Skip to main content
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science logoLink to Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science
. 2020 Aug 25;61(10):43. doi: 10.1167/iovs.61.10.43

Factors Associated with Impaired Motor Skills in Strabismic and Anisometropic Children

Krista R Kelly 1,2,, Sarah E Morale 1, Cynthia L Beauchamp 3, Lori M Dao 3, Becky A Luu 4, Eileen E Birch 1,2
PMCID: PMC7452850  PMID: 32845292

Abstract

Purpose

We evaluated motor skills in children diagnosed with strabismus and anisometropia, with or without amblyopia, and explored factors associated with impairments.

Methods

A total of 143 strabismic and anisometropic children 3 to 13 years of age (96 amblyopic, 47 nonamblyopic) and a group of age-similar 35 control children completed Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance tasks from the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition. Raw scores were converted to standardized scores, and amblyopic and nonamblyopic children were compared to controls. Clinical and sensory factors associated with motor performance were also evaluated.

Results

Overall, amblyopic and nonamblyopic children were three to six times more likely than controls to be at risk for or to have a total motor impairment (≤15th percentile). Although amblyopic children scored lower than controls for the Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance tasks, nonamblyopic children scored lower on Manual Dexterity only. Factors related to manual dexterity deficits include the presence of amblyopia and binocularity deficits typical of these eye conditions. Aiming, catching, and balance deficits were most pronounced in children with an infantile onset of the eye condition, a history of strabismus, and reduced binocularity.

Conclusions

Amblyopia and strabismus disrupt the development of motor ability in children. These findings highlight the widespread effects of discordant binocular input early in life and the visual acuity and binocularity deficits typical of these eye conditions.

Keywords: amblyopia, strabismus, anisometropia, visuomotor development, binocularity


Amblyopia (lazy eye) is the most common cause of poor vision in one eye among children, affecting 2% to 4% of children in the United States.1 The most common causes of amblyopia are strabismus (misalignment of the visual axis) and anisometropia (unequal refractive error). The discordant binocular experience due to these pediatric eye conditions not only impairs visual acuity in the affected eye but also results in fellow eye deficits and disrupts the development of binocularity (e.g., decreased stereoacuity, interocular suppression).14 Amblyopia emerges during a critical period of brain maturation and has the potential to affect the performance of real-world tasks that require vision for development, including fine and gross motor skills.

Coordination of the eyes, hands, and body is essential for object manipulation (fine motor skills) and navigating the environment (gross motor skills). Studies investigating motor skills have focused on the effects of strabismus or the role that binocularity plays in development and have shown that normal stereoacuity and fusion are essential to task performance.516 Binocularity is disrupted in amblyopia; yet, only a small number of studies investigating motor skills have focused on the impact of amblyopia specifically, particularly in children. These studies have found that amblyopic children and adults have impaired fine motor skills, such as reaching, grasping, object manipulation, and transferring test answers to a multiple-choice form.1725 A limited number of studies have also found gross motor impairments in amblyopic adults and children performing tasks designed to assess postural stability and walking,2628 and such impairments can increase the risk of injury.29 However, these studies had heterogeneous groups with limited age ranges and small sample sizes, thus making it difficult to ascertain factors associated with motor impairments.

We evaluated fine and gross motor skills of children 3 to 13 years of age with a history of strabismus or anisometropia, with or without amblyopia, during binocular viewing using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition (MABC-2). Our goal was to determine the extent to which amblyopia affects the maturation of visuomotor ability and to explore clinical (etiology, age at diagnosis) and sensory (severity of amblyopia, binocularity) factors associated with any deficits.

Methods

Participants

Children 3 to 13 years of age diagnosed with strabismus and/or anisometropia (n = 143), with or without amblyopia, were referred to the Retina Foundation of the Southwest by pediatric ophthalmologists in the Dallas-Fort Worth area. Amblyopia was defined as an interocular difference in visual acuity of ≥0.2 logMAR, with best-corrected visual acuity (BCVA) in the fellow eye of ≤0.1 logMAR (20/25 or better; 0.3 logMAR [20/40] for ages 3−4 years).30 Strabismic children were initially diagnosed with esotropia but were aligned with surgery and/or spectacle correction within 6 prism diopters of orthotropia at the time of the test visit. Children with strabismus and anisometropia (i.e., combined mechanism) were included in the strabismus group. Age-similar control children (n = 35) who had age-normal visual acuity and stereoacuity and no history of vision disorders were also enrolled. All children were tested with their habitual spectacle correction, if required, which was confirmed by medical record review. No child enrolled in the study was born preterm (<37 weeks gestational age) or had co-existing ocular or systemic disease, congenital infections/malformations, or developmental delay. Medical records were obtained from referring ophthalmologists to extract information about diagnosis, current alignment, cycloplegic refraction, and prior treatment plans. English was the primary language for all children.

Ethics

The research protocol observed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki, was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, and conformed to the requirements of the U.S. Health Insurance Portability and Privacy Act. Informed consent was obtained from a parent or legal guardian, and assent was obtained from children ≥10 years of age prior to testing and after explanation of the study.

Procedure

Vision Assessment

Prior to motor skills testing, children had a vision assessment that included the following:

  • 1.

    Crowded monocular BCVA using the electronic Early Treatment for Diabetic Retinopathy Study (e-ETDRS) protocol31,32 (≥7 years of age) or the Amblyopia Treatment Study HOTV protocol33,34 (<7 years of age) to provide logMAR BCVA.

  • 2.

    Stereoacuity using the Randot Preschool Stereoacuity and Stereo Butterfly Tests,35,36 converted to log arcsec for analyses (ranging from 1.3 to 3.3 log arcsec); nil stereoacuity was arbitrarily assigned a value of 4 log arcsec.

  • 3.

    Extent of suppression scotoma using the Worth 4 Dot (W4D) test at seven different distances, measured as the farthest distance that the child reported four dots and converted to size of suppression scotoma in log degrees (larger numbers represent larger suppression scotomas; no fusion at the shortest distance was assigned a value of 1.2 log deg).37,38

  • 4.

    Depth of suppression using a computerized dichoptic eye chart that determines the non-preferred eye/preferred eye contrast ratio (i.e., balance point) at which the child can overcome interocular suppression and report letters presented to each eye with equal likelihood (Contrast Balance Index, CBI).2,39

Movement Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition

Each child was tested during binocular viewing with the MABC-240 (administered by KRK or SEM), a standardized test used to identify children with delay or impairment in motor development that is administered in three age bands (3–6, 7–10, and 11–16 years) and consists of Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance subscales with eight tasks in total (Table 1). The raw score for each task was converted into a standardized score using look-up tables provided with the MABC-2. Higher standardized scores indicate better performance. To compare a child's motor proficiency with published age-matched normative data,40 standardized scores per task were summed to produce subscale scores and then a Total Motor score, which was converted to a percentile (standard score of 10 = 50th percentile). According to MABC-2 instructions, children scoring ≤5th percentile have a significant total motor impairment, children scoring between 6th and 15th percentile are at risk for impairment, and children scoring ≥16th percentile are typically developed.

Table 1.

Age Bands and Tasks for the Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance Subscales

Age Band
3–6 y 7–10 y 11–13 y Outcome Measure
Manual dexterity
 Unimanual Post coins, one hand Place pegs, one hand Turn pegs, one hand Latency*
 Bimanual Thread beads Thread lace Build triangle Latency
 Drawing trail Stay inside lines Stay inside lines Stay inside lines Number of errors
Aiming and catching
 Aiming Throw beanbag anywhere on mat Throw beanbag in red circle on mat Throw ball at wall target Number of successful hits
 Catching Catch beanbag, two hands Catch ball, two hands Catch ball, one hand Number of successful catches
Balance
 Static balance Balance, one-leg One-board balance, one leg Two-board balance, two legs Latency
 Walking Walk heels raised Walk heel-to-toe forwards Walk heel-to-toe backwards Number of successful steps
 Jumping Jump on mats, two feet Jump on mats, one foot Zig-zag jump on mats, one foot Number of successful jumps§
*

Only the preferred hand was used for analysis for all age bands.

Total catching score (average of left and right hand) used for analysis for age band 11–13 years.

Total static balance score (average of left and right leg) used for analysis for age bands 3–6 years and 7–10 years.

§

Total jumping score (average of left and right leg) used for analysis for age bands 7–10 years and 11–16 years.

Statistical Analyses

Primary Analyses

Our primary goal was to determine the impact of amblyopia on motor performance. An odds ratios (OR) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) was calculated based on Total Motor percentile scores to determine whether amblyopic or nonamblyopic patients had a higher incidence of being categorized as being at risk or impaired (i.e., ≤15th percentile) compared with controls. We also assessed the difference in standardized scores for the Total Motor score and each subscale (Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, Balance) using a series of independent t-tests to compare amblyopic children and nonamblyopic children to control children (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.025). For subscales that were significantly different from controls, we assessed performance on each individual task.

Secondary Analyses

To determine factors related to Total Motor and subscale (Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, Balance) performance, we compared clinical and sensory factors among the patient group to controls using independent t-tests for the following:2

  • Age at diagnosis—infantile, <12 months; late onset, >12 months (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.025).

  • Etiology—strabismus (including combined mechanism); anisometropia (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.025).

  • Severity of current amblyopia—mild, 0.1 to 0.2 logMAR; moderate, 0.3–0.6 logMAR; severe, ≥0.7 logMAR (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.017)

  • Stereoacuity—normal, ≤60 arcsec; reduced, 100 to 800 arcsec; nil, not measurable (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.017).

  • Extent of suppression (W4D)—bifoveal/macular, −0.15 to 0.45 log deg; peripheral/none, 0.60 to 1.2 log deg (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.025)

  • Depth of suppression (CBI)—no suppression, ≤2; suppression, >2 (Bonferroni-corrected P = 0.025).

Results

A total of 178 children were enrolled and completed the visual acuity, stereoacuity, extent of suppression, and MABC-2 tests (96 patients completed the depth of suppression test). These included 96 amblyopic strabismic and/or anisometropic children (amblyopic; 43 females; mean age = 8.2 ± 2.5 years), 47 nonamblyopic strabismic and/or anisometropic children (nonamblyopic; 22 females; mean age 7.0 ± 2.6 years), and 35 normal control children (control; 18 females; mean age 8.3 ± 2.8 years). Control data for the MABC-2 have previously been published.41 The magnitude of mean ± SD anisometropia was 1.7 ± 0.8 diopters (D) in nonamblyopic anisometropic children and 3.3 ± 2.0 D in amblyopic anisometropic children. The majority of the anisometropic children had hyperopic anisometropia (74%). Amblyopic children did not differ in age from controls (P = 0.85). Nonamblyopic children trended toward being slightly younger than controls, but this trend was not significant (P = 0.033). Because scoring on the MABC-2 is standardized based on age, this was not an issue. Descriptive statistics for clinical and sensory information are provided in Table 2.

Table 2.

Group Characteristics

Characteristic Amblyopic (n = 96) Nonamblyopic (n = 47) Control (n = 35)
Sex (female), n (%) 43 (45) 22 (47) 18 (51)
Strabismus, n (%) 53 (55) 33 (70) n/a
 Strabismus only, n (%) 19 (36) 22 (67) n/a
 Strabismus and anisometropia, n (%) 34 (64) 11 (33) n/a
 Right eye affected, n (%) 25 (47) 22 (47)* n/a
Anisometropia, n (%) 43 (45) 14 (30) n/a
 Hyperopic, n (%) 33 (77) 9 (64) n/a
 Myopic, n (%) 5 (12) 1 (7) n/a
 Astigmatic, n (%) 5 (12) 4 (29) n/a
 Magnitude of anisometropia (D), mean ± SD 3.3 ± 2.0 1.7 ± 0.8 n/a
 Right eye affected, n (%) 24 (56) 7 (50)* n/a
Age (y), mean ± SD (range) 8.2 ± 2.5 (3.8 to 13.1) 7.0 ± 2.6 (3.3 to 13.1) 8.3 ± 2.8 (4.2 to 13.0)
AE BCVA (logMAR), mean ± SD 0.4 ± 0.3 0.1 ± 0.1 0.0 ± 0.1
 Snellen equivalent 20/50 ± 3 lines 20/25 ± 1 lines 20/20 ± 1 lines
 Range 0.1 to 1.9 –0.1 to 0.3 –0.1 to 0.1
FE BCVA (logMAR) mean ± SD 0.0 ± 0.1 0.1 ± 0.1 0.0 ± 0.1
 Snellen equivalent 20/20 ± 1 lines 20/25 ± 1 lines 20/20 ± 1 lines
 Range –0.1 to 0.2 –0.1 to 0.3 –0.1 to 0.1
Stereoacuity (log arcsec), mean ± SD (range) 3.4 ± 0.8 (1.8 to 4) 3.1 ± 1.0 (1.6 to 4) 1.6 ± 0.2 (1.3 to 2.0)
Extent of suppression (log deg), mean ± SD (range) 0.4 ± 0.4 (–0.2 to 1.2) 0.3 ± 0.6 (–0.2 to 1.2) –0.2 ± 0.0 (−0.2 to −0.2)
Depth of suppression (CBI), mean ± SD (range) 4.8 ± 3.6 (0.2 to 11.0) 3.0 ± 2.9 (0.2 to 11.0) n/a

AE, amblyopic eye; FE, fellow eye.

*

For nonamblyopic children, the affected eye was either the at-risk or previously amblyopic eye, or the right eye if the child was never amblyopic.

For nonamblyopic children, either the previously amblyopic eye or the right eye if the child was never amblyopic is listed. For normal control children, the right eye is listed.

For children who were never amblyopic and control children, the left eye is listed.

Total Motor and Subscale Performance

Compared with just 11% (4/35) of controls, 45% (43/96) of amblyopic children and 28% (13/47) of nonamblyopic children were categorized as being at risk for or having a Total Motor impairment (amblyopic, OR = 6.3, 95% CI = 2.1–19.2, Z = 3.2, P = 0.001; nonamblyopic, OR = 3.0, 95% CI = 0.9–10.1, Z = 1.7, P = 0.08). Compared with the Total Motor standardized scores for controls (9.8 ± 2.3), the scores were lower for amblyopic (7.4 ± 2.7; t129 = 4.8, P < 0.001) and nonamblyopic (8.2 ± 2.9; t80 = 2.7, P = 0.009) children (Fig.).

Figure.

Figure.

Mean total motor and subscale standard scores for amblyopic children (light gray bars), nonamblyopic children (white bars), and control children (dark gray bars). Error bars represent ± SEM. Dashed lines represent 50th percentile. *Significantly different from controls (Bonferonni-corrected).

Amblyopic children scored lower than controls on the Manual Dexterity (7.6 ± 2.7 vs. 10.2 ± 3.0; t129 = 4.7, P < 0.001), Aiming and Catching (8.8 ± 3.4 vs. 10.3 ± 2.9; t129 = 2.3, P = 0.023), and Balance (7.8 ± 3.1 vs. 9.3 ± 2.4; t129 = 2.6, P = 0.012) subscales (Fig.). Within the Manual Dexterity scale, amblyopic children scored lower than controls for unimanual dexterity (8.2 ± 3.2 vs. 9.8 ± 2.7; t129 = 2.5, P = 0.012), bimanual dexterity (7.9 ± 3.2 vs. 9.7 ± 3.5; t129 = 2.7, P = 0.008), and the drawing trail (7.0 ± 3.9 vs. 9.7 ± 2.9; t129 = 3.7, P < 0.001). Within the Aiming and Catching scale, amblyopic children scored lower than controls for catching (8.1 ± 3.2 vs. 10.1 ± 2.6; t129 = 3.2, P = 0.002) but not aiming (9.3 ± 3.2 vs. 9.9 ± 2.8; t129 = 1.1, P = 0.28). Within the Balance scale, amblyopic children scored lower than controls for static balance (7.9 ± 2.9 vs. 9.2 ± 2.8; t129 = 2.3, P = 0.022) and jumping (9.3 ± 3.5 vs. 10.9 ± 2.1; t129 = 2.6, P = 0.010) and trended toward scoring lower for walking, but this trend was not significant (7.2 ± 4.0 vs. 8.7 ± 3.8; t129 = 2.0, P = 0.05).

Nonamblyopic children scored lower than controls on the Manual Dexterity subscale (8.5 ± 3.0 vs. 10.2 ± 3.0; t80 = 2.5, P = 0.014) but not on the Aiming and Catching (9.3 ± 3.2 vs. 10.3 ± 2.9; t80 = 1.5, P = 0.14) or Balance (8.3 ± 3.1 vs. 9.3 ± 2.4; t80 = 1.5, P = 0.14) subscales (Fig.). Within the Manual Dexterity scale, nonamblyopic children did not differ from controls for unimanual dexterity (9.2 ± 3.1 vs. 9.8 ± 2.7; t80 = 0.9, P = 0.39) or bimanual dexterity (8.8 ± 3.1 vs. 9.7 ± 3.5; t80 = 1.3, P = 0.21), but they did score lower than controls for the drawing trail (7.5 ± 3.8 vs. 9.7 ± 2.9; t80 = 2.8, P = 0.006).

Clinical Factors Associated with Motor Performance

Etiology

(See Table 3.) Strabismic children (n = 86) scored lower than controls for all three subscales of the MABC-2 (all P ≤ 0.013). Anisometropic children (n = 57) scored lower than controls for Manual Dexterity only (P = 0.002). Those with combined mechanism (strabismus and anisometropia) were categorized to the strabismus group. We explored whether children with strabismus alone (n = 41) differed from those with strabismus and anisometropia (n = 45). No significant differences were found for any of the subscales or the Total Motor score (all P ≥ 0.40).

Table 3.

Factors Affecting Motor Performance

Subscales, Mean (SD)
Factor n Manual Dexterity Aiming and Catching Balance
Control 35 10.2 (3.0) 10.3 (2.9) 9.3 (2.4)
Etiology
 Strabismus 86 7.7 (2.7)* 8.7 (3.2)* 7.7 (3.1)*
 Anisometropia 57 8.2 (2.9)* 9.3 (3.5) 8.4 (3.1)
Age at onset
 Infantile 27 7.2 (3.1)* 8.2 (3.1)* 7.5 (3.4)*
 Late 116 8.1 (2.7)* 9.2 (3.4) 8.1 (3.0)
Severity of amblyopia
 None 47 8.5 (3.0)* 9.3 (3.2) 8.3 (3.1)
 Mild 32 7.6 (2.5)* 8.9 (3.8) 8.9 (3.7)
 Moderate 48 7.5 (2.9)* 8.9 (3.4) 7.3 (2.9)*
 Severe 16 8.0 (2.4)* 8.5 (2.7) 7.1 (1.8)*
Stereoacuity
 Normal 13 9.7 (2.8) 9.9 (3.5) 9.5 (2.6)
 Subnormal 38 7.5 (3.0)* 9.6 (3.7) 7.5 (3.1)*
 Nil 92 7.8 (2.6)* 8.6 (3.1)* 7.9 (3.1)
Extent of suppression
 Bifoveal–macular fusion 92 8.0 (2.8)* 9.3 (3.4) 8.1 (3.0)
 Peripheral–no fusion 51 7.7 (2.8)* 8.4 (3.2)* 7.6 (3.2)*
Depth of suppression
 None 35 8.7 (2.6) 9.1 (3.6) 8.1 (3.0)
 Suppression 61 7.3 (2.8)* 8.8 (3.4) 7.9 (3.3)
*

Indicates significantly lower scores than controls (Bonferonni-corrected).

Age of Diagnosis

Children with infantile onset (n = 27) scored lower than controls for all three subscales (all P ≤ 0.022). Children with late onset (n = 116) scored lower than controls for manual dexterity only (P < 0.001).

Sensory Factors Associated with Motor Performance

Severity of Amblyopia

Children with mild amblyopia (n = 32) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity only (P < 0.001). Children with moderate amblyopia (n = 48) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity (P < 0.001) and Balance (P = 0.001). Children with severe amblyopia (n = 16) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity (P = 0.014) and Balance (P = 0.002). As shown in the primary analysis, nonamblyopic children scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity only. For children with no amblyopia, there was no difference between those who never had amblyopia (n = 16) and those whose amblyopia was successfully treated (n = 31; all P > 0.09).

Stereoacuity

Children with normal stereoacuity (n = 13) scored similar to controls on all three subscales (all P ≥ 0.62). Children with subnormal stereoacuity (n = 38) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity (P < 0.001) and Balance (P = 0.007). Children with nil stereoacuity (n = 92) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity (P < 0.001) and Aiming and Catching (P = 0.006).

Extent of Suppression (W4D)

Children with bifoveal/macular fusion (n = 51) scored lower than controls for Manual Dexterity only (P < 0.001). Children with peripheral/no fusion (n = 38) scored lower than controls for all three subscales (all P ≤ 0.011).

Depth of Suppression (CBI)

A subset of 96 patients competed the depth of suppression task at their motor skills testing visit. Children with no suppression (n = 35) did not score differently than controls on any subscale. Children with suppression (n = 61) scored lower than controls on Manual Dexterity (P < 0.001) only.

Discussion

Our cohort of children with strabismus or anisometropia was more likely to be at risk for or to have a significant Total Motor impairment than controls, with amblyopia doubling the odds. Total Motor scores incorporate individual subscale scores for the Manual Dexterity, Aiming and Catching, and Balance subscales, on all of which amblyopic children scored low compared with controls. Nonamblyopic children had lower scores for Manual Dexterity only, consistent with amblyopia playing a role to increase risk of motor deficits.

Manual dexterity deficits have been reported previously in children with amblyopia.2025 We have also reported lower scores for all manual dexterity tasks in amblyopic children; nonamblyopic children scored lower for the drawing trail only. In the MABC-2 manual dexterity scale, the unilateral (e.g., coins in a slot) and bimanual (e.g., threading beads) tasks are scored by time to completion, whereas the drawing trail task is scored by the number of drawing errors. Webber et al.23 also described more prominent deficits for timed rather than precise manual dexterity tasks in amblyopic children. Amblyopic children are slower at planning and executing reaching movements, with a less precise grasp than controls. However, compensatory strategies may change with age; younger children (5–7 years old) spend longer than controls in the final approach and rely more on visual feedback to guide movement, whereas older children (7–9 years old) spend longer in the object contact phase and rely more on tactile feedback for grasping.20,22 During reaching and grasping, amblyopic adults exhibit reduced hand peak velocity and acceleration and prolonged acceleration duration, in addition to making more errors, indicating that manual dexterity deficits persist with age.1719,42,43

Although the presence of amblyopia was a major factor affecting motor skills, we were unable to identify an association between the severity of amblyopia and manual dexterity performance; all categories scored lower than controls. We did, however, find that poor binocularity also played a large role in manual dexterity deficits. Children with reduced or nil stereoacuity, peripheral or no fusion, and suppression scored lower than controls for the Manual Dexterity subscale, whereas those with normal binocularity did not. Previous studies have shown the importance of good binocularity in manual dexterity performance.18,20,22

Amblyopic children scored lower than controls on the Aiming and Catching subscale, primarily due to poor catching performance. Previous studies have reported worse catching, but not aiming, for amblyopic and strabismic children, as well as children with deprivation amblyopia.10,27,41 Aiming may be a less difficult task than catching. Aiming requires visual input for planning the throw and some visual feedback about the position of the arm and hand during the throw.44 Catching is an interceptive task that requires sophisticated spatiotemporal coordination of the arm and hand motion with target motion to reach the interception point at an appropriate time and configuration for grasping, and then tightening the hand's grip.45 Control of the grasping movement may be the most important indicator of catching performance,46 with poorer catching in stereodeficient individuals being due to less hand closure and lower peak closing hand velocity when grasping a ball.14 In our study, children with reduced or nil stereoacuity and peripheral or no fusion scored lower than controls on the Aiming and Catching subscale, further supporting the importance of binocularity in ball skills.

Amblyopic children in our study also scored lower on the Balance subscale, primarily due to poor static balance (postural stability) and jumping performance. Severity of amblyopia was a factor in poor balance, with moderate and severe amblyopia scoring lower than controls. Previous studies have reported impaired static balance for both amblyopic children and nonamblyopic children with strabismus27,28; however, these studies included strabismic children with ≥10 prism diopters, which could have reduced the field of view during balance testing. Our nonamblyopic group included only strabismic children aligned within 6 prism diopters of orthotropia at the time of testing, which may account for the inconsistency among studies. Amblyopic and strabismic children and adults are more cautious than controls when navigating the environment; for example, they take shorter steps during natural walking, reduce their walking velocity, and increase their toe clearance when navigating high obstacles.9,26 In our study, children with reduced or nil stereoacuity and peripheral or no fusion also scored lower than controls on the Balance subscale, suggesting a large role of binocular vision.

Manual dexterity deficits were pervasive across all factors, with the exception of those with normal stereoacuity and no suppression. Children that were the most impaired on multiple scales were strabismic, had an infantile onset of their eye condition, and had poor binocularity. Children that were the least impaired on multiple scales were anisometropic, had a late onset, and had good binocularity. Strabismus can result in more severe binocularity deficits than anisometropia, even after the eyes are aligned, and especially when the onset is infantile.47 In our study, strabismic children had poorer stereoacuity (3.7 ± 0.64 vs. 2.7 ± 0.89 log arcsec; P < 0.001) and stronger suppression (0.54 ± 0.47 vs. 0.17 ± 0.39 log deg; P < 0.001) than those with anisometropia, although affected eye visual acuity was no different between the groups (0.31 ± 0.30 vs. 0.33 ± 0.33; P = 0.64). Binocular cues such as vergence and stereopsis provide essential information for distance and location judgments, as well as three-dimensional object properties during motor tasks.26,48

Even in stereodeficient individuals, covering one eye affects gait during walking, suggesting that both eyes contribute and that field of view is important during navigation.26 Use of binocular cues emerges in infancy but continues to develop during early childhood.4951 Thus, discordant binocular input during infancy and childhood may disrupt the ability to use these cues during motor development,22,52 which is evident by our finding of poorer performance with an infantile onset of the eye condition. In our study, children with anisometropia had a later mean age at diagnosis than those with strabismus (4.8 ± 2.4 vs. 2.6 ± 2.0 years; P < 0.001). Although better binocularity in anisometropic children likely contributed to the lack of deficits seen for the Aiming and Catching and Balance subscales, the later onset of anisometropia may also play a role.53 Children with normal stereoacuity and normal depth of suppression in our study did not score lower than controls on any subscale, consistent with earlier studies showing better motor performance in those with recovered binocularity19,20 and suggesting that binocularity is essential to task performance.

Motor impairments may adversely influence a child's life and may cause difficulties in the classroom, especially in earlier grades when learning requires manipulating objects for counting and vocabulary. For later grades, children with amblyopia and strabismus are slow to transfer answers to a multiple-choice form,21 which could affect their performance on timed, standardized tests. Motor deficits may also cause difficulties playing sports and navigating the environment in everyday activities, with an increase in the risk of injury and falls.29 Finally, motor impairments may affect self-esteem and self-perception.24,25,54 Lower self-perception is evident in amblyopic children, due in part to lower scores for physical competence and peer acceptance related to worse manual dexterity and ball skills.24,25 Successful treatment of the visual acuity deficits and binocular dysfunction that accompany amblyopia and strabismus may lead to improved motor abilities.55

Our study had limitations. It is difficult to tease apart the individual contributions of sensory and clinical factors as they often co-exist to varying degrees (e.g., strabismus, reduced stereoacuity, suppression). In any case, it is clear that a history of discordant binocular input during visual development also impacts the development of motor ability. Finally, we were unable to control for experience with motor skills; however, many of the children in this study participated in physical recreational activities.

Conclusions

Amblyopia and strabismus disrupt the development of motor ability in children. Manual dexterity deficits appear to be secondary to the visual acuity and binocularity deficits typical of these eye conditions. Factors associated with aiming, catching, and balance deficits include an infantile onset of the eye condition, a history of strabismus, and reduced binocularity. These findings highlight the widespread effects of discordant binocular input early in life.

Acknowledgments

Supported by grants from the National Eye Institute (EY022313, EY028224) and by the Knights Templar Eye Foundation.

Preliminary results were presented at the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus 2016 annual meeting in Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.

Disclosure: K.R. Kelly, None; S.E. Morale, None; C.L. Beauchamp, None; L.M. Dao, None; B.A. Luu, None; E.E. Birch, None

References

  • 1. Birch EE. Amblyopia and binocular vision. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2013; 33: 67–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Birch EE, Morale SE, Jost RM, et al.. Assessing suppression in amblyopic children with a dichoptic eye chart. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57: 5649–5654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Birch EE, Kelly KR, Giaschi DE. Fellow eye deficits in amblyopia. J Binocul Vis Ocul Motil. 2019; 69: 116–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Meier K, Giaschi D. Unilateral amblyopia affects two eyes: fellow eye deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2017; 58: 1779–1800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Alramis F, Roy E, Christian L, Niechwiej-Szwedo E. Contribution of binocular vision to the performance of complex manipulation tasks in 5–13 years old visually-normal children. Hum Mov Sci. 2016; 46: 52–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Gaertner C, Creux C, Orssaud C, Dufier J. Postural control in nonamblyopic children with early-onset strabismus. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2013; 54: 529–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF, Clarke TJ, Elliot KM. Binocular vision and prehension in middle childhood. Neuropsychologia. 2003; 41: 415–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Dickmann A, Di E, Simbolotti C, et al.. Balance in subjects with congenital or early onset strabismus: influence of age. Neurosci Lett. 2016; 623: 28–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Odenrick P, Sandstedt P, Lennerstrand L. Postural sway and gait of children with convergent strabismus. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1984; 26: 495–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Caputo R, Tinelli F, Bancale A, et al.. Motor coordination in children with congenital strabismus: effects of late surgery. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2007; 11: 285–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Hemptinne C, Aerts F, Pellissier T, Vanderveken C. Motor skills in children with strabismus. J AAPOS. 2020; 24: 76.e1–76.e6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Hrisos S, Clarke MP, Kelly T, Hederson J, Wright CM. Unilateral visual impairment and neurodevelopmental performance in preschool children. Br J Ophthalmol. 2006; 90: 836–838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Lions C, Quoc EB, Wiener-Vacher S, Maria P. Postural control in strabismic children: importance of proprioceptive information. Front Physiol. 2014; 5: 156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Mazyn LIN, Lenoir M, Montagne G, Savelsbergh GJP. The contribution of stereo vision to one-handed catching. Exp Brain Res. 2004; 157: 383–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. O'Connor AR, Birch EE, Anderson S, Draper H. Relationship between binocular vision, visual acuity, and fine motor skills. Optom Vis Sci. 2010; 87: 942–947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. O'Connor AR, Birch EE, Anderson S, Draper H. The functional significance of stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 2019–2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Buckley JG, Pacey IE, Panesar GK, Scally A, Barrett BT. Prehension of a flanked target in individuals with amblyopia. Invest Opthalmology Vis Sci. 2015; 56: 7568–7580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Grant S, Melmoth DR, Morgan MJ, Finlay AL. Prehension deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007; 48: 1139–1148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Grant S, Conway ML. Reach-to-precision grasp deficits in amblyopia: effects of object contrast and low visibility. Vision Res. 2015; 114: 100–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Grant S, Suttle C, Melmoth DR, Conway ML, Sloper JJ. Age- and stereovision-dependent eye-hand coordination deficits in children with amblyopia and abnormal binocularity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 5687–5701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Kelly KR, Jost RM, De La Cruz A, Birch EE. Multiple-choice answer form completion time in children with amblyopia and strabismus. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018; 136: 938–941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Suttle CM, Melmoth DR, Finlay AL, Sloper JJ, Grant S. Eye-hand coordination skills in children with and without amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 1851–1864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Webber A, Wood J, Gole G, Brown B. The effect of amblyopia on fine motor skills in children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 594–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Birch EE, Castañeda YS, Cheng-Patel CS, et al.. Self-perception in children aged 3 to 7 years with amblyopia and its association with deficits in vision and fine motor skills. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2019; 137: 499–506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Birch EE, Castañeda YS, Cheng-Patel CS, et al.. Self-perception of school-aged children with amblyopia and its association with reading speed and motor skills. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2019; 137: 167–173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Buckley JG, Panesar GK, MacLellan MJ, Pacey IE, Barrett BT. Changes to control of adaptive gait in individuals with long-standing reduced stereoacuity. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51: 2487–2495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Engel-Yeger B. Evaluation of gross motor abilities and self perception in children with amblyopia. Disabil Rehabil. 2008; 30: 243–248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Zipori AB, Colpa L, Wong AMF, Cushing SL, Gordon KA. Postural stability and visual impairment: assessing balance in children with strabismus and amblyopia. PLoS One. 2018; 13: e0205857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Pineles SL, Repka MX, Yu F, Lum F. Risk of musculoskeletal injuries, fractures, and falls in Medicare beneficiaries with disorders of binocular vision. JAMA Ophthalmol. 2015; 133: 60–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Drover J, Felius J, Cheng C, Morale S, Wyatt L, Birch E. Normative pediatric visual acuity using single surrounded HOTV optotypes on the electronic visual acuity tester following the amblyopia treatment study protocol. J AAPOS. 2008; 12: 145–149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Beck R, Moke P, Turpin A, et al.. A computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the early treatment of diabetic retinopathy study testing protocol. Am J Ophthalmol. 2003; 135: 194–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Cotter SA, Chu RH, Chandler DL, et al.. Reliability of the electronic early treatment diabetic retinopathy study testing protocol in children 7 to <13 years old. Am J Ophthalmol. 2003; 136: 655–661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Holmes JM, Beck RW, Repka MX, et al.. The amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol. Arch Ophthalmol. 2001; 119: 1345–1353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Moke PS, Turpin AH, Beck RW, et al.. Computerized method of visual acuity testing: adaptation of the amblyopia treatment study visual acuity testing protocol. Am J Ophthalmol. 2001; 132: 903–909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Birch E, Williams C, Drover J, et al.. Randot Preschool Stereoacuity Test: normative data and validity. J AAPOS. 2008; 12: 23–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Birch E, Williams C, Hunter J, Lapa M. Random dot stereoacuity of preschool children. ALSPAC “Children in Focus” study team. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1997; 34: 217–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Kelly KR, Jost RM, Wang Y, et al.. Improved binocular outcomes following binocular treatment for childhood amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2018; 59: 1221–1228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Rosenbaum A, Santiago A, eds. Clinical Strabismus Management. Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Webber AL, Wood JM, Thompson B, Birch EE. From suppression to stereoacuity: a composite binocular function score for clinical research. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2019; 39: 53–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Henderson S. E., Sugden D. A., & Barnett AL. Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2. 2nd ed London: Pearson Assessment; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Kelly KR, Morale SE, Wang SX, Stager DR, Birch EE. Impaired fine motor skills in children following extraction of a dense congenital or infantile unilateral cataract. J AAPOS. 2019; 23: 330.e1–330.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Wong AMF. Effects of strabismic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior: part II. Visually guided reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014; 55: 3857–3865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, et al.. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, part 2: visually guided reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011; 52: 795–803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Smirnov AS, Alikovskaia TA, Ermakov PN, et al.. Dart throwing with the open and closed eyes: kinematic analysis. Comput Math Methods Med. 2019; 2019: 4217491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Carnahan H, Mcfadyen BJ. Visuomotor control when reaching toward and grasping moving targets. Acta Psychol. 1996; 92: 17–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Cesqui B, Russo M, Lacquaniti F, Avella A. Grasping in one-handed catching in relation to performance. PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0158606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Fawcett SL, Wang Y-ZZ, Birch EE. The critical period for susceptibility of human stereopsis. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005; 46: 521–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Melmoth DR, Grant S. Advantages of binocular vision for the control of reaching and grasping. Exp Brain Res. 2006; 171: 371–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Giaschi D, Narasimhan S, Solski A, Harrison E, Wilcox LM. On the typical development of stereopsis: fine and coarse processing. Vision Res. 2013; 89: 65–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Birch E, Petrig B. FPL and VEP measures of fusion, stereopsis and stereoacuity in normal infants. Vision Res. 1996; 36: 1321–1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Yang Q, Bucci MP, Kapoula Z. The latency of saccades, vergence, and combined eye movements in children and in adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002; 43: 2939–2949. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Watt SJ, Bradshaw MF. The visual control of reaching and grasping: Binocular disparity and motion parallax. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2003; 29: 404–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Deng L, Gwiazda JE. Anisometropia in children from infancy to 15 years. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012; 53: 3782–3787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Webber AL, Wood JM, Gole GA, Brown B. Effect of amblyopia on self-esteem in children. Optom Vis Sci. 2008; 85: 1074–1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Webber AL, Wood JM, Thompson B. Fine motor skills of children with amblyopia improve following binocular treatment. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016; 57: 4713–4720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science are provided here courtesy of Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology

RESOURCES