Abstract
The structural organization of excitatory inputs supporting spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) remains unknown. We performed a spine STDP protocol using two-photon (2P) glutamate uncaging (pre) paired with postsynaptic spikes (post) in layer 5 pyramidal neurons from juvenile mice. Here we report that pre-post pairings that trigger timing-dependent LTP (t-LTP) produce shrinkage of the activated spine neck and increase in synaptic strength; and post-pre pairings that trigger timing-dependent LTD (t-LTD) decrease synaptic strength without affecting spine shape. Furthermore, the induction of t-LTP with 2P glutamate uncaging in clustered spines (<5 μm apart) enhances LTP through a NMDA receptor-mediated spine calcium accumulation and actin polymerization-dependent neck shrinkage, whereas t-LTD was dependent on NMDA receptors and disrupted by the activation of clustered spines but recovered when separated by >40 μm. These results indicate that synaptic cooperativity disrupts t-LTD and extends the temporal window for the induction of t-LTP, leading to STDP only encompassing LTP.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Physiology
The structural organization of excitatory inputs supporting spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP) in dendritic spines remains unknown. Using a spine STDP protocol, the authors uncover the STDP rules for single, clustered and distributed dendritic spines in the basal dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons in juvenile mice.
Introduction
Dendritic spines, the main recipient of excitatory information in the brain1, are tiny protrusions with a small head (~1 μm in diameter and <1 fL volume) separated from the dendrite by a slender neck. Spines can undergo structural remodeling that is tightly coupled with synaptic function1–4, and are the preferential site for the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP)4–7 and long-term depression (LTD)8, thought to be the underlying mechanisms for learning and memory in the brain9. A variation of LTP and LTD has been described in pyramidal neurons that involve the pairing of pre- and postsynaptic action potentials (APs), known as spike-timing-dependent plasticity (STDP)10–14. In this process, the timing between pre- and postsynaptic APs modulates synaptic strength, triggering LTP or LTD11. The sign and magnitude of the change in synaptic strength depend on the relative timing between spikes of two connected neurons (the pre- and postsynaptic neuron15). The STDP learning rules and their dependency on postsynaptic dendritic depolarization13,16, firing rate13, and somatic distance of excitatory inputs16–19 have been extracted from studies using connected neuronal pairs or by using extracellular stimulating electrodes, but the precise location and structural organization of excitatory inputs that support STDP at its minimal functional unit—the dendritic spine—are unknown.
Activity-dependent spine morphological changes (spine head4, neck2, or both20) have been correlated with changes in synaptic strength in cortical pyramidal neurons by mechanisms involving biochemical and electrical spine changes1,6,21. Thus, we asked what patterns of activity and structural organization of excitatory synaptic inputs support the generation of timing-dependent LTP (t-LTP) and t-LTD, and which morphological, biophysical, and molecular changes observed in dendritic spines can account for the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD?
Here, we provide evidence showing that the induction of STDP in single or distributed spines from layer (L5) pyramidal neurons using two-photon (2P) glutamate uncaging, to mimic synaptic release, follows a bidirectional Hebbian STDP rule. Furthermore, we show that synaptic cooperativity, induced by the co-activation of only two clustered spines using 2P glutamate uncaging, disrupts t-LTD (<40 µm distance between spines) and extends the temporal window for the induction of t-LTP (<5 µm distance between spines) via the generation of differential local N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-dependent calcium signals, leading to an STDP rule for clustered inputs only encompassing LTP.
Results
Induction of t-LTP in single dendritic spines
To induce t-LTP, we used a repetitive spike-timing protocol (40 times, 0.5 Hz) in which 2P uncaging of glutamate at a single spine was closely followed in time (+7 or +13 ms, see “Methods”) by a backpropagating AP (bAP) (Fig. 1a). We monitored spine morphology and uncaging-induced excitatory postsynaptic potential (uEPSP) amplitude for up to 40 min following STDP induction to establish the time course of STDP at individual synapses (Fig. 1c, e).
A repetitive pre–post pairing protocol of +13 ms reliably induced t-LTP (significant increase in uEPSP amplitude over time, P < 0.001, n = 9 spines, Fig. 1b–d, Supplementary Fig. 1), and shortening of the activated spine neck within minutes (P < 0.001), with no significant change in spine head volume (P = 0.25, Fig. 1b–d, Supplementary Fig. 2). Neighboring spines had no appreciable changes in their neck length or head volume (Fig. 1b; neck length = 98.09 ± 5.06%, P = 0.73, n = 13; head volume = 103.01 ± 3.61%, P = 0.51, n = 14, Wilcoxon test). Control experiments showed no significant change in uEPSP amplitude or spine morphology following the STDP protocol when either bAP or synaptic stimulation were applied in isolation, or when these parameters were monitored without any STDP protocol (Supplementary Fig. 3).
A pre–post pairing of +7 ms did not induce any plasticity (P = 0.45, n = 8 spines), or change in the activated spine’s neck (P = 0.09, Fig. 1e, f, Supplementary Fig. 2a). Because the degree of postsynaptic depolarization is an important factor in the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD, we verified that initial uEPSP amplitudes were comparable for pre–post timings of +13 ms versus +7 ms (uEPSP: 0.59 ± 0.11 versus 0.53 ± 0.16 mV, P = 0.47, Mann–Whitney test; Supplementary Fig. 4).
Thus, for the pairing times tested, there is a preferred time window (+13 ms) at which activated spines in basal dendrites from L5 pyramidal neurons undergo a significant increase in synaptic strength, and a concomitant neck shrinkage (Fig. 2g).
Induction of t-LTD in single dendritic spines
We then studied t-LTD in single spines by using a repetitive spike-timing protocol in which 2P uncaging of glutamate at a single spine was preceded in time by a bAP (post–pre protocol, Fig. 2a). When postsynaptic spikes preceded presynaptic firing by 15 ms (i.e., −15 ms), a significant reduction of the uEPSP amplitude occurred within minutes following t-LTD induction (P < 0.001, n = 7 spines, Fig. 2b–d), with no significant changes in spine neck length or head dimension (P = 0.45 and 0.97, respectively, Fig. 2b–d, Supplementary Fig. 2c). To determine the mechanism of t-LTD, we performed the same experiment described above, but with an NMDA receptor blocker (50 µM dl-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid (AP5)) added to the bath, which resulted in no t-LTD induction and no change in spine morphology—indicating that NMDA receptors are indeed needed for the induction of t-LTD in single spines (Supplementary Fig. 5). Interestingly, when postsynaptic spikes preceded presynaptic firing by 23 ms (i.e., −23 ms), there were no significant changes in uEPSP amplitude or in spine neck length and head dimensions for the duration of the recordings (P = 0.28, 0.71, and 0.66, respectively, n = 7 spines, Fig. 2e, f, Supplementary Fig. 2c). Initial uEPSP amplitude for post–pre pairing protocols of −15 ms versus −23 ms were comparable (uEPSP: 0.55 ± 0.07 versus 0.49 ± 0.08 mV, respectively, P = 0.38, Mann–Whitney test; Supplementary Fig. 4). Thus, for the pairing times tested, only a post–pre t-LTD pairing time window of −15 ms can effectively induce LTD in single dendritic spines in the basal dendrites from L5 pyramidal neurons.
Taken together, these results show that the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD in single spines follows a bidirectional Hebbian STDP learning rule favoring presynaptic inputs that precede postsynaptic spikes and depresses presynaptic inputs that follow postsynaptic spikes at a very precise temporal window (+13 ms for the generation of t-LTP and −15 ms for t-LTD, Fig. 2g, h). This single spine STDP rule has a narrower LTD induction time window than previously observed in connected pairs of L2/317,22 and L5 pyramidal neurons13, where the presynaptic control of t-LTD via a metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR) and/or cannabinoid type 1 receptor-dependent mechanism23–26 could plausibly account for these differences.
Induction of t-LTP in clustered dendritic spines
STDP not only depends on spike timing and firing rate but also on synaptic cooperativity and voltage at the postsynaptic site13,16. Furthermore, the induction of t-LTP in a single spine can reduce the threshold for the induction of plasticity of a neighboring spine (separated by <10 µm) activated 90 s later7. However, a direct demonstration of synaptic cooperativity for synchronous inputs, or nearly synchronous synaptic inputs, at the level of single spines and the precise location and structural organization of excitatory inputs that support the induction of STDP (t-LTP and t-LTD) in the dendrites of pyramidal neurons has yet to be obtained. Hence, we directly tested if synaptic cooperativity, marked by the activation of clustered dendritic spines from basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons, can generate a local dendritic depolarization and local calcium signals high enough to disrupt the single spine STDP learning rule described in Fig. 2g. To test this, we performed a two-spine STDP protocol, whereby nearly simultaneous 2P uncaging (interstimulus interval <0.1 ms) of caged glutamate in clustered (distance between spines <5 µm) spines was followed in time by a bAP to trigger t-LTP (Fig. 3a). Specifically, we induced t-LTP in two clustered spines at pre–post timings of +7 ms, and surprisingly found that this protocol was in fact capable of effectively and significantly generating increases in uEPSP amplitude (P < 0.001, n = 8 spine pairs, Fig. 3b, c, h) and the concomitant shrinkage of the activated spine neck (P < 0.001, Fig. 3b, c, h), with no apparent changes in its spine head size (P = 0.47, Fig. 3b, c, h; Supplementary Fig. 2b). These changes last for the duration of the recordings. No significant change in uEPSP amplitude or spine morphology was observed without any STDP protocol (Supplementary Fig. 3b). Hence, the synaptic cooperativity of only two neighboring synaptic inputs onto spines (<5 µm apart) in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons extends the pre–post timing window that can trigger potentiation (Fig. 3h; Supplementary Fig. 6).
Induction of t-LTP in distributed dendritic spines
To precisely study the effect of interspine distance of the nearly simultaneously activated spines on synaptic cooperativity and the induction of t-LTP at pairings of +7 ms, we performed experiments where the activated spines were further away (>5 µm apart) from each other (Fig. 3d). We found a significant correlation between the interspine distance and the uEPSP change, where each data point corresponds to the maximal change in uEPSPs observed after t-LTP induction, such that the induction of t-LTP decayed exponentially as a function of interspine distance with a length constant (λ) of 5.7 µm (Fig. 3e), which we used as the boundary between clustered (<5 µm) and distributed (>5 µm) spines for the induction of t-LTP at pairings of +7 ms. Clustered spines were located in the same basal dendrite (n = 8/8 pairs), while distributed spines were either in sister branches emanating from the same bifurcation point (n = 7/13 pairs) or in the same basal dendrite (n = 6/13 pairs). By separating the data in this manner, the t-LTP protocol (+7 ms) in distributed spines (>5 µm, Fig. 3d) failed to induce t-LTP (P = 0.45, n = 13 spine pairs, Fig. 3f–h) or changes in spine head size (P = 0.07, Fig. 3f–h) and neck length (P = 0.64, Fig. 3f–h) at all the times tested following t-LTP induction (Supplementary Fig. 2a). For comparisons between the activation of two clustered versus two distributed spines with a pre–post timing of +7 ms see Supplementary Fig. 6. In summary, these data show that the induction of t-LTP in nearly synchronously activated spines at pre–post timings of +7 ms is disrupted when the two activated spines were >5 µm apart in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons. These results uncover a two-spine activation crosstalk-spatial barrier of 5 µm for the induction of t-LTP to occur at pre–post timings of +7 ms.
Molecular mechanisms for t-LTP in dendritic spines
AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid) receptor content is one of the major mechanisms underlying LTP (for a review see ref. 27). To experimentally study the contribution of AMPA receptors to these phenomena, we performed t-LTP experiments in two clustered spines from L5 pyramidal neurons recorded via patch pipettes loaded with intracellular solution containing 200 µM PEP1-TGL—a peptide that inhibits AMPA receptor incorporation to the postsynaptic density (PSD) by blocking GluR-1 C-terminus interaction with PDZ domains at the PSD28 (Fig. 4a). PEP1-TGL incubation by itself did not trigger a rundown of uEPSP amplitude or changes in spine morphology over time (Supplementary Fig. 7a, c). A repetitive pre–post pairing protocol of +7 ms used to activate clustered spines in the presence of PEP1-TGL showed that the peptide completely inhibited t-LTP for the duration of the experiment (P = 0.75, n = 5 spine pairs, Fig. 4b–d, Supplementary Fig. 2b), but had no effect on the t-LTP-induced shrinkage of the activated spine necks (P < 0.001, Fig. 4b–d, Supplementary Fig. 2b) or in modifying spine head size (P = 0.96, Fig. 4b–d, Supplementary Fig. 2b). No significant difference was observed between the initial uEPSP amplitude for pre–post pairing protocols of +7 ms with versus without PEP1-TGL (uEPSP: 1.06 ± 0.2 versus 1.16 ± 0.28 mV, P = 0.94, Mann–Whitney test, Supplementary Fig. 4). These results indicate that GluR-1 receptor incorporation into the PSD, via its interaction with PDZ domains, is required for the induction of t-LTP in spines. However, the role of the spine neck shrinkage on AMPA receptor incorporation into the PSD and ultimately on the induction of t-LTP remains open.
To address the role that t-LTP-induced neck shrinkage has on AMPA receptor lateral trafficking to the PSD, and the generation of t-LTP in the activated spines we focused on actin dynamics. We used the actin polymerization inhibitor latrunculin-A (Lat-A)29–31 (Fig. 4e), which did not trigger any rundown of uEPSP amplitude or changes in spine morphology over time in the absence of STDP induction (Supplementary Fig. 7b, d). Induction of t-LTP with 100 nM dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO)-dissolved Lat-A (which allows cellular permeation) added to the bath32 completely blocked the shrinkage of the activated spine necks (P = 0.35, n = 9 spine pairs, Fig. 4f–h, Supplementary Fig. 2b) and the increase in uEPSP amplitude, inducing instead a significant reduction in uEPSP amplitude over time (P < 0.001, Fig. 4f–h, Supplementary Fig. 2b). However, when the t-LTP induction protocol was performed with 100 nM artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF)-dissolved Lat-A (which prevents it from entering the cell), we observed no inhibition of the increase in uEPSP amplitude and shrinkage of the neck of the activated spines (Supplementary Fig. 8a–c).
No significant difference was observed between initial uEPSP amplitudes for pre–post pairing protocols of +7 ms with versus without Lat-A (uEPSP: 0.66 ± 0.1 versus 1.16 ± 0.28 mV, P = 0.15, Mann–Whitney test, Supplementary Fig. 4). The lack of rundown of uEPSP amplitude over time in neurons treated with Lat-A in the absence of STDP induction (Supplementary Fig. 7b, d) and lack of effect of ACSF-dissolved Lat-A on the induction of t-LTP, but the significant depression in uEPSPs after the induction of t-LTP suggests that the induction of plasticity, and the rearrangement of actin filaments destabilized AMPA receptors, leading to removal from the PSD.
We further performed control experiments in actively dividing cells, where the actin cytoskeleton is actively changing, to test cell permeation and the biological action of ACSF-dissolved and DMSO-dissolved Lat-A. Bath applied Lat-A reduced actin filament (f-actin) concentration (Supplementary Fig. 8d) and size only when dissolved in DMSO, but not in ACSF (Supplementary Fig. 8e–g).
In summary, these results show that changes in actin polymerization are required for the t-LTP-dependent neck shrinkage and the induction of plasticity. Our findings further suggest that the induction of t-LTP occurs via a mechanism that involves a neck-shrinkage-dependent facilitated diffusion of GluR-1 subunits from the spine neck to the head, and subsequent incorporation into the PSD. We hypothesize that a shorter and wider neck facilitates the transport of AMPA receptors into the spine head (Fig. 4e), a mechanism that is required for the induction of t-LTP.
Induction of t-LTD in clustered and distributed spines
It has previously been shown that (1) t-LTP induction in the distal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons (layer 3–L5 pyramidal neuron pairs) triggers LTD instead of LTP, and (2) that LTD can be converted into LTP by increasing the local voltage16. Thus, we hypothesized that the induction of t-LTD in single spines depends on the degree of local depolarization, and hence LTD can be disrupted by the activation of neighboring spines. To test this, we performed repetitive spike-timing protocol (40 times, 0.5 Hz) in which 2P uncaging of glutamate at two spines (separated by up to 100 µm) was preceded in time (−15 ms) by a bAP (Fig. 5b and Supplementary Fig. 9). Surprisingly, we found that this t-LTD protocol failed to induce any change in uEPSP amplitude or spine head volume with only a slight but significant reduction in spine neck length (Supplementary Fig. 9a, b). To more precisely characterize the effect of activating two spines on the induction of t-LTD, we correlated the interspine distance and the maximum uEPSP change of all the times tested from each experiment following STDP induction (see “Methods”). We found that the uEPSP change decayed exponentially as a function of interspine distance with a length constant (λ) of 39.7 µm (Fig. 5e, each data point corresponds to the maximal change in uEPSP observed after t-LTD induction). Therefore, we used this value as a boundary between clustered (<40 µm) and distributed (>40 µm) spines. Using this classification, clustered spines were located in the same dendrite (n = 11/12 pairs) or in sister branches emanating from the same bifurcation point (n = 1/12 pairs), while distributed spines were always located on separate dendrites (n = 8/8 pairs). The t-LTD protocol in two clustered spines (Fig. 5a) failed to induce t-LTD (P = 0.24, n = 12 spine pairs, Fig. 5c) or changes in spine head size (P = 0.32; Fig. 5c, d, h; Supplementary Fig. 2d), with only a slight but significant shrinkage of the spine neck (P = 0.47, Fig. 5c, d, h). For comparison between the activation of one versus two clustered spines with a post–pre timing of −15 ms see Supplementary Fig. 10. Interestingly, the t-LTD protocol performed in two distributed spines separated by >40 µm (Fig. 5b) recovered t-LTD (P < 0.001, n = 8 spine pairs, Fig. 5f–h; Supplementary Fig. 2d), without triggering changes in neck length or spine head size (P = 0.67 and 0.56, respectively, Fig. 5f–h). No significant difference was observed between the initial uEPSP amplitude for clustered versus distributed spines activated with post–pre pairings of −15 ms (EPSP: 1.06 ± 0.13 versus 1.05 ± 0.21 mV, P = 0.94, Mann–Whitney test; Supplementary Fig. 4). For comparison between the activation of clustered versus distributed spines after post–pre pairings of −15 ms see Supplementary Fig. 10. These results were surprising since not only did we not observe t-LTD in clustered spines (<40 µm), but we also observed significant neck shrinkage with no LTP (see Figs. 1 and 3). To account for this observation, we explored if there was a correlation between the induction of plasticity in these experiments and both the shrinkage of the spine neck and the distance between the activated clustered spines, since the local voltage, and hence the induction of plasticity, could be affected by the distance between the activated clustered spines. Indeed, we found that the distance between the activated spines is correlated with the induction of plasticity and the shrinkage of the activated spine necks (Eq. 1 in “Methods”; P < 0.01; Supplementary Fig. 9d). This analysis suggests that during t-LTD induction the structural arrangement of clustered spines (<40 µm) determines the sign and magnitude of the change in synaptic strength and concomitant neck shrinkage.
In summary, the induction of t-LTD at pairing times of −15 ms was disrupted when only two clustered spines (<40 μm apart) were nearly simultaneously activated in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons, but was recovered if the activated spines are distributed (>40 μm) in the dendritic tree.
Spine calcium transients during t-LTP and t-LTD induction
Calcium is critical for the induction of synaptic plasticity33–37, and high or low local concentration difference in dendrites and spines are thought to be associated with gating LTP or LTD, respectively38–40. Therefore, to investigate how local calcium accumulations relate to the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD in single versus two clustered spines, we performed 2P calcium imaging in a region of interest (ROI) of the activated spines and their parent dendrites during STDP induction protocols throughout each of the 40 pre–post or post–pre repetitions (see “Methods”). The “before” images correspond to the calcium signals right before the pairing in each repetition, uncovering the lack or presence of local calcium accumulation during the 40 pairing repetitions. The “after” images correspond to the calcium signals right after the pairing in each repetition, uncovering a proxy for the amplitude and local calcium accumulation during the 40 pairing repetitions.
To dissect potential differences in local calcium signals and accumulation that can account for the presence or absence of t-LTP and t-LTD induction in clustered versus distributed spines, we imaged 2P calcium activity during five different STDP induction protocols: (1) pre–post pairing of +7 ms in one spine; (2) pre–post pairing of +7 ms in two clustered spines; (3) pre–post pairing of +13 ms in one spine; (4) post–pre pairing of −15 ms in one spine; (5) post–pre pairing of −15 ms in two clustered spines.
During the pre–post (+7 ms) pairing protocol in single spines, we found that, across the 40 repetitions, there was little to no calcium accumulation in the spine or dendrite (Fig. 6a–c and left panels in Fig. 6d, e, n = 7 spines and dendrites). As expected, there was, however, a significant increase in calcium immediately following the stimulation (left panels in Fig. 6i, j). In contrast, when we applied the exact same pairing protocol (pre–post +7 ms) in two clustered spines, there was a striking calcium accumulation in both the activated spines and dendrite that was evident before (Fig. 6f–h and middle panels in Fig. 6d, e) and after stimulation (middle panels in Fig. 6i, j, n = 12 spines and 6 dendrites). To determine the mechanisms responsible for the calcium accumulation in spines, we performed experiments where NMDA receptors were blocked with AP5 (50 µM) during the induction protocol, revealing that the calcium accumulation was in fact NMDA receptor-dependent (Supplementary Fig. 11). Thus, activating just one additional spine using the same pairing protocol alters the calcium dynamics (compare black and green traces in right panels of Fig. 6d, e, i, j), possibly through a mechanism that is incapable of extruding calcium increases in spines in between pre–post repetitions, leading to its build-up in spines and parent dendrites, which ultimately guide the induction of plasticity.
A pre–post pairing of +13 ms (n = 12 spines and dendrites) resulted in significant calcium accumulation in both the activated spine and dendrite that was evident when we analyzed the images taken before (Fig. 7a, b, left panel, c, d) and after stimulation (Fig. 7a, b, right panel, e, f). In summary, the induction of t-LTP in a single spine at a pre–post pairing protocol of +13 ms (Fig. 7) and in clustered spines at a pairing time of +7 ms (which is otherwise ineffective in triggering t-LTP if only one spine is being activated) (Fig. 6) are correlated with an NMDA receptor-dependent accumulation of calcium in spines and parent dendrites, likely being responsible for the induction of t-LTP.
Next, we performed the same experiments with a post–pre (−15 ms) pairing protocol in both single and clustered spines. In single spines, we observed moderate calcium increases (Fig. 8a, b, n = 10 spines and dendrites experiments, from six neurons, and six mice) before (left panels in Fig. 8c, d) and after the post–pre stimulation (left panels in Fig. 8g, h). Surprisingly, when we applied the same pairing protocol in two clustered spines (Fig. 8e, f, n = 12 spines and 6 dendrites, from four neurons, and three mice) we found calcium levels were significantly higher at the end of the induction protocol compared to those observed with single spine t-LTD induction protocols (P = 0.01 and 0.04 for repetitions 30–35 and 35–40, respectively, right panels in Fig. 8g). We propose that when two clustered spines are activated with a post–pre pairing protocol, calcium accumulation levels reached pass moderate calcium accumulation levels required for the induction of t-LTD.
Discussion
We uncovered the STDP rules for single, clustered, and distributed dendritic spines in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons from juvenile mice. Our results show that the induction of STDP in single spines follows a classical Hebbian STDP learning rule that is bidirectional, in which presynaptic input leading postsynaptic spikes generates t-LTP and postsynaptic spikes preceding presynaptic activation of single dendritic spines results in t-LTD. Furthermore, we found that the induction of t-LTP triggers the shrinkage of the activated spine neck, without any significant changes in the spine head size, extending our previous findings of the activity-dependent shrinkage of the spine neck2. Our results indicate that the induction of t-LTP requires (1) the incorporation of new GluR-1 receptors with PDZ domain-containing proteins in the PSD and (2) an actin polymerization-dependent neck shrinkage of the activated spine neck (Fig. 4). We showed that the induction of t-LTP triggers actin-dependent neck shrinkage, which is likely required for the lateral diffusion of GluR-1 receptors from the spine neck to the spine head, and its incorporation to the PSD—generating plasticity (see Supplementary Discussion). In support of this spine mechanism of LTP induction is a recent report showing that AMPA receptor surface diffusion is fundamental for the induction of hippocampal LTP and contextual learning41. In addition, we found that the induction of t-LTD was not accompanied with spine neck or head changes. We note that these results pertain to juvenile mice and may not hold in mature circuits of older animals.
Although spines have the machinery and do undergo structural head changes in vitro4,8,42 and in vivo43, we propose that our results represent a new form of structural spine plasticity during t-LTP that involves rapid neck shrinkage without head volume enlargements that occurs before structural head volume changes, a process likely linked with memory consolidation. Importantly, our data suggest that during STDP, the use of spine volume changes as the sole proxy for LTP or LTD43 is not a complete representation of plasticity in spines from dendrites in cortical pyramidal neurons (see Supplementary Discussion and Supplementary Fig. 12).
The spine head size has been suggested to be a relevant factor in determining the degree of LTP induction that can be triggered, with head volumes <0.1 µm3 representing the preferential sites for the induction of LTP4. Hence, in the present study, we focused on spines with head sizes ranging between 0.026 and 0.148 fL (average: 0.070 ± 0.0015 fL) and found that within these pool of spines there is no significant correlation between the degree of plasticity induction and head size of the activated spines (pre–post pairing of +13 ms in one spine: R = 0.38, n = 9; pre–post pairing of +7 ms in two clustered spines: R = 0.07, n = 8). However, it is important to mention that changes in endoplasmic reticulum extensions/Ca2+ dynamics44,45 as well as the life history of a neuron could influence the expression of structural and functional forms of synaptic plasticity in spines46, and could account for some of the variability observed between spines.
Somatodendritic distance has also shown to have an effect on STDP induction16–19, although in the present study we did not find any correlation between plasticity and somatodendritic distance (Supplementary Fig. 14). The reason for this discrepancy is likely because we confined our study to spines located in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons with somatodendritic distances ranging between 20 and 80 µm. Previous studies investigating STDP induction dependency on somatodendritic distances16,17,19 investigated a much larger range of synaptic locations (L5: between ~20 and 600 µm from the soma, and L2/3 pyramidal neurons: >100 µm from the soma).
We then explored the functional consequences of synaptic cooperativity of nearly simultaneous excitatory inputs on STDP. Our results show that the induction of t-LTP in two clustered spines—separated by <5 µm—is sufficient to induce LTP and shrinkage of the activated spine necks at a pre–post timing (+7 ms) that is otherwise ineffective at triggering significant morphological changes and synaptic potentiation when only one spine is being activated. To uncover the distance between the nearly simultaneously activated spines capable of supporting synaptic cooperativity and the induction of t-LTP, we varied the distance of the activated spines. Our results show that the induction of t-LTP is suppressed when spines are separated by >5 µm apart (Fig. 3), with an effective length constant (λ) of 5.7 µm. These results show that the nearly simultaneous activation of clustered spines can extend the pre–post timing window that can trigger potentiation. These data, together with results showing that the induction of t-LTP in one spine can change the threshold for the induction of plasticity in a spine located at <10 µm away and activated 90 s later7, indicate that there is a spatially restricted dendritic compartment that is required for spine crosstalk and the broadening of the STDP timing required for triggering t-LTP (Fig. 9).
On the other hand, the induction of t-LTD in two clustered spines disrupts the generation of LTD, leading to an STDP learning rule that is incapable of supporting LTD, but only encompasses LTP (Fig. 9). We next investigated the dendritic mechanisms responsible for the disruption of t-LTD, and found that the induction of t-LTD is recovered when the activated spines are separated by >40 µm (Fig. 5, Fig. 9). Interestingly, the effective electrotonic length constant (referred to here as λe, to distinguish it from the λ obtained to explore synaptic cooperativity in Figs. 3e and 5e) in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons has been reported to be 50 µm47. This value of λe suggests the idea that significant voltage attenuations—capable of recovering LTD—can be expected when the t-LTD induction protocol is triggered in spines that are separated by >40 µm in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons (Fig. 5b, e–h). However, we cannot discard that other mechanisms, such as the diffusion of active molecules5 and/or the engagement of active conductances in spines and parent dendrites1,16, could contribute to the switch from t-LTD to no-LTD induction observed in distributed/single spines and clustered spines, respectively. These results are in discrepancy with observations showing that in connected pairs of L5–L5 pyramidal cells, t-LTD is reliably generated after post–pre pairing protocols13,14,16,26,48. A likely explanation for this apparent discrepancy is that the synaptic inputs from one L5 pyramidal neuron to another are distributed49. In fact, it has been shown in connected pairs of cortical L5 pyramidal neurons that presynaptic boutons form functional synapses onto the proximal dendrites of the postsynaptic neuron in a manner that does not favor the dendrites of a particular target neuron50. Importantly, clustered and distributed excitatory inputs have been described in the dendrites of pyramidal neurons both in vitro and in vivo1,51–53. Our results clearly show the functional importance that the structural and temporal organization of excitatory synaptic inputs have on the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD, and how just two clustered excitatory synaptic inputs are capable of altering the STDP learning rule in the basal dendrites of L5 pyramidal neurons (Fig. 9).
To explore the mechanisms that may be responsible for these observations, we imaged local calcium signals in the activated spines and parent dendrites before and after each of the 40 pairing repetitions performed during t-LTP and t-LTD induction protocols. Our reasoning for performing these experiments was based on findings that different levels of depolarization gate local calcium signals, which depending on its magnitude and kinetics can generate LTP (high calcium) or LTD induction (sustained but moderate calcium signals)10,38,54. In addition, calcium-based modeling studies of STDP have shown that different calcium dynamics mediate the induction of t-LTP versus t-LTD55,56. Specifically, the calcium control hypothesis indicates that large levels of calcium (above a plasticity threshold, ϴp) are thought to lead to t-LTP, whereas more moderate, prolonged levels (between the depression threshold, ϴdSTART, and ϴdSTOP) give rise to t-LTD and a mid-level range in which t-LTD does not occur (below ϴdSTART)55,57,58. A major assumption of these models is infinite time constants for synaptic variables at resting calcium levels so that the synaptic changes do not decay after the presentation of the stimulus56 (a significant constraint for the stabilization of synaptic changes). A potential solution to this problem is the degree of local calcium accumulation observed in the activated spines throughout the t-LTP or t-LTD induction protocol. In fact, these models are consistent, fundamentally, with our results, which show that a pre–post pairing of +7 ms protocol in two clustered spines, and a pre–post pairing of +13 ms protocol in a single spine gives rise to t-LTP accompanied by a substantial NMDA receptor-mediated increase in the intracellular calcium levels following each pairing repetition, and a significant accumulation of local calcium levels throughout the induction protocol, likely mediated by the inability to efficiently extrude the local calcium signals in between each pre–post pairing at this induction frequency (Figs. 6 and 7). We propose that the local spine calcium accumulation we observe provides a new and key variable for the induction of plasticity, which reduces the constraints imposed by calcium-based models for the stabilization of synaptic changes55–58.
Our results further show that a post–pre protocol of −15 ms in a single spine induces t-LTD and moderate intracellular calcium signals in spines and parent dendrites after each pairing, without an evident increase in local calcium accumulation. These results possibly reflect that the calcium signal generated during the induction protocol passed ϴdSTART and remained for several seconds in this permissive calcium concentration window (between ϴdSTART and ϴdSTOP) generating LTD. Activating two clustered spines with the same protocol, however, does not induce plasticity and gives rise to a slow but significant build-up of calcium at the end of the induction protocol in clustered spines. These results likely reflect that the spine calcium levels crossed ϴdSTART and ϴdSTOP reaching higher local calcium levels.
These findings presented here are quite remarkable since stimulating just one additional spine during an STDP protocol can alter the calcium dynamics and the induction of t-LTP and t-LTD. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of the functional relevance that the structural organization and nearly simultaneous subthreshold activation of only a few clustered inputs in the dendrites of pyramidal neurons have on plasticity. We propose the term micro-clusters to describe this structural and functional modality of synaptic connectivity. In fact, the relevance of synaptic micro-clusters on the synaptic cooperativity in a short dendritic segment (<10 µm) of pyramidal neurons is also supported by anatomical51–53,59 and functional studies60–62 (see Supplementary Discussion). Taken together, these reported findings and our data suggest that the functional specificity and structural arrangement of synaptic inputs, distributed or forming micro-clusters in the dendrites of pyramidal neurons, are fundamental for guiding the rules for sensory perception, affecting the STDP learning rule, learning and memory, and ultimately cognition.
Methods
Brain slice preparation and electrophysiology
C57B/6 mice were used in this study and housed on a 12-h light/dark cycle with ambient temperature 20–24 °C and 40–70% humidity. Brains from postnatal day 14–21 mice—anesthetized with isoflurane—were removed and immersed in cold (4 °C) oxygenated sucrose cutting solution containing (in mM) 27 NaHCO3, 1.5 NaH2PO4, 222 sucrose, 2.6 KCl, 1 CaCl2, and 3 MgSO4. Coronal brain slices (300-μm-thick) of visual cortex were prepared. Brain slices were incubated for 1/2 h at 32 °C in ACSF (in mM: 126 NaCl, 26 NaHCO3, 10 dextrose, 1.15 NaH2PO4, 3 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgSO4) and then transferred to a recording chamber. Electrophysiological recordings were performed in whole-cell current-clamp configuration with MultiClamp 700B amplifiers (Molecular Devices) in L5 pyramidal neurons with a patch electrode (4–7 MΩ) filled with internal solution containing (in mM) 0.1 Alexa Fluor 568, 130 potassium d-gluconic acid (potassium gluconate), 2 MgCl2, 5 KCl, 10 HEPES, 2 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, pH 7.4, and 0.4% biocytin. All experiments were conducted at room temperature (~20–22 °C), except for a subset of experiments performed at 32 °C (detailed below). We did not extend our experiments to include voltage-clamp recordings since recent evidence indicates that the high spine neck resistance can prevent the voltage-clamp control of excitatory synapses and that these measurements can be significantly distorted in spiny neurons63.
Two-photon imaging and two-photon uncaging of glutamate
Two-photon imaging was performed with a custom-built two-photon laser scanning microscope64, consisting of (1) a Prairie scan head (Bruker) mounted on an Olympus BX51WI microscope with a ×60, 0.9 NA water-immersion objective; (2) a tunable Ti-Sapphire laser (Chameleon Ultra-II, Coherent, >3 W, 140-fs pulses, 80 MHz repetition rate), (3) two photomultiplier tubes (PMTs) for fluorescence detection. Fluorescence images were detected with Prairie View 5.4 software (Bruker).
Fifteen minutes after break-in, two-photon scanning images of basal dendrites were obtained with 720 nm and low power (~5 mW on sample; i.e., after the objective) excitation light and collected with a PMT. Two-photon uncaging of 4-methoxy-7-nitroindolinyl (MNI)-caged l-glutamate (2.5 mM; Tocris) was performed65. This concentration of MNI-glutamate completely blocked inhibitory postsynaptic currents (IPSCs)66; thus, our results represent excitatory inputs only. Uncaging was performed at 720 nm (~25–30 mW on sample). Note that the laser power used for imaging is not sufficient to result in any partial uncaging of glutamate (Supplementary Fig. 13, and see ref. 64). Activated spines were mostly on the second and third branch of the basal dendrites and were on average 40.22 ± 1.62 µm away from the soma (range of distances: ~10 to 80 μm from soma, n = 113 spines, Supplementary Fig. 14). Only spines with a clear head contour and that were separated by >1 µm from neighboring spines were selected. The location of the uncaging spot was positioned at ~0.3 μm away from the upper edge of the selected spine head (red dot in figures), which has a spatial resolution of 0.71 and 0.88 µm for one and two spines, respectively (Supplementary Fig. 15). Care was taken to maintain the position of the uncaging spot. After each uncaging sequence, the spot was repositioned to keep the same distance of ~0.3 μm from the edge of the spine and to avoid artificial potentiation or depression. The uEPSPs were recorded at the soma of L5 pyramidal neurons. Importantly, the kinetics of uEPSPs from the present study (10/90 rate of rise: 0.07 ± 0.014 mV/ms; duration: 115.5 ± 15.3 ms) are not significantly different from those triggered at 32 °C (Supplementary Fig. 16; 10/90 rate of rise: 0.05 ± 0.01 mV/ms, P = 0.92; duration: 108.8 ± 22.5 ms, P = 0.32, Mann–Whitney test) or 37 °C (P = 0.65)21.
STDP induction protocol
To induce t-LTP in single spines, we used two-photon uncaging of MNI-glutamate (40 times every 2 s, with each uncaging pulse lasting 2 ms), which, after 7 or 13 ms, was followed by a bAP (triggered by 10 ms current injection (0.4–0.6 nA) in the soma). To induce t-LTD in single spines, two-photon uncaging of MNI-glutamate (40 times every 2 s, with each uncaging pulse lasting 2 ms) was preceded for −15 or −23 ms by bAP. When we evaluated t-LTP and t-LTD in two spines, we used similar protocols to those described above, but the spines were activated with two-photon uncaging of MNI-glutamate sequentially with an onset delay of ~2.1 ms for the second spine (i.e., interstimulus interval of <0.1 ms). No significant difference was observed in the in 10/90 rise time of the uEPSPs triggered when one versus two spines were activated (9.05 ± 1.19 versus 9.49 ± 0.54 ms, respectively; P = 0.71, Mann–Whitney test). We used juvenile mice aged 14 to 21 days because cortical synapses are most plastic at this age—critical period for the induction of LTP in primary sensory cortex67–69.
To evaluate the morphological and synaptic strength of the activated spines before and after the STDP induction protocol, we performed 2P imaging, and low-frequency 2P uncaging (0.5 Hz) in single or multiple spines. To establish the time course of the changes in uEPSP amplitude, neck length, and head volume following STDP induction, for each experiment, we interpolated the data taken at different time points using the interp1 function in MATLAB (MathWorks) with the pchip option, which performs a shape-preserving piecewise cubic interpolation. Note that we constrained this fit so that it terminated with a slope of zero following the last data point. Then, for each condition, we averaged the uEPSP amplitude, neck length, and head volume temporal traces. The length of the x-axis was set as the time at which the last data point was obtained for those sets of experiments. Shaded area represents ±SEM. To determine at which time the uEPSP amplitude, neck length, and head volume temporal traces are significantly different from baseline, we binned the temporal traces every 5 min and tested whether it was significantly different from baseline (100%).
Experimental checkpoints and data analysis
Electrophysiological data were analyzed with Igor Pro 7 (Wavemetrics) software and MATLAB R2019b (MathWorks). The resting membrane potential of the recorded L5 pyramidal neurons was −60.25 ± 0.54 mV (analyzed from a random sample of n = 58 neurons from a total of n = 136 neurons tested), which is similar to what others have reported in acute mouse brain slices of a similar age49,70. After taking this measurement, pyramidal neurons were maintained at −65 mV in current-clamp configuration throughout the recording session. Only neurons for which the injected current to hold the cell at −65 mV was <100 pA were included in this study. The series resistance was typically ~25 MΩ and was not compensated. The junction potential measured in our condition was approximately 14 mV and was not corrected. For the generation of bAP, only APs with amplitude of >45 mV from threshold to the peak amplitude were considered for analysis. The AP threshold, measured manually, is reported as the membrane potential measured at the inflection point between the rising potential during the depolarizing pulse and the fast rising phase of the AP. We found a mean AP amplitude from threshold of 63.14 ± 1.73 mV before STDP induction, which was similar to that measured during the STDP protocol (63.17 ± 1.76 mV) and at all time points after the STDP protocol (62.08 ± 1.91, pooled data of AP amplitude measured at all time points from each experiment, P = 0.97; n = 58, Wilcoxon signed-rank test, analyzed from a random sample of n = 58 neurons from a total of n = 136 neurons tested). The absolute AP amplitude, measured from the holding membrane potential to the peak depolarization amplitude, before the STDP protocol was 79.47 ± 2.21 mV (n = 58), and remained stable after the STDP protocol (80.11 ± 1.4 mV, pooled from all times after STDP, P = 0.76, Wilcoxon signed-rank test).
In most experiments, two control tests (each consisting of 10 uncaging pulses at 0.5 Hz), spaced by 5 min were performed to assess the reliability of the uEPSP amplitude. Only experiments for which uEPSP amplitudes were not significantly different before and after 5 min in control conditions were considered for analysis (P > 0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test).
Synaptic plasticity was assessed by two parameters: the uEPSP amplitude and the spine morphology (neck length and head volume). The peak uEPSP amplitude was measured from each individual uEPSPs by taking the peak value and averaging 2 ms before and after using Igor Pro 7 (Wavemetrics). Only uEPSPs that were >0.1 mV when one spine was activated in the control condition (i.e., before the induction of plasticity) were included in the analysis.
Synaptic plasticity was determined by the relative change of uEPSP amplitude (average of 10 uEPSPs) measured before and after the STDP protocol. The spike timings +7 and +13 ms (pre leads post) or −23 and −15 ms (post leads pre) correspond to the delta time offset between the beginning of the uncaging pulse (pre) and the beginning of the bAP pulse (post) repeated 40 times.
The analysis of spine morphology was performed from z-projections of the whole spine using ImageJ 1.52p71 (neck length) and MATLAB R2019b (MathWorks) (head volume). The neck length was measured from the bottom edge of the spine head to the edge of its parent dendritic shaft using the segmented line tool in ImageJ. We selected mostly spines with a spine neck longer than 0.2 µm. For those with a shorter neck, we did not report their length for analysis and statistics due to the diffraction limited resolution of our images. For spines whose necks shrunk after the STDP protocol below the diffraction limited resolution of our microscope, we set their length as the minimal measurement of spine neck length reported by Tonnesen et al.20, using stimulated emission depletion microscopy (0.157 µm)20. We estimated the relative spine head volume using the ratio of the maximum spine fluorescence and the maximum fluorescence observed in the dendrite measured from z-projections of the whole spine72,73. To obtain the spine volume, we then multiplied this ratio by the PSF of our microscope (0.11 fL)74. Linear optimization techniques were used to determine the correlation between EPSP change, neck length change and distance between two activated (clustered) spines following a pairing protocol of −15 ms. Specifically, the change in uEPSP amplitude was modeled using the following equation:
1 |
where uEPSPs and NL are the percent change in uEPSPs and neck length, respectively, following the STDP protocol, D is the distance between the two spines, and c1, c2, and c3 are constant coefficients. These parameters were estimated using a least-squares minimization criterion to obtain an optimal fit of the data that minimized the sum of the residuals squared. The relationship between interspine distance and the percent change in uEPSP was fit with the following exponential equation:
2 |
where α, β, and λ are constants, y represents the change in uEPSP, and x is the interspine distance.
Calcium imaging
During calcium imaging experiments, we performed whole-cell current-clamp recordings of L5 pyramidal neurons with a patch electrode containing calcium indicator Fluo-4 (300 μM; Thermo Fisher) and Alexa Fluor 594 (100 μM) diluted in an internal solution containing (in mM) 130 d-gluconic acid potassium salt, 2 MgCl2, 5 KCl, 10 HEPES, 2 MgATP, 0.3 NaGTP, pH 7.4, and 0.4% biocytin. To perform sequential 2P calcium imaging and 2P uncaging of caged glutamate in selected spines at one wavelength (810 nm), we used ruthenium-bipyridine-trimethylphosphine-caged glutamate (RuBi-glu, Tocris)66, diluted into the bath solution for a final concentration of 800 µM. Uncaging of Rubi-glu was performed at 810 nm (~25–30 mW on sample). The location of the uncaging spot was positioned at ~0.3 μm away from the upper edge of the selected spine head (red dot in Figs. 6–8). Changes in calcium were monitored by imaging 2P calcium signals and detecting the fluorescence with two PMTs placed after wavelength filters (525/70 for green, 595/50 for red). We performed 2P calcium imaging during five different STDP induction protocols triggered at 0.5 Hz: (1) pre–post pairing of +7 ms in one spine; (2) pre–post pairing of +7 ms in two clustered spines; (3) pre–post pairing of +13 ms in one spine; (4) post–pre pairing of −15 ms in one spine; (5) post–pre pairing of −15 ms in two clustered spines. We restricted the image acquisition to a small area (~150 × 150 pixels), which contained the spine(s) that we uncaged and the shaft. Images were acquired at ~30 Hz, averaged eight times, with 8 µs dwell time. Calcium signals were imaged 500 ms before STDP induction protocol and right after (4 ms) the stimulation for >600 ms. We focused our analysis on the images obtained before and immediately after the stimulation in each pairing repetition. ROI drawing was performed using custom algorithms (MATLAB; MathWorks). For spine heads, the ROI was a circle, whereas for dendrites it was a polygon. Fluorescence was computed as the mean of all pixels within the ROI. We quantified the relative change in calcium concentration () using the following formula:
3 |
where G is the fluorescence from the Fluo-4 dye and R is the fluorescence from the Alexa Fluor 594 dye. is the mean of all pixels of Fluo-4 signal within the ROI taken from the first image at the first stimulation.
Statistics and reproducibility
Statistics were performed with GraphPad Prism 5. Statistical significance was determined using the Wilcoxon signed-rank test when we analyzed the change in uEPSP amplitude and spine morphology 15–25 min after the induction of t-LTP or t-LTD and a Mann–Whitney test when we compare the raw initial uEPSP or spine morphology between two different set of experiments. Statistical significance was determined using one-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) when we analyzed the time course of the uEPSP amplitude and spine morphological changes after induction of t-LTP or t-LTD with post hoc pairwise comparisons using Dunnett’s test or a Tukey’s multiple comparison test for Supplementary Figs. 4 and 14, *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001. Sample sizes were similar to those generally employed in the field2,8,75. No technical replicates were used in this study. Values are expressed as mean ± SEM.
Experiments at physiological temperature
We performed a series of experiments where we induced t-LTP in two clustered spines at pre–post timings of +7 ms at 32 °C. The temperature of the bath was controlled by an inline solution heater and temperature controller (Warner Instruments). We found that this protocol resulted in significantly increased uEPSP amplitudes (113.8 ± 4.5%, P = 0.014; Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test) and shrinkage of the activated spine necks (89.2 ± 5.8, P = 0.022; Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test), with no apparent changes in spine head size (101.2 ± 2.8%, P = 0.91; Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Therefore, the plasticity we observe at room temperature does not differ from that evoked at the physiological temperature of 32 °C.
Pharmacology
Lat-A (Tocris Bioscience), soluble in organic compounds and water with a concentration ≤0.024 mg/mL (Drugbank database), was dissolved in DMSO at 1/1000 and added to the recording chamber containing the brain slice at 100 nM32 for 15 min before starting the STDP protocol. PEP1-TGL (Tocris Bioscience) was added in the pipette at 200 µM; after 15 min in whole-cell condition, electrophysiological recording and synaptic plasticity experiments were started. MNI-glutamate (Tocris Bioscience) was diluted in ACSF from stock solution and bath applied at 2.5 mM. Fresh vials of MNI-glutamate were used for each experiment. AP5 from Tocris Bioscience was diluted in ACSF (50 µM) from stock solution.
Cell culture and actin imaging
The 293T cells were subcultured into a 12-well plate at 3.5 × 105 cells/well over poly-d-lysine-coated glass coverslips (EMS 72294-12) 24 h before the experiment. Cells were treated with 100 nM Lat-A (Tocris 3973) dissolved in DMSO or ACSF, or vehicle alone (control) for 1 h and then fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 10 min at 4 °C. Fixed cells were permeabilized in 0.1% Triton X-100 for 10 min and then incubated with a 1:500 dilution of Alexa Fluor 647 Phalloidin (Thermo Fisher A22287) to label f-actin and 5 μg/mL Hoechst 33342 (Thermo Fisher H1399) to label nuclei for 30 min. Cells were mounted in ProLong Glass Antifade Mountant (Thermo Fisher P36984), left to cure, and imaged in a Leica TCS SP8 laser scanning confocal microscope with DLS light sheet module using ×10 or ×63 oil objectives. The results of these experiments are presented in Supplementary Fig. 8.
Image processing and analysis of actin
The z-stacks stored in LIF format were visualized and exported as three-dimensional images using Leica LASX software or opened and processed for quantification using FIJI as follows: (1) image stacks z-projected (standard deviation projection type), (2) transformed to 8-bit image, (3) image threshold adjusted (limits always 100/255 for actin and variable depending on the image 20–60/255 for nuclei), (4) nuclei images were also subject to binary processing to convert to mask and then to watershed, to separate attached nuclei. (5) Finally, images were analyzed using the “analyze particle” macro, modifying the preset parameters as follows, for f-actin: size 0.1–3000 μm2; for nuclei: size 20-infinity μm2. The size of all the particles with a size over 10 μm2 counted in each experimental condition was used for statistics (using Mann–Whitney t test) and plotted using custom MATLAB software. The results of these experiments are presented in Supplementary Fig. 8.
Ethics
Animal experimentation
These studies were performed in compliance with experimental protocols (13-185, 15-002, 16-011, 17-012, 18-011, and 19-018) approved by the Comité de déontologie de l’expérimentation sur les animaux (CDEA) of the University of Montreal and protocol 2020-2634 approved by the Comité institutionnel des bonnes pratiques animales en recherche (CIBPAR) of the Centre de Recherche, CHU Ste-Justine.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank P.J Sjöström, A. Kolta, C. Pack, and P. Drapeau for critical discussion and reading of the manuscript, and are grateful to all other members of Roberto Araya’s laboratory for kind support. We also thank members of the Groupe de Recherche sur le système nerveux central (GRSNC) for support and equipment sharing. Confocal microscopy experiments were carried out at the Platform of Imaging by Microscopy (PIM) of the CHU Sainte-Justine Research Center (CHUSJRC) that is supported by the expertise of E. Küster-Schöck and funded by CHUSJRC, the Quebec government (MSSS), the CHUSJ Foundation, and Canada Foundation for Innovation. This work was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) grant MOP-133711 to R.A., a Canada Foundation for Innovation (CFI) equipment grant Fonds des leaders 29970 to R.A., and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC Discovery Grant) grant application no. 418113-2012 (NSERC PIN 392027) to R.A. S.T. was supported in part by a salary support from the GRSNC at Université of Montréal. D.E.M. was supported in part by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Fonds de recherche du Québec—Santé (FRQS).
Author contributions
R.A. conceived the project. S.T. and D.E.M. and S.M.-R. performed the experiments. S.T., D.E.M., and S.M.-R. performed data analyses. R.A., S.T., and D.E.M., and S.M.-R. designed experiments. R.A., S.T., and D.E.M. wrote the manuscript. R.A. supervised the project. All authors read and approved the contents of the manuscript.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available in figshare with the identifier doi: 10.6084/m9.figshare.1262742276.
Code availability
All the codes used in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Sabrina Tazerart, Diana E. Mitchell.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41467-020-17861-7.
References
- 1.Araya, R. Input transformation by dendritic spines of pyramidal neurons. Front. Neuroanat.8, 10.3389/fnana.2014.00141 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 2.Araya, R., Vogels, T. P. & Yuste R. Activity-dependent dendritic spine neck changes are correlated with synaptic strength. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA111, E2895–E904 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 3.Matsuzaki M, et al. Dendritic spine geometry is critical for AMPA receptor expression in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2001;4:1086–1092. doi: 10.1038/nn736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Matsuzaki M, Honkura N, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H. Structural basis of long-term potentiation in single dendritic spines. Nature. 2004;429:761–766. doi: 10.1038/nature02617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Harvey CD, Yasuda R, Zhong HN, Svoboda K. The spread of Ras activity triggered by activation of a single dendritic spine. Science. 2008;321:136–140. doi: 10.1126/science.1159675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Nishiyama J, Yasuda R. Biochemical computation for spine structural plasticity. Neuron. 2015;87:63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Harvey CD, Svoboda K. Locally dynamic synaptic learning rules in pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nature. 2007;450:1195–1200. doi: 10.1038/nature06416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Oh WC, Hill TC, Zito K. Synapse-specific and size-dependent mechanisms of spine structural plasticity accompanying synaptic weakening. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:E305–E312. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1214705110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hayashi-Takagi A, et al. Labelling and optical erasure of synaptic memory traces in the motor cortex. Nature. 2015;525:333–+. doi: 10.1038/nature15257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Feldman DE. The spike-timing dependence of plasticity. Neuron. 2012;75:556–571. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bi GQ, Poo MM. Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:10464–10472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-24-10464.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Debanne D, Gahwiler BH, Thompson SM. Long-term synaptic plasticity between pairs of individual CA3 pyramidal cells in rat hippocampal slice cultures. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 1998;507:237–247. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.237bu.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Sjöström PJ, Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Rate, timing, and cooperativity jointly determine cortical synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2001;32:1149–1164. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00542-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Markram H, Lubke J, Frotscher M, Sakmann B. Regulation of synaptic efficacy by coincidence of postsynaptic APs and EPSPs. Science. 1997;275:213–215. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5297.213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Zhang LI, Tao HW, Holt CE, Harris WA, Poo MM. A critical window for cooperation and competition among developing retinotectal synapses. Nature. 1998;395:37–44. doi: 10.1038/25665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sjöström PJ, Hausser M. A cooperative switch determines the sign of synaptic plasticity in distal dendrites of neocortical pyramidal neurons. Neuron. 2006;51:227–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Froemke RC, Poo MM, Dan Y. Spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity depends on dendritic location. Nature. 2005;434:221–225. doi: 10.1038/nature03366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Froemke RC, Letzkus JJ, Kampa BM, Hang GB, Stuart GJ. Dendritic synapse location and neocortical spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 2010;2:29. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Letzkus JJ, Kampa BM, Stuart GJ. Learning rules for spike timing-dependent plasticity depend on dendritic synapse location. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:10420–10429. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2650-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tønnesen J, Katona G, Rozsa B, Nagerl UV. Spine neck plasticity regulates compartmentalization of synapses. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:678–685. doi: 10.1038/nn.3682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Araya R, Jiang J, Eisenthal KB, Yuste R. The spine neck filters membrane potentials. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:17961–17966. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608755103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Feldman DE. Timing-based LTP and LTD at vertical inputs to layer II/III pyramidal cells in rat barrel cortex. Neuron. 2000;27:45–56. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00008-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nakamura T, Barbara JG, Nakamura K, Ross WN. Synergistic release of Ca2+ from IP3-sensitive stores evoked by synaptic activation of mGluRs paired with backpropagating action potentials. Neuron. 1999;24:727–737. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81125-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hashimotodani Y, et al. Phospholipase Cbeta serves as a coincidence detector through its Ca2+ dependency for triggering retrograde endocannabinoid signal. Neuron. 2005;45:257–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bender VA, Bender KJ, Brasier DJ, Feldman DE. Two coincidence detectors for spike timing-dependent plasticity in somatosensory cortex. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:4166–4177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0176-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Sjöström PJ, Turrigiano GG, Nelson SB. Neocortical LTD via coincident activation of presynaptic NMDA and cannabinoid receptors. Neuron. 2003;39:641–654. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00476-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Diering GH, Huganir RL. The AMPA receptor code of synaptic plasticity. Neuron. 2018;100:314–329. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hayashi Y, et al. Driving AMPA receptors into synapses by LTP and CaMKII: requirement for GluR1 and PDZ domain interaction. Science. 2000;287:2262–2267. doi: 10.1126/science.287.5461.2262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Allison DW, Gelfand VI, Spector I, Craig AM. Role of actin in anchoring postsynaptic receptors in cultured hippocampal neurons: differential attachment of NMDA versus AMPA receptors. J. Neurosci. 1998;18:2423–2436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02423.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kim CH, Lisman JE. A role of actin filament in synaptic transmission and long-term potentiation. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:4314–4324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-11-04314.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ramachandran B, Frey JU. Interfering with the actin network and its effect on long-term potentiation and synaptic tagging in hippocampal CA1 neurons in slices in vitro. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:12167–12173. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2045-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Honkura N, Matsuzaki M, Noguchi J, Ellis-Davies GCR, Kasai H. The subspine organization of actin fibers regulates the structure and plasticity of dendritic spines. Neuron. 2008;57:719–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Lynch G, Larson J, Kelso S, Barrionuevo G, Schottler F. Intracellular injections of EGTA block induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Nature. 1983;305:719–721. doi: 10.1038/305719a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Malenka RC, Kauer JA, Zucker RS, Nicoll RA. Postsynaptic calcium is sufficient for potentiation of hippocampal synaptic transmission. Science. 1988;242:81–84. doi: 10.1126/science.2845577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Artola A, Singer W. Long-term depression of excitatory synaptic transmission and its relationship to long-term potentiation. Trends Neurosci. 1993;16:480–487. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(93)90081-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cummings JA, Mulkey RM, Nicoll RA, Malenka RC. Ca2+ signaling requirements for long-term depression in the hippocampus. Neuron. 1996;16:825–833. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80102-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fino E, et al. Distinct coincidence detectors govern the corticostriatal spike timing-dependent plasticity. J. Physiol. 2010;588:3045–3062. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2010.188466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lisman J. A mechanism for the Hebb and the anti-Hebb processes underlying learning and memory. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1989;86:9574–9578. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.23.9574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ismailov I, Kalikulov D, Inoue T, Friedlander MJ. The kinetic profile of intracellular calcium predicts long-term potentiation and long-term depression. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:9847–9861. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0738-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Nevian T, Sakmann B. Spine Ca2+ signaling in spike-timing-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:11001–11013. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1749-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Penn AC, et al. Hippocampal LTP and contextual learning require surface diffusion of AMPA receptors. Nature. 2017;549:384–388. doi: 10.1038/nature23658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Tanaka JI, et al. Protein synthesis and neurotrophin-dependent structural plasticity of single dendritic spines. Science. 2008;319:1683–1687. doi: 10.1126/science.1152864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.El-Boustani S, et al. Locally coordinated synaptic plasticity of visual cortex neurons in vivo. Science. 2018;360:1349–1354. doi: 10.1126/science.aao0862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cooney JR, Hurlburt JL, Selig DK, Harris KM, Fiala JC. Endosomal compartments serve multiple hippocampal dendritic spines from a widespread rather than a local store of recycling membrane. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:2215–2224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-06-02215.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Holbro N, Grunditz A, Oertner TG. Differential distribution of endoplasmic reticulum controls metabotropic signaling and plasticity at hippocampal synapses. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:15055–15060. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905110106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wiegert, J. S., Pulin, M., Gee, C. E. & Oertner T. G. The fate of hippocampal synapses depends on the sequence of plasticity-inducing events. Elife7, e39151 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 47.Nevian T, Larkum ME, Polsky A, Schiller J. Properties of basal dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons: a direct patch-clamp recording study. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:206–214. doi: 10.1038/nn1826. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Normann C, et al. Associative long-term depression in the hippocampus is dependent on postsynaptic N-type Ca2+ channels. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:8290–8297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-08290.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Markram H, Lubke J, Frotscher M, Roth A, Sakmann B. Physiology and anatomy of synaptic connections between thick tufted pyramidal neurones in the developing rat neocortex. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 1997;500:409–440. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1997.sp022031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kalisman N, Silberberg G, Markram H. The neocortical microcircuit as a tabula rasa. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:880–885. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407088102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schmidt H, et al. Axonal synapse sorting in medial entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2017;549:469–475. doi: 10.1038/nature24005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kasthuri N, et al. Saturated reconstruction of a volume of neocortex. Cell. 2015;162:648–661. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.06.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bloss EB, et al. Single excitatory axons form clustered synapses onto CA1 pyramidal cell dendrites. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:353–363. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0084-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Yang SN, Tang YG, Zucker RS. Selective induction of LTP and LTD by postsynaptic [Ca2+]i elevation. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;81:781–787. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.2.781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rubin JE, Gerkin RC, Bi GQ, Chow CC. Calcium time course as a signal for spike-timing-dependent plasticity. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93:2600–2613. doi: 10.1152/jn.00803.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Graupner, M. & Brunel, N. Mechanisms of induction and maintenance of spike-timing dependent plasticity in biophysical synapse models. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4, 136 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 57.Karmarkar UR, Buonomano DV. A model of spike-timing dependent plasticity: one or two coincidence detectors? J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:507–513. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.1.507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Shouval HZ, Bear MF, Cooper LN. A unified model of NMDA receptor-dependent bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:10831–10836. doi: 10.1073/pnas.152343099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lee WC, et al. Anatomy and function of an excitatory network in the visual cortex. Nature. 2016;532:370–374. doi: 10.1038/nature17192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Wilson DE, Whitney DE, Scholl B, Fitzpatrick D. Orientation selectivity and the functional clustering of synaptic inputs in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1003–1009. doi: 10.1038/nn.4323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Scholl, B., Wilson, D. E. & Fitzpatrick, D. Local order within global disorder: synaptic architecture of visual space. Neuron96, 1127–1138.e4 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 62.Lee, K. S., Vandemark, K., Mezey, D., Shultz, N. & Fitzpatrick, D. Functional synaptic architecture of callosal inputs in mouse primary visual cortex. Neuron101, 421–428.e5 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 63.Beaulieu-Laroche L, Harnett MT. Dendritic spines prevent synaptic voltage clamp. Neuron. 2018;97:75–82 e73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Mitchell DE, Martineau E, Tazerart S, Araya R. Probing single synapses via the photolytic release of neurotransmitters. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 2019;11:19. doi: 10.3389/fnsyn.2019.00019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Araya R, Eisenthal KB, Yuste R. Dendritic spines linearize the summation of excitatory potentials. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2006;103:18799–18804. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609225103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Fino E, et al. RuBi-glutamate: two-photon and visible-light photoactivation of neurons and dendritic spines. Front. Neural Circuits. 2009;3:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.002.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kirkwood A, Lee HK, Bear MF. Co-regulation of long-term potentiation and experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in visual cortex by age and experience. Nature. 1995;375:328–331. doi: 10.1038/375328a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Crair MC, Malenka RC. A critical period for long-term potentiation at thalamocortical synapses. Nature. 1995;375:325–328. doi: 10.1038/375325a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Fox K. The critical period for long-term potentiation in primary sensory cortex. Neuron. 1995;15:485–488. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90136-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Le Bon-Jego M, Yuste R. Persistently active, pacemaker-like neurons in neocortex. Front. Neurosci. 2007;1:123–129. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.009.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rasband WS, Bright DS. NIH image—a public domain image-processing program for the Macintosh. Microbeam Anal. 1995;4:137–149. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Nimchinsky EA, Yasuda R, Oertner TG, Svoboda K. The number of glutamate receptors opened by synaptic stimulation in single hippocampal spines. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:2054–2064. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5066-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Holtmaat AJ, et al. Transient and persistent dendritic spines in the neocortex in vivo. Neuron. 2005;45:279–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ruttinger S, et al. Comparison and accuracy of methods to determine the confocal volume for quantitative fluorescence correlation spectroscopy. J. Microsc. 2008;232:343–352. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2818.2008.02105.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Weber JP, et al. Location-dependent synaptic plasticity rules by dendritic spine cooperativity. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11380. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Tazerart, S., Mitchell, D. E., Miranda-Rottmann, S. & Araya R. A spike-timing-dependent plasticity rule for single, clustered and distributed dendritic spines. 10.6084/m9.figshare.12627422 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available in figshare with the identifier doi: 10.6084/m9.figshare.1262742276.
All the codes used in the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.