Abstract
This study uses recent data to investigate if smoking initiation diffuses through friendship networks over the high school period and explores if diffusion processes differ across schools. One thousand four hundred and twenty-five racially and ethnically diverse youth from four high schools in Los Angeles were surveyed four times over the high school period from 2010 to 2013. Probit regression models and stochastic actor-based models for network dynamics tested for peer effects on smoking initiation. Friend smoking was found to predict adolescent smoking, and smoking initiation diffused through friendship networks in some but not all of the schools. School differences in smoking rates and the popularity of smokers may be linked to differences in the diffusion of smoking through peer networks. We conclude that there are differences in peer effects on smoking initiation across schools that will be important to account for in network-based smoking interventions.
Keywords: adolescent, diffusion, high school, smoking initiation, social network, stochastic actor-based model
Most adult smokers start using cigarettes before they are 18 years of age (Giovino et al. 1995), making adolescence a critical stage for prevention. Data from the National Youth Tobacco Survey indicate that youth cigarette smoking rates in the United States have declined in the past decade, but despite these encouraging changes, there remain several concerning trends, particularly among high school students who have not shown significant declines in smoking since 2009 and whose rates of smoking remained stable (9.2%–9.3%) between 2014 and 2015 (Dutra and Glantz 2017).
Although risk factors for youth smoking are complex, influence from peers and friends has been identified as a consistent and important predictor of smoking initiation and cigarette use among early and late adolescents (Ennett et al. 2006; Fujimoto and Valente 2012; Go et al. 2012; Green et al. 2013; Haas, Schaefer, and Kornienko 2010; Kobus 2003). Longitudinal social network studies that model social selection and social influence effects jointly (Alexander et al. 2001; Go et al. 2012; Green et al. 2013; Haas and Schaefer 2014; Hall and Valente 2007; Kiuru et al. 2010; Lakon et al. 2015; Mercken et al. 2009; Ragan 2016; Schaefer, Haas, and Bishop 2012; Steglich, Snijders, and Pearson 2010; Valente et al. 2013) found that youth tend to select friends whose smoking behaviors are similar to their own (controlling for other characteristics predicting friendship choices, such as gender; Green et al. 2013; Haas and Schaefer 2014; Kiuru et al. 2010; Mercken et al. 2009; Schaefer et al. 2012) and that youth were also influenced by their friends in some (but not all) studies, adopting smoking (or nonsmoking) practices that were similar to their friends’ behaviors (Go et al. 2012; Green et al. 2013; Haas and Schaefer 2014; Ragan 2016; Schaefer et al. 2012). Friends’ smoking has also been identified as a strong predictor of smoking initiation such that youth rarely initiated smoking without exposure to smoking friends (Haas and Schaefer 2014).
These studies have also linked popularity within peer social networks to youth smoking, albeit with mixed findings. Some research has found smoking to be associated with social isolation or having few friends (Copeland, Bartlett, and Fisher 2017; Ennett et al. 2006; Valente, Unger, and Johnson 2005), while other studies have shown smoking is positively associated with popularity (Alexander et al. 2001; Valente et al. 2005), with evidence that popularity can lead to smoking (Valente et al. 2005) and that smoking can enhance adolescent popularity over time (Haas and Schaefer 2014; Schaefer et al.2012; Valente et al. 2013). The extent to which smoking is associated with social status in a given setting, such as a school, is likely to be an important feature of the social-ecological system that shapes social selection and influences processes (Green et al. 2013; Lakon et al. 2015; Osgood et al. 2013; Schaefer, adams, and Haas 2013). When popular youth smoke, it is a behavior associated with social status, and it is likely to have increased “norm salience” (Dijkstra and Gest 2015). This can increase the salience of smoking as a characteristic on which to base friendship choices, and youth may be more motivated to adhere to the smoking norms of their friends (Green et al. 2013; Mercken et al. 2009; Rambaran, Dijkstra, and Stark 2013). Positive associations between popularity and smoking can also make this behavior more visible to network members and lead to biased prosmoking norms because of the “majority illusion” (Lerman, Yan, and Wu 2016). Network simulation studies have provided valuable insight into the interaction between smoking prevalence and the social status of smokers: Studies suggest that the strength of the effect that smoking has on friend selection has little impact on overall smoking rates (Lakon et al. 2015) but that lower associations between popularity and smoking can lead to lower smoking prevalence when there is peer influence (Schaefer et al. 2013). The PROSPER intervention also showed that by reducing the peer network centrality of students who engaged in “problem behaviors” (that included substance use and smoking), the overall rates of problem behaviors within school districts declined (Osgood et al. 2013). They attributed this result to having reduced the “peer influence potential” of students engaging in problem behaviors by limiting their social status in their peer network.
Limitations of these studies are that (1) all of the data were collected prior to the year 2010 (with the majority collected in the 1990s), when smoking rates were higher and smoking norms differed; (2) the majority of these studies tracked youth for just one year; (3) some of these samples lacked racial and ethnic diversity; and (4) about half of the studies cited relied on data from just one or two schools, limiting the ability to explain if different settings gave rise to different peer and smoking dynamics. The PROSPER study is an exception to many of these limitations; it assessed 28 school districts over four years, beginning in the early 2000s, although this sample was predominantly rural and white (Osgood et al. 2013; Ragan 2016). Given the changes in youth smoking rates in the past decade as well as current trends in high school smoking rates among youth of color (Dutra and Glantz 2017), it is valuable to investigate the role of friends and peers in the diffusion of smoking initiation among high school students from racially and ethnically diverse communities, using recent data, and considering how these social processes unfold over the full high school period across multiple high schools. The current study addresses these gaps by investigating the diffusion of smoking initiation among 1,425 racially and ethnically diverse high school students from four U.S. high schools and tracking changes in their friendships and smoking behaviors over four time points. This investigation is warranted because there are differences in smoking rates and norms since the 1990s and early 2000s and in low-income communities and communities of color that may give rise to different smoking dynamics in peer networks. For example, as smoking rates have decreased and smoking cigarettes has become less socially accepted, it is plausible that the association between smoking and popularity has waned. This could decrease the norm salience of smoking and thus its importance as a basis for youths’ friendship choices as well as their motivation to adopt the (rarer) smoking behaviors of their peers. Lower smoking rates would also result in fewer opportunities for youth to be exposed to friends who smoke and may dampen the diffusion of smoking initiation through peer networks. Indeed, simulation studies suggest that when rates of risk behaviors are low in a social network, peer influence tends to have an overall effect of promoting nonadoption of the behavior (adams and Schaefer 2016).
Although these changes may have lessened the diffusion of smoking through peer networks in the past decade, we anticipate that there will continue to be some evidence that friend smoking influences the initiation of cigarette smoking in high school students and that youth will also select their friends based on similarity in smoking behaviors given the consistent evidence for these phenomena in the literature. However, although the participating high schools come from one low-income urban neighborhood, there are demographic differences between the schools that may lead to different smoking rates and social dynamics linked to smoking. Based on the literature reviewed, we anticipate that school smoking rates and the social status associated with smoking could impact the salience of smoking for friendship selection, motivations to adopt the smoking behaviors of peers, and the peer influence potential of smokers. We will therefore explore school differences in network dynamics linked to smoking initiation and characteristics of schools that may be linked to these differences.
DATA AND METHODS
Procedure
Data for this study were drawn from a larger cohort study investigating the social networks and substance use of 10th-through 12th-grade students from all five high schools located in one eastern Los Angeles County neighborhood. In 2010, this neighborhood had a population of 113,484 (living within 9.6 square miles), and the community was characterized as low income and predominantly Hispanic/Latino. Students who were enrolled in Grade 10 in the fall of 2010 at these five high schools were invited to participate in the study. Of the 2,290 Grade 10 students, 78% (1,795) obtained parental consent and provided personal assent to participate in the study. Baseline (Wave 1) assessments occurred in the fall of students’ Grade 10 year, Wave 2 in the spring of 10th grade, Wave 3 in the spring of 11th grade, and Wave 4 in the spring of 12th grade. Students new to the school at Wave 3 who were in Grade 11 were also recruited and enrolled in the study using the same protocols employed at baseline. The study protocol was approved by the University of Southern California’s Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.
The current analyses focus on data from four of the five high schools (N = 1,425 students) because one of the schools opted to withdraw from the 11th-grade assessment, resulting in too much missing data for the longitudinal network modeling procedure. Study participants (n = 365) in this omitted school were similar to students from the four schools retained in this analytic sample in terms of demographics (45% female, 83% Hispanic), but this school had a higher smoking rate (31% of students had ever smoked at Wave 1).
Participants
Only students who completed at least two of the four study assessments were included in this analytic sample. All students were in Grade 10 at baseline. Of the total sample, 51.2% (729) were female, and 76.4% (963) were Hispanic/Latino.
Measures
Participants completed self-report measures via paper-based survey at all four study waves (see Appendix A in the online version of the article for additional detail). Measures of smoking were taken from the U.S. Center for Disease Control’s Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System and assessed lifetime cigarette smoking (1 = initiated cigarette smoking, 0 = has not initiated cigarette smoking) and past-month cigarette smoking (1 = smoked cigarettes on one or more days, 0 = zero days).
Friendship networks were measured using a roster of all students in the participating grade cohort at the school (including photos and names) and asking participants to nominate 7 of their best friends who were in their same grade at school and up to 12 additional close friends. They completed this same measure at each assessment. Both best friend and close friend nominations were coded as friendships (thus a maximum of 19 friendships were measured at each wave), and an adjacency matrix of friendships was generated for each school at each assessment period where 1 = a directed friendship nomination between a given pair of students and 0 = the absence of a friendship nomination.
Control variables.
Participants reported on their gender (male/female), race-ethnicity (1 = Latino/Hispanic), and socioeconomic status (SES; 1 = family owns its home; 1 = child receives a free school lunch). At each wave, they reported their typical school grades (1 = lowest grades, 5 = highest grades), parent/caregiver smoking (1 = one or two of these adults smoked every day/most days), and sibling smoking (1 = they have any siblings that smoke). We also obtained records for their home group class, which is the group of students they attended core courses with and that was included as a predictor of friendship choices.
Analytic Methods
The extent to which high school student friendship networks predict smoking initiation was tested using two modeling strategies: a probit regression (Wooldridge 2010) with lagged exposure that tested if adolescent exposure to friends who had initiated smoking predicted adolescent’s own smoking initiation, and a stochastic actor-based model (SABM) for network dynamics (Snijders, Steglich, and Schweinberger 2007) that modeled more nuanced social selection and network influence processes. We first fit a pooled probit model for all schools and then explored school differences by estimating separate probit models and SABMs for each school. Analyses were conducted using R (R Core Team 2017), and figures were generated using netdiffuseR (Vega Yon et al. 2017).
Probit regression model.
The probit model (Wooldridge 2010) predicted smoking initiation conditional on time-constant covariates (gender, Latino/Hispanic, SES proxied by the indicator variables free lunch and own home, sibling smoking, parent smoking) and a time-varying predictor: lagged exposure to smokers, defined as the proportion of friends who had initiated smoking in the previous wave. While network exposure was calculated using all observations, in the estimation procedure, we only included individuals who either initiated smoking during the study or were never observed as having initiated smoking and thus explicitly exclude individuals who had initiated smoking prior to Wave 1. This model was estimated both as a pooled regression using all schools adding fixed effects per school and separately for each school to explore school differences. The model also included fixed effects for year. For each estimate, we report probit coefficients as well as average marginal effects as the latter estimates the average difference in probability for the outcome Y given a unit change in the predictor variable (estimated using the margins postestimation command in Stata13; StataCorp 2013).
In this probit model, network exposure was explicitly exogenous as we are using lagged exposure rather than contemporaneous exposure. One potential problem is the existence of unobserved covariates that codetermine smoking initiation and network structure. Hence, if friendship selection depended on some unobserved covariate that also predicted smoking initiation, our estimates would be biased because we would not be able to disentangle selection from influence (Shalizi and Thomas 2011).
SABM for network and behavior dynamics.
SABM for network and behavior dynamics (Snijders et al. 2007; Snijders, van de Bunt, and Steglich 2010) were applied to identify temporal processes most likely to give rise to the observed social networks and differentiate social selection and social influence effects related to smoking. SABMs are implemented in the RSiena package in R (Ripley et al. 2017), and technical descriptions of the models are available from several sources (Snijders et al. 2007; Steglich et al. 2010). The current analyses implement SABMs for network diffusion, which extends the SABM framework as a hazard model for outcomes that are dichotomous and nondecreasing and estimates the time to an event (hazard rate; Greenan 2015). Models were estimated using a method of moments procedure, and specification of the models was based on a forward selection approach to parameter selection described in Snijders et al. (2010).
Parameters of the SABMs were specified to predict both friendship network dynamics and smoking initiation dynamics and were tested for significance based on a t-ratio (estimate divided by the standard error). Parameters predicting changes to the friendship network included effects of student smoking, student covariates, and endogenous effects of the network structure. Three effects were specified for lifetime smoking: (1) the effect of adolescents’ smoking on their tendency to nominate friends (smoke ego), (2) the effect of peer smoking on the tendency for adolescents to nominate them as a friend (smoke alter), and (3) an effect capturing if adolescents and school peers with the same smoking behavior were more likely to nominate each other as friends (smoke same). The ego, alter, and same (or similarity for continuous variables) effects were also estimated for covariates known to predict adolescent friendships (gender, race-ethnicity, school grades, and SES). An effect of being in the same home group class (class same) was also included as a predictor of friendship choices. The structural effects predicting friendship choices were selected based on empirical and theoretical evidence of endogenous network effects in friendship networks and assessments of model fit.
Effects predicting smoking initiation included exposure to friends who smoked and covariates. Friend influence was tested using a parameter that assessed the impact of friends’ average smoking (average exposure) on changes to adolescent smoking (i.e., a change from having never smoked to smoking initiation) that was based on the average of similarity scores between actors and their friends (Ripley et al. 2017). The following covariate effects on smoking initiation were also included in the models: gender, race-ethnicity, SES, academic grades, parent smoking, and sibling smoking.
Assumptions of heterogeneity in the parameter estimates across time were tested using the RSiena TimeTest function (Lospinoso et al. 2010), and dummy variables for time periods (i.e., the period of time between two consecutive study waves) were included for parameters with evidence of differences in effects across time. Model goodness of fit (GOF) was assessed using the sienaGOF function in RSiena (Ripley et al. 2017).
RESULTS
Descriptive Statistics
Participant characteristics.
Table 1 summarizes participant smoking, demographics, and covariates. Within each school, roughly half of participants were female, and the proportion of Hispanic/Latino students varied from 53.4% (School 3) to 95.6% (School 4). Often the minority of students (26%–53%) came from households where their parents/caregivers owned the home that they lived in, and the vast majority (84% to 97%) received free or reduced-priced lunch. Rates of parent smoking and sibling smoking varied across schools (11%–30% and 7%–15%, respectively).
Table 1.
School 1 | School 2 | School 3 | School 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 449) | (n = 288) | (n = 371) | (n = 317) | |
Characteristics | M (SD)/n (%) | M (SD)/n (%) | M (SD)/n (%) | M (SD)/n (%) |
Age | 15.1 (0.4) | 15.0 (0.3) | 15.1 (0.5) | 15.0 (0.4) |
Female | 235 (52.3%) | 158 (54.9%) | 173 (46.6%) | 163 (51.4%) |
Hispanic | 318 (70.8%) | 269 (93.4%) | 198 (53.4%) | 304 (95.6%) |
SES | ||||
Own home | 226 (50.3%) | 76 (26.4%) | 195 (52.6%) | 141 (44.5%) |
Free lunch | 388 (86.4%) | 279 (96.9%) | 313 (84.4%) | 283 (89.3%) |
M (SD) academic gradesa | 3.7 (1.0) | 3.5 (0.9) | 3.7 (1.0) | 3.6 (1.0) |
Parent smoking | ||||
Wave 1 | 114 (25.4%) | 60 (20.8%) | 110 (29.7%) | 64 (20.2%) |
Wave 2 | 122 (27.2%) | 65 (22.6%) | 94 (25.3%) | 61 (19.2%) |
Wave 3 | 66 (14.7%) | 61 (21.2%) | 96 (25.9%) | 42 (13.3%) |
Wave 4 | 74 (16.5%) | 44 (15.3%) | 59 (15.9%) | 35 (11.0%) |
Sibling smoking | ||||
Wave 1 | 69 (15.4%) | 39 (13.5%) | 46 (12.4%) | 45 (14.2%) |
Wave 2 | 67 (14.9%) | 29 (10.1%) | 35 (9.4%) | 40 (12.6%) |
Wave 3 | 46 (10.2%) | 21 (7.3%) | 38 (10.2%) | 34 (10.7%) |
Wave 4 | 52 (11.6%) | 22 (7.6%) | 41 (11.1%) | 28 (8.8%) |
Lifetime smokingb | ||||
Wave 1 | 102 (22.7%) | 79 (27.4%) | 73 (19.7%) | 76 (24.0%) |
Wave 2 | 135 (30.1%) | 102 (35.4%) | 89 (24.0%) | 103 (32.5%) |
Wave 3 | 151 (33.6%) | 127 (44.1%) | 116 (31.3%) | 123 (38.8%) |
Wave 4 | 168 (37.4%) | 137 (47.6%) | 128 (34.5%) | 135 (42.6%) |
Past-month smoking | ||||
Wave 1 | 22 (4.9%) | 21 (7.3%) | 23 (6.2%) | 21 (6.6%) |
Wave 2 | 27 (6.0%) | 21 (7.3%) | 19 (5.1%) | 20 (6.3%) |
Wave 3 | 18 (4.0%) | 15 (5.2%) | 25 (6.7%) | 16 (5.1%) |
Wave 4 | 30 (6.7%) | 16 (5.6%) | 28 (7.6%) | 25 (7.9%) |
Note: Survey data were collected from four high schools in one Los Angeles neighborhood from 2010 to 2013. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; SES = socioeconomic status.
Academic grades: 1 = F, 2 = D, 3 = C, 4 = B, 5 = A.
The number of participants that smoked for the first time between Wave 1 and Wave 4 was 56 in School 1, 40 in School 2, 47 in School 3, and 52 in School 4.
At baseline, about one in four students had smoked cigarettes in their lifetime, although this percentage ranged from 19.7% in School 3 to 27.4% in School 2. Over the high school period, the rates of participants that had ever smoked increased so that between one third and one half of participants had initiated smoking by the end of high school. Again School 2 had the highest rates, with 47.6% of students having tried smoking by the end of high school. Rates of past-month smoking were much lower, ranging from 4.9% (School 1) to 7.3% (School 2) at baseline, and increasing negligibly (<2%) by Wave 4 in all schools except in School 2, where there was a small decline (1.7%). Descriptive statistics for past-month smoking are provided to characterize the study sample, but this variable is not included in the subsequent analyses, which focus on smoking initiation.
Network characteristics.
Characteristics of the school friendship networks are summarized in Table 2. On average, students nominated 2 to 6 friends at each wave, although the full range was from 0 to 19 friends. The range of friend nominations received by students was 0 to 23. Across all waves and schools, about 1 in 3 friendship nominations were reciprocated (reciprocity index). The friendship networks changed substantially over the three years of high school (and four study waves): On average, during each period transition between two study waves, approximately one quarter of friendship ties remained stable, one third of friendships were newly formed, and 40% of friendships were dissolved. Observed similarities on lifetime smoking behavior among friends, or network autocorrelation, is also described in Table 2 using Moran’s i. Coefficient values for this measure of spatial correlation that are close to 0 indicate that connected individuals (i.e., friends) are not more similar on smoking than would be expected if they were randomly paired, and values close to 1 indicate that connected individuals are very similar. Friend similarities on smoking were typically found to increase over time and were lowest in School 2.
Table 2.
School 1 (n = 449) | School 2 (n = 288) | School 3 (n = 371) | School 4 (n = 317) | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristics | W1 | W2 | W3 | W4 | W1 | W2 | W3 | W4 | W1 | W2 | W3 | W4 | W1 | W2 | W3 | W4 |
M friends nominated | 5.68 | 4.89 | 3.45 | 2.26 | 4.30 | 3.99 | 5.31 | 3.93 | 5.48 | 5.61 | 5.59 | 3.18 | 6.39 | 6.12 | 5.19 | 2.21 |
Range friend outdegreea | 0–19 | 0–18 | 0–16 | 0–17 | 0–18 | 0–19 | 0–19 | 0–19 | 0–19 | 0–19 | 0–19 | 0–18 | 0–18 | 0–18 | 0–17 | 0–10 |
Range friend indegreeb | 0–22 | 0–17 | 0–16 | 0–10 | 0–18 | 0–16 | 0–20 | 0–17 | 0–19 | 0–16 | 0–18 | 0–12 | 0–23 | 0–17 | 0–16 | 0–12 |
Reciprocity index | .36 | .33 | .34 | .36 | .34 | .34 | .41 | .41 | .36 | .35 | .38 | .41 | .34 | .32 | .39 | .49 |
Transitivity indexc | .17 | .15 | .16 | .15 | .17 | .19 | .21 | .25 | .18 | .18 | .18 | .22 | .16 | .16 | .17 | .2 |
Moran’s i smokingd | .17 | .13 | .17 | .21 | .09 | .10 | .10 | .10 | .19 | .18 | .30 | .22 | .08 | .04 | .13 | .27 |
P1 | P2 | P3 | P1 | P2 | P3 | P1 | P2 | P3 | P1 | P2 | P3 | |||||
% Stable friend ties | 26.9 | 18.7 | 18.9 | 3.2 | 25.1 | 25.8 | 32.5 | 27.0 | 25.5 | 26.4 | 2.7 | 18.4 | ||||
% New friend ties | 31.7 | 43.8 | 26.0 | 33.0 | 52.8 | 27.2 | 35.8 | 38.5 | 22.0 | 37.2 | 42.2 | 25.1 | ||||
% Friend ties dissolved | 41.4 | 37.5 | 55.1 | 36.7 | 22.2 | 47.0 | 31.8 | 34.5 | 52.5 | 36.4 | 37.1 | 56.5 | ||||
Actors joining | 0 | 31 | 0 | 0 | 38 | 0 | 0 | 27 | 0 | 0 | 25 | 0 | ||||
Actors leaving | 0 | 80 | 106 | 0 | 21 | 64 | 0 | 23 | 85 | 0 | 41 | 105 | ||||
Jaccard coefficiente | .27 | .19 | .19 | .30 | .25 | .26 | .33 | .27 | .26 | .26 | .21 | .18 |
Note: Survey data were collected from four high schools in one Los Angeles neighborhood from 2010 to 2013. M = mean; W = Wave, P = time period between two consecutive study waves.
Outdegree is the number of friend nominations made by a student.
Indegree is the number of friend nominations received by a student.
Transitivity index represents the proportion of friendship two paths (where A nominates B as a friend and B nominates C) that formed friendship triads (where there are friendship ties between A, B, and C).
Moran’s i is a measure of spatial correlation where coefficient values close to 0 indicate that connected individuals (i.e., friends) are not more similar on smoking than would be expected if they were randomly paired and values close to 1 indicate that connected individuals are very similar.
The Jaccard index measures the amount of change between consecutive waves and is used to express quantitatively whether the data collection points are not too far apart. Values of .2 to .3 and greater are desired to meet assumptions that the network change process is gradual (Snijders et al. 2010).
Probit Regression Models Testing Effects of Exposure to Friend Smoking on Adolescent Smoking Initiation
Probit regression models tested if friend smoking predicted adolescent smoking. The results are summarized in Table 3. The lagged exposure term, capturing exposure to friends that had initiated smoking, was strongly and significantly associated with adolescent smoking initiation in the full sample (all schools). The average marginal effect of exposure to friend smoking in the full sample indicates that a unit change in exposure (i.e., moving from 0% of friends who smoked to 100% of friends who smoked) increased the chance of a student initiating smoking by 16 percentage points. In models fit to schools individually, this effect was significant in School 1 and School 4 but not in School 2 and School 3. The average marginal effects of exposure to friend smoking indicate that moving from no exposure (0% of friends) to full exposure (100% of friends) increased the chance of a student initiating smoking by 20 percentage points in School 1 and 28 percentage points in School 4. Two covariates fairly consistently and significantly predicted smoking initiation: Across all schools, students were more likely to initiate smoking if they had lower academic grades and siblings who smoked (significant in School 3 and School 4).
Table 3.
All Schools | School 1 | School 2 | School 3 | School 4 | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(N = 1,425) | (n = 449) | (n = 288) | (n = 371) | (n = 317) | ||||||
Variable | PE | ME | PE | ME | PE | ME | PE | ME | PE | ME |
Lagged exposure to friend smoking | 1.06** (.32) | .16** (.05) | 1.42* (.60) | .20* (.09) | .54 (.84) | .09 (.14) | .76 (.83) | .10 (.11) | 1.53* (.73) | .28* (.14) |
Covariats | ||||||||||
Female | −.13* (.06) | −.02* (.01) | .03 (.16) | .00 (.02) | −.19 (.18) | −.03 (.03) | −.12 (.16) | −.01 (.02) | −.29† (.17) | −.05† (.03) |
Hispanic | .10* (.04) | .02* (.01) | −.02 (.22) | −.00 (.03) | .18 (.49) | .03 (.08) | .14 (.19) | .02 (.02) | .13 (.51) | .02 (.09) |
Grades | −.29** (.06) | −.04** (.01) | −.45** (.11) | −.06** (.02) | −.29* (.14) | −.05* (.02) | −.28** (.11) | −.03** (.01) | −.20† (.11) | −.04† (.02) |
Own home | −.03 (.05) | −.00 (.01) | −.00 (.16) | −.00 (.02) | −.27 (.25) | −.05 (.04) | .03 (.17) | .00 (.02) | .01 (.17) | .00 (.03) |
Free lunch | .01 (.15) | .00 (.02) | .26 (.27) | .04 (.04) | −.88† (.53) | −.15† (.09) | −.15 (.22) | −.02 (.03) | .18 (.35) | .03 (.07) |
Parent smoking | .13 (.10) | .02 (.02) | .25 (.16) | .04 (.02) | .08 (.20) | .01 (.03) | .31† (.16) | .04† (.02) | −.14 (.18) | –.03 (.03) |
Sibling smoking | .38** (.03) | .06** (.00) | .32† (.18) | .05† (.03) | .41† (.22) | .07† (.04) | .41* (.18) | .05* (.02) | .40* (.19) | .07* (.03) |
Constant | −.60** (.20) | −.32 (.54) | .53 (.97) | −.84 (.53) | −.97 (.82) | |||||
Events | 195 | 56 | 40 | 47 | 52 |
Note: Standard errors in parentheses: Robust standard errors are reported with PE, and standard errors estimated using the Delta-method are reported with the ME. Year fixed effects included but not reported. Survey data were collected from four high schools in one Los Angeles neighborhood from 2010 to 2013. ME = average marginal effect; PE = parameter estimate.
p < .10
p < .05
p < .01.
SABM for Friendship Network and Smoking Initiation Dynamics
SABM results for the effects of primary interest are summarized in Table 4. A description of results for the full model are provided in Appendix B and Supplementary Table B1 (available in the online version of the article). These models were fit to identify the most probable temporal processes within school social contexts, including mechanisms of social selection and network influence, which gave rise to the observed dependencies between friend smoking and adolescent smoking found in the probit regression models. Because of the school differences in network effects identified by the probit models, a separate SABM was fit to each school.
Table 4.
School 1 (n = 449) | School 2 (n = 288) | School 3 (n = 371) | School 4 (n = 317) | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model Parameter | PE | (SE) | PE | (SE) | PE | (SE) | PE | (SE) | ||||
Friendship network dynamics | ||||||||||||
Effects of lifetime smoking | ||||||||||||
Smoke ego | .04 | (.03) | −.02 | (.04) | −.08 | (.04) | † | .02 | (.04) | |||
Smoke alter | .13 | (.04) | ** | −.80 | (.04) | * | .07 | (.04) | † | .05 | (.04) | |
Smoke same | −.02 | (.05) | −.05 | (.05) | .00 | (.05) | .03 | (.05) | ||||
Smoking initiation dynamics | ||||||||||||
Effects of the friend network | ||||||||||||
Average exposure to friends’ smoking | 3.09 | (1.46) | * | .53 | (1.83) | 2.76 | (1.13) | * | 3.48 | (1.91) | † | |
Effects of individual covariates | ||||||||||||
Female | ||||||||||||
Hispanic | −.78 | (.36) | * | |||||||||
Grades | −.36 | (.09) | ** | −.22 | (.13) | † | .26 | (.21) | ||||
Own home | ||||||||||||
Free lunch | ||||||||||||
Parent smoke | ||||||||||||
Sibling smoke |
Note: Parameter estimates that are not included in the table were all tested during the forward selection approach used to specify the models and were excluded from the final model because they were not marginally or statistically significant. Results of the full model are provided in the Supplemental Files available in the online version of the article. Survey data were collected from four high schools in one Los Angeles neighborhood from 2010 to 2013. PE = Parameter estimate; SE = standard error.
p < .10
p < .05
p < .01.
Predictors of friendship network dynamics.
Generally, friendship choices were not predicted by student’s lifetime smoking status over and above the effects of network structure and covariates that were included in the model. In all four schools, students were not found to select friends who were similar to them in lifetime smoking (smoke same), and lifetime smoking status did not predict the students’ tendency to nominate friends (smoke ego). Smoking was associated with popularity: In School 1, students who had tried smoking became more popular (positive smoke alter), and this effect was also positive but not statistically significant in School 3 and School 4; however, in School 2, there was a significant but opposite effect indicating that students who had tried smoking became less popular (negative smoke alter).
Effects of covariates and network structure that also predicted the friendship network dynamics are described in Appendix B in the online version of the article. In brief, students preferred to befriend peers who were similar to them in terms of gender, school grades, and race-ethnicity. In School 1, 2, and 4, students with lower school grades attracted more friendship nominations, while in School 3, students with higher school grades attracted more friendship nominations.
Predictors of smoking initiation.
Controlling for effects predicting friendship selection in the SABM, smoking initiation among students was predicted by network effects and student attributes (covariates). Initiating smoking was significantly predicted by exposure to friends who had tried smoking in their lifetime (positive average exposure to friends’ smoking) in School 1 and School 3, and this effect was marginally significant (p = .06) in School 4 (see Figure 1 for a visualization of diffusion of smoking initiation in one school network). By exponentiating the estimates of the friend exposure effect and multiplying this by the average rate of change across time periods (see Supplementary Table B1 in the online version of the article), we can interpret them as division factors for participant-specific times until adoption of smoking initiation (with one unit of time being the average time period between each observation of the study; i.e., 10 months), under different exposure scenarios (Greenan 2015).1
For a nonsmoking student to adopt smoking in School 1, it would take 20.2 years if their friends’ average lifetime smoking was 0 (i.e., none initiated smoking), 4.3 years if their friends’ average lifetime smoking was .5 (i.e., half initiated smoking), and .9 years if their friends’ average lifetime smoking was 1 (i.e., all initiated smoking). The corresponding participant-specific times until smoking initiation in School 3 are 13.2 years (no friends initiated smoking), 3.3 years (half of friends initiated smoking), and .8 years (all friends initiated smoking); and in School 4, they are 19.2 years (no friends initiated smoking), 3.4 years (half of friends initiated smoking), and .6 years (all friends initiated smoking).
The effect of friend smoking on adolescent smoking initiation was not significant in School 2.
Males were significantly more likely than females to initiate smoking in School 4 (negative effect of female). Students with lower school grades were significantly more likely to initiate smoking than students with higher grades in School 1, and there was a similar but nonsignificant trend in School 2. No other covariates significantly predicted smoking initiation.
GOF.
GOF for the SABMs was assessed using a method that compares auxiliary statistics of the observed networks to the simulated values at the end of each time period (Ripley et al. 2017). The differences are assessed by combining the auxiliary statistics using the Mahalanobis distance, a measure of the distance between an observed statistic P from a simulated distribution of that statistic D. We assessed four key auxiliary GOF statistics (indegree distribution, outdegree distribution, triad census, and geodesic distribution), and the Mahalanobis distance scores are summarized in the Supplemental Table B2 (available in the online version of the article). In all four schools, the model fit was adequate for the indegree distribution, outdegree distribution, and the majority of the triad censuses. Model fit for the lowered end of the geodesic distribution was good, but the upper end of this distribution (geodesics of 4 and 5) had inadequate fit (a common challenge with SABMs). The addition of other structural parameters did not improve model fit or resulted in models that would not converge. The different specifications of these models did not result in different findings for our study hypotheses.
DISCUSSION
This study found evidence of smoking initiation diffusing through friendship networks over the high school period in some but not all of the schools included in our sample. This parallels the mixed findings of peer influence on smoking in previous research (Alexander et al. 2001; Go et al. 2012; Green et al. 2013; Haas and Schaefer 2014; Hall and Valente 2007; Kiuru et al. 2010; Lakon et al. 2015; Mercken et al. 2009; Schaefer et al. 2012, Steglich et al. 2010; Valente et al. 2013). Probit models found evidence of associations between exposure to friends who had smoked and adolescent smoking initiation in two schools (Schools 1 and 4), while SABMs that modeled dynamics of the friendship network and adolescent smoking found evidence that smoking initiation was positively predicted by exposure to friends who had smoked in Schools 1, 3, and 4 (this effect was marginally significant in School 4), accounting for processes that predicted network dynamics.
In the three schools where friend’s lifetime smoking predicted or marginally predicted smoking initiation in the SABMs (Schools 1, 3, and 4), students who had not tried smoking but who were friends with smoking initiators were more likely to experiment with smoking themselves relative to nonsmoking students who were not friends with smoking initiators. In these same three schools, we did not find evidence that students befriended peers who were similar to them in lifetime smoking status, and so students who had not tried smoking were likely to become friends with both students who had and who had not initiated smoking. This mixing would have increased opportunities for nonsmokers to be exposed to lifetime smokers and the diffusion of smoking initiation through the friendship network.
Additionally, in these three schools, there was mixed evidence that trying cigarette smoking influenced students’ popularity during high school: Initiating smoking increased students’ popularity in School 1, and this was a nonsignificant but similar trend in School 3 and School 4. This combination of selection and influence effects may give us insights into the underlying mechanisms of peer influence in these schools. It seems plausible that students who initiated smoking could be motivated by goals to fit in with their close friends or attain popularity and social status among their school peers (particularly in School 1). Additionally, the mechanisms of interpersonal influence may be complex: Youth may copy the smoking behaviors recently modeled by their friends based on social learning processes, and/or they may be motivated to adopt an identity of “someone who has tried smoking” if this is the norm among their friends.
There was one school (School 2) in which smoking initiation was not influenced by the friendship network, where having friends who had tried smoking in their lifetime did not predict smoking initiation over the course of high school (in the probit and SABM). Despite being in the same neighborhood, this school had notably higher lifetime smoking rates than in the other three schools (47.6% had tried smoking by the end of high school). Additionally, students in this school had notably lower SES, with just 26.4% of students living in homes that their parents owned and 97% of students receiving free or reduced-price school lunch. There were also differences in the social meaning of smoking in this school: Students who had initiated smoking were unpopular.
The higher rates of smoking initiation at this high school, even at the beginning of Grade 10, may have arisen because youth with lower SES are more likely to smoke (Melotti et al. 2011). Because higher smoking rates can amplify network diffusion (adams and Schaefer 2016), smoking initiation may also have diffused through these networks earlier, with the early stages of the adoption curve playing out before high school for these students. It is possible that the negative association between smoking initiation and popularity was something that emerged at the later stages of diffusion, after the “innovators” and early majority adopters had already tried smoking (Valente 2005). In this scenario, the negative association between popularity and smoking initiation may be a result of smoking having already been adopted by a majority of students rather than being a protective feature of the social system that could limit smoking influence in the earlier stages of diffusion (Schaefer et al. 2013). This could be a topic for future longitudinal research that follows the full adoption curve.
In this specific school context, nonsmoking students may not be influenced to smoke by their smoking friends given the apparent social disincentive to experiment with smoking and low norm salience of this behavior because it is performed by unpopular youth (Dijkstra and Gest 2015). Alternately, because students who had tried smoking were less popular, smokers may have had less peer influence potential because they had fewer friendship connections and were less central in the friendship network (Osgood et al. 2013). The results from the PROSPER intervention found that reducing the centrality and peer influence potential of risky youth in middle school could decrease problem behaviors among peers overall. However, this mechanism may need to be implemented in the early stages of the adoption curve as it does not appear to be at play in School 2, which had the highest rates of lifetime and current smoking.
To better understand school differences that may be linked to peer effects on smoking initiation, we conducted ad hoc analyses exploring differences in participants’ perceived pro-tobacco norms (assessed at Wave 1 and Wave 2 only) across schools. This measure was an average score of four items (e.g., “How many of your five best friends think it’s OK for someone your age to smoke” scored on 4-point scales), where scores of 1 reflected the weakest pro-tobacco norms. Prosmoking norms at Wave 1 were indeed the highest in School 2 (M = 1.9, SD = .8) and were significantly higher than norms in School 1 (M = 1.7, SD = .8) and School 3 (M = 1.6, SD = .7), although not significantly higher than School 4 (M = 1.8, SD = .7). A similar trend (although without statistically significant differences) was found at Wave 2. This suggests that pro-tobacco norms were initially higher School 2, yet initiation of smoking came with a cost of decreased social prestige.
Finally, friendship network effects were notably one of the most consistent significant predictors of smoking initiation, while other covariates such as gender and school grades were less consistently predictive.
Limitations
The measures used in this study have some limitations. Friendship nominations were restricted to school peers who were in the same grade, a network sampling strategy that was based on practical motivations to bound the sample on one grade cohort in each school and measure them as they transitioned through the high school period. This sampling strategy misses potentially influential friends in higher or lower grades or outside of the school. Also, because we did not measure a broad range of cognitions that may have mediated the social influence and selection mechanisms, such as motivations to comply with peer norms and changes in beliefs about smoking identities, we could not explicitly test these as mediators in this study.
A further limitation is that this study was not designed to test for school- or network-level characteristics that explain differences in network dynamics given the sample of four school networks. Finally, one assumption of the SABMs that can be problematic for larger social networks is that actors consider the state of the complete network (i.e., the ties and attributes of all other actors) when making choices to change or maintain friendship ties or behaviors. This is a limitation of the models when applied to the large school friendship networks in this study.
CONCLUSIONS
Based on recent longitudinal data from four urban U.S. high schools with racially and ethnically diverse students, the current study finds evidence that smoking initiation diffuses through adolescent friendship networks over the high school period in some schools but not in others. Although these four schools were based in the same neighborhood and school district and thus represent a fairly homogenous subset of the overall population of high school students in the United States, we observed differences in school characteristics and smoking norms that may result in variations in peer networks effects on smoking. These results point to mechanisms—such as smoking rates, the popularity of smokers, and the stage of diffusion for the initiation of smoking—that should be explored in future work as possible moderators of peer smoking dynamics using a large and representative sample of high school friendship networks.
The two modeling approaches adopted—probit regression models and SABMs that simulate and estimate network and smoking dynamics (Snijders et al. 2007, 2010)—provide valuable insights into temporal dependencies between networks and smoking initiation and unfolding social processes that give rise to these dependencies, respectively. The network dynamics models point to social processes whereby smoking is linked to popularity and social status, and this co-occurs with the diffusion of smoking through peer networks.
Identifying the dependencies between peer and adolescent smoking as well as social processes that give rise to conditions under which smoking diffuses through adolescent social networks, is important to understand and intervene on in smoking prevention initiatives (Valente 2012). Simulation studies show that for interventions that target peer influence processes to effectively reducing smoking, they will need to be tailored to the initial smoking rates and the popularity of smokers (adams and Schaefer 2016). For example, engaging opinion leaders in contexts where smoking behaviors are not (or are positively) associated with popularity may be effective to dampen peer effects on smoking (Campbell et al. 2008), and this may be particularly effective in contexts with higher smoking rates (vs. those with lower rates, where peer influence has an overall effect of promoting nonsmoking; adams and Schaefer 2016). However, we must also be mindful that if the risk behavior gives rises to popularity, recruiting popular students to promote smoking abstinence may actually reduce their popularity and social influence. In contexts where smoking is negatively associated with popularity, opinion leader interventions may not be effective, and segmentation strategies (that target at-risk peer groups) may be more appropriate (Valente 2012).
Overall, network interventions for smoking prevention in youth are likely to be important given the pervasive effect of smoking diffusion in peer networks (Go et al. 2012; Green et al. 2013; Haas and Schaefer 2014; Schaefer et al. 2012), but tailoring strategies to local norms and social environments may also be essential. One social size does not fit all settings.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Drafts of these results were presented at several annual meetings of the International Network for Social Network Analysis, and we thank attendees for comments on our preliminary findings.
FUNDING
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was supported by NIH grant number RC1AA019239 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and R01CA157577 from the National Cancer Institute.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHIES
Kayla de la Haye is an assistant professor in the Department of Preventive Medicine, Keck School of Medicine, at the University of Southern California (USC), where she directs the USC Center for Applied Network Analysis. Her research targets family and community social networks to promote healthy behaviors and prevent disease; particularly among priority populations. It also explores the role of social networks in group problem solving in families, teams, and coalitions.
Hee-sung Shin recently completed her PhD in Health Behavior Research in the Department of Preventive Medicine, at the University of Southern California. Her research interests are to apply quantitative analytic techniques in disease prevention research, with an emphasis on social network analysis, multilevel modeling, mixture growth curve modeling, propensity score matching, and latent class analysis. Applied areas of interest include health disparities, immigrant health, and media influences on health behaviors.
George G. Vega Yon is a research programmer and Ph.D. student of Biostatistics in the Department of Preventive Medicine, at the University of Southern California. His research is centered on statistical computing and applications in the context of social networks and phylogenetics.
Thomas W. Valente is a Professor in the Department of Preventive Medicine, Keck School of Medicine, at the University of Southern California. His research focuses on the influence of social networks on a wide variety of behaviors with a particular interest in using social network theory and methods to accelerate diffusion of innovations.
Footnotes
SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIAL
Appendices A and B and Table B1 are available in the online version of the article.
Participant-specific rates = average smoking initiation rate effect × exp(friend’s average smoking value × estimate for avg. exposure to friends’ smoking).
REFERENCES
- adams jimi, and Schaefer David R.. 2016. “How Initial Prevalence Moderates Network-Based Smoking Change: Estimating Contextual Effects with Stochastic Actor-Based Models.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 57(1):22–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander Cheryl, Piazza Marina, Mekos Debra, and Valente Thomas. 2001. “Peers, Schools, and Adolescent Cigarette Smoking.” Journal of Adolescent Health 29(1):22–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell Rona, Starkey Fenella, Holliday Jo C., Audrey Suzanne, Bloor Michael J., Nina Parry-Langdon Rachael A. Hughes, and Moore Laurence. 2008. “An Informal School-Based Peer-Led Intervention for Smoking Prevention in Adolescence (Assist): A Cluster Randomised Trial.” Lancet 371(9624):1595–602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Copeland Molly, Bartlett Bryce, and Fisher Jacob C.. 2017. “Dynamic Associations of Network Isolation and Smoking Behavior.” Network Science 5(3):257–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dijkstra Jan Kornelis, and Gest Scott D.. 2015. “Peer Norm Salience for Academic Achievement, Prosocial Behavior, and Bullying: Implications for Adolescent School Experiences.” The Journal of Early Adolescence 35(1):79–96. [Google Scholar]
- Dutra Lauren M., and Glantz Stanton A.. 2017. “E-cigarettes and National Adolescent Cigarette Use: 2004–2014.” Pediatrics 139(2):e20162450. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennett Susan T., Bauman Karl E., Hussong Andrea, Faris Robert, Foshee Vangie A., Cai Li, and Robert H. DuRant. 2006. “The Peer Context of Adolescent Substance Use: Findings from Social Network Analysis.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 16(2):159–86. [Google Scholar]
- Fujimoto Kayo, and Valente Thomas W.. 2012. “Social Network Influences on Adolescent Substance Use: Disentangling Structural Equivalence from Cohesion.” Social Science & Medicine 74(12):1952–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giovino Gary A., Henningfield Jack E., Tomar Scott L., Escobedo Luis. G., and Slade John. 1995. “Epidemiology of Tobacco Use and Dependence.” Epidemiologic Reviews 17(1):48–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Go Myong-Hyun, Tucker Joan S., Green Harold D. Jr., Pollard Michael, and Kennedy David. 2012. “Social Distance and Homophily in Adolescent Smoking Initiation.” Drug and Alcohol Dependence 124(3):347–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green Harold D., Horta Mariana, de la Haye Kayla, Tucker Joan S., Kennedy David R., and Pollard Michael. 2013. “Peer Influence and Selection Processes in Adolescent Smoking Behavior: A Comparative Study.” Nicotine & Tobacco Research 15(2):534–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenan Charlotte C. 2015. “Diffusion of Innovations in Dynamic Networks.” Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society) 178(1):147–66. [Google Scholar]
- Haas Steven A., and Schaefer David R.. 2014. “With a Little Help from My Friends? Asymmetrical Social Influence on Adolescent Smoking Initiation and Cessation.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 55(2):126–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haas Steven A., Schaefer David R., and Kornienko Olga. 2010. “Health and the Structure of Adolescent Social Networks.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(4):424–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall Jeffrey A., and Valente Thomas W.. 2007. “Adolescent Smoking Networks: The Effects of Influence and Selection on Future Smoking.” Addictive Behaviors 32(12):3054–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiuru Noona, Burk William J., Laursen Brett, Salmela-Aro Katariina, and Nurmi Jari-Erik. 2010. “Pressure to Drink but Not to Smoke: Disentangling Selection and Socialization in Adolescent Peer Networks and Peer Groups.” Journal of Adolescence 33(6):801–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kobus Kimberly. 2003. “Peers and Adolescent Smoking.” Addiction 98(Suppl. 1):37–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lakon Cynthia M., Hipp John R., Wang Cheng, Butts Carter T., and Jose Rupa. 2015. “Simulating Dynamic Network Models and Adolescent Smoking: The Impact of Varying Peer Influence and Peer Selection.” American Journal of Public Health 105(12):2438–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lerman Kristina, Yan Xiaoran, and Wu Xin-Zeng. 2016. “The ‘Majority Illusion’ in Social Networks.” PLoS ONE 11(2):e0147617. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0147617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lospinoso Joshua, Schweinberger Michael, Snijders Tom, and Ripley Ruth. 2010. “Assessing and Accounting for Time Heterogeneity in Stochastic Actor Oriented Models.” Advances in Data Analysis and Classification 5(2):147–76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melotti Roberto, Heron Jon, Hickman Matthew, Macleod John, Araya Ricardo, and Lewis Glyn. 2011. “Adolescent Alcohol and Tobacco Use and Early Socioeconomic Oosition: The Alspac Birth Cohort.” Pediatrics 127(4):e948–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mercken L, Tom AB Snijders, Christian Steglich, and Hein de Vries. 2009. “Dynamics of Adolescent Friendship Networks and Smoking Behavior: Social Network Analyses in Six European Countries.” Social Science & Medicine 69(10):1506–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osgood D. Wayne, Feinberg Mark E., Gest Scott D., Moody James, Ragan Daniel T., Spoth Richard, Greenberg Mark, and Redmond Cleve. 2013. “Effects of PROSPER on the Influence Potential of Prosocial Versus Antisocial Youth in Adolescent Friendship Networks.” Journal of Adolescent Health 53(2):174–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. 2017. “R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing.” Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Ragan Daniel T. 2016. “Peer Beliefs and Smoking in Adolescence: A Longitudinal Social Network Analysis.” The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse 42(2):222–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rambaran Ashwin J., Dijkstra Jan Kornelis, and Stark Tobias H.. 2013. “Status-Based Influence Processes: The Role of Norm Salience in Contagion of Adolescent Risk Attitudes.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 23(3):574–85. [Google Scholar]
- Ripley Ruth, Snijders Tom A. B., Boda Zsofia, Voros Andras, and Preciado Paulina. 2017. “Manual for RSiena.” http://www.stats.ox.ac.uk/~snijders/siena/RSiena_Manual.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer David R., adams jimi, and Haas Steven A.. 2013. “Social Networks and Smoking: Exploring the Effects of Peer Influence and Smoker Popularity through Simulations.” Health Education & Behavior 40(Suppl. 1):24S–32S. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer David R., Haas Steven A., and Bishop Nicholas J.. 2012. “A Dynamic Model of U.S. Adolescents’ Smoking and Friendship Networks.” American Journal of Public Health 102(6): e12–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shalizi Cosma Rohilla, and Thomas Andrew C.. 2011. “Homophily and Contagion Are Generically Confounded in Observational Social Network Studies.” Sociological Methods & Research 40(2):211–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders Tom A. B., Steglich Christian E. G., and Schweinberger Michael. 2007. “Modeling the Co-evolution of Networks and Behavior” Pp. 41–71 in Longitudinal Models in the Behavioral and Related Sciences, edited by van Montfort K, Oud H, and Satorra A. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Snijders Tom A. B., van de Bunt Gerhard G., and Steglich Christian E. G.. 2010. “Introduction to Stochastic Actor-Based Models for Network Dynamics.” Social Networks 32(1):44–60. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. 2013. “Stata Statistical Software: Release 13.” College Station, TX: StataCorp LP. [Google Scholar]
- Steglich Christian, Snijders Tom A. B., and Pearson Michael. 2010. “Dynamic Networks and Behavior: Separating Selection from Influence.” Sociological Methodology 40(1):329–93. [Google Scholar]
- Valente Thomas W. 2005. “Network Models and Methods for Studying the Diffusion of Innovations” Pp. 78–116 in Models and Methods in Social Network Analysis, edited by Carrington PJ, Scott J, and Wasserman S. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Valente Thomas W. 2012. “Network Interventions.” Science 337(6090):49–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valente Thomas W., Fujimoto Kayo, Soto Daniel, Ritt-Olson Anamara, and Unger Jennifer B.. 2013. “A Comparison of Peer Influence Measures as Predictors of Smoking among Predominately Hispanic/Latino High School Adolescents.” Journal of Adolescent Health 52(3):358–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valente Thomas W., Unger Jennifer B., and Johnson CA. 2005. “Do Popular Students Smoke? The Association between Popularity and Smoking among Middle School Students.” Journal of Adolescent Health 37(4):323–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yon Vega, George Stephanie Dyal, Hayes Timothy, and Valente Thomas W.. 2017. “netdiffuseR: Analysis of Diffusion and Contagion Processes on Networks. R Package Version 1.19.0.” https://rdrr.io/cran/netdiffuseR/ [Google Scholar]
- Wooldridge Jeffrey M. 2010. Econometric Analysis of Cross Section and Panel Data. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.