, the answers could be influenced by the subjective perception of the owner [17, 24].
Relationship between dogs and cats living in the same household
The second section of the questionnaire deepened the owner’s perception of the relationship between dogs and cats living in the same household. Our findings suggested that cats can develop interspecific social bonds provided there is a familiarity with each other. This bond emerged in social play and physical proximity of cats and dogs living in the same household although they both retain species-specific characteristics. Dogs and cats living together have different feeding and sleeping habits but show signs of mutual good relationship. Usually, dogs and cats eat in separate locations but, in a quarter of the dyads, the animal that first finishes his/her meal waits close to the animal who is still eating. Moreover, more than half of the respondents stated that their pets sleep and play together. Perhaps they are not best friends, but neither mortal enemies.
The most popular games that engage dogs and cats living together include making ambushes and chasing each other, the so-called “play-fighting”. This is not surprising since it is a typical intraspecific play in both cats and dogs [48, 49, 54]. In the domestic dog, play can be a result of the domestication, therefore humans selected them based on a general tendency towards playfulness. Moreover, it often involves a canine or human partner assuming a predominant social motivation: playing helps to establish, reinforce or test social bonds [26, 54, 55]. In particular, some authors [26, 54] sustain that dog-human play is a tool to form and maintain an emotionally-based bond.
Play for the cat is structurally and motivationally more similar to hunting behaviour since it involves often prey-like objects and exploratory behaviours [54]. However, there are playful interactions within colonies which continue in adult cats and a social motivation cannot be excluded even for the feline species [47, 48]. To our knowledge, there are no previous studies on play dynamics between dogs and cats. Moreover, numerous functions have been attributed to animal play [18, 55, 56]. “Play-fighting” may also be stressful [48, 55] and not always the distinction between play fighting and real fighting is clear [55]. Anyway, we considered the play between dogs and cats living together as a positive social indicator. Indeed, play behaviour has generally been identified as a potential welfare indicator since it is associated to positive emotions [57], it occurs when conditions are optimum and other needs are met [55, 57], it can turn a stranger into a familiar animal [56], and may indicate an interspecific attachment [26].
How do dogs and cats living together interact? Only 18% of owners stated that their pets ignore each other and not even 1% (9 owners) reported reciprocal aggressive interactions. Conversely, more than half of the owners referred that their dog and cat showed friendly interactions. However, there were differences in the dog and cat approaches, even though they lived in the same household. Owners reported that their dogs lick their roommate more frequently than cats while more cats ignore the cohabiting dog, move away or escape than vice versa. In the owner opinion, when the dog returns home from the walk, most cats ignore him while more often the dog approaches the cat wagging his/her tail or smelling the cat’s back. Then, a playful attitude of the cat towards his/her dog roommate prevails but the comparison between species shows a more frequent indifferent approach and minor interactivity for the cat. In the cat, the aggressive interactions towards the dog (hissing) are also greater than vice versa (growling), confirming the results of Thomson et al. [13].
As mentioned above, interspecific relationships in the cat are influenced by his evolutionary process, early socialization and later negative experiences, familiarity and individual personality [18, 48, 53]. In the Fox's experiment [58], some pups were raised with a litter of kittens together with their mother throughout the critical period of socialization. Fox [58] showed that cats not precociously socialized with dogs rarely interacted spontaneously with them, whereas the socialization with an alien species facilitated their social reactivity and playful interactions. In contrast, dog-raised dogs interact spontaneously with cats inviting them to play. In our survey, we can hypothesize that some cats exhibited few interactions with dogs due to poor familiarity or lack of early socialization. However, we found agreement on the age when dogs and cats living in the same household firstly met each other (in most pets, the first meeting occurred before six months). Unfortunately, we do not know the number of interspecific contacts during their socialisation period, as well as we do not have information about how long the pets of our survey have been cohabitating and which of the two species have been firstly introduced in the home. Anyway, species-specific personality traits seem to modulate the approach also between pets living together. Menchetti et al. [17], by using this same questionnaire, compared the owners’ perception of the personality traits of cats and dogs living in the same household finding dogs more sociable and protective whilst cats more neurotic. The results of this study, through a multifactorial approach, confirmed that learning and social context modulate the pet’s sociability but it is primarily a species-specific characteristic [17].
Dogs and cats’ response to different approaches of the pets living with them
In the last part of the questionnaire, we analysed the reactions of dogs and cats, observed by the owners, in response to different postures and approaches of their roommate. Most respondents stated that their dogs and cats react amicably (“Wags tail”, “Get close amicably” or “Stays quiet”) in response to the roommate's different behaviours.
Tail wagging in dogs occurs in several situations. It can indicate excitement, stress [28, 59] or subordination [60], but usually communicates friendly intentions and an affectionate greeting [59–61]. Our results show that tail wagging mainly occurs when the cat bends on the front limbs, approaches him for a nose-to-nose greeting or with tail up and lies down beside him. Therefore, the dog sees these cat's attitudes as a positive signal to which he responds with sociability. Indeed, cats touch the nose and hold their tail up as a greeting and affiliative behaviours [18, 27, 48, 62]. Many dogs wag their tails even when the owner pampers the cat. In this context, tail wagging could also indicate stress and attention-seeking behaviour [28, 59]. Conversely, the owners reported that the dog reacts negatively, barks or attacks, mainly when the cat gets on his bed (even when it is empty), approaches his bowl, approaches while the owner is cuddling him. Then, some dogs also showed territorial and possessive attitudes. Moreover, differently from what Thomson et al. [13] report, we did not find a greater willingness of the dog to share his food than the cat.
Similarly, to the dog, the cat positively interpreted the dog bowing on the front limbs and lying by his side as well as the tail wagging. Again, the cat reacted negatively, hissing or attacking, when the dog entered his bed while he was sleeping (but not when the bed was empty) or approached his bowl. However, there were also many cats preferring to get away. The different findings obtained for the empty bed suggest that the cat, rather than possessive, could be annoyed by the presence of the dog and prefers to remain on his own.
Furthermore, unlike the dog, cats show aggressive behaviour when the dog approaches with the tail up. Then, we confirm that the tail position has different meanings in the species, but they understand each other. The tail held high is one of the dominant communicative features in the dog [60, 61, 63] and the cat can react negatively. Conversely, the vertical “tail up” is associated with intra and inter-specific affiliative behaviour in the cat signalling intention to interact amicably and inhibiting aggressiveness of the others [18, 27, 62]. Indeed, the dog’s response to the cat’s tail up was friendly.
Interestingly, the cat reacts negatively when the dog approaches while he is being pampered by the owner. This can suggest possessiveness and attachment to the owner similarly to the dog’s, confirming the results of Edwards et al. [25]. However, further studies about cat-owner relationship are needed.
Limitations of the study
The present study investigated the behaviours of cats and dogs living together as perceived by the owner. The perception of the owner is an important aspect for understanding the human-animal relationships as it could influence the owner's approach to the two species. Moreover, the questionnaire is a widely used methodological approach to study behaviour and temperament of pets as owners can observe them in a variety of situations and over an extended period [17, 64]. However, the questionnaire inevitably involves a degree of subjectivity and some biases in the owners' responses. The voluntary participation in the questionnaire and the imbalance in the gender of participants could further aggravate these expected biases. For example, owners who are experiencing a peaceful coexistence of their animals may be more willing to fill in a questionnaire; in addition, women spend more time with their pets and generally tend to show more affective and empathic relationships [12]. Further sources of bias could be the popular breed stereotypes, owner’s personality, sensibility, and expectations as well as cultural aspects [12, 17, 64]. On the other hand, though, the large sample of owners and the possibility of examining the perception of a single owner on two paired animals supports the reliability of our results. Indeed, they agreed with most of the previous observational studies.
Conclusions
Although there is no difference between dogs and cats in the relationship with familiar people, the approach to strangers and conspecifics is friendlier in dogs and more disinterested in cats. The dog has a more active approach with heterospecifics, with sociable or aggressive responses toward cats, while the cat prefers to ignore or escape at the sight of a dog. Species-specific features, different domestication processes and attitudes of people can motivate these differences. Dogs and cats retain their ethological characteristics of the species in living habits and social approaches, even when they share the same social and physical environment. The dog often looks for physical contact with the cat while the cat's approach is less interactive. However, cohabitation and familiarity allow the cat to form interspecific social bonds. Many dogs and cats living in the same household sleep and play together and few owners reported mutual aggressive interactions. Moreover, though the two species have different body languages, dogs and cats understand each other and respond accordingly: if the cat approaches with his tail up, the dog responds amicably while if the dog approaches with his tail up, the cat reacts with aggressive behaviours. A better understanding of the communicative signals of our pets would help improve management choices and relationship but observational studies should confirm our findings. Further studies should clarify the contribution of species-specific traits, learning abilities and owner's personality in the development of the pets’ sociability.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank all the cats’ owners who generously fulfilled the questionnaire making this study possible: their collaboration has been invaluable for us. We also wish to thank Margherita Angelucci for her wonderful assistance in the English revision of the manuscript.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
The authors received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1.Driscoll CA, Macdonald DW, O’Brien SJ. From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106 Suppl: 9971–8. 10.1073/pnas.0901586106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Diverio S, Guelfi G, Barbato O, Di Mari W, Egidi MG, Santoro MM. Non-invasive assessment of animal exercise stress: real-time PCR of GLUT4, COX2, SOD1 and HSP70 in avalanche military dog saliva. Animal. 2015;9: 104–9. 10.1017/S1751731114002304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Diverio S, Menchetti L, Riggio G, Azzari C, Iaboni M, Zasso R, et al. Dogs’ coping styles and dog-handler relationships influence avalanche search team performance. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017;191: 67–77. 10.1016/j.applanim.2017.02.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Olsen C, Pedersen I, Bergland A, Enders-Slegers MJ, Patil G, Ihlebaek C. Effect of animal-assisted interventions on depression, agitation and quality of life in nursing home residents suffering from cognitive impairment or dementia: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31: 1312–1321. 10.1002/gps.4436 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Guelfi G, Casano AB, Menchetti L, Bellicci M, Suvieri C, Moscati L, et al. A cross-talk between blood-cell neuroplasticity-related genes and environmental enrichment in working dogs. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 6910 10.1038/s41598-019-43402-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Menchetti L, Guelfi G, Speranza R, Carotenuto P, Moscati L, Diverio S. Benefits of dietary supplements on the physical fitness of German Shepherd dogs during a drug detection training course. PLoS One. 2019;14 10.1371/journal.pone.0218275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Feuerstein N, Terkel J. Interrelationships of dogs (Canis familiaris) and cats (Felis catus L.) living under the same roof. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;113: 150–165. 10.1016/j.applanim.2007.10.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Serpell JA. Anthropomorphism and Anthropomorphic Selection—Beyond the “Cute Response.” Society & Animals. 2003. pp. 83–100. 10.1163/156853003321618864 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Orlandi M, Trangeled K, Mambrini A, Tagliani M, Ferrarini A, Zanetti L, et al. Pet therapy effects on oncological day hospital patients undergoing chemotherapy treatment. Anticancer Res. 2007;27: 4301–4303. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18214035 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.American Pet Products Association (APPA). APPA National Pet Owners Survey Statistics: Pet Ownership & Annual Expenses [Internet]. [cited 3 May 2020]. Available: https://www.americanpetproducts.org/press_industrytrends.asp
- 11.The European Pet Food Industry. FACTS & FIGURES 2018 [Internet]. 2018 [cited 5 Mar 2020]. Available: http://www.fediaf.org/images/FEDIAF_Facts__and_Figures_2018_ONLINE_final.pdf
- 12.Diverio S, Boccini B, Menchetti L, Bennett PC. The Italian perception of the ideal companion dog. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2016;12: 27–35. 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.02.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Thomson JE, Hall SS, Mills DS. Evaluation of the relationship between cats and dogs living in the same home. J Vet Behav. 2018;27: 35–40. 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.06.043 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Murray JK, Gruffydd-Jones TJ, Roberts MA, Browne WJ. Assessing changes in the UK pet cat and dog populations: Numbers and household ownership. Vet Rec. 2015;177: 259 10.1136/vr.103223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Arahori M, Kuroshima H, Hori Y, Takagi S, Chijiiwa H, Fujita K. Owners’ view of their pets’ emotions, intellect, and mutual relationship: cats and dogs compared. Behav Processes. 2017; 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Serpell JA. Evidence for an association between pet behavior and owner attachment levels. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1996;47: 49–60. 10.1016/0168-1591(95)01010-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Menchetti L, Calipri S, Guelfi G, Catanzaro A, Diverio S. My Dog Is Not My Cat: Owner Perception of the Personalities of Dogs and Cats Living in the Same Household. Animals. 2018;8: 80 10.3390/ani8060080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Bradshaw JWS. Sociality in cats: A comparative review. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2016;11: 113–124. 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.09.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biol Rev. 2010;85: 327–345. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00104.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.O’Connor R, Coe JB, Niel L, Jones-Bitton A. Effect of Adopters’ Lifestyles and Animal-Care Knowledge on Their Expectations Prior to Companion-Animal Guardianship. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2016;19: 157–170. 10.1080/10888705.2015.1125295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Vitale Shreve KR, Mehrkam LR, Udell MAR. Social interaction, food, scent or toys? A formal assessment of domestic pet and shelter cat (Felis silvestris catus) preferences. Behav Processes. 2016; 0–1. 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Saito A, Shinozuka K. Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis catus). Anim Cogn. 2013;16: 685–690. 10.1007/s10071-013-0620-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Miklósi A, Pongrácz P, Lakatos G, Topál J, Csányi V. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J Comp Psychol. 2005;119: 179–86. 10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pongrácz P, Szapu JS. The socio-cognitive relationship between cats and humans–Companion cats (Felis catus) as their owners see them. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018;207: 57–66. 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.07.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Edwards C, Heiblum M, Tejeda A, Galindo F. Experimental evaluation of attachment behaviors in owned cats. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2007;2: 119–125. 10.1016/j.jveb.2007.06.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rooney NJ, Bradshaw JWS. Links between play and dominance and attachment dimensions of dog-human relationships. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2003;6: 67–94. 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0602_01 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stanton LA, Sullivan MS, Fazio JM. A standardized ethogram for the felidae: A tool for behavioral researchers. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2014;173: 3–16. 10.1016/j.applanim.2015.04.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Beerda B, Schilder MBH, Bernadina W, Hooff JANARAMVAN, Vries HWDE, Mol JANA. Chronic Stress in Dogs Subjected to Social and Spatial Restriction. I Behavioural Resposes. 1999;66: 243–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bennett V, Gourkow N, Mills DS. Facial correlates of emotional behaviour in the domestic cat (Felis catus). Behav Processes. 2017;141: 342–350. 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Menchetti L, Righi C, Guelfi G, Enas C, Moscati L, Mancini S, et al. Multi-Operator Qualitative Behavioural Assessment for dogs entering the shelter. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2019;213: 107–116. 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.02.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Menchetti L, Mancini S, Catalani M, Boccini B, Diverio S. RandAgiamoTM, a Pilot Project Increasing Adoptability of Shelter Dogs in the Umbria Region (Italy). Animals. 2015;5: 774–792. 10.3390/ani5030383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gácsi M, Győri B, Miklósi Á, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Topál J, et al. Species-specific differences and similarities in the behavior of hand-raised dog and wolf pups in social situations with humans. Dev Psychobiol. 2005;47: 111–122. 10.1002/dev.20082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V. A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at Humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol. 2003;13: 763–766. 10.1016/s0960-9822(03)00263-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Hare B, Tomasello M. Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9: 439–444. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V. Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Anim Behav. 2004;67: 995–1004. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Foyer P, Wilsson E, Jensen P. Levels of maternal care in dogs affect adult offspring temperament. Sci Rep. 2016;6: 19253 10.1038/srep19253 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Howell TJ, King T, Bennett PC, Press D. Puppy parties and beyond: the role of early age socialization practices on adult dog behaviour. Vet Med Res Reports. 2015;6: 143–153. 10.2147/VMRR.S62081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Freedman DG, King JA, Elliot O. Critical period in the social development of dogs. Science (80-). 1961;133: 1016–1017. 10.1126/science.133.3457.1016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Cushing BS, Kramer KM. Mechanisms underlying epigenetic effects of early social experience: The role of neuropeptides and steroids. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29: 1089–1105. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Gazzano A, Bianchi L, Campa S, Mariti C. The prevention of undesirable behaviors in cats: Effectiveness of veterinary behaviorists’ advice given to kitten owners. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2015;10: 535–542. 10.1016/j.jveb.2015.07.042 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Gazzano A, Mariti C, Alvares S, Cozzi A, Tognetti R, Sighieri C. The prevention of undesirable behaviors in dogs: effectiveness of veterinary behaviorists’ advice given to puppy owners. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2008;3: 125–133. 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.04.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Scott JP, Fuller JL. Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. Chicago.: The University Of Chicago Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Udell MAR, Wynne CDL. Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim Behav. 2010;79: e9–e14. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.033 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mariti C, Ricci E, Zilocchi M, Gazzano A. Owners as a secure base for their dogs. Behaviour. 2013;150: 1275–1294. 10.1163/1568539X-00003095 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gácsi M, Topál J, Miklósi A, Dóka A, Csányi V. Attachment behavior of adult dogs (Canis familiaris) living at rescue centers: forming new bonds. J Comp Psychol. 2001; 10.1037/0735-7036.115.4.423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Carlone B, Sighieri C, Gazzano A, Mariti C. The dog (Canis familiaris) as part of the family: A pilot study on the analysis of dog bond to all the owners. Dog Behav. 2019;5: 1–14. 10.4454/db.v5i1.90 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Crowell-Davis SL, Curtis TM, Knowles RJ. Social organization in the cat: A modern understanding. J Feline Med Surg. 2004;6: 19–28. 10.1016/j.jfms.2003.09.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rochlitz I. The Welfare Of Cats [Internet]. Springer. I R, editor. Dordrecht, The Netherlands; 2007. 10.1007/978-1-4020-3227-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Strickler BL, Shull EA. An owner survey of toys, activities, and behavior problems in indoor cats. J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2013;9: 207–214. 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.06.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Potter A, Mills DS. Domestic cats (felis silvestris catus) do not show signs of secure attachment to their owners. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–17. 10.1371/journal.pone.0135109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Barry KJ, Crowell-Davis SL. Gender differences in the social behavior of the neutered indoor-only domestic cat. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1999;64: 193–211. 10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00030-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Curtis TM, Knowles RJ, Crowell-Davis SL. Influence of familiarity and relatedness of proximity and allogrooming in domestic cats (Felis catus). Am J Vet Res. 2003;64: 1151–1154. 10.2460/ajvr.2003.64.1151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Lowe SE, Bradshaw JWS. Responses of pet cats to being held by an unfamiliar person, from weaning to three years of age. Anthrozoos. 2002. pp. 69–79. 10.2752/089279302786992702 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Bradshaw JWS, Pullen AJ, Rooney NJ. Why do adult dogs “play”? Behav Processes. 2015;110: 82–87. 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Held SDE, Spinka M. Animal play and animal welfare. Anim Behav. 2011;81: 891–899. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Antonacci D, Norscia I, Palagi E. Stranger to familiar: Wild strepsirhines manage xenophobia by playing. PLoS One. 2010;5 10.1371/journal.pone.0013218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Boissy A, Manteuffel G, Jensen MB, Moe RO, Spruijt B, Keeling LJ, et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol Behav. 2007;92: 375–397. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Fox MW. Behavioral effects of rearing dogs with cats during the “Critical Period of Socialization.” Behaviour. 1969;35: 273–280. 10.1163/156853969X00242 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Konok V, Dóka A, Miklósi Á. The behavior of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) during separation from and reunion with the owner: A questionnaire and an experimental study. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2011;135: 300–308. 10.1016/j.applanim.2011.10.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Van Der Borg JAM, Schilder MBH, Vinke CM, De Vries H, Petit O. Dominance in domestic dogs: A quantitative analysis of its behavioural measures. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–18. 10.1371/journal.pone.0133978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Bradshaw JWS, Blackwell EJ, Casey RA. Dominance in domestic dogs-useful construct or bad habit? J Vet Behav Clin Appl Res. 2009;4: 135–144. 10.1016/j.jveb.2008.08.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cafazzo S, Natoli E. The social function of tail up in the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus). Behav Processes. 2009;80: 60–66. 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Bonanni R, Natoli E. Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behav Ecol. 2010;21: 443–455. 10.1093/beheco/arq001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Duffy DL, Hsu Y, Serpell JA. Breed differences in canine aggression. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2008;114: 441–460. 10.1016/j.applanim.2008.04.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.