Skip to main content
Nutrition Journal logoLink to Nutrition Journal
. 2020 Aug 28;19:92. doi: 10.1186/s12937-020-00609-w

Perceived stress and diet quality in women of reproductive age: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Karim Khaled 1, Fotini Tsofliou 1,2,, Vanora Hundley 2, Rebecca Helmreich 3, Orouba Almilaji 4
PMCID: PMC7456060  PMID: 32859204

Abstract

Background

Poor diet quality is associated with obesity-related morbidity and mortality. Psychological stress can increase unhealthy dietary choices, but evidence pertinent to women of reproductive age remains unclear. This paper systematically reviewed the literature to determine the association between psychological stress and diet quality in women of reproductive age.

Methods

Medline, CINAHL, Scopus, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, and Sciencedirect were searched. Data extraction was determined by the PEO. Inclusion criteria consisted of: English language, stress (exposure) measured in combination with diet quality (outcome), healthy women of reproductive age (18–49 years old (population)). Observational studies, due to the nature of the PEO, were included. Quality assessment used the Risk of Bias in Non-randomised Studies from the Cochrane Handbook of Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Meta-analysis was conducted using random-effect model to estimate the Fisher’s z transformed correlation between stress and diet quality with 95% confidence interval (CI).

Results

From 139,552 hits, 471 papers were screened; 24 studies met the inclusion criteria and were conducted in different countries: 8 studies on diet quality and 16 on food intake and frequency of consumption. Studies of diet quality consisted of six cross-sectional and two longitudinal designs with a total of 3982 participants. Diet quality was measured with diverse indices; Alternate Healthy Eating Index (n = 2), Healthy Eating Index (n = 2), Dietary Approach to Stop Hypertension (DASH) Diet Index (n = 2), Dietary Quality Index- Pregnancy (n = 2), and Dietary Guideline Adherence Index (n = 1). Most studies used Cohen’s perceived stress scale and no study measured biological stress response. After sensitivity analysis, only 5 studies (3471 participants) were included in the meta-analysis. Meta-analysis revealed a significant negative association between stress and diet quality with substantial heterogeneity between studies (r = − 0.35, 95% CI [− 0.56; − 0.15], p value < 0.001, Cochran Q test P < 0.0001, I2 = 93%).

The 16 studies of food intake and frequency of consumption were very heterogeneous in the outcome measure and were not included in the meta-analysis. These studies showed that stress was significantly associated with unhealthy dietary patterns (high in fat, sweets, salt, and fast food and low in fruits, vegetables, fish, and unsaturated fats).

Conclusion

Future studies that explore diet quality/patterns should include both diet indices and factor analysis and measure biological markers of stress and dietary patterns simultaneously.

Keywords: Diet quality, Diet, Stress, Women, Reproductive age, Systematic review, Meta-analysis

Background

The rate of obesity has increased alarmingly in the past twenty years across all age groups, especially among young adults [1]. In women of reproductive age, obesity is associated with type-2-diabetes, hypertension, decreased fertility and delayed conception, high birthweight and congenital anomalies [24]. These women are at increased risk of obesity related morbidity and mortality especially during pregnancy when metabolic complications might deteriorate and cause gestational diabetes, pre-eclampsia, miscarriage, and various cardiovascular disorders putting both the mother and baby at increased health risk [5]. Preventing weight gain in women of reproductive age through healthy diet is crucial and would benefit the next generation [6, 7]. Poor dietary patterns are major predictors of increased adiposity and a higher diet quality is associated with reduced risk of obesity-related metabolic disorders [6, 8]. Recently, diet patterns have been derived in nutrition epidemiological studies by measuring the whole diet instead of single nutrients [9]. Indeed, the overall food pattern is considered a more realistic approach to investigate the association between diseases and food consumption rather than single nutrients [9]. Diet patterns/quality can be estimated via a posteriori approach based on statistical methods such as factor analysis, or a priori- defined diet quality score which measures adherence to specific dietary pattern indices such as the Mediterranean Diet Index [10]. These healthy dietary patterns (e.g. Mediterranean diet) have been associated with decreased risk of cardiovascular disease, diabetes, cancer, and hypertension in women of reproductive age, and this is why they are used to measure diet patterns/quality in recent epidemiologic studies [11, 12].

There are several factors that might affect diet patterns/quality such as adiposity, smoking, age, income, educational level, race/ ethnicity, marital status, and psychological factors [13, 14]. Particularly, there has been a growing interest in the role of stress in relation to human health [15, 16]. Stress is defined as an individual’s perception, appraisal, and response to a stimulus exhibited by the surrounding environment [17], and it happens when the person’s adaptive capacity is surpassed by the stimuli and demands of the environment [18]. Stress has been associated with diet patterns in young adults, and the dietary responses to stress are individualized [19, 20]. For example, some reviews and longitudinal studies investigated the effects of stress on energy intake and have found that with high levels of stress, 40% of people eat more, 40% eat less, and 20% eat the same amount of food compared to that consumed in the absence of stress [2123]. The variance in the response to stress might be due to the duration of exposure to stress, the type of stressor, and the variation in the level of hunger and satiety at the start of the studies [24]. For example, mild/chronic stressors (such as long-term poverty, unemployment, unhappy marriage, etc.) increase the desire for food intake and binge eating, while sever/acute stressors (such as an upcoming work deadline or exam) induce restriction of food intake [24]. It is fundamental in this context to understand the types of food that are consumed and restricted under stress in order to estimate its health consequences. In general, studies have reported that highly stressed participants tend to consume hyper-palatable foods that are high caloric, low nutrient-dense (e.g. butter, cream cheese, full-fat products), and high fat foods even when there is no hunger or bodily demand for food [2527]. The effects of stress have been found to be exacerbated in obese (BMI > 30 kg/m2) compared to normal weight individuals because the former have higher insulin resistance than the latter and demonstrate significantly higher activation of brain reward regions when exposed to stress [24, 28].

Recent studies among young adults and university students have found that perceived stress is a serious contributor to low diet quality [29, 30]. The majority of these studies have focused on food groups (such as fat intake) as a result of stress, rather than assessing the diet quality (a priori/ a posteriori) [3032]. For example, there is evidence that females (18–29 years old), who report high levels of perceived stress (measured through the 14-item perceived stress scale), consume more fat than non-stressed females as assessed by the Night Eating Questionnaire [3032]. When fruits and vegetables consumption was assessed in women of reproductive age, perceived stress was found to significantly decrease their intake [15, 16, 3336]. Studies that have examined stress and diet have been limited in their approach. Habhab et al. [31] assessed the association between perceived stress and diet in females of reproductive age and found that participants in the high stress group (given unsolvable Sudoku) consumed more fats and sweets (measured through the Emotional Eating subscale) than individuals in the low stress group (given easy Sudoku). However, the sample size was small (40 participants), baseline hunger status was not measured, and the assignment of participants to low or high stress groups might have by chance assigned stressed individuals to the high stress group. In a study by Barrington et al. [37], higher levels of perceived stress were associated with higher fast food consumption in young women. However, the study used non validated single item scale to measure fast food intake.

In summary, the picture regarding the association between stress and diet in women of reproductive age remains unclear. This has gained attention recently, especially that diet-related diseases have been trending over the past few years among these women and studying the factors that might affect diet (such as stress) became crucial. To our knowledge, this is the first review of the association between stress and dietary patterns/quality specifically in women of reproductive age. The aim of this systematic review is to critically appraise the current literature and identify whether women who exhibit higher levels of stress have a poorer diet pattern/quality than women who exhibit lower levels of stress.

Methods

The Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) was used to guide this systematic review [38]. The association between psychological stress and diet quality was examined using the PEO (Population, Exposure, and Outcome) model: Population (women aged 18–49 years old), Exposure (Psychological Stress), Outcome (Diet Quality/Patterns of women of reproductive age).

Search strategy

A literature search was conducted in December 2019 in Medline complete, CINAHL Complete, Scopus, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, and Sciencedirect. These databases were searched using appropriate key words and index terms where the PEO (Population, Exposure, and Outcome) model framed the search process (Table 1 in Additional file 1). The key words were then combined by the EBSCO host operator AND/OR. The databases search was limited to human studies and English language articles published between 2000 and 2019. The search strategy (Title/Abstract) is demonstrated in Additional file 1.

Table 1.

Characteristics extracted from the 24 included studies: BS (Breakfast skippers), BE (Breakfast eaters), CS (Cross-Sectional), LG (Longitudinal), y (years), m (months), FFQ (Food Frequency Questionnaire), WFR (Weigh food record), SES (Socioeconomic status), PA (Physical Activity), AM (Anthropometric measures), − (not reported)

Author, Year Country Age and Number of Participants Study Design Participants in Study Dietary Assessment Tool Confounding Factors Identified
8 studies on Diet Quality
Richardson et al. 2015 [39] USA 18–44 y, N = 101 CS Women who had a child up to age 5 24-h Dietary recalls SES, AM
Ferranti et al. 2013 [10] USA Mean age 48 y, N = 433 LG (5 y follow up) University and health center employees FFQ SES, PA, AM,
Isasi et al. 2015 [40] USA 18–74 y, N = 3141 LG (9 m follow up) Hispanic/Latino males and females 24-h Dietary recalls SES, PA, AM
El Ansari et al. 2015 [41] Egypt 16–30 y, N = 1483 CS Undergraduate students males and females FFQ SES, PA, AM
Valipour et al. 2017 [42] Iran 28–45 years old, N = 2134 CS General Adults FFQ SES, PA, AM
Fowles et al. 2012 [43] USA Mean age 24.7 y, N = 71 CS Low income pregnant women 24-h Dietary recalls SES, AM
Fowles et al. 2011 [44] USA Mean age 25 y, N = 118 CS Low income pregnant women 24-h Dietary recalls SES, AM
Widaman et al. 2016 [45] USA

Mean Age 25.1, N = 35 (BS)

Mean Age 24.1, N = 40 (BE)

CS Female habitual breakfast eaters and breakfast skippers 24-h Dietary recalls PA, AM
16 studies on Food Intake and Frequency of Consumption
Vidal et al. 2018 [1] Peru Mean Age: 19 y, N = 272 CS Undergraduate students Block fat screener SES
Nastaskin et al. 2015 [46] Canada Mean age: 20 y, N = 113 CS Students Block fat screener/ Block sodium screener SES, AM
Pettit et al. 2011 [47] USA 18–24 y, N = 78 CS Undergraduate students Energy drink intake questions SES
Mikolajczyk et al. 2009 [34] Germany, Poland, Bulgaria Mean age: 20 y, N = 1201 CS Fist year undergraduate students FFQ
Errisuriz et al. 2016 [48] USA Mean age: 18.9 y, N = 433 CS Freshman students Food and beverage frequency questions SES, AM
El Ansari et al. 2014 [15] UK Mean age: 24.9 y, N = 2699 CS Students FFQ
Ng et al. 2003 [49] USA Mean age: 40 y, N = 6620 CS Working adults Block Fat Screener/ Alcohol frequency questions SES, PA
Barrington et al. 2012 [37] USA 18–65 y, N = 357 CS Working adults Single-item question for fast food intake/ 5-A-Day fruit & vegetable assessment tool SES, PA, AM
Grossniklaus et al. 2010 [50] USA Mean age: 41.3 y, N = 64 CS Working adults 3-day WFR SES, AM
Papier et al. 2015 [16] Australia Mean Age 21.2 y, N = 397 CS Students FFQ SES, PA, AM
Roohafza et al. 2013 [35] Iran Mean age: 38.4–39.5 y, N = 9549 CS General adults FFQ SES, PA, AM
Gonzalez et al. 2013 [51] Puerto Rico 21–30 y, N = 186 CS First and second year students Alcohol frequency questions SES
Tseng et al. 2011 [36] USA Mean age 43.9 y, N = 426 CS Premenopausal women 48- h Dietary recalls SES
Hinote et al. 2009 [33] 8 post-Soviet republics > 18 y, N = 10,454 CS General adults Questions about frequency of consumption SES
Hwang et al. 2010 [52] Korea Mean age: 23.7 y, N = 570 CS Vietnamese female marriage immigrants 1-day Dietary recalls SES, PA, AM
Wardle et al. 2000 [53] UK Mean Age: 36.29 y, N = 58 CS Staff of a store 24-h Dietary recalls SES, AM

Alongside title and abstract searching, Medical subject headings (MeSH) were used when searching MEDLINE and CINAHL subject headings when searching CINAHL. The key terms used were: “psychological stress” AND “Diet”. Additionally, reference lists were checked, and authors of unpublished papers were contacted by email.

Selection of studies

The reviewer (KK) screened the full texts of all potentially relevant papers, including those over which there was doubt, with excluded articles also reviewed by the second reviewer (FT) to ensure that studies are not erroneously excluded. Any disagreements were resolved by discussion, or arbitrated if necessary, by a third reviewer (VH). Similarly, if eligibility was unclear, this was discussed across the wider team (KK, FT, and VH).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Studies were included in the review if they: i) enrolled healthy women aged 18–49 years old, ii) measured psychological stress (subjective and/or objective) as an exposure in combination with diet, iii) comprised observational quantitative studies looking at the association between stress and diet quality, iv) were in English language. Due to the limited resources available, it was not possible to translate non-English papers.

For studies in which the sample’s age range may in part be below or over the specified age range for this review, they were included if the mean age of the sample was between the age range of 18–49 years.

Articles were excluded if they: i) used qualitative methods, ii) enrolled exclusively men or participants with mean age outside the age range of 18–49 years old; iii) did not report stress data in a format that could be extracted; iv) comprised study sample with health conditions that may confound the diet stress relationship (e.g. depression, mental disorders, heart disease, diabetes, cancer, coeliac disease, eating disorders). Abstracts and unpublished studies were not included in this systematic review.

Data extraction

Data extraction and coding stages of the review were completed by the first reviewer (KK) using structured data extraction forms. The following information was extracted from the manuscripts: first author, year of publication, location, study design, number of subjects, period of enrolment and follow-up, age, the exposure (self-reported stress measured via validated stress scales and/or via biological marker (e.g. cortisol levels in blood, hair or saliva)). A proportion of the extracted data (30%) was checked for accuracy by second reviewer (FT).

For the purpose of meta-analysis, a dataset containing the 7 studies [3945] that initially qualified for meta-analysis was built. Ferranti et al. [10] was not among these studies as it did not report any effect size and hence should not be qualified for meta-analysis. The dataset was developed with the help of reviewer (OA) and included the following information from the studies: effect size, number of participants, first author surname, and year of publication. When only β coefficient was reported in any study, a proper conversion was carried out to transform β coefficient to correlation coefficient “r”. This was undertaken using the formula of imputing r value from β [54]: r = 0.98 β + 0.05 λ (restricted only to linear models and β values between ±0.5), where λ is an indicator variable that equals 1 when β is nonnegative and 0 when β is negative [54]. In the study by Richardson et al. [39]: r = 0.98 (− 0.18) + 0.05 (0) = − 0.1764. The β coefficient in Isasi et al. [40] is not within the exact range (± 0.5), however due to the large sample size in the study and the proximity of its β coefficient value to the range in the formula of imputing r from β, the formula was applied as follows: r = 0.98 (− 0.61) + 0.05 (0) = − 0.5978. The formula was not applied to Valipour et al. [42] as it is based on categorical dependent variable model, so this study was also excluded from the meta-analysis.

Study outcomes

Study outcomes included: dietary components (e.g. fat intake, alcohol intake, healthy versus unhealthy diet patterns) or adherence to diet indices (e.g. Alternate Healthy Eating Index (AHEI), the Dietary Approaches to Stopping Hypertension (DASH), and the Mediterranean Diet Score (MDS)).

Quality evaluation

The first and second reviewers (KK, FT) assessed bias in all eligible studies using the Risk of Bias in Non-randomised Studies [55], which is recommended by the Cochrane Handbook of Systematic Reviews of Interventions [56]. The bias domains included in the quality assessments were bias due to confounding, bias in selection of participants, bias in classification of interventions, bias due to deviations from intended interventions, bias due to missing data, bias in measurement of outcome, bias in selection of the reported results. Any conflicting opinion of quality of studies was discussed with the third reviewer (VH).

Meta-analysis

Meta-analysis was performed based on the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions and Borenstein book on meta-analysis [57, 58]. Fisher’s z transformation of correlation was used as a summary measure of the association between diet quality and stress, whereby correlation coefficients were converted to Fisher’s z scale. Due to heterogeneity of the studies, particularly with respect to studies’ participants and the methods of measuring the exposure and the outcome, a random effect model has been applied for the meta-analysis. Higgin’s & Thompson’s I2 and Cochran’s Q measures were used to assess the between-study heterogeneity [58]. Outliers and influential studies were detected by identifying any study with a confidence interval that did not overlap with the confidence interval of the pooled effect through Baujat plot [57]. Publication bias was assessed through a Funnel plot. Sensitivity analysis was performed by applying trim and fill method [57, 58]. Following the Cochrane Handbook recommendations, a risk-of-bias assessment was performed for all included studies by creating a “weighted bar” which plots the distribution of risk-of-bias judgements within each bias domain. The figure was formatted according to the risk-of-bias assessment tool (ROBINS-I).

Results

The databases identified 139,552 hits; only 471 had a relevant title (Fig. 1; (MOOSE Checklist in Additional file 2). The titles and abstracts of these articles were screened further and 382 were deemed not relevant which yielded 89 articles for full-text screening. A further 65 studies were subsequently excluded as they did not meet the criteria. Three studies were eliminated after quality assessment for the following reasons: one study did not have a methods section [59] and two studies measured the emotional/psychological domain of eating as an outcome (disordered eating/emotional eating) [60, 61]. A total of 24 studies were included in the review: 8 studies on diet quality (measured the adherence to specific dietary indices as outcome) and 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption which reported consumption of different food components and nutrients as proxy measure for dietary patterns (Tables 1, 2, and 3 in Additional files 3, 4 and 5 respectively).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) flow chart

Table 2.

Data values extracted from the included eight studies on Diet Quality: β (Beta coefficients), r (correlation coefficient), OR (Odd Ratio), ↑ (increase), ↓ (decrease), <= > (no association)

Author, Year Stress Assessment Tool Diet Quality Index Association between Stress and Diet Quality β coefficient, r, or OR
Richardson et al. 2015 [39] - 14-item Perceived Stress Scale - Healthy Eating Index 2010 <=> β = − 0.18 (S.E 0.10, p = 0.08)
Ferranti et al. 2013 [10]

- 14-item Perceived Stress Scale

- Beck Depression Inventory II

- Alternate Healthy Eating Index

- Mediterranean Diet Index

- Dietary Approach to Stop Hypertension Index

<=> Not reported
Isasi et al. 2015 [40]

- 10-item Perceived Stress Scale

- 8-item Chronic stress burden

- Alternate Healthy Eating Index 2010 β = − 0.61 (− 1.18 to − 0.03)
El Ansari et al. 2015 [41] - 4-item Perceived Stress Scale - Dietary Guideline Adherence Index <=>

r = 0.00, p = 0.98

β = 0.00 (− 0.13 to 0.13)

Valipour et al. 2017 [42] - 12-item General Health Questionnaire - Dietary Approach to Stop Hypertension Index <=> OR: 1.02 (0.78–1.33)
Fowles et al. 2012 [43]

- Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale

- Prenatal Psychosocial Profile-stress subscale

- Dietary Quality Index- Pregnancy r = −0.35, p is not reported
Fowles et al. 2011 [44]

- Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale

- Prenatal Psychosocial Profile-stress subscale

- Dietary Quality Index- Pregnancy r = − 0.293, p < 0.01
Widaman et al. 2016 [45] - Wheaton Chronic Stress Inventory - Healthy Eating Index 2010

↓ in breakfast skippers

<= > in breakfast eaters

Empty calories (r = − 0.392, p = 0.027)

Empty calories (r = − 0.104, p = 0.53)

Table 3.

Data values extracted from the included studies on food intake and frequency of consumption: ↑ (increase), ↓ (decrease), <= > (no association)

Author, Year Stress Assessment Tool Association between Stress and the measured Food intake and frequency of consumption Values
Vidal et al. 2018 [1] 14-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Fat intake p = 0.005
Nastaskin et al. 2015 [46] 14-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Fat intake r=. 35, p < 0.01
↑Sodium intake r=. 23, p = 0.07
Pettit et al. 2011 [47] 14-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Energy Drink intake r=. 235, p < 0.01
Mikolajczyk et al. 2009 [34] 14-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Sweets, cookies, snacks, fast food p = 0.03
↓ Fruits/vegetables p < 0.01
Errisuriz et al. 2016 [48] Perceived stress single item scale (0–10) ↑ Soda, coffee, energy drink, salty snack, sweet snack, frozen food, and fast food consumption p < 0.05
El Ansari et al. 2014 [15] 4-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Sweets, cookies, snacks, fast food P = 0.017
↓ Fruits and vegetables P = 0.002
Ng et al. 2003 [49] 4-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ High Fat diet p < 0.01
<= > Alcohol intake p = 0.4
Barrington et al. 2012 [37] 10-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ Fast food intake z = 3.00, P = .003
↓ Fruits and vegetables intake z = − 3.01, P = .003
Grossniklaus et al. 2010 [50] Perceived Stress Scale <= > food and beverage intake p > 0.05
Papier et al. 2015 [16] Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS) ↑ processed foods p < 0.01
↓ meat alternatives p < 0.05
↓vegetables and fruits p < 0.01
Roohafza et al. 2013 [35] -A12-item General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12) ↑ Saturated oils p < 0.01
↓ Unsaturated oils p < 0.01
↓ Fruits p < 0.01
↓ Vegetables p = 0.02
↓ Meat p = 0.03
↓ dairy products p < 0.01
Gonzalez et al. 2013 [51] Cognitivist Systemic Model Academic Stress scale ↑ Alcohol intake p < 0.05
Tseng et al. 2011 [36] Migration–Acculturation Stressor Scale ↑ Energy density -(β = 0.002, p = 0.04)
↑ % energy from fat -(β = 0.06, p = 0.05)
↓ total grams of grains -(β = −11.3, p < 0.0001)
↓ Overall grain intake -(β = −0.18, p = 0.03)
Hinote et al. 2009 [33] 12-item distress scale ↓ Meat, fish, vegetables, fruits, animal fat p < 0.001
Hwang et al. 2010 [52]

Psychological

Well-Being Index

↓ energy intake -p = 0.011
↓ carbohydrates -p = 0.004
↓ protein -p = 0.021
↓ fat -p = 0.021
↓ calcium -p = 0.042
↓ vitamin A -p = 0.039
↓ zinc -p = 0.005
↓ thiamine -p = 0.006
↓ riboflavin -p = 0.013
↓ folate -p = 0.004
Wardle et al. 2000 [53] 10-item Perceived Stress Scale ↑ energy intake, ↑ saturated fats intake, ↑ fat intake p < 0.05, p < 0.01, p < 0.05

Characteristics of included studies

Two out of the eight studies that assessed diet quality were longitudinal cohort studies: [10] included 5 years of follow-up (n = 429), while [40] followed participants for 9 months (n = 3141) (Table 1). Both studies investigated psychological stress via the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) at baseline; however, diet quality was investigated through different methods: [10] used food frequency questionnaire at baseline while [40] used two 24-h dietary recalls. The other six studies were cross-sectional, published between 2011 and 2017, and included a total of 3982 participants [39, 4145]. Only two out of the eight studies were conducted outside of the USA [41, 42]. Two studies included pregnant women of reproductive age who fall in the age range 19–49 years old [43, 44]. Four studies recruited females only (18–45 years old) [39, 4345] while the other four recruited both males and females (16–74 years old) [10, 4042].

The 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption did not assess diet quality, but instead measured the different food components and nutrients. As a result, the studies were very heterogeneous. Studies were all of a cross sectional design and published between 2000 and 2018. Six studies were conducted in USA, two in UK, and the remaining eight were conducted in other countries. Two studies took place in more than one country: Mikolajczyk et al. [34] was done in three European countries (Germany, Poland, Bulgaria) and Hinote et al. [33] was done in eight post-Soviet republics. In only two studies, participants were 100% females; the rest had both males and females with more than half of the participants were females in all of these studies. One study did not specify the percentage of females in its sample [35]. Mean age of participants was between 18.9 and 43.9 years and the number of female participants ranged from 52 to 10,454 per study.

Findings of the studies

In four of the eight studies on diet quality, stress was not associated with diet quality [10, 39, 41, 42], while in another three studies; stress was significantly associated with poorer diet quality [42.40.41] (Table 2). Interestingly, one study found that stress was significantly associated with lower diet quality in breakfast skippers only while no association was found in breakfast eaters [45].

The three studies that reported β coefficients indicated mixed results; two found no association [10, 41] and one found poorer diet quality when individuals were stressed [40]. Studies that reported correlation coefficient “r” found negative association between stress and diet quality [43, 44], no association [41], and mixed results (negative association in breakfast skippers/no association in breakfast eaters) [45] as shown in Table 2.

The outcomes of the 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption were very heterogeneous and thus it was not possible to perform a meta-analysis (Table 3). All studies that assessed fat intake found that perceived stress was significantly associated with increased fat consumption [1, 36, 46, 49, 53]. Only Hwang et al. [52] reported a significant decrease in fat intake, along with decreased intake of energy, carbohydrates, protein, calcium, vitamin A, zinc, thiamine, riboflavin, and folate, as a result of high stress (p < 0.05). The intake of fruits, vegetables, and grains was found to be significantly lower in individuals with higher stress (p < 0.02) [15, 16, 3337]. Some studies assessed the intake of fast food, sweets, snacks, and energy drinks and found a direct association between these foods and perceived stress (p < 0.05) [15, 34, 37, 47, 48]. The consumption of meat and meat alternatives was measured in three studies and was inversely correlated with stress (p < 0.05) [16, 33, 35]. Mixed results were found in two studies that assessed alcohol intake: Gonzalez et al. [51] found that perceived stress was significantly associated with greater consumption of alcohol (p < 0.05) whereas Ng et al. [49] found no significant association (p = 0.4).

Meta-analysis

Using the aforementioned methods for meta-analysis, 6 studies on diet quality were eligible for the meta-analysis [3941, 4345].

Assessment of heterogeneity

Outliers and influential analysis identified one outlier study [41]. Before removing this study from the analysis, the pooled effect was r = − 0.28 (95% CI [− 0.45; − 0.08], p value< 0.01). The overall effect size estimate (pooled correlation) was recalculated after removing this study and revealed a medium, negative, and very significant correlation (r = − 0.34, 95% CI [− 0.51; − 0.15], p value < 0.001) with 95% prediction interval of [− 0.80; 0.37]. These results (Fig. 2) suggest that a higher stress level was associated with poorer diet quality, and vice versa. The I2 heterogeneity measure in this analysis was substantial (93%), indicating significant variability across the studies (heterogeneity) and supporting the use of a random-effects model. Additionally, this conclusion was supported by Cochran’s Q test of heterogeneity which showed a very significant P value (< 0.0001).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Association between stress and diet quality (five studies based on correlation coefficient “r” and converted β coefficients to “r”)

Given the broad prediction interval in Fig. 2, which stretched well above zero, we cannot be 100% confident that the negative correlation between stress and diet quality found in this meta-analysis will be robust in every context.

Publication Bias

The funnel plot created was asymmetrical (Additional file 6). The asymmetry was mainly driven by one small size study [45] that has a large standard error and was shown in the bottom-right corner of the plot. This resembles a publication bias. Although this might occur due to chance, it might have also been comprised as a result of heterogeneity. The number of studies included in the meta-analysis was too small (5 studies) to test for significance of funnel plot asymmetry.

Sensitivity analysis

Trim-and-fill procedure identified three studies (Additional file 7) and assumed that initial results were underestimated due to publication bias. The true effect might be r = − 0.57 (95% CI [− 0.75; − 0.31], p value< 0.01) rather than r = − 0.34. Due to the assumed missing studies (small size studies reporting large effect sizes) and the small number of studies in this meta-analysis, the result of sensitivity analysis (r = − 0.57) is not considered a more valid estimate of the pooled correlation.

Quality assessment

Using “robvis” package, a weighed bar plot of the distribution of risk-of-bias judgments within each bias domain (Fig. 3) was generated to visualize the quality assessment performed for the 24 studies that were included in this systematic review.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Weighed bar plot of the distribution of risk-of-bias judgments within each bias domain

Fig. 3 shows that most studies scored moderate with regards to bias in measurement of outcomes, selection of the reported results, and the overall risk of bias. More than 75% of studies had a critical risk of bias due to missing data. When it came to the bias due to confounding and selection of participants, around 90% of studies had a low risk, and most studies scored not available (NA) risk with regards to bias due to classification of interventions and deviations from intended interventions.

Recruitment procedure

Recruitment procedures were very different among studies. In the eight studies on diet quality, three used data from participants enrolled in large cohorts from previous projects [10, 40, 42] while Fowles et al. [43, 44] recruited low income pregnant women in clinics using recruitment cards and forms (Table 1). The staff of a nutrition program helped Richardson et al. [39] identify women eligible for the study and the study staff asked them for their interest. Widaman et al. [45] recruited participants through advertisements on local newspapers, websites, and posted flyers while university students were recruited by distributing questionnaires during lectures [41]. Ethical approval was granted in seven studies and one study [40] did not give information regarding the ethical approval of the study.

Among the 16 studies of food intake and frequency of consumption, five studies used previous data of large cohort studies [33, 35, 37, 49, 52]. Eight studies recruited participants who were students through posters, flyers, or classroom visits at different university campuses [1, 15, 16, 34, 4648, 51]. Participants of the three remaining studies were recruited differently; through community organizations [36, 50] or from staff of a large department store [49]. Three studies did not provide information regarding ethical approval [33, 49, 53], whereas all other thirteen studies mentioned that ethical approval was given prior to conducting the studies.

Exposure: perceived stress

In four of the eight studies that assessed diet quality [10, 3941], the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) was used as a measure of psychological stress, whereas the other four studies used different scales such as: the General Health Questionnaire [42], the Prenatal Psychosocial Profile stress sub-scale [43, 44], and Wheaton Chronic Stress Inventory [45]. None of the studies used biomarkers of psychological stress (e.g. salivary cortisol) as a measure of the exposure.

All 16 studies that assessed food intake and frequency of consumption measured stress through self-reported measures: 10 studies used the Perceived Stress Scale [1, 15, 34, 37, 4650, 53] and the six remaining studies used different other scales (Table 2).

Dietary assessment

A variety of dietary instruments were used to assess habitual dietary intake in the eight studies that assessed diet quality. Three studies [10, 41, 42] used different Food Frequency Questionnaires (FFQs) to assess dietary intake (Table 1). The other five studies used 24-h dietary recalls for either: three days [4345], two days [40], or one-to-two days [39].

With respect to diet quality, all studies used the a priori defined method (using diet indices) to derive the diet quality. A variety of diet quality indices were included: i) Alternate Healthy Eating Index [10, 40], ii) Healthy Eating Index [39, 45], iii) The Dietary Approach to Stop Hypertension (DASH) Diet Index [10, 42], iv) Dietary Quality Index- Pregnancy [43, 44], v) Dietary Guideline Adherence Index [41]. Interestingly, only one study combined three diet quality indices to measure diet quality [10], while all other studies used only one index. No study was found to assess diet quality via a posteriori approach i.e. to define diet patterns with statistical methods such as Factor Analysis.

There was also diversity in the tools used to assess food intake and frequency of consumption. Four of the 16 studies used food frequency questionnaires [15, 16, 34, 35], three used dietary recalls [36, 52, 53], another three used Block fat screener [1, 46, 49], two used alcohol intake frequency questions [49, 51], one used Block sodium screener [46], and one used weighed food records [50]. The remaining studies used different questions about food and beverages consumption (Table 1).

Confounding factors

Table 1 indicates that seven of the eight studies of diet quality identified and corrected for socioeconomic status of participants as confounding factor. The exception was the study by Widaman et al. [45]. One study identified only age and educational level as means of socioeconomic status [44]. Three out of the eight studies did not assess the physical activity level of participants [39, 43, 44]. The anthropometric measures of participants were measured in all eight studies, either through BMI [10, 39, 41, 4345] or both Waist Circumference and BMI [40, 42]. Smoking status was reported in three studies [4244], marital status in five [10, 39, 4244], and energy intake in three [10, 40, 45].

In the 16 studies of food intake and frequency of consumption, two studies did not identify or correct for confounding factors [15, 34]. All remaining studies identified socioeconomic status and demographic information of participants. Only five studies measured physical activity among participants [16, 35, 37, 49, 52]. BMI was reported in seven studies as a measure of adiposity [16, 35, 37, 46, 48, 50, 53] and only one study reported both waist circumference and BMI [52].

Discussion

Our findings suggest that stress appears to impact diet negatively regardless of the various dietary outcomes measured among studies. Stress decreased diet quality and contributed to unhealthy dietary patterns, particularly high fat, fast food, sweets, and energy dense foods. In contrast stress lowered the intake of fruits, vegetables, fish and unsaturated oils.

The mixed results, especially in the eight studies on diet quality, highlights the disparity of evidence that exists in the literature regarding the association between stress and diet quality for the general population. In other populations, such as adolescents, perceived stress has been associated with poorer diet quality, measured through Diet Quality Index for Adolescents (DQI-A) (β = − 0.04, p < 0.01), [62]. An inverse association has been also reported in a systematic review with regards to mental health (including stress) and diet quality in children and adolescents [63] while Sims et al. [61] found no association between perceived stress and diet quality among female African American adults.

In almost all 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption included in our review, higher perceived stress was associated with an unhealthy eating pattern, characterised by increased consumption of sweets, fast food, fats and lower consumption of fruits and vegetables. This is in line with studies of other populations. Increased stress in female undergraduate students and peri-menopausal women has been linked with greater consumption of high calorie foods [64, 65]. Similarly, O’conner et al. [32] showed that daily stress was associated with a higher intake of high fat/sugar food and a reduced intake of fruits and vegetables in women. Wichianson et al. [30] found that stress was associated with unhealthy night-eating syndrome (NES) in a sample of 95 college students (β = 0.259, p < 0.05). Interestingly, one of the 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption found that stress was linked with decreased fat intake (along with all macro- and micro-nutrients) [52]. This contradicts the majority of studies in the field with only Torress et al. [23] finding an inverse association between stress and fat. Torres et al. [23] assessed daily record of stress and diet among male and female students and found that participants consumed less food and dietary fat when they were stressed. These conflicting results indicate that there might be inter-individual variation in response to stress.

The differences in results presented in Tables 1 and 2 must be interpreted with caution due to the challenges in assessing dietary intake. The eight studies on diet quality used different methods to collect dietary data: five studies used 24-h recalls [39, 40, 4345] and mainly found negative association between stress and diet quality, while three studies [10, 41, 42] used food frequency questionnaires and found no association between stress and diet quality, which might explain the variance in the findings. Similarly, the 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption used food frequency questionnaires [15, 16, 34, 35], dietary recalls [36, 52, 53], block fat screener [1, 46, 49], and other different tools to assess dietary intake and found that stress was associated with the intake of unhealthy diet (higher fat, sweets, fast food, salt; lower fruits, vegetables, whole grains, and seafood). Although the use of food frequency questionnaires, 24-h dietary recalls, and the above-mentioned tools in nutrition epidemiology is quite common, measurement errors caused by self-reporting (under-reporting or over-reporting) of food intake occur leading to the manipulation of the expected associations. Furthermore, these dietary assessment methods might not be ideal for investigating the response to perceived stress; different methods such as ecologic momentary assessment, which aims to minimise recall bias, might be better in reporting dietary/behavioural responses to stress that take place in real time [40, 66].

Disparities exist between the two groups of studies in our review. Most of the 16 studies on food intake and frequency of consumption indicate that stress increases energy intake and food consumption [15, 36, 47, 48, 5153]. In contrast, the majority of the eight studies on diet quality found no association between diet quality, which depends on food consumption, and stress. This can be explained mainly due to the diet quality indices used in the studies. Of the three studies that measured diet quality through the Healthy Eating Index (HEI) (including the Alternative HEI), two found no association between stress and diet quality [10, 39] and one found an inverse association [40]. However, out of the twelve scoring components of the HEI, nine will be scored higher if the intake of certain foods is higher which means that participants might have a higher energy and food consumption than they need and still score high on the HEI and have a higher diet quality. Moreover, the mixed findings could be related to the socioeconomic status of the participants as low socioeconomic populations tend to be more stressed than socially advantaged populations. A previous meta-analysis found that socioeconomically disadvantaged individuals had increased odds of being stressed and depressed (odds ratio = 1.81, p < 0.001) [67].

Two studies on diet quality were conducted among pregnant women [43, 44] and were included in the review since prenatal stress and diet are considered important for the intrauterine environment that affects several developmental outcomes [6870]. The variation in diet quality of women during pregnancy has been associated with health outcomes of the fetus [7176]. Similarly, maternal stress during conception is linked to disease risk and developmental outcomes of the fetus [68, 7781]. More studies looking on diet and stress in this population and in the preconception stage are needed and should be conducted across different countries and with unified methodologies to allow comparison and confirm the stress/diet association.

Strengths and limitations of the study

With diet quality and food intake in women of reproductive age being significant predictors of obesity and complications during pregnancy, the present systematic review adds to the body of knowledge by providing evidence on the role of psychological stress in manipulating diet quality. This will help in developing stress reducing strategies and guide future health care. The large sample size of most studies is a major strength of the present review. Another strength is restricting the sample to healthy women where studies with sample that had health conditions such as depression, metabolic diseases, and eating disorders were excluded, because these conditions might manipulate the diet quality and are considered significant confounding factors.

However, the 24 studies in the review are very heterogeneous in both participants that they recruited and the methods that they used, making pooling of these results challenging. Most of the eight studies on diet quality were conducted in USA and only two studies were conducted in the Middle East; no studies were conducted in Europe or Asia. This highlights the importance of conducting similar studies on diet quality among populations with different ethnicity and cultural backgrounds to confirm any possible differences. Another limitation is that in the 24 studies, stress was measured by self-reported stress scales and dietary intake was measured using 24-h recalls, food frequency questionnaires, or other self-reported questionnaires, which could lead to errors during dietary reporting and classification. A study measuring physiological markers of stress (such as serum or salivary cortisol) and biomarkers of dietary intake (such as urinary nitrogen, plasma vitamin C, and serum carotenoids) would provide stronger evidence. Moreover, differences in diet quality indices, dietary outcomes measured, and methodologies between the 24 studies made it difficult to compare the results of the studies. This issue has been highlighted by Mikolajczyk et al. [34] who recommended that research looking on stress and diet should be conducted across diverse population groups and amongst different countries which can enable the use of unified methodology and meaningful comparison of comparable outcomes. At present, it is challenging to compare results derived from studies conducted in single countries due to variation in methodologies and measures of diet and stress. The study design was a major limitation where studies were cross-sectional and longitudinal; hence, no causation or definitive conclusions can be drawn about the association between psychological stress and diet. A case-control study could provide more accurate evidence on the relationship between stress and diet. Including studies that are only in English language might be another limitation where evidence from studies published in other languages was not considered. Moreover, a prospective registration of this systematic review (for example on PROSPERO) was not done and this was also considered a limitation of this paper. The authors also declare that a thorough review/search of unpublished literature was not done, however the authors of unpublished papers were contacted and there were only 3 non-English abstracts found during the literature search.

Conclusions

Studies exploring the association between stress and diet in women of reproductive age reported mixed results. This review adds to the current knowledge by highlighting the inverse association between stress and diet. However, there was substantial heterogeneity in both methods and outcomes, which made it difficult to pool the study results and draw a solid conclusion about the association between stress and diet quality/patterns. Studies of rigorous design and robust methodology are needed to determine the role of stress in manipulating the dietary patterns/quality of women of reproductive age. In particular, it is crucial to conduct studies in different countries, with larger number of participants, and with well-designed, unified and standardised methodologies.

Although some studies reported a significant association between stress and diet, this systematic review cannot determine causation of this association. At the clinical level, results from this systematic review, that showed inverse association between stress and healthy dietary patterns/quality in women of reproductive age, might be useful to implement stress coping strategies aimed at lowering stress levels and improving the quality of diet, and vice versa.

Supplementary information

12937_2020_609_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13.8KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table 1. Search strategy.

Additional file 2. (19.3KB, docx)
12937_2020_609_MOESM3_ESM.docx (18.6KB, docx)

Additional file 3 Table 2. Characteristics extracted from the 24 included studies: BS (Breakfast skippers), BE (Breakfast eaters), CS (Cross-Sectional), LG (Longitudinal), y (years), m (months), FFQ (Food Frequency Questionnaire, WFR (Weigh food record), SES (Socioeconomic status), PA (Physical Activity), AM (Anthropometric measures), − (not reported).

12937_2020_609_MOESM4_ESM.docx (16.6KB, docx)

Additional file 4: Table 3. Data values extracted from the included eight studies on Diet Quality: β (Beta coefficients), r (correlation coefficient), OR (Odd Ratio).

12937_2020_609_MOESM5_ESM.docx (17.8KB, docx)

Additional file 5: Table 4. Data values extracted from the included studies on food intake and frequency of consumption: ↑ (increase), ↓ (decrease), <= > (no association).

Additional file 6. (58.2KB, png)
Additional file 7. (61.2KB, png)

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

UK

United Kingdom

PRISMA

The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-analysis Approach

PEO

Population, Exposure, and Outcome

AHEI

Alternate Healthy Eating Index

DASH

Dietary Approach to Stopping Hypertension

MDS

Mediterranean Diet Score

CASP

Critical Appraisal Skills Programme

PSS

Perceived Stress Scale

USA

United States of America

CI

Confidence Intervals

FFQ

Food Frequency Questionnaires

BMI

Body Mass Index

CS

Cross sectional

LG

Longitudinal

SES

Socioeconomic status

BS

Breakfast skippers

BE

Breakfast eaters

y

years

m

months

WFR

Weighed Food Records

PA

Physical activity

AM

Anthropometric measures

OR

Odd ratio

DQIA

Diet Quality Index for Adolescents

HEI

Healthy Eating Index

Authors’ contributions

Conceptualization, F.T. and V.H.; methodology, K.K., F.T., V.H., and R.H.; formal analysis of study findings, K.K., O.A., F.T., and V.H.; interpretation, K.K., F.T., and V.H.; writing—original draft preparation, K.K.; writing—review and editing, K.K., F.T., V.H., R.H. and O.A.; supervision, F.T., and V.H. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding and was conducted as part of a PhD project.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Karim Khaled, Email: Khaledk@bournemouth.ac.uk.

Fotini Tsofliou, Email: ftsofliou@bournemouth.ac.uk.

Vanora Hundley, Email: vhundley@bournemouth.ac.uk.

Rebecca Helmreich, Email: Rebecca.J.Helmreich@uth.tmc.edu.

Orouba Almilaji, Email: oalmilaji@bournemouth.ac.uk.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s12937-020-00609-w.

References

  • 1.Vidal EJ, Alvarez D, Martinez-Velarde D, Vidal-Damas L, Yuncar-Rojas KA, Julca-Malca A, et al. Perceived stress and high fat intake: a study in a sample of undergraduate students. PLoS One. 2018;13(3):e0192827. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0192827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.CMACE . Maternal obesity in the UK: findings from a national project: maternal, Confidential Enquiry. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Poston L, Caleyachetty R, Cnattingius S, Corvalán C, Uauy R, Herring S, et al. Preconceptional and maternal obesity: epidemiology and health consequences. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2016;4(12):1025–1036. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(16)30217-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Reynolds RM, Allan KM, Raja EA, Bhattacharya S, McNeill G, Hannaford PC, et al. Maternal obesity during pregnancy and premature mortality from cardiovascular event in adult offspring: follow-up of 1 323 275 person years. BMJ. 2013;347:f4539. doi: 10.1136/bmj.f4539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kanguru L, McCaw-Binns A, Bell J, Yonger-Coleman N, Wilks R, Hussein J. The burden of obesity in women of reproductive age and in pregnancy in a middle-income setting: a population based study from Jamaica. PLoS One. 2017;12(12):e0188677. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Martin J, Moran L, Teede H, Ranasinha S, Lombard C, Harrison C. Exploring diet quality between urban and rural dwelling women of reproductive age. Nutrients. 2017;9(6):586. doi: 10.3390/nu9060586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Organization WH . Rural poverty and health systems in the WHO European region. WHO Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Thangaratinam S, Rogozińska E, Jolly K, Glinkowski S, Roseboom T, Tomlinson J, et al. Effects of interventions in pregnancy on maternal weight and obstetric outcomes: meta-analysis of randomised evidence. BMJ. 2012;344:e2088. doi: 10.1136/bmj.e2088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hu F. Dietary pattern analysis: a new direction in nutritional epidemiology. Curr Opin Lipidol. 2002;13(1):3–9. doi: 10.1097/00041433-200202000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ferranti EP, Dunbar SB, Higgins M, Dai J, Ziegler TR, Frediani JK, et al. Psychosocial factors associated with diet quality in a working adult population. Res Nurs Health. 2013;36(3):242–256. doi: 10.1002/nur.21532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kretowicz H, Hundley V, Tsofliou F. Exploring the perceived barriers to following a Mediterranean style diet in childbearing age: a qualitative study. Nutrients. 2018;10(11):1694. doi: 10.3390/nu10111694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Martini D. Health benefits of Mediterranean diet. Nutrients. 2019;11(8):1802. doi: 10.3390/nu11081802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Boynton A, Neuhouser M, Sorensen B, McTiernan A, Ulrich C. Predictors of diet quality among overweight and obese postmenopausal women. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008;108(1):125–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2007.10.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Giskes K, van Lenthe F, Avendano-Pabon M, Brug J. A systematic review of environmental factors and obesogenic dietary intakes among adults: are we getting closer to understanding obesogenic environments? Obes Rev. 2010;12(5):e95–e106. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00769.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.El Ansari W, Adetunji H, Oskrochi R. Food and mental health: relationship between food and perceived stress and depressive symptoms among university students in the United Kingdom. Cent Eur J Public Health. 2014;22(2):90. doi: 10.21101/cejph.a3941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Papier K, Ahmed F, Lee P, Wiseman J. Stress and dietary behaviour among first-year university students in Australia: sex differences. Nutrition. 2015;31(2):324–330. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2014.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fink G. Stress: definition and history. Stress Sci: Neuroendocrinol. 2010:3–9.
  • 18.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. JAMA. 2007;298(14):1685–1687. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.14.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Adam TC, Epel ES. Stress, eating and the reward system. Physiol Behav. 2007;91(4):449–458. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lee M-J, Fried SK. The glucocorticoid receptor, not the mineralocorticoid receptor, plays the dominant role in adipogenesis and adipokine production in human adipocytes. Int J Obes. 2014;38(9):1228. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2014.6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Block JP, He Y, Zaslavsky AM, Ding L, Ayanian JZ. Psychosocial stress and change in weight among US adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2009;170(2):181–192. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwp104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pasquali R. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and sex hormones in chronic stress and obesity: pathophysiological and clinical aspects. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1264(1):20–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06569.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Torres S, Nowson C. Relationship between stress, eating behavior, and obesity. Nutrition. 2007;23(11–12):887–894. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2007.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Yau YH, Potenza MN. Stress and eating behaviors. Minerva Endocrinol. 2013;38(3):255. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Oliver G, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Stress and food choice: a laboratory study. Psychosom Med. 2000;62(6):853–865. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200011000-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rutters F, Nieuwenhuizen AG, Lemmens SG, Born JM, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Acute stress-related changes in eating in the absence of hunger. Obesity. 2009;17(1):72–77. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Zellner DA, Loaiza S, Gonzalez Z, Pita J, Morales J, Pecora D, et al. Food selection changes under stress. Physiol Behav. 2006;87(4):789–793. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, Potenza MN. Neural correlates of stress-and food cue–induced food craving in obesity: association with insulin levels. Diabetes Care. 2013;36(2):394–402. doi: 10.2337/dc12-1112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Bayram N, Bilgel N. The prevalence and socio-demographic correlations of depression, anxiety and stress among a group of university students. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2008;43(8):667–672. doi: 10.1007/s00127-008-0345-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wichianson JR, Bughi SA, Unger JB, Spruijt-Metz D, Nguyen-Rodriguez ST. Perceived stress, coping and night-eating in college students. Stress and Health: J Int Soc Investigation Stress. 2009;25(3):235–240. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Habhab S, Sheldon JP, Loeb RC. The relationship between stress, dietary restraint, and food preferences in women. Appetite. 2009;52(2):437–444. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.O'connor DB, Jones F, Conner M, McMillan B, Ferguson E. Effects of daily hassles and eating style on eating behavior. Health Psychol. 2008;27(1S):S20. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.S20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hinote BP, Cockerham WC, Abbott P. Psychological distress and dietary patterns in eight post-soviet republics. Appetite. 2009;53(1):24–33. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mikolajczyk RT, El Ansari W, Maxwell AE. Food consumption frequency and perceived stress and depressive symptoms among students in three European countries. Nutr J. 2009;8(1):31. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-8-31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Roohafza H, Sarrafzadegan N, Sadeghi M, Rafieian-Kopaei M, Sajjadi F, Khosravi-Boroujeni H. The association between stress levels and food consumption among Iranian population. Arch Iran Med. 2013;16(3):145–148. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tseng M, Fang CY. Stress is associated with unfavorable patterns of dietary intake among female chinese immigrants. Ann Behav Med. 2011;41(3):324–332. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9259-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Barrington WE, Ceballos RM, Bishop SK, McGregor BA, Beresford SA. Perceived stress, behavior, and body mass index among adults participating in a worksite obesity prevention program, Seattle, 2005-2007. Prev Chronic Dis. 2012;9:E152. doi: 10.5888/pcd9.120001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Stroup D, Berlin J, Morton S, Olkin I, Williamson C, Rennie D, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology a proposal for reporting. JAMA. 2000;283(15):2008–2012. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.15.2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Richardson AS, Arsenault JE, Cates SC, Muth MK. Perceived stress, unhealthy eating behaviors, and severe obesity in low-income women. Nutr J. 2015;14(1):122. doi: 10.1186/s12937-015-0110-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Isasi CR, Parrinello CM, Jung MM, Carnethon MR, Birnbaum-Weitzman O, Espinoza RA, et al. Psychosocial stress is associated with obesity and diet quality in Hispanic/Latino adults. Ann Epidemiol. 2015;25(2):84–89. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2014.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.El Ansari W, Berg-Beckhoff G. Nutritional correlates of perceived stress among university students in Egypt. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12(11):14164–14176. doi: 10.3390/ijerph121114164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Valipour G, Esmaillzadeh A, Azadbakht L, Afshar H, Hassanzadeh A, Adibi P. Adherence to the DASH diet in relation to psychological profile of Iranian adults. Eur J Nutr. 2017;56(1):309–320. doi: 10.1007/s00394-015-1081-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fowles ER, Stang J, Bryant M, Kim S. Stress, depression, social support, and eating habits reduce diet quality in the first trimester in low-income women: a pilot study. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(10):1619–1625. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.07.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Fowles ER, Bryant M, Kim S, Walker LO, Ruiz RJ, Timmerman GM, et al. Predictors of dietary quality in low-income pregnant women: a path analysis. Nurs Res. 2011;60(5):286. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0b013e3182266461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Widaman AM, Witbracht MG, Forester SM, Laugero KD, Keim NL. Chronic stress is associated with indicators of diet quality in habitual breakfast skippers. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2016;116(11):1776–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2016.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Nastaskin RS, Fiocco AJ. A survey of diet self-efficacy and food intake in students with high and low perceived stress. Nutr J. 2015;14:42. doi: 10.1186/s12937-015-0026-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Pettit ML, DeBarr KA. Perceived stress, energy drink consumption, and academic performance among college students. J Am Coll Heal. 2011;59(5):335–341. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2010.510163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Errisuriz VL, Pasch KE, Perry CL. Perceived stress and dietary choices: the moderating role of stress management. Eat Behav. 2016;22:211–216. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ng DM, Jeffery RW. Relationships between perceived stress and health behaviors in a sample of working adults. Health Psychol. 2003;22(6):638. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.6.638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Grossniklaus DA, Dunbar SB, Tohill BC, Gary R, Higgins MK, Frediani J. Psychological factors are important correlates of dietary pattern in overweight adults. J Cardiovascular Nurs. 2010;25(6):450. doi: 10.1097/JCN.0b013e3181d25433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.González AM, Cruz SY, Ríos JL, Pagán I, Fabián C, Betancourt J, et al. Alcohol consumption and smoking and their associations with socio-demographic characteristics, dietary patterns, and perceived academic stress in Puerto Rican college students. P R Health Sci J. 2013;32(2):82–8. [PubMed]
  • 52.Hwang J-Y, Lee SE, Kim SH, Chung HW, Kim WY. Psychological distress is associated with inadequate dietary intake in Vietnamese marriage immigrant women in Korea. J Am Diet Assoc. 2010;110(5):779–785. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Wardle J, Steptoe A, Oliver G, Lipsey Z. Stress, dietary restraint and food intake. J Psychosom Res. 2000;48(2):195–202. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(00)00076-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Peterson RA, Brown SP. On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(1):175. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sterne J, Hernán M, Reeves B, Savović J, Berkman N, Viswanathan M, et al. ROBINS-I: a tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ. 2016;355:i4919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 56.Chapter 25: Assessing risk of bias in a non-randomized study [Internet]. Training.cochrane.org. 2020 [cited 16 March 2020]. Available from: https://training.cochrane.org/handbook/current/chapter-25.
  • 57.Borenstein M, Hedges L, Higgins J, Rothstein H. Introduction to meta-analysis. Ltd, UK: Wiley; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA (editors). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.0 (updated July 2019). Cochrane, 2019. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 59.Roberts C. The effects of stress on food choice, mood and bodyweight in healthy women. Nutr Bull. 2008;33(1):33–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ball K, Lee C. Psychological stress, coping, and symptoms of disordered eating in a community sample of young Australian women. Int J Eat Disord. 2002;31(1):71–81. doi: 10.1002/eat.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sims R, Gordon S, Garcia W, Clark E, Monye D, Callender C, et al. Perceived stress and eating behaviors in a community-based sample of African Americans. Eat Behav. 2008;9(2):137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2007.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.De Vriendt T, Clays E, Huybrechts I, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Moreno L, Patterson E, et al. European adolescents’ level of perceived stress is inversely related to their diet quality: the healthy lifestyle in Europe by nutrition in adolescence study. Br J Nutr. 2011;108(2):371–380. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511005708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.O’Neil A, Quirk S, Housden S, Brennan S, Williams L, Pasco J, et al. Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Am J Public Health. 2014;104(10):e31–e42. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.O'Connor D, O'Connor R. Perceived changes in food intake in response to stress: the role of conscientiousness. Stress Health. 2004;20(5):279–291. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tryon M, Carter C, DeCant R, Laugero K. Chronic stress exposure may affect the brain's response to high calorie food cues and predispose to obesogenic eating habits. Physiol Behav. 2013;120:233–242. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kirkpatrick SI, Vanderlee L, Raffoul A, Stapleton J, Csizmadi I, Boucher BA, et al. Self-report dietary assessment tools used in Canadian research: a scoping review. Adv Nutr. 2017;8(2):276–289. doi: 10.3945/an.116.014027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(2):98–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Entringer S, Buss C, Wadhwa P. Prenatal stress and developmental programming of human health and disease risk: concepts and integration of empirical findings. Curr Opinion Endocrinol Diabetes Obesity. 2010;17(6):507–516. doi: 10.1097/MED.0b013e3283405921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hobel C, Culhane J. Role of psychosocial and nutritional stress on poor pregnancy outcome. J Nutr. 2003;133(5):1709S–1717S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.5.1709S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Spencer S. Perinatal nutrition programs neuroimmune function long-term: mechanisms and implications. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 71.Horan M, McGowan C, Gibney E, Donnelly J, McAuliffe F. Maternal low glycaemic index diet, fat intake and postprandial glucose influences neonatal adiposity – secondary analysis from the ROLO study. Nutri J. 2014;13(1):78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 72.Dabelea D, Crume T. Maternal environment and the Transgenerational cycle of obesity and diabetes. Diabetes. 2011;60(7):1849–1855. doi: 10.2337/db11-0400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Reynolds C, Gray C, Li M, Segovia S, Vickers M. Early life nutrition and energy balance disorders in offspring in later life. Nutrients. 2015;7(9):8090–8111. doi: 10.3390/nu7095384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Pet M, Brouwer-Brolsma E. The impact of maternal vitamin D status on offspring brain development and function: a systematic review. Advances in Nutrition: An Int Rev J. 2016;7(4):665–678. doi: 10.3945/an.115.010330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Emmett P, Jones L, Golding J. Pregnancy diet and associated outcomes in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Nutr Rev. 2015;73(suppl 3):154–174. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Devakumar D, Fall C, Sachdev H, Margetts B, Osmond C, Wells J, et al. Maternal antenatal multiple micronutrient supplementation for long-term health benefits in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Med. 2016;14(1):90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 77.Buss C, Entringer S, Wadhwa P. Fetal Programming of Brain Development: Intrauterine Stress and Susceptibility to Psychopathology. Science Signaling. 2012;5(245):pt7. doi: 10.1126/scisignal.2003406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Dunkel SC. Psychological science on pregnancy: stress processes, biopsychosocial models, and emerging research issues. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011;62(1):531–558. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Entringer S, Buss C, Swanson J, Cooper D, Wing D, Waffarn F, et al. Fetal programming of body composition, obesity, and metabolic function: the role of intrauterine stress and stress biology. J Nutri Metabol. 2012;2012:1–16. doi: 10.1155/2012/632548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.KINSELLA M, MONK C. Impact of maternal stress, depression and anxiety on fetal neurobehavioral development. Clin Obstet Gynecol. 2009;52(3):425–440. doi: 10.1097/GRF.0b013e3181b52df1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Marques A, O'Connor T, Roth C, Susser E, Bjørke-Monsen A. The influence of maternal prenatal and early childhood nutrition and maternal prenatal stress on offspring immune system development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

12937_2020_609_MOESM1_ESM.docx (13.8KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table 1. Search strategy.

Additional file 2. (19.3KB, docx)
12937_2020_609_MOESM3_ESM.docx (18.6KB, docx)

Additional file 3 Table 2. Characteristics extracted from the 24 included studies: BS (Breakfast skippers), BE (Breakfast eaters), CS (Cross-Sectional), LG (Longitudinal), y (years), m (months), FFQ (Food Frequency Questionnaire, WFR (Weigh food record), SES (Socioeconomic status), PA (Physical Activity), AM (Anthropometric measures), − (not reported).

12937_2020_609_MOESM4_ESM.docx (16.6KB, docx)

Additional file 4: Table 3. Data values extracted from the included eight studies on Diet Quality: β (Beta coefficients), r (correlation coefficient), OR (Odd Ratio).

12937_2020_609_MOESM5_ESM.docx (17.8KB, docx)

Additional file 5: Table 4. Data values extracted from the included studies on food intake and frequency of consumption: ↑ (increase), ↓ (decrease), <= > (no association).

Additional file 6. (58.2KB, png)
Additional file 7. (61.2KB, png)

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.


Articles from Nutrition Journal are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES