Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2020 Sep 4;44(6):819–831. doi: 10.1007/s11031-020-09853-3

Helping when the desire is low: Expectancy as a booster

Małgorzata Kossowska 1,, Ewa Szumowska 1, Paulina Szwed 1, Aneta Czernatowicz-Kukuczka 2, Arie W Kruglanski 3
PMCID: PMC7472690  PMID: 32921845

Abstract

One might assume that the desire to help (here described as Want) is the essential driver of helping declarations and/or behaviors. However, even if desire to help (Want) is low, intention to help may still occur if the expectancy regarding the perceived effectiveness of helping is high. We tested these predictions in a set of three experimental studies. In all three, we measured the desire to help (Want) and the Expectancy that the aid would be impactful for the victim; in addition, we manipulated Expectancy in Study 3. In Studies 1 and 3, we measured the participants’ declaration to help while in Study 2, their helping behavior was examined. In all three studies, we used variations of the same story about a victim. The results supported our hypothesis. Thus, the studies help to tease apart the determinants of helping under conditions of lowered desire to do so, an issue of great importance in public policymaking.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1007/s11031-020-09853-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Intention to help, Helping behavior, Motivation to help, Social judgment, Social cognition

Introduction

Vast research on helping focuses on the desire to help, i.e., the feeling of wanting to do something to help victims (for an overview see: Butts et al. 2019). However, eliciting a high desire to help is not always easy or even possible. This is usually the case in public policy decisions. Moreover, factors such as personal values, religion, personal experience (Charities Aid Foundation 2013), or demographic variables (Wunderink 2002) may lower people’s desire to help. Indeed, recent reports show that these factors systematically decrease the levels of charity in many countries (Charities Aid Foundation 2019; Roy Morgan’s Data 2019). Thus, the question arises as to how to foster helping in situations where the initial desire to do so is relatively weak.

In this paper, we build on motivational readiness theory (Kruglanski et al. 2014) by examining the role of Expectancy in making decisions about charity donations. Expectancy in this case can be defined as the subjective assessment of the extent to which any aid will be impactful for the victim. One might assume that the desire to help (here described as Want) is the essential driver of helping declarations and/or behaviors. However, even if Want is low, declared or actual help may still occur if the Expectancy regarding the perceived effectiveness of helping is high.

Although some researchers have implicated Expectancy as an influence on charitable-giving decisions (e.g., Bendapudi et al. 1996; Cryder et al. 2013a, b; Sharma and Morwitz 2016), their focus was on the separate (or additive) effects of affective (Want) and cognitive (Expectancy) responses to a person in need. In contrast to this, we demonstrate the interplay between these two factors, i.e., Want and Expectancy, via the results of three experimental studies presented in this paper. These help to tease apart the determinants of helping under conditions of lowered desire to do so, an issue of great importance in public policymaking, and in the creation of successful fundraising strategies of many charity organizations.

What motivates helping?

Many researchers consider the desire to help victims as one of the most prominent motive driving declared or actual help (Hofmann and Dillen 2012; Butts et al. 2019). Desire is usually defined as an affectively charged motivation toward a certain object, person, or activity, whose satisfaction is associated with pleasure or a relief from displeasure (see Kavanagh et al. 2005). Indeed, different emotions, both negative and positive may accompany the desire to help and most psychological literature on helping focuses on analyzing these affective responses to people in need (e.g., Cialdini et al. 1987; Loewenstein and Small 2007; Batson 2011; Kogut and Ritov 2005; Genevsky et al. 2013; Cameron and Payne 2011; Butts et al. 2019).

On the other hand, studies have demonstrated that non-affective factors may play a role here, too. For example, people are more likely to help if they believe that their contribution will make a major impact (e.g., Bendapudi et al. 1996). Helping motivation decreases if the overhead costs are perceived as high (Sargeant and Woodliffe 2007), but increases when people believe the benefits of helping outweigh the costs (Erlandsson et al. 2014). This motivation to help also increases when campaigns align with people’s goals; in this circumstance, donors feel that their contribution will make a more substantial impact (Cryder et al. 2013a, b). Other research has shown that people donate more money when they believe they are the only possible helper who can assist an identified child as opposed to when the belief is that there is a shared responsibility to help all children (Basil et al. 2006; Cryder and Loewenstein 2012). Sharma and Morwitz (2016), distinguishing between perceived self-efficacy (i.e., the belief that one can take the steps required to achieve an outcome) and response efficacy (i.e., the belief that the steps taken will result in the desired outcome), demonstrated that increasing perceived self-efficacy increases perceived response efficacy, and donations to multiple beneficiaries. Also, recently, Touré-Tillery and Fishbach (2017) showed that the intention to help increases as the perceived impact of actions increases, such that the greater the expected impact of a contribution, that is, the greater the likelihood that the contribution will alleviate suffering, the more willingness there exists to take an action. In addition, Erlandsson et al. (2014) analyzed the role of perceived impact (also referred to as perceived utility or perceived efficacy) in explaining helping, showing that the higher the believed impact of the contribution, the more likely people are to help (see also Butts et al. 2019). All of the abovementioned results are in line with a survey conducted by the Charities Aid Foundation (2014); 72% of respondents declare that they ‘invest in charities who demonstrate their impact clearly’ and 81% of them believe that ‘more hard evidence of the impact of charities’ work’ would positively influence their decision to donate money.

Interplay between affective and non-affective factors in motivating helping

In the studies mentioned above, researchers tested the affective and non-affective factors underlying help separately; we propose to look at the interplay between them. According to motivational readiness theory (Kruglanski et al. 2014), the willingness or inclination to act in the service of a desire is determined by two factors: Want and Expectancy. The Want state is defined as an outcome that a person desires at a given moment (e.g., a desire to help). Expectancy is a subjective assessment of the gratification of the Want (e.g., a subjective assessment of the extent to which aid will be impactful for the victim). Although both Want and Expectancy influence the inclination to act, they are not functionally equivalent in their effects. Certainly, desire (Want) seems critical and indispensable for undertaking any action. Expectancy, though also contributing to motivational readiness, according to the theory, plays more of an assisting role. The theory allows for the possibility of motivational readiness even where the Expectancy is absent. In contrast, if Want is entirely gone, no amount of Expectancy would suffice to rekindle one’s extinguished readiness.

It is worth stressing however that unlike in classic motivational models in which the Want and Expectancy factors are typically portrayed as fully independent from each other (e.g., Atkinson 1964; Hull 1943; Spence 1956; Tolman 1955), the motivational readiness theory indicated that they are in fact interdependent and that each may partially determine the other under some conditions (see also Dunning 1999; Kunda 1990; Kunda and Sinclair 1999). For example, at high degree of Want the individual may develop a hope of attainment even where objective Expectancy of attainment was low, demonstrating wishful thinking (McGuire 1960; Bélanger et al. 2016). Similar “motivated distortions” were discussed also in other areas and suggest that when people really want something, they find it more achievable and likely (or unlikely in case of something they find highly undesirable, see Kruglanski 1999; Kruglanski and Ajzen 1983). The higher the Want, the greater the tendency to distort reality, or subordinate it to the dominant desire. Nonetheless, the Want factor has major determinants other than expectancy, for example the degree of deprivation (as in hunger) or magnitude of the incentive (e.g. the tastiness of the food). Thus, we expect that Want and Expectancy even though partially linked, are still largely independent of each other, hence they constitute mostly separate sources of behavioral intentions.

In summary then, we assume that Want is a fundamental factor in predicting declared or actual help. If desire is at a high level, Expectancy is not essential to take an action, as long as it reaches a minimal threshold level. However, if Want to help others is flagging, declared or actual help may still occur when prompted by high degrees of Expectancy. In such a circumstance (i.e., low Want, high Expectancy), the magnitude of readiness to help is smaller compared to the situation of high Want, due to the crucial role of the latter; however, the declared or actual help should still be exhibited.

Overview of the studies

We tested the foregoing predictions in a set of three experimental studies.1 In all three, we measured the desire to help (Want) and the Expectancy that the aid would be impactful for the victim; in addition, we manipulated Expectancy in Study 3. In Studies 1 and 3, we measured the participants’ declaration of their intention to help. We are aware that desire to help (Want) and declaration/intention to help might be seen as difficult to distinguish. It is because within attitude theory the concepts of desires and intentions are not differentiated but are often treated as synonyms. However, Perugini and Bagozzi (2004) demonstrated that there are theoretical reasons for distinguishing between desires and intentions. Also, an emerging literature shows that desires strongly influence intentions and substantially mediate most of the effects of attitudes, subjective norms, perceived behavioral control, and other personal reasons for acting on intentions (e.g. Bagozzi and Edwards 1998a, b). Trying to distinguish between desire and intention, Perugini and Bagozzi (2004) indicate that intentions have a stronger connection to goals or outcomes than desires because they imply a commitment, and encompass at least some form of partial planning to achieve the goals or outcomes (Bratman 1987), whereas desires do not. The emphasis on goals, representing combinations of Wants and Expectancies have inspired recent advances in the theory of reasoned action (see, Kruglanski et al. 2016; Ajzen and Kruglanski 2019).

Intentions to act are directly connected to a multitude of activities and outcomes related to the choice of means for action implementation, impediments to action, temptations to perform other actions or consider other goals, cues for retrieval of the intention at a future point in time, and so forth (Bagozzi and Edwards 2000). Also, within the motivational readiness theory the two constructs can be distinguished. Want produces motivational readiness, however, intention is formed only after the commitment point is passed, that is a goal is formed. For the latter, a minimum level of Expectancy is also needed. For instance, one might desire to climb Mount Everest, however, they may form the intention to do so (and actually climb it) only when they find it feasible (i.e. expectancy of success is above threshold), hence committing to the goal to climb.

Our three studies contained common elements allowing convergence but also contained unique features that complemented each other. Whereas in Study 1 we examined behavioral intentions in Study 2 we additionally examined participants helping behavior, instead of mere declarations of intention. In Studies 1 and 3, we also measured negative affect. As we did not have specific predictions about its impact, the results for this variable are presented in Supplementary Materials.

In all three studies, we used variations of the same story about a victim. The basic version of the story read as follows:

A 5-year old boy/girl, who suffers from a serious kidney condition, is now in hospital. The condition will soon lead to kidney failure, which will put the boy’s/girl’s life in danger. For medical reasons, a kidney transplant is impossible as is hemodialysis. However, a medication which can stop the disease has been recently discovered. Unfortunately, health insurance will not pay for it in Poland, and the treatment is very expensive. If the amount of 500,000 PLN is not collected shortly, the progress of the disease will be so advanced that it will be impossible to save the boy/girl.

In all our studies, we tested the interactive effect of the desire to help (Want) and Expectancy, and hypothesized that, at high levels of Want, Expectancy would not matter; that is, the desire to help would be the sole significant predictor of the magnitude of the (intended) help. At the same time, we also hypothesized that at lower levels of Want, Expectancy would significantly predict helping declarations and behaviors.

In order to determine the samples size, we ran an a priori power analysis with the use of G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al. 2009). As for estimates of effect sizes in Studies 1 and 3, they were based on what is typically done in the discipline when the effect size is unknown (i.e. medium effect size is assumed). Since in our studies we expected a significant effect of Expectancy only at low levels of Want, this is where we assumed a medium effect size (β = 0.5). At high levels of Want we assumed no significant effect of Expectancy (effect equal to β = 0), so the expected difference between conditions was equal to 0.5. Such a difference produces an effect size of f2 = 0.07 for the interaction (rather small effect). This is what we entered in our analyses. In Study 2, we also assumed a rather small effect for the whole interaction (odds ratio equals to 2). In all studies, we strived for achieving at least 0.90 power.

The Local Research Ethics Committee approved all studies. Thus, data collection complied with current APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. The data is available at https://osf.io/g5pe6/?view_only=375446b4157a4823b814a8505c191825.

Method

Study 1

In Study 1, we checked if increased Expectancy predicts declarations regarding readiness to help even when the desire to help is low. We thus ran an online study in which we presented participants with a helping scenario and recorded their declared intentions to help. Their desire to help (Want) and the Expectancy that the help would be impactful were measured as well. We investigated whether the interaction between the two factors predicts the declared magnitude of the help.

Participants

A priori power analysis with the use of G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al. 2009) showed that a sample of at least 153 would be necessary to obtain the assumed power. Since the study was conducted online, we increased the sample size by 30% to take into account possible attritions and non-attentive responding. Accordingly, we aimed to recruit at least 204 participants for this study.

Two hundred and nine registered users of the Research Online platform took part in the online experiment. There were 126 women and 83 men, aged between 18 and 84 (M = 33.94, SD = 11.35). Sixty-eight participants had a high school education, and 5 had occupational education; 21 were students, and 115 had completed their higher education at the time of the study. Participants were paid for their participation in line with the rules of the platform (approx. 2 EUR). All participants gave informed consent before participation in the study, and they were debriefed on completion of the study.

Fourteen participants failed our attention check (see below). Therefore, the final sample consisted of N = 195 participants (116 women, 79 men; with a mean of age M = 33.93, SD = 10.85).

Materials and procedure

In the study, we presented participants with a story of a child, or children, in need (it transpired that the version of the story was unimportant, see Supplementary Materials). Then, the participants’ desire to help, and expectancy in relation to the satisfaction of this desire, were measured. Want was measured using one item: To what extent is it important for you to help [the victim]? (1–7 scale anchored with not at all important and definitely important). We also used an additional item: Do you want to help in this situation? But we decided not to include it to the analysis as it is linked too close to measure of intention. We however re-analyzed data using both items and the results were the same. Alongside this, Expectancy2 (Cronbach’s α = 0.81, M = 5.08, SD = 0.93) was measured with four items: (1) In your opinion, by donating, will you effectively help [the victim]? (2) In your opinion, will the necessary amount be collected? (3) In your opinion, if the necessary amount is collected, will it be possible to save…? (4) In your opinion, are actions like this effective? (1–7 Scale anchored with definitely not and absolutely). Indices of Want and Expectancy were obtained by averaging responses to the respective items. The order of Want and Expectancy items was counterbalanced.

Finally, participants were asked about their donation declarations with the following question: What amount would you be willing to donate to this cause? (Participants were asked to mark an amount on a slider). The minimum amount was 0 PLN, and the maximum was 100 PLN, with responses possible in 1 PLN increments. We decided to set an upper limit to make sure that all participants operated within a given range and that their donation decisions would not be influenced by other factors (e.g., their socio-economic status).

In addition, we decided to control for the participants’ gender and attitude towards money as these variables may influence the declarations to donate or actual donation. Indeed, some research show that women are likely to give and give more than man in similar situations (Mesch et al. 2011; Cox and Deck 2006; Anderoni and Vesterlund 2001). Besides, it is already known that people differ in their attitudes towards money, that money attitudes are mostly independent from income, and money perceptions are an additional important factor in the understanding of charitable behavior (Wiepking and Breeze 2011). Thus, even though participants did not donate actual money, their decisions could be influenced by their attitude towards spending. Therefore, we asked participants to rate to what extent they agreed with the following statements: (1) I attach a lot of importance to money. (2) I appreciate the value of money. (3) I carefully think about each zloty spent. (4) Money is important in my life (1–7 scale from definitely disagree to definitely agree). Responses to all items were averaged and the resulting score was entered into the analyses as a statistical control (reported in the Supplementary Materials).

Since the study was run online, we also included an attention check. Specifically, we added a question in which we asked participants to mark response number 3.

Results and discussion

Means and intercorrelations between variables are presented in Table 1. As Want and Expectancy are correlated, we explored the variance inflation factor (VIF) to be sure that performing interaction is accurate (Freund et al. 2003). As VIF equals 1.37, we performed interaction analysis. To test the interactional hypothesis, we regressed Want, Expectancy, and the product of the two on the donation (log transformed prior to the analyses due to the skewness in the data, DV’s skewness = 1.28). The analyses were performed with the use of Process macro for SPSS version 2.13 (Hayes 2013). In all analyses, bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals are reported; 10,000 bootstrap samples were used in all analyses. All variables were standardized prior to analyses.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations

M SD 1 2
Study 1 (N = 195)
 (1) Want [1–7] 4.81 1.31
 (2) Expectancy [1–7] 5.08 0.93 .48*
 (3) Money donated [1–100 PLN] 27.01 26.07 .49* .36*
Study 2 (N = 134)
 (1) Want [1–7] 4.63 1.25
 (2) Expectancy [1–7] 5.02 1.35 .44*
 (3) Money donated [1–20 PLN] 3.62 4.72 .37* .26*
Study 3 (N = 190)
 (1) Want [1–7] 4.71 1.71
 (2) Expectancy [1–7] 5.13 1.13 .65*
 (3) Money donated [1–100 PLN] 26.11 24.14 .50* .34*

*Significant

The results showed that both Want and Expectancy significantly predicted declarations to donate, β = 0.48, t = 7.26, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.35, 0.61] for Want, and β = 0.21, t = 3.23, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.08, 0.34], for Expectancy. Importantly, however, there was a significant interaction, β =  − 0.10, t =  − 2.10, p = 0.04, 95% CI [− 0.19, 0.00]. Conditional effects analysis showed that, whereas there were no significant effects of Expectancy at high (+ 1 SD) values of Want, β = 0.12, t = 1.45, p = 0.15, 95% CI [− 0.04, 0.28], there was a significant effect of Expectancy at low (− 1 SD) values of Want, β = 0.27, t = 3.94, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.14, 0.41]. The interaction is graphically presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Interactive effect of Want and Expectancy on donation (Study 1)

We re-ran the analyses while controlling for gender and attitudes towards money, and they yielded similar results (see Table 1 in Supplementary Materials).

The results thus demonstrated that, although a desire to help (Want) is a crucial factor influencing on declarations to donate, if it is low, Expectancy that the donation is effective matters as well. Specifically, when the desire to help was high, Expectancy did not seem to affect the donation. However, when the desire to help was low, Expectancy became a significant predictor, with high Expectancy levels significantly increasing the declared amount.

Study 2

The aim of Study 2 was to replicate the results described above in a laboratory setting. In addition, we also focused on actual helping behavior, not merely on declarations to donate as in Study 1. It is important extension of Study 1 as declarations made by people about how they behave might not always be consistent with their overt behavior (see Doliński 2018). Thus, it is crucial to demonstrate that our predictions hold also when actual helping behavior is taken into account. Therefore, we set up a laboratory experiment connected with a charity fundraiser in which participants could donate money earned in the lab to a person in need. As in the previous study, Want and Expectancy were measured, and we tested whether they interactively predicted participation in the fundraiser (i.e., in the form of offering help).

Participants

We ran an a priori power analysis for logistic regression with a power of 0.90 and odds ratio of 2. The analysis showed that a sample size of at least 106 participants would be necessary to obtain the assumed power. We intended to recruit more participants in case there were any data losses; we therefore endeavored to enroll at minimum 130 participants in the study.

We recruited 134 participants to take part in a laboratory study on solving cognitive tasks. The sample was comprised of young adults who answered an announcement at university websites and local internet portals. There were 111 women and 21 men (2 participants did not provide their demographics) and they were aged between 18 and 35 (M = 23.66, SD = 3.54). 63 Participants had received a high school education; 2 had occupational education, 2 basic education, and 65 higher education. 50 Participants were students, 20 were employed, 44 both studied and worked, and 18 were doing neither at the time of the experiment.

Participants were paid 20 PLN (around 5 Euro) for participation in the study. All subjects gave informed consent before participation in the study and were debriefed on its completion.

Materials and procedure

Participants were recruited to take part in a study on task solving. In this study, which took about an hour to complete, they solved a set of computer tasks of low to medium difficulty. Participants worked in sessions of up to six participants at the same time, each in a separate cubicle. After completing the task, they were paid the amount of 20 PLN (in one zloty coins; app. 4.5 Euro) and thanked for participation in the study. However, they were also informed that the study was run in cooperation with a local university charity organization which was currently raising money to help children in need. Each participant was presented with a description of the cause in writing, i.e., they read about a child suffering from a kidney disease (in one of the three versions, as presented in the Supplementary Materials; the version did not matter).

Participants were informed that if they decided to support the cause, their money would be given to the charity in full. They were also told that the fund-raiser was not part of the study and that donations were entirely voluntary so their decision to take part was entirely up to them. Nevertheless, all participants were asked to answer several questions, allegedly for evaluation purposes, even if they decided not to donate any money. The questionnaire aimed to measure the desire to help (Want), the expectancy that this desire would be satisfied (Expectancy), and to assess their attitude towards money. Since we wanted the participants to believe this was not a part of a study, we kept the questionnaire short and asked one question per each variable: To what extent is it important for you to help the child?—To measure Want; In your opinion, are campaigns like this effective?—To measure Expectancy; and I carefully think about each zloty spent—to measure their attitude towards money (each responded on a 1–7 scale).

Participants answered the questions in their cubicles so that they could have some privacy and not feel pressured in any way. After answering the questions, they were asked to put the questionnaire, along with the amount (if any) they were willing to donate, in the envelope and leave it on the desk (as participants were paid in single zloty coins, they could leave any amount ranging from 1 to 20 PLN). After completing the study, they were informed that they would soon receive detailed information regarding the purpose of the study and confirmation of their financial donation to the charity organization via email.

In the follow-up email, the purpose of the study was explained to them, and they were provided with more information about the charity organization to which the money was donated (the organization was the Association for Aid to Children with Kidney Diseases associated under the auspice of Professor Marta Uszycka-Karcz, www.nefrologiadziecieca.pl). A receipt for the money transfer was also attached.

Results and discussion

Means and intercorrelations between variables measured in this study are presented in Tables 1 and 2. As Want and Expectancy are correlated, we explored VIF index to be sure that performing interaction is accurate. As VIF equals 1.22, we performed interaction analysis.

Table 2.

Mean differences for variables tested in Study 2 depending on decision to donate (N = 134)

M SD t p-value
Want
 No donation 4.08 1.41  − 3.88  < .001
 Donation 4.96 1.01
Expectancy
 No donation 4.61 1.60  − 2.5 .015
 Donation 5.26 1.12

Since it has been argued that observed behavior, unlike declarations, is often binary, and that it is the decision to donate or not that is of crucial importance (Doliński 2018), we analyzed the effects of Want and Expectancy on the decision to help. To that end, we ran logistic regression on the decision to help (0 when a participant did not donate anything, 1 if they donated something).

The results showed that there was a significant effect of Want, b = 0.77, Z = 2.96, p = 0.003, 95% CI [0.27, 1.28], whereas Expectancy was non-significant, b = 0.20, Z = 0.86, p = 0.390, 95% CI [− 0.25, 0.65]. Importantly, however, there was a non-significant Want × Expectancy interaction, b =  − 0.53, Z =  − 1.86, p = 0.063, 95% CI [− 1.08, 0.03]. Conditional effects analysis showed that, whereas there were no significant effects of Expectancy at high (+ 1 SD) values of Want, b =  − 0.33, Z =  − 0.91, p = 0.364, 95% CI [− 1.05, 0.39], there was a significant effect of Expectancy at low values of the moderator, b = 0.72, Z = 1.99, p = 0.047, 95% CI [0.01, 1.44]. The interaction is graphically presented in Fig. 2. Similar results were obtained when controlling for gender and attitudes towards money (see Table 11 in Supplementary Materials).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Probability of donating depending on Expectancy at different level of want (Study 2)

To test if Want and Expectancy predicts the sum of donation (log transformed prior to the analyses due to the skewness in the data, skewness = 1.58) we ran similar analyses as we did in Study 1. The results showed that there was no interactional effect of Want and Expectancy on donation, β =  − 0.02, t =  − 0.32, p = 0.748, 95% CI [− 0.16, 0.12]. Want was a significant predictor of donation, β = 0.35, t = 3.93, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.17, 0.53], whereas Expectancy was not, β = 0.10, t = 1.08, p = 0.280, 95% CI [− 0.08, 0.28].

Thus, we demonstrated that, although it was the desire to help that mainly predicted helping behavior, when it was low, high Expectancy predicted donations as well. The study adds to the previous one by showing that the interactional effect of Want and Expectancy not only predicts declarations, but also the actual behavior subsequently displayed. Unlike in the previous study, we didn’t find significant effects on the amount of money donated, which might stem from the fact that this time we asked participants for real donations not just declarations. It is in line with the results of other studies investigating actual helping behaviors (vs. declarations), showing that the decision whether to donate differs from the decision on how much to donate (Parsons 2007; Karlan and Wood 2017; Bergh and Reinstein 2020).

Study 3

In Study 3, to more directly test if increased Expectancy may predict donations if desire to help is low, we manipulated the level of Expectancy by providing participants with information about the chances of success of collecting the money for the given victim.

Participants

We calculated power for this study in a similar manner to that carried out for Study 1 but the interaction was calculated for the high and low Expectancy conditions rather than for high and low values of the moderator. The analysis yielded similar results and we aimed to recruit 204 participants for this study.

The sample was comprised of 205 participants. There were 167 women and 38 men ranging in age from 18 to 44 (M = 23.19, SD = 3.91). All participants were enrolled in a lottery in which they could win three prizes of 50 PLN each. All subjects gave informed consent before participation in the study and were debriefed upon completion of the study.

Fifteen participants failed our attention check (see below). Therefore, the final sample comprised N = 190 participants (156 women, 34 men; with the mean age M = 23.06, SD = 3.68).

Materials and procedure

Participants read a story about a child (or children) in need. Additionally, we manipulated the Expectancy of the fund-raiser’s success by adding information about the amount of money that had been collected so far. One group of participants was told that: The money collection is ending soon and, so far, only 50,000 PLN has been collected; this was the low Expectancy condition. Another group of participants was told: The money collection is ending soon and, so far, 450,000 PLN has already been collected; this was the high Expectancy condition. All participants were given the information that 500,000 PLN needed to be raised. We assumed that by informing participants about the progress of the fund-raiser, we would influence their expectations of success regarding the whole campaign. The manipulations used in each condition are presented in the Supplementary Materials.

We used the same measures of Want, Expectancy (Cronbachs α = 0.75, M = 5.13, SD = 1.13), and donations as in Study 1. The participants’ attitudes toward money were also measured. An attention check question was also included (participants were asked to mark answer number 4).

Results and discussion

Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between the variables are presented in Table 1. As Want and Expectancy are correlated, we explored VIF index to be sure that performing interaction is accurate. VIF equals 1.74, thus we performed analysis.

As predicted, the manipulation significantly affected Expectancy, F(1, 188) = 11.52, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.06 (M = 4.84 in the low and M = 5.38 in the high Expectancy condition). There was no effect on Want, F(1, 188) = 0.80, p = 0.372.

Next, we tested whether our manipulation significantly predicted the magnitude of the donation (log transformed prior to the analyses due to the skewness in the data, skewness = 1.16) via Expectancy at different levels of Want. We thus tested a moderated mediation model (Hayes 2013, model 14), with the manipulation as the IV, Expectancy as the mediator, Want as the moderator, and the donation as the DV (the model is pictured in Fig. 3). As in Study 1, the variables were log transformed prior to the analyses due to the skewness in the data. The analyses were performed with the use of Process macro for SPSS version 2.13 (Hayes 2013). In all analyses, bias corrected bootstrap confidence intervals are reported; 10,000 bootstrap samples were used in all analyses. All variables were standardized prior to analyses.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Model tested in Study 3

The results showed a significant moderated mediation effect, IMM =  − 0.06, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [− 0.14, − 0.01], with the manipulation significantly predicting donations at low levels of Want, IE = 0.11, SE = 0.06, 95% CI [0.02, 0.27]. The effect was not significant at high values of Want, IE =  − 0.004, SE = 0.06, 95% CI [− 0.12, 0.12], suggesting that when Want was high, it was of no consequence what our participants were told about the likelihood of collecting the necessary amount. The Want × Expectancy interaction is presented in Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Interactive effect of Want and Expectancy on donation (Study 3)

Thus, this study replicated our previous findings and provided further support for the interactive influence of Want and Expectancy on the declaration of donating. As in previous studies, Want was the main predictor of declarations to help, and when it was high, the level of Expectancy was immaterial. However, when Want was low, the amount of declared donations significantly increased with increasing Expectancy. Moreover, we showed that experimentally manipulating Expectancy translated into an increased willingness to help among participants who were not otherwise inclined to do so (i.e., those feeling low levels of Want).

Summary of the results

To present the results obtained in a more illustrative manner, we statistically summarized the results of Studies 1, 2, and 3. The summary of the study results was performed in Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Software (CMA; Borenstein et al. 2013). We used Pearson’s r as a measure of the effect size and the sample size to calculate sampling variance. We fitted a fixed-effects model with the level of Want as a moderator. The heterogeneity among the effects was significant [I2 = 60.86%; Q(5) = 12.76, p = 0.003] and expected, given the differences in studies designs.

The results showed that the overall effect for the three studies followed the same pattern as the individual studies. To be specific, Expectancy was not associated with helping behaviors when Want levels were high (r =  − 0.02, p = 0.703, 95% CI [− 0.10, 0.07]). However, it was significant and positive at lower levels of Want (r = 0.17, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.09, 0.25]). These results are summarized in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Summarization of Studies 1, 2 and 3 using meta-regression technique. Note in Study 2 the decision to donate (not the decision how much to donate) was included into the analysis

General discussion

The overall goal of this study was to gain a better understanding of the psychological underpinnings associated with helping people in need. We built on motivational readiness theory (Kruglanski et al. 2014) to test the role of Expectancy regarding the perceived effectiveness of helping in predicting helping declarations as well as behavior. In line with the theoretical assumptions, we systematically demonstrated that, although a desire to help (Want) is the essential driver of helping, when this factor is low, both helping declarations (and behaviors) may still occur if the Expectancy is high that the aid provided will be impactful for the victim(s). Despite the fact that a similar notion was previously investigated by other researchers (e.g., Sargeant and Woodliffe 2007; Erlandsson et al. 2014; Cryder et al. 2013a, b; Sharma and Morwitz 2016), in those studies, it was suggested that separate mechanisms underlay the act of helping a victim—with emotional mechanisms being responsible for helping identified victims, and cognitive mechanisms being responsible for helping statistical victims. We however have demonstrated that both components are essential in predicting declared or actual help with the desire to help (Want) being the pivotal predictor of helping, irrespective of the levels of Expectancy that the helping action will be effective. However, when the desire to help (Want) is low, helping is still possible via high Expectancy. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first systematic demonstration of an interactive effect of cognitive and affective components in predicting helping intentions and behaviors.

However, we should also note that, although we treat Want and Expectancy as separate mechanisms, these factors are not independent. This line of thought is in accordance with the motivational readiness theory which assumes that a relation between the two components might exist. This is exemplified in ‘wishful thinking’: when a person really wants something and thus perceives it as more attainable, or when a person’s desire increases when they perceive something as easily attainable or readily available. Such a relation is also seen in our studies, in which we found a moderate correlation between the two variables (see also Caviola et al. 2020). The correlations, however, suggest that these are still two distinct variables, and our further analyses show that they differentially predict helping motivation and behavior.

It is worth noting that in Study 2, although we found the expected effects on decision whether to donate, we didn’t find them on decision how much to donate. As mentioned above, it is consistent with other studies on actual donations, which demonstrated that the decision to donate differs from the decision on amount of the donation (Parsons 2007; Karlan and Wood 2017; Bergh and Reinstein 2020). Also, Dickert et al. (2011) suggest that donation decisions should be regarded as two-stage process, in which the initial decision to donate (stage 1) is separated from the donation amounts (stage 2). Authors suggested that different emotional mechanisms govern each stage (mood management in stage 1 and empathy in stage 2). However, in case of real (vs. hypothetical) donations other factors may play a role as well. As CAF’s report suggests (Charities Aid Foundation 2019), over half of people donate money for altruistic purposes, but the amount of money given was influenced by one’s personal values, sense of morality, faith, etc. (Charity Aid Foundation 2014). Given that we did not focus on the individual differences in our studies (except from attitude toward money), we were able to detect an effect in stages 1 and 2 in case of hypothetical donations, which are in fact only declarations, but not in case of real donations. In addition, we asked for donations only people who decided to donate (of 130 participants, 50 didn’t offer anything), which significantly reduced the sample size. Thus, it is possible that for this analysis our study was underpowered. Also, the variation of our dependent variable was quite small. Finally, there were differences between the format of the scales: in Study 1 the scales ranged between 0 and 100, but in Study 2 between 0 and 20. It means, that in Study 2 the variation of the results was lower than in Study 1.

There are limitations to the research reported here. One limitation is that we used self-reported measures of desire to help (Want), and expectancy to satisfy this desire. Such measures always decrease the ecological validity of the studies, and because many scholars believe they exhibit less construct validity, they should be used with caution (Kuncel et al. 2005). In future studies, it would be beneficial to focus on more implicit measures—behavioral or psychophysiological. It is also worth noting that we conceptualize Want in a rather general fashion, as the importance of helping. However, the desire to help might be determined by a variety of factors (e.g. sympathy, empathy, the desire to be perceived in a positive manner) which tap into the affective component to help. In future studies, it would be of benefit if Want was operationalized in a different way, with different sources of Want being measured.

Another limitation may reside in the fact that our research does not focus on the emotional mechanism underlying the desire to help. Thus, we simply demonstrated that in the case of our manipulation (the invariable cause of the victim’s plight was a disease) negative affect is related to the desire to help. As emotional reactions to victims may be influenced by the particular circumstances that victims are facing, future research should investigate emotional responses to victims experiencing various sorts of plights to corroborate the results of our studies. Besides, we also think that it is possible that after answering the questions about Want and Expectancy, participants would respond highly to giving/donating due to social desirability. Indeed, the participants presented high willingness to help (see the skewness of the donation variable). In the future studies, the role of social desirability needs to be tested to rule out this possibility. Finally, we believe that an additional study with manipulation of Want and Expectancy separately might make our argument stronger. There are two reasons however why we were reluctant to run additional study. First, in Studies 1 and 2 we tried to manipulate Want via identifiability. Above we discuss why this manipulation didn’t work and suggest that future research should investigate emotional responses to victims experiencing various sorts of plights. To meet this goal, we would have to change the procedure of the study completely, thus the new study wouldn’t fit to the present series of the studies. In fact, in the meantime we did additional study with completely different procedure when we manipulate Want. Although this time the manipulation worked, and we replicate the previous results, we decided not to include this study into the paper to keep this paper more concise. However, we included this study into the Supplementary Material (see Study 0). Second, we also think that running the study about helping in COVID-19 situation can heavily influence the results (e.g., the helping mindset might be currently more salient than before). Thus, the replication of the previous results using exact the same procedures wouldn’t be possible and the results wouldn’t be comparable with the previous studies as the social context has changed drastically.

From a pragmatic standpoint, our results suggest ways of designing an effective charitable fundraising or crisis relief campaigns, that is, campaigns that increase the audience’s willingness to donate to victims or bodies tasked with handling the crisis (e.g. COVID-19 pandemic). Our results suggest that the message should address the prospects of goal attainment if just evoking emotions with regards to the victim is not possible. Past studies have suggested that presenting the victim by means of a photo depicting the victim’s circumstances (e.g., a photo of a hungry child expressing sadness) or with a specific background story about him/her (e.g., the hungry child’s parents died when he was 1 year old) will increase the audience’s emotional reactions to the victim’s plight, thereby also increasing their willingness to donate. Indeed, workers of non-governmental charities do their best to make people want to help others. However, other studies have demonstrated that many different factors can influence Want itself, such as outgroup biases (Stürmer et al. 2006), prejudices against some groups (Abrams et al. 2016), and the identifiability of the person in need (Lee and Feeley 2016). It is difficult to successfully address all of these factors in any single charitable campaign. We suggest that charitable actions could be more effective when their work focuses on enhancing Expectancy, especially if it is difficult to evoke a desire to help. As shown here, even when desire is low, helping behavior might still be possible when expectancy is high. Therefore, our recommendation for NGO’s, or any other fundraising organization, will be to always bear in mind that, by increasing the level of expectancy of success, the potential for helping behavior is thereby increased among donating recipients. This could be achieved, for example, by assuring the recipients that the money donated will be adequately invested, and that they will eventually obtain detailed information about how their donation helped. Indeed, recent studies show that informing people about effectiveness of the help increases donations, that is, charities presented as more effective are supported more compared to those presented as less effective (Caviola et al. 2020).

Additionally, we argue that Expectancy is also crucial for public policy makers. Schelling (1968) pointed out that one of the biggest obstacles in effective policy decision-making is the identified victim effect, i.e., peoples’ tendency to preferentially give to identified versus anonymous victims of misfortune. He argued that, from a normative point of view, this preference for identified vs. statistical (anonymous) victim exerts a considerable impact on how public funds are spent, often leading to ineffective uses of resources in the long run. This inefficiency is based on the fact that decision-makers tend to maximize those goals which they consider to be the most valuable in the moment, as determined by affective reactions while overlooking more abstract goals, which are often of greater utilitarian value for society. We argue that increasing expectancy at this macro level (e.g., via training and workshops for health officials), can result in more utilitarian decision-making by reducing biases against prevention policies.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank members of the Centre for Social Cognitive Studies for their comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

Author contributions

MK developed rationale for the studies, MK, ES, PS, AC equally contributed to the study concept and study design. PS and ES contributed to data collection, and analyses. MK, ES and PS wrote the manuscript. AC and AK critically read the manuscript and provided comments that helped improve its final version. All four authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Funding

The research was supported from the Grant NPRH 2bH 15 0068 83 awarded to Tomasz Żuradzki.

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

The local Research Ethics Committee approved all studies. Thus, data collection complied with current APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. The data is available at https://osf.io/g5pe6/?view_only=375446b4157a4823b814a8505c191825.

Conflict of interest

Authors declare no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1

In Studies 1 and 2, we manipulate the Want by identifiability of the victim (i.e., whether identifying information, such as the name, was present or not) in order to evoke emotions related to the victim. Since there were no effects stemming from this manipulation on any of the variables described in the paper (see Supplementary Materials), this manipulation has been omitted from further analyses. It is worth noting that the fact that manipulation was not effective does not mean that the studies are not reliable, since other researchers have also failed to confirm the identified victim effect (see meta-analyses by Hart et al. 2018; Lee and Feeley 2016). No additional manipulations or measures were included.

2

It is worth noting that Expectancy refers not only to the assessment of an individual’s impact on the victim, but also to the assessment of the extent to which aid will be impactful for the victim.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Abrams, D., Swift, H. J., & Mahmood, L. (2016). Prejudice and unlawful behaviour: Exploring levers for change (Report No. 101). Centre for the Study of Group Process, University of Kent.
  2. Ajzen I, Kruglanski AW. Reasoned action in the service of goal pursuit. Psychological Review. 2019;126(5):774–786. doi: 10.1037/rev0000155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderoni J, Vesterlund L. Which is the fair sex? Gender differences in altruism. Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2001;116:293–312. doi: 10.1162/003355301556419. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Atkinson JW. An introduction to motivation. Oxford: Van Nostrand; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bagozzi R, Edwards E. Goal setting and goal pursuit in the regulation of body weight. Psychology and Health. 1998;13:593–621. doi: 10.1080/08870449808407421. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bagozzi R, Edwards E. Goal-striving and the implementation of goal intentions in the regulation of body weight. Psychology and Health. 1998;15:255–270. doi: 10.1080/08870440008400305. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Basil DZ, Ridgway NM, Basil MD. Guilt appeals: The mediating effect of responsibility. Psychology and Marketing. 2006;23(12):1035–1054. doi: 10.1002/mar.20145. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Batson CD. Altruism in humans. New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bélanger JJ, Schumpe BM, Lafrenière M-AK, Giacomantonio M, Brizi A, Kruglanski AW. Beyond goal commitment: How expectancy shapes means evaluation. Motivation Science. 2016;2(2):67–84. doi: 10.1037/mot0000031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bergh R, Reinstein D. Empathic and numerate giving: The joint effects of victim images and charity evaluations. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2020 doi: 10.1177/1948550619893968. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bendapudi N, Singh SN, Bendapudi V. Enhancing helping behavior: An integrative framework for promotion planning. Journal of Marketing. 1996;60(3):33–49. doi: 10.2307/1251840. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Borenstein M, Hedges L, Higgins J, Rothstein H. Comprehensive meta-analysis, version 3. Englewood, NJ: Biostat; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bratman M. Intention, plans, and practical reason. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  14. Butts MM, Lunt DC, Freling TL, Gabriel AS. Helping one or helping many? A theoretical integration and meta-analytic review of the compassion fade literature. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2019;151:16–33. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.12.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cameron CD, Payne BK. Escaping affect: How motivated emotion regulation creates insensitivity to mass suffering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2011;100(1):1–15. doi: 10.1037/a0021643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Caviola L, Schubert S, Nemirow J. The many obstacles to effective giving. Judgment and Decision Making. 2020;15(2):159–172. [Google Scholar]
  17. Charities Aid Foundation. (2013). World Giving Index. A global view of giving trends.https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-publications/worldgivingindex2013_1374aweb.pdf?sfvrsn=e215f440_4.
  18. Charities Aid Foundation. (2014). Why we give.https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-publications/caf-why-we-give-jan14.pdf.
  19. Charities Aid Foundation. (2019). CAF UK Giving 2019. An overview of charitable giving in the UK.https://www.cafonline.org/docs/default-source/about-us-publications/caf-uk-giving-2019-report-an-overview-of-charitable-giving-in-the-uk.pdf?sfvrsn=c4a29a40_4.
  20. Cialdini RB, Schaller M, Houlihan D, Arps K, Fultz J, Beaman AL. Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly or selfishly motivated? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1987;52(4):749–758. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.4.749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cox J, Deck C. When are women more generous than men? Economic Inquiry. 2006;44:587–598. doi: 10.1093/ei/cbj042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cryder CE, Loewenstein G. Responsibility: The tie that binds. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2012;48(1):441–445. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cryder CE, Loewenstein G, Scheines R. The donor is in the details. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2013;120(1):15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cryder CE, Loewenstein G, Seltman H. Goal gradient in helping behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2013;49(6):1078–1083. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.07.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dickert S, Sagara N, Slovic P. Affective motivations to help others: A two-stage model of donation decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2011;24(4):361–376. doi: 10.1002/bdm.697. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Doliński, D. (2018). Is psychology still a science of behaviour? Psychological Bulletin, 13(2), Article e25025.
  27. Dunning D. A newer look: Motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychological Inquiry. 1999;10:1–11. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1001_1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Erlandsson A, Björklund F, Bäckström M. Perceived utility (not sympathy) mediates the proportion dominance effect in helping decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2014;27(1):37–47. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1789. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G* Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Freund RJ, Littell RC, Creighton L. Regression using JMP. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  31. Genevsky A, Västfjäll D, Slovic P, Knutson B. Neural underpinnings of the identifiable victim effect: Affect shifts preferences for giving. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33(43):17188–17196. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2348-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hart PS, Lane D, Chinn S. The elusive power of the individual victim: Failure to find a difference in the effectiveness of charitable appeals focused on one compared to many victims. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0199535. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hayes AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Publications; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hofmann W, Van Dillen L. Desire: The new hot spot in self-control research. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:317–322. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453587. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hull CL. Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: D. Appleton-Century Company, Incorporated; 1943. [Google Scholar]
  36. Karlan D, Wood DH. The effect of effectiveness: Donor response to aid effectiveness in a direct mail fundraising experiment. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics. 2017;66:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2016.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kavanagh DJ, Andrade J, May J. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review. 2005;112:446–467. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kogut T, Ritov I. The “identified victim” effect: An identified group, or just a single individual? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2005;18(3):157–167. doi: 10.1002/bdm.492. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kruglanski A. Motivation, cognition, and reality: Three memos for the next generation of research. Psychological Inquiry. 1999;10:54–58. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1001_8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kruglanski AW, Jasko K, Chernikova M, Milyavsky M, Babush M, Baldner C, Pierro A. The rocky road from attitudes to behaviors: Charting the goal systemic course of actions. Psychological Review. 2016 doi: 10.1037/a0039541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kruglanski AW, Chernikova M, Rosenzweig E, Kopetz C. On motivational readiness. Psychological Review. 2014;121(3):367–388. doi: 10.1037/a0037013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kruglanski AW, Ajzen I. Bias and error in human judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology. 1983;13:1–44. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420130102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kuncel NR, Credé M, Thomas LL. The validity of self-reported grade point averages, class ranks, and test scores: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75(1):63–82. doi: 10.3102/00346543075001063. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kunda Z. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;108:480–498. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kunda Z, Sinclair L. Motivated reasoning with stereotypes: Activation, application, and inhibition. Psychological Inquiry. 1999;10:12–22. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1001_2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lee S, Feeley TH. The identifiable victim effect: A meta-analytic review. Social Influence. 2016;11(3):199–215. doi: 10.1080/15534510.2016.1216891. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Loewenstein G, Small DA. The Scarecrow and the Tin Man: The vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Review of General Psychology. 2007;11(2):112–126. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.112. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. McGuire WJ. Cognitive consistency and attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1960;60:345–353. doi: 10.1037/h0048563. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mesch DJ, Brown MS, Moore ZI, Hayat AD. Gender differences in charitable giving. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. 2011;16:342–355. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.432. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Morgan, R. (2019). Slowdown in giving to charity a major issue.https://www.roymorgan.com/findings/7885-slowdown-in-giving-to-charity-a-major-issue-201902212238.
  51. Parsons LM. The impact of financial information and voluntary disclosures on contributions to not-for-profit organizations. Behavioral Research in Accounting. 2007;19:179–196. doi: 10.2308/bria.2007.19.1.179. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Perugini M, Bagozzi RP. The distinction between desires and intentions. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2004;34:69–84. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.186. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Sargeant A, Woodliffe L. Gift giving: an interdisciplinary review. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. 2007;12(4):275–307. doi: 10.1002/nvsm.308. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schelling TC. The life you save may be your own. In: Chase S, editor. Problems in public expenditure analysis. Washington, DC: The Brookings Institute; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  55. Sharma E, Morwitz VG. Saving the masses: The impact of perceived efficacy on charitable giving to single vs. multiple beneficiaries. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2016;135:45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Spence KW. Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1956. [Google Scholar]
  57. Stürmer S, Snyder M, Kropp A, Siem B. Empathy-motivated helping: The moderating role of group membership. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32(7):943–956. doi: 10.1177/0146167206287363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Tolman EC. Principles of performance. Psychological Review. 1955;62:315–326. doi: 10.1037/h0049079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Touré-Tillery M, Fishbach A. Too far to help: The effect of perceived distance on the expected impact and likelihood of charitable action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2017;112(6):860–876. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wiepking P, Breeze B. Feeling poor, acting stingy: The effect of money perceptions on charitable giving. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. 2011 doi: 10.1002/nvsm.415. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wunderink SR. Individual financial donations to charities in The Netherlands: Why, how and how much? Journal of Nonprofit and Public Sector Marketing. 2002;10(2):21–39. doi: 10.1300/J054v10n02_02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials


Articles from Motivation and Emotion are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES