Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Med. 2020 Jul-Sep;46(3-4):189–201. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2020.1729086

When is Hope Enough? Hopefulness, Discrimination and Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Allostatic Load

Uchechi A Mitchell 1, Elinam D Dellor 2, Mienah Z Sharif 3, Lauren L Brown 4, Jacqueline M Torres 5, Ann Nguyen 6
PMCID: PMC7458691  NIHMSID: NIHMS1578958  PMID: 32787721

Abstract

Hopefulness is associated with better health and may be integral for stress adaptation and resilience. Limited research has prospectively examined whether hopefulness protects against physiological dysregulation or does so similarly for U.S. whites, blacks and Hispanics. We examined the association between baseline hopefulness and future allostatic load using data from the Health and Retirement Study (n = 8,486) and assessed differences in this association by race/ethnicity and experiences of discrimination. Four items measured hopefulness and allostatic load was a count of seven biomarkers for which a respondent’s measured value was considered high-risk for disease. A dichotomous variable assessed whether respondents experienced at least one major act of discrimination in their lifetime. We used Poisson regression to examine the association between hopefulness and allostatic load and included an interaction term in our model to test racial/ethnic differences in this association. Subsequent analyses were stratified by race/ethnicity and tested the interaction between hopefulness and discrimination within each group. Hopefulness was associated with lower allostatic load scores (b=−0.043, SE=0.007) but its effects varied significantly by race/ethnicity. Race-stratified analyses suggested that hopefulness was protective among whites (b=−0.051, SE=0.008) and not associated with allostatic load among Hispanics (b=−0.013; SE=0.022) irrespective of experiencing discrimination. Hopefulness was associated with lower allostatic load among blacks reporting discrimination but associated with higher allostatic load among those who did not. Findings suggest that hopefulness plays differing roles for older whites, blacks and Hispanics and, for blacks, its protective effects on physiological dysregulation are intricately tied to their experiences of discrimination.

INTRODUCTION

There is growing interest in the role psychosocial factors play in promoting resilience and the context in which this process occurs among older adults. Nonetheless, a vast majority of health research focuses on identifying and addressing risk factors for poor health and disease. Far less has been done on internal, psychosocial resources that support healthy functioning, foster resilience and promote longevity. Hopefulness is an understudied resource that may be particularly beneficial in the face of persistent stressors and adversity across the life course. Individuals who are hopeful are goal-oriented, hold a positive outlook towards the future and a high sense of agency or control over one’s life and goals.1 In other words, hopefulness conceptually combines goal-directed thinking with a positive outcome expectancy2 and self-efficacy.3,4 Although the constructs are not mutually exclusive, hopefulness can be differentiated from: 1) optimism5 which solely focuses on positive outcome expectancy without considering a person’s motivation and agency, and 2) an internal sense of control1,6 which solely focuses on personal agency without considering a person’s outlook, expectations and motivation. Thus, the integration of these two constructs with goal-oriented thinking potentially makes hopefulness a more protective psychosocial factor compared to either of the two constructs alone.

Prior research has documented associations between hopefulness and composite measures of mental and physical health7, treatment adherence in primary care settings8, and cancer survivorship in adolescents and adults.9 The lack of hope, hopelessness, has been studied more extensively and has been shown to be associated with cardiovascular disease,1012 depressive symptoms,1316 suicidal behavior and ideation,1719 and mortality.20,21

Multiple mechanisms plausibly link hopefulness and health. Hopefulness may promote healthy behaviors or minimize unhealthy behaviors22 because hopeful thinking is associated with the desire to prevent negative experiences, including poor health, the belief that those experiences can be avoided and the identification of pathways to avoid them.3 Hopefulness may also influence health via cognitive processes that effect the physiological stress response, specifically perceptions of fear and threat.23 According to the allostatic load hypothesis, repeated or prolonged exposure to experiences considered threatening or challenging can hinder the body’s ability to adapt to stress.24,25 Stress exposure triggers the release of “primary mediators” from the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system. These mediators, including cortisol, epinephrine and norepinephrine, then trigger the release of “secondary mediators” from the immune, cardiovascular and metabolic systems. Over time, the frequent activation of the physiological stress response creates “wear-and-tear” on these body systems that leads to the dysregulation of or impairments in functioning that manifests as allostatic load.24 Allostatic load is a known predictor of poor health2628 and mortality.29,30 Thus, the extent to which hopefulness protects against increases in allostatic load has important implications for death, disease and disability.

Although extant research on the protective effects of psychosocial resources on allostatic load are mixed31, hopefulness has not been thoroughly examined and may be an integral part of healthy stress adaptation, resilience and the maintenance of normal physiological functioning. For example, hopefulness may lead to proactive coping—the accumulation of resources and skills prior to potentially stressful experiences to prevent or avoid exposure, which then prevents the physiologic stress response in the first place.32 Alternatively, in the face of an existing stressor, hope can diminish stress reactivity by minimizing perceptions of how threatening or severe a stressor may be,23 thus limiting the activation of the stress response system.23 Few studies have considered associations between hopefulness and the intermediary physiological processes underlying changes in health. One exception is a study that found a positive association between hope and allostatic load.33 However, this study was specific to a small sample of newly admitted nursing home residents and not representative of the larger United States (U.S.) population. Other population-based studies have linked hopelessness to individual biomarkers16,34,35 but were not representative of the U.S. and failed to examine racial and ethnic differences in this association.

Considering stark racial and ethnic disparities for multiple health outcomes,36 the mechanisms through which hopefulness affects physiological functioning likely vary across race/ethnicity. Theoretical and empirical research suggest that racial and ethnic minorities and other marginalized populations are disproportionately exposed to social stressors,3740 including discrimination,41,42 and tend to have higher levels of allostatic load than non-Hispanic whites.43 Discrimination is associated with multiple health outcomes and multifaceted in its effects.44,45 It influences socioeconomic status, residential location, health care quality and access, health behaviors and physiological functioning directly.4649 This latter pathway is substantiated by prior research showing that the stress of discrimination is associated with allostatic load and other measures of multi-system dysregulation5052 and individual biomarkers, including markers of immune function,5358 blood pressure,5962 and metabolism.63 Individuals may utilize distinct coping mechanisms and psychosocial resources in the face of discrimination and other social stressors,64,65 which may in turn influence the stress response.66 The combined effects of discrimination and coping resources on the physiological functioning of racial and ethnic minorities may vary from whites due to differences in lived experiences, cognitive appraisal processes and cultural norms about coping strategies.67 Moreover, a lifetime of exposure and adaptation to discrimination may, in fact, facilitate resilience in late life in these populations through the development of psychosocial resources,68 such as hopefulness. Therefore, examination of racial and ethnic heterogeneity in the relationships between hopefulness, discrimination and allostatic load is warranted.

The current study is the first to prospectively examine the relationships between hopefulness, discrimination and allostatic load using data from a nationally representative sample of community-dwelling U.S. whites, blacks and Hispanics in mid-life and old age. It addresses the stated gaps and furthers our understanding of the role hopefulness and discrimination play in physiological functioning. We hypothesize that hopefulness will be inversely associated with allostatic load and that its effects will be more pronounced among older blacks and Hispanics—two populations that over time have experienced significant social and economic disadvantage69 and, as a result, may be more dependent on internal psychosocial resources in later life when faced of adversity. Identifying modifiable psychosocial factors, such as hopefulness,70 that protect against increases in allostatic load may inform future policies and programs focused on the treatment and prevention of disease, particularly among older racial and ethnic minorities. Even more consequential, however, is the potential impact of hopefulness and other resources on racial and ethnic health disparities. Thus, the current study is timely in elucidating how hopefulness facilitates resilience and prevents physiological dysregulation among a racially diverse population of U.S. adults.

METHODS

Study population and data source

The data come from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), an ongoing national survey of the health and aging experiences of U.S. adults aged 51 and older residing in the community. This prospective cohort study is conducted by the University of Michigan with funding by the National Institute on Aging. Data on economic, social and physical well-being have been collected biennial since 1992 using face-to-face and phone interviews, and new cohorts are recruited every six years. Additional details on study design and protocol of HRS are available elsewhere.71 The Institutional Review Board of the University of Michigan approved this study.

In 2006, the HRS started administering supplemental psychosocial and biomarker assessments.72,73 Separate written consent forms were completed for both assessments. Psychosocial data were collected after the core HRS interview using a leave-behind, self-administered questionnaire. The biomarker assessment included the collection of physical measures (e.g., blood pressure, height, weight, waist circumference, etc.), saliva for DNA sequencing and dried blood spots, for measuring cholesterol, hemoglobin A1c and C-reactive protein.

One half of the HRS cohort completed the assessments in 2006, while the other half-sample completed them in 2008. Both cohorts repeat each assessment every four years (i.e., in 2010 and 2012, respectively). We combined the 2006 and 2008 half-samples for each wave to increase the sample size and statistical power of the current study. At baseline (i.e., 2006/2008), there were 15,885 eligible respondents and 13,805 of them participated in the psychosocial assessment. At the four-year follow-up (i.e., 2010/2012), 8,933 of baseline participants were alive and eligible for the biomarker assessment and all of them completed the biomarker assessment. We excluded 188 respondents whose racial/ethnic background was classified as “other” due to small cell sizes and high racial/ethnic heterogeneity within this classification. Individuals missing on any study variable were also excluded (n = 259), resulting in a final analytic sample of 8,486.

Measures

Our measure of physiological dysregulation is allostatic load, which we assessed at follow-up using the following seven biomarkers: pulse pressure (i.e., the difference between systolic and diastolic blood pressure), heart rate, hemoglobin A1c, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, total cholesterol, C-reactive protein (CRP), and waist circumference. We followed previously established methods for measuring allostatic load by dichotomizing each biomarker at cut-points considered “high-risk” for cardiovascular and metabolic diseases (see Supplemental Table S1) and then counting the number of high-risk biomarkers74. Values ranged from 0 to 7. Other constructions of allostatic load have been used that incorporate primary mediators such as epinephrine and norepinephrine, additional immune factors and systolic and diastolic blood pressure instead of pulse pressure. However, the HRS does not include epinephrine, norepinephrine or additional immune markers in the 2010/2012 biomarker assessment. Also, we used pulse pressure, a measure of arterial stiffness,75 instead of systolic and diastolic blood pressure in our measure of allostatic load because pulse pressure has been shown to be a better predictor of cardiovascular functioning among older adults compared to its components.7678

Baseline hopefulness was assessed by reverse-coding and averaging the following four items: “I feel it is impossible for me to reach the goals that I would like to strive for”, “The future seems hopeless to me and I can’t believe that things are changing for the better”, “I don’t expect to get what I really want” and “There’s no use in really trying to get something I want because I probably won’t get it”. These items came from the Beck Hopelessness Scale79 and the Kuopio Ischaemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study20, both of which have been shown to be valid in clinical and nonclinical samples. Possible response options for each item ranged from 0=strongly agree to 5=strongly disagree, such that higher levels of the scale represented higher levels of hopefulness. The internal reliability of the scale was high in the total sample and for each race/ethnic group (Cronbach’s α range: 0.80–0.88), and factor analyses identified a single factor from the four items (Eigenvalue = 2.70 in 2006 and 2.85 in 2008).

We focused on major acts of discrimination that unfairly and negatively impact status attainment; the items were adapted from the Lifetime Discrimination Scale.41,80 At baseline, respondents were asked to indicate if any of the following six experiences had ever happened in their lifetime: unfairly dismissed from a job, not hired for unfair reasons, unfairly denied a promotion, unfairly prevented from moving into a neighborhood, unfairly denied a bank loan, and unfairly stopped, searched, threatened or abused by the police. For each respondent we counted the number of reported experiences. We then dichotomized the variable such that 0 = no, does not report experiencing major act of discrimination and 1 = yes, reports experiencing at least one major act of discrimination.

Depressive symptoms are correlated with feelings of hopefulness and allostatic load.33,81 Thus, we included baseline depressive symptoms in our analyses as a potential confounder. Respondents were asked to indicate whether they experienced each of the following eight symptoms, which were derived from a modified version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale:82 felt depressed, everything was an effort, sleep was restless, felt lonely, felt sad, could not get going, was happy, enjoyed life. The last two items were reverse-coded, and we counted the number of symptoms reported. Values ranged from 0 to 8.

Additional covariates were self-reported and assessed at baseline. For race/ethnicity, whites were compared to blacks and Hispanics. Age at baseline was measured in years. Males were compared to females and U.S.-born respondents were compared to foreign-born respondents. Education was assessed as years of school completed. Supplemental analyses added controls for prevalent cardiovascular and metabolic conditions including hypertension, stroke, heart disease, and diabetes.

Statistical Analysis

All analyses were conducted using Stata 15® and statistical procedures and weights that account for the complex HRS sample design. A Wald F-test was used to examine race/ethnic differences in all variables. Multivariate analyses used Poisson regression to determine whether baseline hopefulness was associated with allostatic load, a count variable, at the four-year follow-up, net of covariates (Model 1). An interaction term between hopefulness and race/ethnicity was added (Model 2) and its significance was tested with an omnibus F-test. We then stratified our models by race/ethnicity and included an interaction term between hopefulness and discrimination to determine whether the effect of hopefulness on allostatic load differed by experiences of discrimination within each racial/ethnic group.

Subsequent sensitivity analyses examined an alternative way of measuring allostatic load. Specifically, we included separate measures of systolic and diastolic blood pressure in our allostatic load measure instead of pulse pressure (i.e., the difference between one’s systolic and diastolic blood pressures). Substantive findings using this revised allostatic load measure did not differ from findings using pulse pressure in our measure of allostatic load. Additionally, we examined the interaction between gender and hopefulness and between gender, race and hopefulness (i.e., a 3-way interaction), because resiliency pathways may be gendered83. However, none of these interactions were statistically significant (Supplemental Table S2); thus, the only interaction retained in non-stratified analyses was between race/ethnicity and hopefulness.

RESULTS

Characteristics of the sample by race/ethnicity are presented in Table 1. Differences by race/ethnicity existed for all variables except age. Blacks and Hispanics had higher allostatic load than whites at follow-up and whites had the highest levels of hopefulness at baseline. While 1 in 3 whites and Hispanics reported experiencing discrimination, 45% of blacks reported experiencing at least one major act of discrimination in their lifetime. The average age for the sample and all racial/ethnic groups was 66 years old. There was a significantly greater proportion of women among blacks (63.9%) and Hispanics (61.8%) compared to whites (55.4%), and nearly 52% of Hispanics were foreign born. Years of education were highest for whites (13.5 years) and lowest for older Hispanics (9.8 years); the reverse was true for depressive symptoms with Hispanics on average reporting two times the number of symptoms than whites.

Table 1.

Weighted Sample Characteristics by Race/Ethnicity: HRS (n=8,486)

  Total (n=8,486) White (n=6,803) Black (n=1,039) Hispanic (n=644) p-value

Variables Mean (SE) or % Mean (SE) or % Mean (SE) or % Mean (SE) or %

Allostatic Load, 2010/2012 (range: 0–7) 1.7 (0.02) 1.7 (0.02) 2.0 (0.04) 2.0 (0.05) <0.001
Hopefulness, 2006/2008 (range: 0–5) 3.7 (0.02) 3.8 (0.02) 3.6 (0.07) 3.2 (0.07) <0.001
Experienced Discrimination, 2006/2008 (%) 31.8 31.0 45.1 28.4 <0.001
Age, 2006/2008 (range: 52–96, years) 66.0 (0.21) 66.0 (0.24) 66.0 (0.44) 66.0 (0.61) 0.910
Female (%) 56.3 55.4 63.9 61.8 <0.001
Foreign (%) 6.3 3.8 4.9 51.5 <0.001
Education (range: 0–17, years) 13.2 (0.07) 13.5 (0.05) 12.1 (0.14) 9.8 (0.43) <0.001
Depressive Symptoms, 2006/2008 (range: 0–8) 1.2 (0.03) 1.2 (0.03) 1.7 (0.09) 2.1 (0.13) <0.001

p-value test significance of race difference for each variable

Table 2 shows the association between hopefulness and allostatic load (Model 1) and tests whether these associations differ by race/ethnicity (Model 2). Baseline hopefulness was inversely associated with allostatic load, such that higher levels of hopefulness at baseline were associated with lower allostatic load at follow-up. Blacks had higher allostatic load compared to whites and those who reported experiencing discrimination had higher allostatic load than those who did not. Model 2 adds the interaction between race/ethnicity and hopefulness and shows that the effects of hopefulness on allostatic load varies by race/ethnicity (adjusted Wald Test: F(2,55) = 6.65, p<0.01).

Table 2.

Poisson Regression of Allostatic Load (2010/2012) on Hopefulness (2006/2008): HRS (n=8,486)

Model 1
Model 2
b SE b SE

Hopefulness, 2006/2008 −0.043*** 0.007   −0.054*** 0.008
Blacka 0.126*** 0.022 −0.112 0.072
Hispanica 0.058 0.039 −0.129 0.079
Black-x-Hopefulness, 2006/2008 0.067*** 0.019
Hispanic-x-Hopefulness, 2006/2008 0.057* 0.022
Experienced Discrimination, 2006/2008 0.059* 0.023 0.057* 0.023
Age, 2006/2008 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001
Femaleb 0.042* 0.016 0.043** 0.016
Foreign-bornc −0.072 0.045 −0.07 0.045
Education −0.032*** 0.003 −0.032*** 0.003
Depressive Symptoms, 2006/2008 0.009 0.006 0.008 0.006
Constant 0.953*** 0.104   1.000*** 0.107
*

p<0.05,

**

p<0.01,

***

p<0.001;

a

ref=whites,

b

ref=males,

c

ref=U.S.-born

The interaction between race/ethnicity and hopefulness is depicted in Figure 1, which shows the predicted allostatic load score by level of hopefulness for each race/ethnic group. While baseline hopefulness is inversely associated with allostatic load at follow-up for whites (slope: −0.09, p<.001), the association is positive but not significant for blacks and Hispanics.

Figure 1. Association between Hopefulness and Allostatic Load by Racial/Ethnic Group: HRS (n=8,486).

Figure 1.

To better understand the differential effects of hopefulness on allostatic load for older whites, blacks and Hispanics, we tested the interaction between hopefulness and discrimination within race-stratified models (Table 3). For whites, hopefulness was negatively associated with allostatic load (Model 1) but the interaction between hopefulness and discrimination was not significant (Model 2). For blacks, the opposite was true: hopefulness was not associated with allostatic load in the main effects model (Model 3) but the interaction between hopefulness and discrimination was significant (Model 4). The association was null for Hispanics irrespective of the interaction (Models 5 and 6).

Table 3.

Race-stratified Regression of Allostatic Load (2010/2012) on the Interaction between Hopefulness (2006/2008) and Discrimination (2006/2008), HRS (n=8,486)

White (n=6,803) Black (n=1,039) Hispanic (n=644)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6

  b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE

Hopefulness 2006/2008 −0.051*** 0.008 −0.042*** 0.011 0.017 0.018 0.067** 0.024 −0.013 0.022 −0.005 0.027
Discrimination 2006/2008 0.067* 0.028 0.162 0.085 −0.019 0.053 0.362** 0.126 0.059 0.056 0.154 0.154
Hopefulness-x-Discrimination −0.026 0.021 −0.107*** 0.03 −0.03 0.049
Age 2006/2008 0.002 0.001 0.002 0.001 0.001 0.003 0.001 0.002 −0.001 0.003 −0.001 0.003
Femalea 0.028 0.018 0.028 0.018 0.207*** 0.051 0.203*** 0.049 0.017 0.06 0.019 0.06
Foreign-bornb −0.069 0.064 −0.069 0.065 −0.234 0.143 −0.244 0.137 0.001 0.051 0.003 0.052
Education −0.038*** 0.004 −0.038*** 0.004 −0.009 0.01 −0.008 0.009 −0.022** 0.007 −0.022** 0.008
Depressive Symptoms, 2006/2008 0.007 0.007 0.006 0.007 0.037** 0.012 0.038** 0.012 −0.004 0.014 −0.005 0.014
Constant 1.054*** 0.117 1.017*** 0.126 0.489* 0.243 0.286 0.233 1.003*** 0.254 0.981*** 0.264
*

p<0.05,

**

p<0.01,

***

p<0.001;

a

ref=males,

b

ref=U.S.-born

Figure 2 shows the interaction between hopefulness and discrimination for blacks. Among blacks who report experiencing major acts of discrimination, the association between hopefulness and allostatic load is negative (slope: −0.081; p=0.093); but, for blacks who report not experiencing any major acts of discrimination in their lifetime, the association is positive (slope: 0.138; p<0.01).

Figure 2. Association between Hopefulness and Allostatic Load among Older Blacks by Experienced Discrimination: HRS (n=1,039).

Figure 2.

DISCUSSION

This study revealed significant associations between hopefulness and allostatic load among a racially diverse sample of adults in mid-life and old age. Our findings align with previous research suggesting hopefulness is beneficial for physiological functioning33 and health.9,84,85 However, we offer novel findings on racial and ethnic differences in this association, which contribute to our understanding of resilience in mid-life and old age.

Specifically, higher levels of hopefulness at baseline were associated with lower allostatic load scores four years later. This association, however, was not consistent across all groups. While greater hopefulness was associated with lower allostatic load scores among whites, it is not associated with allostatic load among Hispanics. For blacks, the association depended on their experiences of discrimination: hopefulness was inversely associated with allostatic load among blacks who reported experiencing at least one major act of discrimination in their lifetime, but positively associated with allostatic load among blacks who did not report experiencing discrimination. These associations persisted even after taking into account group differences in depressive symptoms and socioeconomic status. Supplemental analysis additionally controlled for prevalent cardiovascular health conditions and revealed similar findings.

A plausible explanation for the mixed results is that the mechanisms through which hopefulness affects physiological functioning are multifactorial, involving social, psychological and behavioral processes and, therefore, are neither universal nor uniform. For example, hopeful individuals may be more likely to actively engage in planning and problem-solving strategies that mitigate the degree of stress they are exposed to or foster greater resources for dealing with anticipated stressors. This is supported by research showing that hopeful individuals tend to have a positive problem orientation and rational problem-solving style compared to those characterized as hopeless.86 Having a more hopeful perspective can also facilitate perceiving problems as challenges that are within one’s capacity to solve and the use of a deliberate and systematic approach to solving these problems.87 This proactive coping style may minimize or prevent a physiological response to stressful encounters prior to exposure,32 thus leading to less wear-and-tear on the body over time.

Additionally, hopefulness may buffer the effects of a stressor vis-à-vis the influence on other psychological and social factors or health behaviors. One pathway for this relationship is through greater positive emotions,85 which facilitate adaptation to stress.88 Hopeful individuals are also less likely to have an external locus control6 and are more likely to engage in health-promoting behaviors when diagnosed with chronic conditions,84 both of which are associated with lower allostatic load scores.8991 Collectively, these mechanisms protect the body from damage caused by constant activation of the physiological stress response, help maintain normal physiological functioning and adaptability, and promote resilience to future stressors.

A novel finding of our study is that the salience of the aforementioned mechanisms differs for older whites, blacks and Hispanics. While whites may directly benefit from hopefulness irrespective of experiencing discrimination, the protectiveness of being hopeful is intricately tied to experiences of discrimination among blacks. Our initial examination of race differences in the relationship between hopefulness and allostatic load suggested that baseline hopefulness was not associated with future allostatic load among blacks, which would be an erroneous conclusion because heterogeneity in how hopefulness affects the physiological functioning of blacks was not considered. Additional analyses revealed that, on the one hand, greater hopefulness was protective for blacks who report experiencing major acts of discrimination; on the other hand, it was associated with higher allostatic load scores and detrimental for the physiological functioning of blacks who did not report having these experiences. This latter finding was unexpected and is counter to our hypotheses. However, previous studies have documented worse health outcomes among individuals reporting few or no experiences of discrimination compared to those with higher rates of exposure.92,93

The unexpected findings among blacks should be interpreted within the context of their lived experiences. Namely, our study focused on reports of discrimination among adults born between 1914 and 1960, during the Jim Crow Era and Civil Rights Movement, which was a politically-charged and racially-divisive period in the U.S. distinguished by pervasive acts of racism. Thus, one explanation for our findings is that older blacks who have experienced and overcome past instances of discrimination may be more hopeful in later life than those who did not. More specifically, for blacks in our sample, it could be that overcoming these earlier experiences of discrimination and “making it” to old age despite the adversity they faced helped foster hope and other internal resources that more effectively protect against stress and physiological dysregulation in later life. This aligns with the hypothesis that resilience—the capacity to adapt to a threat—is a multifactorial quality forged over time through a process of repeated success over obstacles and challenges.94,95 Thus, older blacks who have experienced major acts of discrimination throughout their lives may be better able to hold on to hope in the face of new challenges than those who have not, which would provide greater protection against physiological dysregulation.

The idea that experiencing discrimination facilitates the development of psychosocial resources is counter to conventional stress theory positing that stressors erode internal resources and thereby provide a way through which stressors exerts their deleterious effects.96 However, this hypothesis fails to consider the positive effects of stress adaptation over time.97 The “shift-and-persist” hypothesis98 suggests that the adverse effects of early life adversity, as measured by low socioeconomic status, on physiological functioning can be averted through a learned reappraisal process that involves accepting the stressor and adjusting oneself to the exposure (i.e., “shifting”), and finding meaning in the experience and maintaining optimism (i.e., “persisting”). Learning this strategy earlier in life may reduce the emotional and physiological response to stressful situations acutely, which, in turn, mitigates physiological dysregulation in later life. Previous research has documented differences in stress appraisal by race and ethnicity99 but appraisal processes likely vary within racial/ethnic groups as well. Therefore, it is possible that blacks in our sample who have experienced discrimination have learned to reappraise their experiences and find a greater purpose or meaning in them that helps them maintain the protective effects of hope.

Conversely, another explanation for our findings is that older blacks who state that they have not experienced any major acts of discrimination in their lifetime may in fact be denying and/or internalizing their experiences. This failure to acknowledge one’s experiences of discrimination may interfere with stress adaptation and promote physiological dysregulation. Prior research suggests that racial and ethnic minorities perceive lower rates of personally-experienced discrimination compared to rates of discrimination they perceive members of their social group experience.100 In other words, they tend to deny their own disadvantage while recognizing the disadvantage of their own social group.101 Minimizing or denying discrimination may be psychologically protective if it maintains a person’s sense of personal control and self-esteem.68,102 However, discrimination is an uncontrollable stressor that directly threatens intrinsic characteristics of oneself.103 Therefore, attempting to be hopeful when faced with discrimination while actively denying its existence may be more taxing on the body and detrimental to physiological functioning than being hopeful while acknowledging its presence. The importance of acknowledging and thoroughly comprehending the magnitude of the adversity one faces is echoed in research on John Henryism—a high-effort, active-coping style characterized by a determined perseverance to succeed despite limited resources104 that, for blacks, paradoxically leads to adverse health including higher blood pressure105108 and worse physiological functioning.109111 As Brody and colleagues explain, this can be due to “outwardly undetectable wear and tear” that manifests as either premature mortality rates and/or cardiometabolic health impairments.111 This phenomenon is attributed to the accelerated aging due to insufficient access to resources, most noticeably low socioeconomic status among blacks, as a result of sustained efforts for upward mobility. In this case, the combination of dealing with stressors associated with low socioeconomic status and coping in a goal-oriented manner adversely affects the health of black men and women and may stem from a failure to fully comprehend the enormity of the structural barriers perpetuated along racial lines. Future research in this area would benefit from examining how different cognitive approaches to coping with discrimination and other adversities impact internal resources and their effects on physiological functioning.

An important contribution of this study was the inclusion of a nationally representative sample of older Hispanics. Despite the fact that Hispanics are the largest racial and ethnic minority group in the country, constituting approximately one-fifth of the U.S. population, there is a noted dearth of empirical research on the relationships between psychosocial resources, discrimination and physiological functioning in this population.112 Our study addressed this gap in the literature and yielded unexpected findings, specifically, the absence of an association between hopefulness and allostatic load irrespective of experiences of discrimination. These findings align with previous research demonstrating inconsistent associations between psychosocial resources and physiological outcomes in this population.113,114 Together, these findings suggest that other psychosocial and sociocultural resources, besides hopefulness, may protect against physiological dysregulation among Hispanics. For instance, cultural values, such as valuing family and social harmony, have been identified as important sources of resilience among Hispanic communities.115 These values foster social support and capital across familial relationships and community networks that, in turn, protect against social disadvantage and stressors. Additionally, the life-course stressors that older Hispanics have faced may be qualitatively different from those of older U.S. blacks. Our sample of Hispanics is highly heterogeneous—more than half are foreign-born and Hispanics from multiple countries of origin have been aggregated due to sample size limitations. Thus, it is possible that resilience to physiological dysregulation among this sample of Hispanics may be more greatly influenced by immigration- and acculturation-related stressors than the experiences of discrimination assessed in this study (e.g., discrimination related to one’s job). Thus, to better understand resilience to physiological dysregulation among older Hispanics, future research should examine the relationships between cultural factors, immigration-related stress, acculturation and allostatic load in this population.

As with any study, there are limitations to the present analysis that should be considered when interpreting the results and addressed in future research. First, some of our measures were self-reported (e.g., hopefulness, discrimination, prevalent health conditions) and are subject to reporting bias. Hopefulness, in particular, may be influenced by social desirability bias and could itself be impacted by health and functioning. Moreover, there are multiple methods to construct the allostatic load measure,28,116 including the count-based method used in the current study. However, counting the number of “high-risk” biomarkers assumes an equal influence (i.e., equal weight) of each biomarker on multisystem dysregulation, which may not be a valid assumption. Weighing methods have been proposed to address this limitation, however, these methods are less straightforward and sample dependent which limits replication in other samples. Moreover, the definition of “high-risk” may vary across population subgroups. Where available, we took into consideration known subgroup differences in high-risk cut-points (i.e., gender differences in high-risk waist circumference) to improve the clinical relevance of our allostatic measure. Also, our analyses are based on data from individuals present at baseline and follow-up, which may bias our sample towards a healthier population and, subsequently, underestimate the effects of hopefulness on allostatic load. Lastly, the time period between baseline and follow-up for our study was limited to four years due to changes in how the HRS collected biomarker and physical measures data after the 2010/2012 assessment. This change in data collection also limited our ability to examine the effects of baseline hopefulness on trajectories of allostatic load. Nonetheless, our study provides initial evidence for the prospective link between hopefulness and physiological functioning.

There are several noteworthy methodological and conceptual strengths of this study. First, this study is the first, to our knowledge, to prospectively assess the impacts of hopefulness on allostatic load in a nationally representative and racially diverse sample of adults in mid-life and old age. Examining the effects of baseline hopefulness on future allostatic load limits issues of reverse causality and demonstrates that the physiological benefits of hopefulness persist over short periods of time. Additionally, our outcome measure, allostatic load, is an objective measure of health status that is less prone to reporting bias. Hopefulness was assessed using a validated scale and, in our sample, exhibited high levels of internal reliability. More importantly, our study highlighted the importance of examining between- and within-group heterogeneity in the effects of psychosocial resources on physiological functioning. Doing so has advanced our understanding of how resilience factors operate across racial/ethnic groups and in the context of social stressors.

CONCLUSION

Hopefulness may be an important, yet understudied, indicator of resilience. Its effects on physiological functioning, however, may be intricately tied to the types of stressors a person is exposed to and differences in cognitive appraisal processes. Collectively, our study provides novel findings that advance our understanding of the complex relationship between psychosocial resources, stressors and physiological dysregulation, specifically highlighting the nuances of how hopefulness potentially contributes to resilience across race/ethnicity. For older blacks, the protective effects of being hopeful depend on stress exposure. Lifetime experiences of discrimination may foster resilience by facilitating the development of hopefulness as a coping mechanism in later life. Conversely, being hopeful may be detrimental for older blacks who fail to report or recognize the discriminatory experiences they have overcome. Given that hope is modifiable and can be incorporated into health programs,70 future research should further explicate the nuanced role hopefulness plays on health among racially and ethnically diverse older adults.

Supplementary Material

Supp 1
Supp 2

Acknowledgements:

UAM is supported by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number U54MD012523. JMT is supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institute of Health under Award Number K01AG056602. LLB is supported by a training grant awarded to Institute for Social Research at University of Michigan by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institute of Health under Award Number T32AG000221. The content of this research is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The Health and Retirement Study data is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging (grant number U01AG009740) and is conducted by the University of Michigan.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interests: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Snyder C, Irving LM, Anderson JR. Hope and health. Handbook of social and clinical psychology: The health perspective. 1991;162:285–305. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Scheier ME, Carver CS. Dispositional optimism and physical well‐being: The influence of generalized outcome expectancies on health. Journal of personality. 1987;55(2):169–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Snyder C, Feldman DB, Taylor JD, Schroeder LL, Adams VH III. The roles of hopeful thinking in preventing problems and enhancing strengths. Applied and Preventive Psychology. 2000;9(4):249–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bandura A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist. 1982;37(2):122–147. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gallagher MW, Lopez SJ. Positive expectancies and mental health: Identifying the unique contributions of hope and optimism. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2009;4(6):548–556. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brackney BE, Westman AS. Relationships among Hope, Psychosocial Development, and Locus of Control. Psychol Rep. 1992;70(3):864–866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barnett MD. Future orientation and health among older adults: The importance of hope. Educational gerontology. 2014;40(10):745–755. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nsamenang SA, Hirsch JK. Positive psychological determinants of treatment adherence among primary care patients. Primary health care research & development. 2015;16(4):398–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Scheier MF, Carver CS. Adapting to cancer: The importance of hope and purpose In: Baum A, Andersen BL, eds. Psychosocial interventions for cancer. Washington DC: American Psychological Association; 2001:15–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Anda R, Williamson D, Jones D, et al. Depressed affect, hopelessness, and the risk of ischemic heart disease in a cohort of US adults. Epidemiology. 1993;4(4):285–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen JT. Hypertension incidence is predicted by high levels of hopelessness in Finnish men. Hypertension. 2000;35(2):561–567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen R, Salonen JT. Hopelessness and 4-Year Progression of Carotid Atherosclerosis The Kuopio Ischemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology. 1997;17(8):1490–1495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Assari S, Lankarani MM. Depressive symptoms are associated with more hopelessness among white than black older adults. Frontiers in public health. 2016;4:82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Johnson JG, Alloy LB, Panzarella C, et al. Hopelessness as a mediator of the association between social support and depressive symptoms: findings of a study of men with HIV. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2001;69(6):1056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mitchell AM, Possel P, Sjogren E, Kristenson M. Hopelessness the “active ingredient”? Associations of hopelessness and depressive symptoms with interleukin-6. Int J Psychiatry Med. 2013;46(1):109–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pössel P, Mitchell AM, Sjögren E, Kristenson M. Do depressive symptoms mediate the relationship between hopelessness and diurnal cortisol rhythm? International journal of behavioral medicine. 2015;22(2):251–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Britton PC, Duberstein PR, Conner KR, Heisel MJ, Hirsch JK, Conwell Y. Reasons for living, hopelessness, and suicide ideation among depressed adults 50 years or older. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2008;16(9):736–741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Daniel SS, Goldston DB. Hopelessness and lack of connectedness to others as risk factors for suicidal behavior across the lifespan: Implications for cognitive-behavioral treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2012;19(2):288–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ron P. Depression, Hopelessness, and Suicidal Ideation Among the Elderly. Journal of Gerontological Social Work. 2004;43(2–3):97–116. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Everson SA, Goldberg DE, Kaplan GA, et al. Hopelessness and risk of mortality and incidence of myocardial infarction and cancer. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1996;58(2):113–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stern SL, Dhanda R, Hazuda HP. Hopelessness Predicts Mortality in Older Mexican and European Americans. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2001;63(3):344–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Taylor SE, Seeman TE. Psychosocial resources and the SES-health relationship. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;896:210–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Taylor SE, Burklund LJ, Eisenberger NI, Lehman BJ, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman MD. Neural bases of moderation of cortisol stress responses by psychosocial resources. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2008;95(1):197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McEwen B, Lasley EN. Allostatic load: when protection gives way to damage. Advances in mind-body medicine. 2003;19(1):28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McEwen BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences-Paper Edition. 1998;840:33–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Karlamangla AS, Merkin SS, Crimmins EM, Seeman TE. Socioeconomic and ethnic disparities in cardiovascular risk in the United States, 2001–2006. Annals of epidemiology. 2010;20(8):617–628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Juster R-P, Marin M-F, Sindi S, et al. Allostatic load associations to acute, 3-year and 6-year prospective depressive symptoms in healthy older adults. Physiol Behav. 2011;104(2):360–364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Juster R-P, McEwen BS, Lupien SJ. Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav R. 2010;35(1):2–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Karlamangla AS, Singer BH, Seeman TE. Reduction in allostatic load in older adults is associated with lower all-cause mortality risk: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Psychosomatic medicine. 2006;68(3):500–507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Borrell LN, Dallo FJ, Nguyen N. Racial/ethnic disparities in all-cause mortality in US adults: the effect of allostatic load. Public Health Reports. 2010:810–816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wiley JF, Bei B, Bower JE, Stanton AL. Relationship of psychosocial resources with allostatic load: a systematic review. Psychosomatic medicine. 2017;79(3):283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Aspinwall LG, Taylor SE. A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological bulletin. 1997;121(3):417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Meeks S, Van Haitsma K, Mast BT, et al. Psychological and social resources relate to biomarkers of allostasis in newly admitted nursing home residents. Aging Ment Health. 2016;20(1):88–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Do DP, Dowd JB, Ranjit N, House JS, Kaplan GA. Hopelessness, depression, and early markers of endothelial dysfunction in US adults. Psychosomatic medicine. 2010;72(7):613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Valtonen M, Laaksonen DE, Tolmunen T, et al. Hopelessness—novel facet of the metabolic syndrome in men. Scand J Public Healt. 2008;36(8):795–802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Chang M-H, Moonesinghe R, Athar HM, Truman BI. Trends in Disparity by Sex and Race/Ethnicity for the Leading Causes of Death in the United States—1999–2010. Journal of Public Health Management and Practice. 2016;22:S13–S24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Aneshensel CS. Social Stress: Theory and Research. Annu Rev Sociol. 1992;18:15–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Meyer IH, Schwartz S, Frost DM. Social patterning of stress and coping: Does disadvantaged social statuses confer more stress and fewer coping resources? Social science & medicine. 2008;67(3):368–379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Turner RJ, Avison WR. Status variations in stress exposure: Implications for the interpretation of research on race, socioeconomic status, and gender. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2003:488–505. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Myers HF. Ethnicity-and socio-economic status-related stresses in context: an integrative review and conceptual model. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;32(1):9–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kessler RC, Mickelson KD, Williams DR. The Prevalence, Distribution, and Mental Health Correlates of Perceived Discrimination in the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1999;40(3):208–230. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Williams DR. Race, socioeconomic status, and health the added effects of racism and discrimination. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1999;896(1):173–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Beckie TM. A systematic review of allostatic load, health, and health disparities. Biological research for nursing. 2012;14(4):311–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Williams DR, John DA, Oyserman D, Sonnega J, Mohammed SA, Jackson JS. Research on discrimination and health: an exploratory study of unresolved conceptual and measurement issues. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102(5):975–978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lewis TT, Cogburn CD, Williams DR. Self-reported experiences of discrimination and health: scientific advances, ongoing controversies, and emerging issues. Annu Rev Clin Psycho. 2015;11:407–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gee GC. A multilevel analysis of the relationship between institutional and individual racial discrimination and health status. American Journal of Public Health. 2002;92(4):615–623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sawyer PJ, Major B, Casad BJ, Townsend SSM, Mendes WB. Discrimination and the stress response: psychological and physiological consequences of anticipating prejudice in interethnic interactions. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102(5):1020–1026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Shavers VL, Fagan P, Jones D, et al. The State of Research on Racial/Ethnic Discrimination in The Receipt of Health Care. American Journal of Public Health. 2012;102(5):953–966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Brondolo E, Blair IV, Kaur A. Biopsychosocial mechanisms linking discrimination to health: A focus on social cognition. The Oxford handbook of stigma, discrimination, and health. 2017:219. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ong AD, Williams DR, Nwizu U, Gruenewald TL. Everyday unfair treatment and multisystem biological dysregulation in African American adults. Cultural diversity and ethnic minority psychology. 2017;23(1):27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Allen AM, Thomas MD, Michaels EK, et al. Racial discrimination, educational attainment, and biological dysregulation among midlife African American women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;99:225–235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kaestner R, Pearson JA, Keene D, Geronimus AT. Stress, Allostatic Load, and Health of Mexican Immigrants*. Social science quarterly. 2009;90(5):1089–1111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Saban KL, Mathews HL, Bryant FB, et al. Perceived discrimination is associated with the inflammatory response to acute laboratory stress in women at risk for cardiovascular disease. Brain, behavior, and immunity. 2018;73:625–632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Simons RL, Lei M-K, Beach SR, et al. Discrimination, segregation, and chronic inflammation: Testing the weathering explanation for the poor health of Black Americans. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(10):1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stepanikova I, Bateman LB, Oates GR. Systemic inflammation in midlife: race, socioeconomic status, and perceived discrimination. Am J Prev Med. 2017;52(1):S63–S76. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Kershaw KN, Lewis TT, Roux AVD, et al. Self-reported experiences of discrimination and inflammation among men and women: The multi-ethnic study of atherosclerosis. Health Psychology. 2016;35(4):343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Beatty Moody DL, Brown C, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT. Everyday Discrimination Prospectively Predicts Inflammation across 7-Years in Racially Diverse Midlife Women: Study of Women’s Health across the Nation. Journal of Social Issues. 2014;70(2):298–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lewis TT, Aiello AE, Leurgans S, Kelly J, Barnes LL. Self-reported experiences of everyday discrimination are associated with elevated C-reactive protein levels in older African-American adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2010;24(3):438–443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Albert MA, Ravenell J, Glynn RJ, Khera A, Halevy N, de Lemos JA. Cardiovascular risk indicators and perceived race/ethnic discrimination in the Dallas Heart Study. Am Heart J. 2008;156(6):1103–1109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lewis TT, Barnes LL, Bienias JL, Lackland DT, Evans DA, Mendes de Leon CF. Perceived discrimination and blood pressure in older African American and white adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(9):1002–1008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Krieger N. Racial and gender discrimination: risk factors for high blood pressure. Soc Sci Med. 1990;30(12):1273–1281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Guyll M, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT. Discrimination and unfair treatment: relationship to cardiovascular reactivity among African American and European American women. Health Psychology. 2001;20(5):315–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hickson DA, Lewis TT, Liu J, et al. The associations of multiple dimensions of discrimination and abdominal fat in African American adults: The Jackson Heart Study. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2012;43(1):4–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Nguyen AW. African American Elders, Mental Health, and the Role of the Church. Generations. 2018;42(2):61–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Nguyen AW, Chatters LM, Taylor RJ, Aranda MP, Lincoln KD, Thomas CS. Discrimination, serious psychological distress, and church-based emotional support among African American men across the life span. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2017;73(2):198–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mwendwa DT, Sims RC, Madhere S, et al. The influence of coping with perceived racism and stress on lipid levels in African Americans. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2011;103(7):594–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Ungar M. The social ecology of resilience: Addressing contextual and cultural ambiguity of a nascent construct. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2011;81(1):1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Taylor SE. Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation. American psychologist. 1983;38(11):1161. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Nazroo JY. The structuring of ethnic inequalities in health: economic position, racial discrimination, and racism. Journal Information. 2003;93(2). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Duggleby WD, Degner L, Williams A, et al. Living with hope: initial evaluation of a psychosocial hope intervention for older palliative home care patients. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2007;33(3):247–257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Sonnega A, Faul JD, Ofstedal MB, Langa KM, Phillips JW, Weir DR. Cohort Profile: the Health and Retirement Study (HRS). International Journal of Epidemiology. 2014;43(2):576–585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Crimmins EM, Faul J, Kim JK, et al. Documentation of Biomarkers in the 2006 and 2008 Health and Retirement Study. Ann Arbor, MI; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Smith J, Fisher G, Ryan L, Clarke P, House J, Weir D. Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire 2006 – 2010: Documentation Report. Ann Arbor, Michigan: Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan;2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Crimmins EM, Johnston M, Hayward M, Seeman T. Age differences in allostatic load: an index of physiological dysregulation. Exp Gerontol. 2003;38(7):731–734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Safar ME, Levy BI, Struijker-Boudier H. Current perspectives on arterial stiffness and pulse pressure in hypertension and cardiovascular diseases. Circulation. 2003;107(22):2864–2869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Assmann G, Cullen P, Evers T, Petzinna D, Schulte H. Importance of arterial pulse pressure as a predictor of coronary heart disease risk in PROCAM. European heart journal. 2005;26(20):2120–2126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Blacher J, Staessen JA, Girerd X, et al. Pulse pressure not mean pressure determines cardiovascular risk in older hypertensive patients. Archives of internal medicine. 2000;160(8):1085–1089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Glynn RJ, Chae CU, Guralnik JM, Taylor JO, Hennekens CH. Pulse pressure and mortality in older people. Archives of internal medicine. 2000;160(18):2765–2772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Beck AT, Weissman A, Lester D, Trexler L. The measurement of pessimism: the hopelessness scale. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1974;42(6):861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology. 1997;2(3):335–351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Arnau RC, Rosen DH, Finch JF, Rhudy JL, Fortunato VJ. Longitudinal effects of hope on depression and anxiety: A latent variable analysis. Journal of personality. 2007;75(1):43–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Hirani S, Lasiuk G, Hegadoren K. The intersection of gender and resilience. J Psychiatr Ment Hlt. 2016;23(6–7):455–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Feldman DB, Sills JR. Hope and cardiovascular health-promoting behaviour: Education alone is not enough. Psychology & health. 2013;28(7):727–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Munoz RT, Hellman CM, Buster B, et al. Life Satisfaction, Hope, and Positive Emotions as Antecedents of Health Related Quality of Life Among Homeless Individuals. International Journal of Applied Positive Psychology. 2016;1(1):69–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Chang EC. Hope, problem-solving ability, and coping in a college student population: some implications for theory and practice. J Clin Psychol. 1998;54(7):953–962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.D’Zurilla TJ, Nezu A. Social problem solving in adults Advances in cognitive–behavioral research and therapy: Elsevier; 1982:201–274. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ong AD, Bergeman C, Bisconti TL, Wallace KA. Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2006;91(4):730. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Satariano WA, McAuley E. Promoting physical activity among older adults: from ecology to the individual. Am J Prev Med. 2003;25(3):184–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Mattei J, Bhupathiraju S, Tucker KL. Higher adherence to a diet score based on American Heart Association recommendations is associated with lower odds of allostatic load and metabolic syndrome in Puerto Rican adults. The Journal of nutrition. 2013;143(11):1753–1759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Roepke SK, Grant I. Toward a more complete understanding of the effects of personal mastery on cardiometabolic health. Health Psychology. 2011;30(5):615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Chae DH, Lincoln KD, Adler NE, Syme SL. Do experiences of racial discrimination predict cardiovascular disease among African American men? The moderating role of internalized negative racial group attitudes. Social Science & Medicine. 2010;71(6):1182–1188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Krieger N, Sidney S. Racial discrimination and blood pressure: the CARDIA Study of young black and white adults. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86(10):1370–1378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Southwick SM, Bonanno GA, Masten AS, Panter-Brick C, Yehuda R. Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: interdisciplinary perspectives. European journal of psychotraumatology. 2014;5(1):25338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Bonanno GA. Resilience in the face of potential trauma. Current directions in psychological science. 2005;14(3):135–138. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Aneshensel CS, Mitchell UA. The Stress Process: Its Origins, Evolution, and Future In: Johnson RJ, Turner RJ, Link BG, eds. Sociology of Mental Health: Selected Topics from Forty Years 1970s to 2010s. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer International Publishing; 2014:53–74. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Bower JE, Low CA, Moskowitz JT, Sepah S, Epel E. Benefit finding and physical health: Positive psychological changes and enhanced allostasis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2(1):223–244. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Chen E, Miller GE. “Shift-and-Persist” Strategies Why Low Socioeconomic Status Isn’t Always Bad for Health. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2012;7(2):135–158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Brown LL, Mitchell UA, Ailshire JA. Disentangling the stress process: Race/ethnic differences in the exposure and appraisal of chronic stressors among older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Taylor DM, Wright SC, Moghaddam FM, Lalonde RN. The personal/group discrimination discrepancy: Perceiving my group, but not myself, to be a target for discrimination. Personality and social psychology bulletin. 1990;16(2):254–262. [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Crosby F. The denial of personal discrimination. Am Behav Sci. 1984;27(3):371–386. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Ruggiero KM, Taylor DM. Why minority group members perceive or do not perceive the discrimination that confronts them: the role of self-esteem and perceived control. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1997;72(2):373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist. 1999;54(10):805–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.James SA. John Henryism and the health of African-Americans. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry. 1994;18(2):163–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Clark R, Adams JH. Moderating effects of perceived racism on John Henryism and blood pressure reactivity in Black female college students. Ann Behav Med. 2004;28(2):126–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.James SA, Keenan NL, Strogatz DS, Browning SR, Garrett JM. Socioeconomic status, John Henryism, and blood pressure in black adults: The Pitt County Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1992;135(1):59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.James SA, Strogatz DS, Wing SB, Ramsey DL. Socioeconomic status, John Henryism, and hypertension in blacks and whites. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1987;126(4):664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Merritt MM, Bennett GG, Williams RB, Sollers JJ III, Thayer JF. Low educational attainment, John Henryism, and cardiovascular reactivity to and recovery from personally relevant stress. Psychosomatic medicine. 2004;66(1):49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Merritt MM, McCallum T, Fritsch T. How much striving is too much? John Henryism active coping predicts worse daily cortisol responses for African American but not white female dementia family caregivers. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2011;19(5):451–460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Merritt MM, Bennett GG, Williams RB, Sollers JJ, Thayer JF. Low educational attainment, John Henryism, and cardiovascular reactivity to and recovery from personally relevant stress. Psychosomatic medicine. 2004;66(1):49–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Brody GH, Yu T, Miller GE, Ehrlich KB, Chen E. John Henryism Coping and Metabolic Syndrome Among Young Black Adults. Psychosomatic medicine. 2018;80(2):216–221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Almeida J, Biello KB, Pedraza F, Wintner S, Viruell-Fuentes E. The association between anti-immigrant policies and perceived discrimination among Latinos in the US: A multilevel analysis. SSM-population health. 2016;2:897–903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Gallo LC, de los Monteros KE, Ferent V, Urbina J, Talavera G. Education, psychosocial resources, and metabolic syndrome variables in Latinas. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2007;34(1):14–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Gallo LC, Fortmann AL, Roesch SC, et al. Socioeconomic status, psychosocial resources and risk, and cardiometabolic risk in Mexican-American women. Health Psychology. 2012;31(3):334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Ruiz JM, Campos B, Garcia JJ. Special issue on Latino physical health: Disparities, paradoxes, and future directions. Journal of Latina/o Psychology. 2016;4(2):61. [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Seeman TE, Epel E, Gruenewald T, Karlamangla A, McEwen BS. Socio-economic differentials in peripheral biology: cumulative allostatic load. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1186:223–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp 1
Supp 2

RESOURCES