Abstract
This study examines inter- and intra-personal problems associated with being overweight among 1164 6- to 7-year-olds (49% boys) in 29 rural schools. Socio-emotional data include child self-reports, peer sociometrics, and teacher reports. Results support the hypothesis that children with weight problems struggle socially and emotionally, and extend current understanding of child obesity by demonstrating that problems appear early; are evident in a community sample; can be identified using standard sociometric methods; and are worse among children with severe obesity. Sociometric status difference between levels of obesity also were found. Whereas obese children were neglected by peers, severely obese children were rejected.
Childhood obesity impacts bioecological systems at almost every level. At the societal level, it is a public health threat that has almost quadrupled among 6- to 11-year-olds since 1980 (Ogden, Carroll, Curtin, Lamb, & Flegal, 2010) and that costs the U.S. government billions of dollars annually (Marder & Chang, 2006). It impacts schools, as children with obesity have elevated rates of absenteeism and academic problems. It impacts families, carrying with it high medical costs (an average of $19,000 across the child’s life; Finkelstein, Graham, & Malhotra, 2014) as well as parental stress due to concerns about child well-being (Booth, King, Pagnini, Wilkenfeld, & Booth, 2009). Obesity also impacts the children themselves in multiple developmental domains, including increased risk for metabolic, dental, and asthma health problems (see review by Pulgarón, 2013), as well as childhood myocardial deformation and elevations in blood pressure (Nguyen et al., 2014). Increased child adiposity also predicts later reduced child physical activity which, in turn, is linked to increased risk of childhood obesity (see review by Kellou, Sandalinas, Copin, & Simon, 2014). Additionally, there is a growing literature supporting the increased risk of socio-emotional problems for overweight and obese children (see review by Sgrenci & Faith, 2011).
The current investigation adds to the developing understanding of the social and emotional lives of young obese and overweight children by examining a non-clinical sample of first graders using an inter- and intra-personal risk (IIPR) model of child obesity (Harrist et al., 2012) as a conceptual framework. This model highlights the fact that the unhealthy eating and sedentary behaviors that lead to or support obesity in children occur primarily in inter-personal contexts such as family relationships and peer groups. The model also highlights child intra-personal factors such as child negative affect and poor child self-regulation that might mediate between what is happening in the child’s inter-personal contexts (e.g., home, school) and the development or maintenance of weight problems. In the current study, we explore both peer group dynamics and child intra-personal characteristics (negative affectivity) in a community sample using data collected from teachers, peers, and the children themselves.
Social Life: School as an Inter-Personal Context for Weight Problems
A report from the Institute of Medicine (2012) describes school as a “gateway to healthy weights” because it is where children can learn about nutrition, eat healthy food, and exercise. Yet, for obese children, school can be an undesirable place. Obese children are more likely than other children to have poor school attendance, perhaps avoiding school because of negative feelings about it (see review by Daniels, 2008), as is the case for many children who are disliked or rejected at school.
A child who is considered “different” by the peer group is at increased risk of being rejected, and obesity is one of the most highly stigmatized forms of being different among children, more so than having a physical disability (Latner, Stunkard, & Wilson, 2005). As early as preschool, children prefer very thin to overweight figures in drawings and stories (e.g., Su & Aurelia, 2012). By elementary school, obese children report unsatisfactory peer relations, including social rejection (e.g., Gable, Krull, & Chang, 2009; Gunnarsdottir, Njardvik, Olafsdottir, Craighead, & Bjarnason, 2012). However, the great majority of studies of the association between obesity and peer relations have used either “hypothetical peers” or self-report of peer relations rather than asking the peers themselves about their feelings regarding their actual overweight classmates. Use of this methodology—assessing peer relations by asking about children’s real peers—is especially rare when examining weight problems in the early years of school.
Negative social standing.
The use of peer informants via sociometric interviews and the subsequent formation of sociometric status classifications has long been the gold standard method when studying young children’s classroom-level social structure. With this method, peer nominations (responses to the questions, “Who do you like/not like to play with?”) are tallied across classmates and converted to “liked most” and “liked least” scores, then to social preference (“liked most” minus “ liked least”) and social impact (“liked most” plus “ liked least”) scores, and finally five sociometric statuses (rejected, popular, neglected, controversial, and average) are computed (Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982). Together, these statuses represent a classroom-level social hierarchy which should be useful for understanding the peer relations of obese and overweight school children. Yet, we identified no studies that have compared weight levels to sociometric status using this social classification method. This gap is likely due to the fact that: (a) peer relations scholars have not commonly assessed obesity in their sociometric status studies; and (b) a large number of schools and classrooms is necessary to provide a sample big enough to allow identification of patterns among five sociometric status categories and three or four child weight status categories. We did locate five studies (reviewed below) where peer interview methods were used, although social standing was not operationalized in the standard way (i.e., as sociometric status classifications based on peer nominations) in any of them.
Strauss, Smith, Frame, and Forehand (1985) collected sociometric data using ratings (children rated how much they liked each classmate on a 5-point scale) and nominations (detailed above) in a sample of 36 2nd–5th graders. Two groups were compared: One group who had been classified as obese by observers, and one group who had been classified as non-obese and matched to the obese group on grade, race, and sex. Strauss et al. found that the obese children received significantly lower mean “like” ratings and significantly more “like-least” nominations (but not fewer “ like-most” nominations) than did the non-obese group.
Similarly, Zeller, Reiter-Purtill, and Ramey (2008) assessed social standing in the classrooms of 90 clinically-referred 8- to 16-year-old obese patients (Body Mass Index [BMI] percentile ≥ 95th percentile) and a comparison group who were observed to be non-overweight and who matched on sex, race, and age. Classmates of participants rated how much they liked them, resulting in a mean “ like” rating score. The children and adolescents also completed the Revised Class Play assessment to nominate classmates who fit the descriptions of various class roles. Zeller et al. found obese children received significantly lower “ like” ratings, fewer popular-leader nominations, and more sensitive-isolated nominations than the comparison group. Cohen, Klesges, Summerville, and Meyers (1989) conducted sociometric interviews with 136 1st, 3rd, and 5th graders and, as in the other studies, analyzed data at the level of “ like” and “dislike” ratings and nominations, not sociometric status. These authors found that in 1st grade, overweight boys received significantly fewer “like” (but not more “ dislike”) nominations than overweight girls and normal-weight classmates. In a U.K. sample of 313 9- and 10-year-old girls, Phillips and Hill (1998) developed their own peer-nomination measure of popularity (3 questions: “Which girls in your class would you most like to… play with at break time/sit next to at lunch/take home for tea?”) and other child characteristics, and found that heavier-weight girls did not receive fewer popularity nominations than normal-weight girls (although they were less likely to be nominated as “pretty”). Finally, using a sample of 522 11th and 12th graders, Wang, Houshyar, and Prinstein (2006) computed social preference and perceived popularity scores based on responses to the respective questions (but referring to grade-mates, not class-mates): “Who do you like/not like to spend time with?” and “Who is the most popular?” They found self-reported heavier body size was associated with lower perceived popularity—in a linear fashion for girls, curvilinear for boys—but not with actual social preference.
Thus, although somewhat mixed, there is some evidence that peer informants report not liking their obese classmates as much as their non-obese classmates. However, it is not clear—if sociometric status had been assessed—which “low-liked” status (or statuses) obesity would predict: rejected status (disliked by many, liked by few), neglected status (disliked by few, liked by few), or controversial (disliked by some, liked by some). These distinctions are important given the different concurrent and future correlates associated with each status. For example, children with a rejected sociometric status in sixth grade have been found to be less prosocial and more irresponsible in eighth grade, to significantly increase in risk for poor grades and school dropout, and to have greater risk for later conduct disturbance and substance abuse. Neglected children report less peer support and are more withdrawn than their peers, while children with a controversial status have been found to have lower grades, exert less effort in learning, and have a higher risk of delinquency (see Wentzel, 2003).
It is also noteworthy that three of the available studies used preadolescents or adolescents only. Consequently their findings might not generalize to younger children. The relation between weight and peer relations may differ by age, given findings that appearance-related peer pressure and the importance of physical appearance increase significantly with age (Helfert & Warshburger, 2013). Thus, the current study fills two gaps in the child obesity literature: it extends the research on social preference and social impact to a younger age group than has been studied, and it pioneers the application of sociometric status.
Difficulty with friendship.
Besides examining classroom-level peer standing, some researchers have provided evidence that school-age overweight children have fewer friendships (dyadic relationships with mutual liking) than their peers, although this has been examined more often among adolescents than younger children. Among younger children with weight problems, the likelihood of not having friends is unclear: For example, 32 clinic-referred obese 9- to 11-year-olds reported that, because of their weight, they do not have as many friends as other children (Pierce & Wardle, 1997). However, other studies (e.g., Zeller et al., 2008) have not found fewer reciprocated friendships among obese versus non-obese children. In the current study, we assess children’s perceptions of having friends or supportive relationships in the classroom and teachers’ reports of children’s skills in making and interacting with friends.
Teasing and victimization.
We also examine peer behaviors that are known to be damaging for the social and emotional lives of obese and overweight children. A large group of studies show that children with obesity are at increased risk for being targets of weight-related teasing (see review by Gray, Kahhan, & Janicke, 2009), non-weight related teasing and bullying (e.g., Gunnarsdottir et al., 2012) and other forms of victimization (e.g., physical and relational aggression; Hayden-Wade et al., 2005). In fact, a recent cross-national study of almost 3000 adults (Puhl et al., 2015) found that they consider weight-related bullying to be the most prevalent type of bullying among youth. These victimization phenomena clearly take place during adolescence: For example, in a large school-based study (n = 4,746), approximately 43% of overweight-or-obese adolescents reported being teased by peers (Van den Berg, Neumark-Sztainer, Eisenberg, & Haines, 2008). However, relatively few studies have examined teasing in the early elementary years (e.g., kindergarten through 2nd grade). Three exceptions include Davison and Birch’s (2002) prospective study using a community sample of 192 girls, in which they found BMI was unrelated to teasing at age 5 but became marginally related by age 7; Williams et al.’s (2013) study, where authors reported that, among 5- to 7-year-old urban children, heavier weight was linked to more victimization for girls; and recently, Qualter et al.’s (2015) population study of 3–10 year olds that showed that, for girls over 6 but not boys, higher BMI was associated with greater victimization. The current study extends this literature by assessing teasing and victimization among our sample of 1st graders based on reports of peers, teachers, and the children themselves.
Hypotheses.
Although there is a popular perception that obese children are stigmatized and oppressed by peers, relatively few large multiple-method studies have examined actual social relations. We hypothesize that, according to children, their teachers, and their peers: Overweight and obese children will have lower levels of peer preference (like minus dislike nominations), higher rates of classification of sociometric rejection and/or neglect, poorer sense of relationship support and friendship in the class, and higher levels of peer teasing when compared to non-overweight children. We also examine the relation between child weight and social impact and controversial status, but due to lack of past research, do not hypothesize about how the relations might differ by weight level. We include sex and ethnicity as covariates in our analyses. Because this is the first study to examine social relations as a function of weight status groupings ranging from healthy weight to severely obese, we view the current study as a necessary precursor to the generation of hypotheses about how sex or ethnicity might moderate main effects of weight status.
Emotional Life: Intra-Personal Characteristics of Children with Weight Problems
As proposed in the IIPR model, child obesity not only impacts and is impacted by negative peer experiences, but it also has implications for the obese child’s intra-personal or psychological and emotional well-being. Therefore, in addition to the exploration of the social, inter-personal lives of obese children in the current study, we examine their emotional, intra-personal lives, as well.
One group of intra-personal factors identified in the IIPR model is negative affective mediators such as low self-esteem and body-esteem, depression and loneliness, and social avoidance (Harrist et al., 2012). There are hundreds of papers examining self- and body-esteem among obese children. Here we focus on other aspects of negative affectivity including loneliness, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. We do so in our analytic model by including self- and teacher-report, given that teachers and 1st grade children have been shown to be reliable informants about children’s emotional experiences (see Varni, Limbers, & Burwinkle, 2007).
Loneliness.
Community-based research among school children indicates that, relative to peers, children who are overweight and obese experience significantly elevated levels of loneliness and negative affectivity (e.g., Gable et al., 2009). Rejection and teasing of children has been correlated with loneliness, as well (see review by Storch & Ledley, 2005), providing potential indirect evidence linking overweight and loneliness. In the current study, both self- and teacher-reports of loneliness and negative feelings are assessed.
Anxiety.
Although obese children have been found to be at increased risk for both externalizing and internalizing problems (e.g., Braet, Mervielde, & Vandereycken, 1997), more research finds internalizing problems than externalizing. In particular, children with obesity have been found to be more anxious and withdrawn than their non-overweight peers (e.g., Young-Hyman et al., 2006). In the current study, we include measures of anxiety and difficulties with regulation of social anxiety as reported by teachers and the children themselves.
Hypothesis.
In the current study, overweight and obese children are hypothesized to have elevated rates of negative affectivity (loneliness, anxiety, depressive symptoms) relative to non-overweight children.
Levels of Obesity and the Severely Obese Child
Finally, in the current study, we are interested in examining whether degree (or level) of weight problem is related to degree of social and emotional problems. Research has suggested the importance of different BMI percentiles as indicators of physical health comorbidities in children. Harrington, Staiano, Broyles, Gupta, and Katzmarzyk (2013) found support for the 94th to 96th percentiles as predictors of elevated cardiometabolic risk, visceral adipose tissue, and fat mass among children and adolescents; whereas Freedman, Mei, Srinivasan, Berensen, and Dietz (2007) found that children and adolescents with BMI ≥ 99th percentile had more abnormalities in insulin, lipids, and blood pressure than children and adolescents with BMI ≥ 95th percentile. Halfon, Larson, and Slusser (2013) investigated the incidence of multiple general health (e.g., missed school days), physical health (e.g., activity restrictions), and psychosocial (e.g., conduct disorder) comorbidities for children classified as obese (≥ 95th percentile) or overweight (85th to < 95th percentile) compared to children and adolescents of normal weight. While normal weight children had a 10% prevalence of three or more comorbidities, overweight children had a 13% prevalence and obese children had an 18% prevalence, suggesting obese children were at greater risk. Turer, Lin, and Flores (2013, a study with n = 17,224) reported more psychiatric referrals for obese children than both overweight and normal weight children and adolescents. In the current study, we assess whether socio-emotional factors differ significantly for obese versus overweight children. Additionally, and importantly, we are able to differentiate severely obese from other obese children. Thus, we are able to compare four groups of children, those who are severely obese, obese, overweight, and non-overweight.
Children with severe (or “morbid”) obesity comprise between 4% and 6% percent of the current U.S. youth population (Kelly et al., 2013). The prevalence of severe obesity among children and adolescents is growing, increasing by more than 300% since 1976 and over 70% since 1994 (Skelton, Cook, Auinger, Klein, & Barlow, 2009). Yet children with severe obesity have not been the focus of much research, especially research examining socio-emotional correlates of obesity. Two exceptions are a study of 96 8- to 17-year-olds with severe obesity whose self-reported victimization was related to loneliness and depression (Lim et al., 2011) and a qualitative study of 20 severely obese Mexican-American female adolescents who reported problems with peer relations, including self-isolation (Taylor et al., 2013).
Our reading of the literature is that most studies of severely obese children are medically oriented, examining health consequences or discussing bariatric surgery; in both cases, the samples are almost all clinical rather than community samples, and they are typically studies of adolescents. Because our sample is large enough to include 61 severely obese children, we have the rare opportunity to develop a socio-emotional profile of them, and compare it to the profiles of obese, overweight, and non-overweight children.
Hypothesis.
Whether severely obese, obese, and overweight groups differ from each other, or differ only from non-overweight children, is an exploratory question. However, because a few studies have examined BMI as a continuous variable (e.g., using BMI Z-scores) and found a linear association between adiposity and level of psychosocial distress (e.g., Gibson et al, 2008), we anticipate a linear (or step-function) trend, where the greater the level of weight, the greater the socio-emotional problems.
The Current Study
The nature of the sample for the current study adds to an understanding of childhood obesity for several reasons. First, ours is a community rather than a clinical sample. Second, the sample was gathered from rural elementary schools with a relatively large proportion of American Indian students, both risk factors for obesity (Kumanyika, 1993). Third, we use multiple informants (teachers, classmates, and the children themselves) and ask children directly about their peers rather than assessing stereotypes or hypothetical relationships. Finally, because our sample is large, we have enough children to compare socio-emotional factors among those in severely obese, obese, and overweight categories, something that, to our knowledge, has not been done.
Method
Participants.
Data were gathered as the first wave in the Families and Schools for Health (FiSH) Project, a longitudinal study of two cohorts of children at 29 schools in 20 towns in eight counties with adult obesity rates from 28 to 41%, placing children in those communities at elevated risk for obesity, the focus of the current project. Counties and adult obesity rates from 2005–2009 were Creek (31.3%), Kay (31.7%), Lincoln (28.4%), Logan (31.7%), Noble (40.5%), Pawnee (32.6), Payne (28.9), and Pottawatomie (34.4%; Oklahoma State Department of Health, 2013). Cohort 1 wave 1 data collection took place in Fall 2005 and early Spring 2006; cohort 2 wave 1 data collection took place in Fall 2006 and early Spring 2007. All families with a first grade child were asked to participate in the interview and anthropometric parts of the study. Participation in the FiSH Project averaged 55.6% per first grade class (SD = 18.7%). In total, 1171 students were interviewed; 1186 children completed anthropometric assessments. The schools were rural, with all but two of the 20 towns having a population < 10,000 (2004 to 2006 populations of the two larger towns were between 20,000 and 30,000). The average proportion of children on free or reduced price lunch—a proxy for poverty at the school-level—was 65%. April 1, 2010, United States census data for the eight counties indicate countywide percentages for race or ethnic groups ranged from 74.4% to 84.6% white non-Hispanic, 3.3%% to 12.9% American Indian, 2.0% to 6.4% Hispanic or Latino, and 0.5% to 9.1% African American, with the range of individuals reporting multiracial or multiethnic identity being 4.1% to 6.6%. In 2006, the Oklahoma state poverty rate was 23.5%; county poverty rates for the eight counties with schools participating in the project ranged from 19.1% to 26.1% with four counties above the state poverty rate and four below it.
Procedures
Data collection.
Prior to data collection researchers met with each school principal individually to obtain permission for school involvement. After obtaining consent from school administration, families of first-grade children were recruited from 20 elementary schools in north-central Oklahoma. All rural schools within a 90-mile radius of the university were targeted. If the superintendent had given approval, any principal agreeing was accepted into the study. Superintendents provided verbal approval of the project; principals, teachers, and parents provided written informed consent; and children provided assent before participating in the study.
Parents were recruited at kindergarten graduation (spring), first-grade registration (fall), and back-to-school (fall) events, as well as via send-home letters in children’s backpacks. In late fall or early spring of the children’s first grade year, trained project research assistants (graduate or undergraduate students) completed anthropometric assessments in the elementary school gymnasium and conducted one-on-one interviews in private areas of the school building (e.g., in the library or at the end of a quiet hall). Teachers completed questionnaires during the same period and were financially compensated.
Data collection of covariates occurred as follows: Child sex and ethnicity data were collected on parent questionnaires. If parents did not respond to the child ethnicity question or did not return questionnaires, a graduate student traveled to each school with a form to obtain the child ethnicity data, bringing information on parent provision of consent for demographic data, and obtained the ethnicity data from the school office. Because 58% of parents did not complete the parent questionnaire packet, information on child sex was recorded by research assistants during the one-on-one child interviews.
Data management.
The current report is based solely on data from the first wave of data collection. Data management of the first wave (as approved by the Oklahoma State University Institutional Review Board) included multiple components. Raw data were stored in locked file cabinets in the project lab and signed consent forms in the primary PI’s office. De-identified computer versions of the data were stored on a password-protected drive with a back-up version of the de-identified data stored in a separate co-PI’s office. When student research assistants brought data from the schools to the lab, they locked it in a special cabinet and checked off identification numbers and status of data. The project data manager received the incoming data forms, de-identified them, and readied the data for processing.
Weight Classification
Anthropometric assessments were conducted by trained research assistants at the children’s schools, usually during physical education class the same day as the child interview, or soon thereafter. Children’s height and weight were measured in order to calculate BMI-for-age percentile using Epi Info software (CDC, 2008). Measurements of height were taken two times during the same visit using a portable board to measure each child’s height to the nearest 0.2 cm. If the height was not within ±0.3 cm then it was measured a third time. The average of the height measurements was the value used in analysis. Weight was determined to the nearest ±0.2 pounds using a portable digital scale (Tanita Electronic Scale, BWB-800).
BMI-for-age percentile (BMI%ile) was used to group children into four weight groups (CDC, 2007: severely obese (BMI ≥ 99%ile), obese (95%ile ≤ BMI < 99%ile), overweight (85%ile ≤ BMI < 95%ile), and non-overweight (5%ile ≤ BMI < 85%ile). Children who were underweight (i.e., BMI < 5%ile) were excluded from analyses due to their different health and peer experience profiles compared to both normal-weight and overweight or obese children in past studies and because our focus was on overweight and obese children.
Inter-Personal Characteristics
Child measures.
Child measures of inter-personal experiences were assessed with four measures. These (including measure names and abbreviations) are summarized in Table 1. Briefly, the Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Questionnaire (LSDQ) assesses children’s perception of what their peer relationships at school are like (including friendships and group dynamics) as well as feelings of loneliness at school. The Weight-Related Teasing subscale of the Perception of Peer Teasing Survey (POTS) assesses children’s perception of being teased about their weight (e.g., “Kids pointed at you because you were overweight”). The Harter Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance for Young Children (PCSA) assesses child self-perception by having children identify with a set of hand-drawn figures and has four subscales, including one focused on perceived peer acceptance (e.g., “This girl has lots of friends to play with.”).
Table 1.
Measure | Source | Reporter | Scales Used | Validity | Reliability | Scale |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Loneliness and Social Dissatisfaction Questionnaire (LSDQ) | Asher, Hymel, & Renshaw, 1984 | Child | Full 16 item | Convergence with teacher reported peer problems (Cassidy & Asher, 1992) | α=. 80 | 1 (Not true about you) to 3 (True about you) |
Weight-Related Teasing subscale of the Perception of Peer Teasing Survey (POTS) | Thompson, Cattarin, Fowler, & Fisher, 1995 | Child | Full 4 item | Predictive of body image (Thompson et al., 1995) | α=. 79 | 1 (Never) to 3 (Very often |
Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence and Social Acceptance (PCSA) | Harter & Pike, 1984 | Child | Perceived Peer Acceptance, 6 items | Convergence with actual performance (Harter & Pike, 1984) | α=. 79 | 1 (Most negative) to 4 (Most positive) |
Sociometric Nominations | Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli 1982 | Peer | Social Preference, Social Impact, & Social Descriptors | Prediction of later peer status (McKown, Gumbiner, & Johnson, 2011) | NA | NA |
Perceived Peer Status | Andrade, Waschbusch, King, Thurston, McNutt, & Terrio, 2005 | Teacher | 3 ratings: Well-liked, disliked, teased | Convergence with peer ratings (Andrade et al., 2005) | NA | NA |
Behavior Assessment System for Children, 2nd edition (BASC-2) | Reynolds & Kamphaus, 1992 | Teacher | Child Withdrawal (8 items), Child Somatization (9 items), & Child Depression (11 items) | Convergence with similar measures (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). | α=. 82 α=. 88 α=. 83 |
1 (Never) to 4 (Almost Always) |
Modified Child Depression Inventory (M-CDI) | Modified from Kovacs, 1985 | Child | 4 items | Good model fit statistics of CFA | α=. 62 | 1 (No, not true of you); 2 (Sometimes this is true and sometimes not); 3 (Yes, it is true for you) |
Note. Reported reliability coefficients are calculated for the current sample.
Peer measures.
Peer measures of Inter-Personal Factors included sociometric nominations and ratings. Because children were not necessarily all accurate readers, individual rather than group interviews were conducted, and photographs of participating classmates were used rather than written rosters. Selecting from the photographs of their classmates in a one-on-one interview, children were asked to point to the photographs of three people whom they liked the most and three children they liked the least. Social preference and social impact scores were created from these nominations in accordance with the procedure outlined by Coie et al. (1982). Each of these scores (“liked most” nominations, “liked least” nominations, social preference, and social impact) was standardized by classroom. Using social preference and social impact scores, sociometric statuses were created with children designated as popular (standardized social preference > 1, standardized “liked most” > 0 and standardized “liked least” < 0), average (standardized social preference and social impact values between −0.5 and 0.5), rejected (standardized social impact < −1, standardized “liked most” < 0, and standardized “liked least” > 0), neglected (standardized social impact < −1, = “liked most” = 0), or controversial (standardized social impact > 1, standardized “liked most” > 0 and standardized “liked least” > 0).
During the peer ratings portion of the sociometric interview, children also were asked to pick from among the photographs three children in their class who fit a series of social descriptions (e.g., “Which of the people in your class gets made fun of? ). The number of classmates who nominated each child was tallied for each description and, for comparison across different sized classes, the tallies were standardized as Z-scores.
Teacher measures.
Teachers completed two measures for each child which are included in the Table 1 summary. The perceived peer status measure included three descriptions of a child in the class: “This child is well-liked by his or her peers;” “This child is disliked by his peers;” and “This child is picked on by other kids, and teased by them.” On the Behavior Assessment System for Children (BASC-2), teachers responded to a series of short descriptions of student behavior and attitudes (e.g., “Says, ‘Nobody likes me’”). In the current study, the Child Withdrawal sub-scale was included with the inter-personal variables model.
Intra-personal Characteristics
Child measures.
A modified version of the Child Depression Inventory (CDI, Kovacs, 1985) was used to assess children’s depressive thoughts. On this modified measure (M-CDI), children were asked to rate the frequency of the most severe response from the original CDI (e.g., “Terrible things will happen to me”). Of the original 26 items, 4 were selected as most indicative of the low efficacy, low esteem, and anhedonia aspects of the CDI (Steele et al., 2006) and combined into a single scale. A confirmatory factor analysis with robust maximum likelihood estimation was conducted to verify the proposed scale as indicative of an underlying construct. The Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) for the measurement model was 0.03 (CI =.00; .05); the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) was .03; and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) was .99. The model chi-square (χ 2 (5) = 9,27, p = .10) was non-significant (χ 2 (38) = 50.15, p =.09). Thus, this model exhibited adequate fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Standardized factor loadings ranged from .50 to .57 and were all significant.
Teacher measures.
Subscales reflecting teacher perception of Child Somatization and Child Depression were used from the BASC-2. These scales were developed to indicate internalizing problems for children.
Analytic Approach
Analyses were conducted to reflect the inter-personal and intra-personal socio-emotional experiences of children in the sample. Multiple informants’ (viz., child, peer, and teacher) reports on constructs of interest were then included in MANOVA analyses for inter-personal and intra-personal constructs to determine whether socio-emotional experiences differed across weight groups.
A MANOVA approach was selected to account for the inherent inter-correlations of the variables between and within informants. MANOVA analyses create a combined vector of the dependent variables that allows for a test of group differences on the set of dependent variables as a whole before examination of individual dependent variables. This allows for removal of the shared correlation between dependent variables (i.e., error) and greater ability to detect valid, significant findings. Pairwise multivariate ANOVAs with covariates were conducted as follow-up analyses when omnibus group differences were at statistically significant levels. Cohen’s d is reported as an indicator of effect size when group differences were observed. These values were calculated using the corrected mean square error from the omnibus analyses. When homogeneity between groups was not a tenable assumption as tested by Box’s Test of Equality of Covariance matrices, Pillai’s Trace values for examining significance were used (Olson, 1974). Wilks’ lambda values were used when homogeneity between groups was satisfied. Models included sex (0 = female, 1 = male) and ethnicity as controls. Ethnicity was dummy coded to compare American Indian and other races to a European American referent group.
In addition to the two MANOVAs, a chi-square test of independence was conducted to determine whether the distribution of weight groups was independent from, or related to, sociometric status groups. Significant chi-squares were interpreted based on standardized residual scores for each cell. This determined the contributing sources of significance. Observed values with Z-score beyond +/−1.96 were interpreted as significantly different from the expected values at the 95% confidence level. This serves as a secondary representation of inter-personal social experience and potentially adds to the literature by using a categorical rather than continuous peer-preference measure. A significance level of .05 was adopted for all analyses. Data were cleaned and analyzed in SPSS Version 19 (IBM Corp, 2010).
All available cases with complete data were included in each model (i.e., listwise deletion). In total, 963 cases had complete data for the model with inter-personal variables; teacher-rated child withdrawal had the greatest proportion missing (n = 1,035). For the intra-personal model, 1,101 had complete data for the model with intra-personal variables. Teacher-rated child somatization had the greatest missingness in this model (n =1,131).
RESULTS
Participant Demographics
Children were 49% male and 51% female with a mean age of 6.88 years (SD = .42). Children were 72.6% European American, 18.8% American Indian, 2.1% Multiethnic, and 6.5% “other” race or ethnicity. In comparison to the ethnicity distributions of the eight counties from which our child sample was drawn, we had a higher participation rate by American Indian children and a slightly lower participation rate by children from all other racial groups. Distribution among weight groups was as follows: severely obese, n = 67; obese, n =125; overweight, n = 204; and non-overweight, n = 768. Among the total sample, 22 underweight children were identified and excluded.
Inter-Personal Characteristics
Three-informant multivariate analysis.
For the MANOVA model representative of inter-personal social experience, a significant omnibus effect for weight groups was found suggesting that weight groups significantly differed on the set of independent variables, controlling for sex and ethnicity (Pillai’s Trace = .09, F = 2.49, p <.001). Sex was a significant covariate (Pillai’s Trace = .09, F = 7.97, p <.001), but ethnicity was not significant as a covariate. Pairwise multivariate tests revealed that normal weight and overweight groups were not different on the set of dependent variables; overweight and obese groups were also not different on the set of dependent variables.
However, normal weight and obese groups were significantly different on the set of dependent variables while controlling for ethnicity and sex (Wilks’ λ = .97, F = 2.23, p =.009). Specifically, social preference (F = 7.41, p = .007) and social impact (F = 11.28, p =.001) values were significantly higher for normal weight children than obese children (Cohen’s ds = .10 and .15, respectively).
Normal weight and severely obese children also differed on the set of dependent variables with sex and ethnicity as controls (Pillai’s Trace = .08, F = 5.14, p <.001). Several specific significant differences were found between these groups: perceptions of peer teasing (F = 16.87, p < .001), social preference (F = 17.56, p < .001), social impact (F = 7.07, p = .008), gets made fun of (F = 5.02 p = .03), teacher-rated liking (F = 13.64, p < .001), teacher-rated disliking (F = 18.14, p < .001), teacher-rated being picked on or teased (F = 14.32, p < .001), and teacher-rated withdrawal (F = 11.77, p = .001). For each measure, normal weight children’s scores indicated more positive inter-personal experiences than severely obese children’s. Cohen’s d ranged in absolute value from .10 (teacher-rated withdrawal) to .23 (perceptions of peer teasing, being made fun of, and teacher-rated disliking) for these mean differences.
The overweight group differed significantly from the severely obese group on the set of dependent variables adjusting for sex and ethnicity (Wilks’ λ = .84, F = 3.10, p <.001). Specifically, differences were observed on one self-reported measure, one peer report measure, and all teacher-report measures: perceptions of peer teasing (F = 6.76 p = .01), social preference (F = 10.62, p =.001), teacher-rated liking (F = 13.58, p < .001), teacher-rated disliking (F = 16.38, p < .001), teacher-rated being picked on or teased (F = 10.90, p = .001), and teacher-rated withdrawal (F = 10.20, p = .002). Severely obese children fared worse than overweight children in each of these inter-personal areas. For group differences on these measures, Cohen’s d ranged in absolute value from .11 (teacher-rated withdrawal) to .25 (teacher-rated liking and disliking).
Finally, obese and severely obese groups were significantly different on the set of dependent variables with sex and ethnicity covariates (Wilks’ λ = .83, F = 2.66, p = .003). Specifically, severely obese children had greater perceptions of peer teasing (F = 4.80, p =.03), higher ratings of peer acceptance (F = 6.47, p =.01), and poorer ratings on every teacher measure: liking (F = 6.55, p = .01), disliking (F = 11.31, p = .001), picked on or teased (F = 11.05, p = .001), and withdrawal (F = 6.69, p = .01). Cohen’s d values for these significant differences ranged in absolute value from .09 (teacher-rated withdrawal) to .25 (child-reported peer acceptance). Adjusted means by weight group and results of post hoc pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 2.
Table 2.
Obese | Normal weight | Overweight | Obese | Severely |
---|---|---|---|---|
Child Report | ||||
LSDQ | 1.59 (.02) a | 1.60 (.03) a | 1.62 (.04) a | 1.56 (.05) a |
POTS | 1.26 (.02) a | 1.32 (.04) a | 1.34 (.05) a | 1.55 (.07) b |
PCSA Peer Acceptance | 2.96 (.03) a b | 3.08 (.06) a b | 2.87 (.07) b | 3.15 (.10) a |
Peer Report | ||||
Social preference | .08 (.04) b | .05 (.08) bc | −.19 (.09)ac | −.46 (.12)a |
Social impact | .07 (.04) b | −.03 (.08) ab | −.26 (.09) a | −.28 (.12) a |
Gets made fun of | −.02 (.04) a | −.03 (.08) a b | .05 (.09) a b | .26 (.12) b |
Kids do mean things to | −.02 (.04) a | −.05 (.07) a | .07 (.09) a | .22 (.12) a |
Gets called names by other kids | −.003 (.04) a | .04 (.08) a | −.15 (.09) a | .10 (.13) a |
Teacher Report | ||||
This child is well-liked by his/her peers. | 3.41 (.03) a | 3.44 (.07) a | 3.32 (.08) a | 2.98 (.11) b |
This child is disliked by his/her peers. | 1.89 (.04) a | 1.87 (.08) a | 1.92 (.09) a | 2.48 (.13) b |
This child is picked on by other kids & teased by them | 1.64 (.03) a | 1.65 (.07) a | 1.65 (.08) a | 2.10 (.11) b |
BASC Withdrawal | 1.54 (.02) a | 1.50 (.04) a | 1.55 (.04) a | 1.74 (.06) b |
Notes: Estimated Marginal Means (SEs) in columns adjusted for sex and ethnicity. Homogeneous groups are indicated by superscripts based on pairwise multivariate post-hoc comparisons. That is, significantly different groups have different superscripts.
Sociometric analysis.
Findings from the chi-square test of independence further suggest that children’s sociometric status was not independent of weight classification, χ2(1, n = 1079) = 44.99, p < .000 (see Table 3). Children with severe obesity were significantly more likely to experience rejection from their peers (Z = 3.0) compared to other weight groups and also were significantly less likely to be popular (Z = −2.0). Children at the obese weight status were significantly more likely to be neglected by their peers (Z = 4.2).
Table 3.
Rejected | Neglected | Controversial | Average | Popular | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Normal Weight | −1.2 | −1.9 | 1.1 | .1 | 1.1 |
Overweight | .4 | .1 | .1 | .1 | −.7 |
Obese | .3 | 4.2* | −1.8 | −.3 | −.5 |
Severe obese | 3.0* | .6 | −1.5 | −1. | −2.0* |
p < .05
Intra-Personal Characteristics
For the MANOVA model representative of intra-personal factors, the omnibus effect adjusting for sex and ethnicity was significant, suggesting that weight groups differed on the set of intra-personal factors (Pillai’s Trace = .02= .98, F = 2.66, p = .005). Sex, but not ethnicity, was significant as a covariate (Pillai’s Trace = .05, F = 20.35, p <.001). Findings from subsequent pairwise multivariate tests suggest that normal weight children were not different on the set of dependent variables from overweight or obese children.
Normal weight children were significantly different than severely obese children on the set of dependent variables adjusting for sex and ethnicity (Wilks’ λ = .98, F = 3.96, p = .008). Specifically, severely obese children had higher teacher-rated depression (F = 9.91, p = .002), a mean difference corresponding to a Cohen’s d of .22.
Overweight and obese children were significantly different on the set of dependent variables with sex and ethnicity as covariates (Pillai’s Trace = .04, F = 3.62, p =.01). The source of this effect was due to a significant difference on teacher-rated somatization (F = 8.47, p = .004), with obese children having higher scores (Cohen’s d = .19).
Overweight children also were significantly different from severely obese children on the set of measures (Wilks’ λ = .96, F = 3.09, p =.03). Both teacher-rated measures were significantly different between these groups with severely obese children having higher scores on teacher-rated somatization (F = 6.01, p = .02) and teacher-rated depression (F = 5.59, p = .02). Cohen’s d effect sizes were .19 and .22 for these measures, respectively.
Finally, obese children and severely obese children differed significantly on this set of dependent variables with sex and ethnicity covariates (Pillai’s Trace = .05, F = 2.83, p =.04). The child self-reported M-CDI score (F = 4.89, p = .03) and teacher-rated depression measure (F = 3.93, p = .049) were different between groups with severely obese children having greater indication of intra-personal problems. Cohen’s d effect sizes for these respective measures were .26 and .19. Means by weight level and results of post hoc pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 4.
Table 4.
Normal weight (n = 729) | Overweight (n = 191) | Obese (n = 117) | Severely Obese (n = 64) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Child Report | ||||
Modified CDI | 1.55 (.02)ab | 1.56 (.04)ab | 1.46 (.05)a | 1.66 (.07)b |
Teacher Report | ||||
BASC Somatization | 1.32 (.01)ab | 1.25 (.03)b | 1.38 (.04)a | 1.39 (.05)a |
BASC Depression | 1.30 (.01)a | 1.30 (.02)a | 1.34 (.03)a | 1.43 (.04) b |
Notes: Estimated Marginal Means (SEs) in columns adjusted for sex and ethnicity. Homogeneous groups are indicated by Superscripts based on pairwise multivariate post-hoc comparisons. That is, significantly different groups have different superscripts.
Discussion
The results of the current study offer a clear and consistent message: The socio-emotional lives of children with weight problems differ from those of non-overweight children, even as early as first grade. This finding emerged whether looking at child, peer, or teacher reports, and when looking at social (inter-personal) as well as emotional (intra-personal) variables. While popular-press media has recently been educating the public about the psychosocial consequences of obesity, so far these consequences have not been well-studied empirically. Past studies have tended to focus on self-esteem and body-esteem or clinical-type behavior problems, and have focused mostly on adolescents and older children. Studies that have focused on the social and emotional lives of young school-age children tend not to use multiple informants and often do not ask about actual peer relationships, but rather assess bias or prejudice at the abstract or hypothetical level.
Using multiple informants, we found a good deal of consensus that the children with weight problems are not well-liked and not well-treated, that they struggle with friendships, and that they exhibit somatic complaints and depressive symptoms at higher rates than lesser-weight peers. In general, the higher the weight status, the worse children were doing (the step-wise pattern we tentatively hypothesized), although often obese and severely obese children did not differ. Interestingly, it was teachers who seemed to report distinctions between the obese and severely obese children. It could be that teachers are identifying subtle differences in the treatment of children with different levels of obesity, or the teachers might be biased against severely obese children and therefore their perceptions are skewed by that bias (see, e.g., Lynagh, Cliff, & Morgan, 2015, for evidence of anti-fat attitudes among teachers).
Perhaps the most striking result of the current study is the compelling pattern of social and emotional difficulties being experienced by the severely obese children in our study. Severe obesity is a clear psychosocial risk for children, even as early as six years old. In the past, studies that have found psychosocial risk among obese versus non-overweight children have not analyzed the severely obese and the obese groups separately; it may be that findings from these studies were actually driven by the severely obese group, as were many of our findings. Some researchers studying psychosocial correlates of obesity among adults have found the same trend, for example finding that the link between obesity and depression or anxiety is mostly accounted for by the severely obese portion of an obese sample (e.g., Onyike, Crum, Lee, Lyketsos, & Eaton, 2003). Our findings suggest that this pattern begins in early childhood. The self-perceptions of these obese and severely obese children—who were neglected and rejected by peers—may worsen as they children age: Gable et al. (2009) point out that, across the elementary school years, children’s social-cognitive development results in a more and more sophisticated understanding of stereotypes, and at some point children realize that stereotypes are being applied to them by their peers and by society at large.
Our study advances the current understanding of the social and emotional experiences of overweight and obese children by utilizing a non-clinical sample of young elementary school-aged children. The fact that we used a community sample, not a clinical sample, adds much needed information to the field, given that the predominance of research about severe obesity in childhood has been based on small clinical samples, and has been primarily focused on physical health rather than socio-emotional well-being (see Boschemail, Stradmeijer, & Seidell, 2004, for discussion of clinical vs. community samples and preventative vs. curative approaches). While rates of child obesity are stabilizing somewhat in the U.S., severe obesity is becoming increasingly prevalent, with approximately one in 20 children in the U.S. currently classified as severely obese (Kelly et al., 2013). This may suggest that recent intervention efforts are effective for children at-risk for obesity or with non-morbid obesity, but may not be effective for severely obese children. If true, this is a serious concern, given our evidence of children’s painful experiences with severe obesity, and findings such as increased risk of suicide among populations of severely obese adults (e.g., Chen et al., 2012). The theme that emerged in our data about severely obese children being rejected, made fun of, teased, picked on, and disliked, suggests that interventions aimed at change in the peer group’s behavior (e.g., Harrist & Bradley, 2003) might be necessary to improve severely obese children’s quality of life; these changes might ultimately improve their physical health, as well (see Swindle et al., 2014). Interventions that facilitate friendship formation might also be effective, given that some studies (e.g., Reiter-Purtill, Ridel, Jordan, & Zeller, 2010) have found that an obese child’s having just one friend in a class can buffer the negative effect of poor treatment by peers.
Our findings on weight status and peer rejection and neglect are particularly noteworthy given the evidence that ostracism, a concept that has been used in reference to being both ignored (neglected) and actively excluded (rejected), has an even greater negative effect on children than bullying (Saylor et al., 2013). When examining our sociometric findings, it is interesting to note that severely obese children were significantly more likely to be rejected than their peers, but obese children were significantly more likely to be neglected. To our knowledge, ours is the first study to be able to make this important distinction. Rejected and neglected children are both unpopular, but they have different profiles and outcomes. Rejected children are actively disliked, while neglected children just do not have many friends. Rejected children are more likely to retaliate against peer rebuffs and are more likely to be lonely, depressed, and aggressive than neglected children, who are likely to become increasingly withdrawn when rebuffed by peers (Juvonen & Gross, 2005) and rejected children are more at-risk for future problems such as school drop-out and delinquency (see review by Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). Because of this, intervening with obese children in an effort to prevent a move into the severely obese group might be a worthwhile goal of future interventions.
A limitation of the current study is its correlational nature. Although the participants in our study were young, it is not clear whether weight problems are creating socio-emotional problems, or vice versa. It is probably true that they impact each other in a “vicious cycle” over time. While it seems unlikely that a 6-year-old could have developed severe obesity solely in reaction to peer maltreatment, it is possible to imagine an overweight child withdrawing, engaging in less play and physical activity, and eating for emotional reasons in response to peer teasing, thereby increasing the weight problem.
Another limitation of the current study is the fact that we did not explore the impact of demographic factors. Future research might explore sex as a possible moderator of the link between children’s weight level and socio-emotional characteristics. One study (Griffiths, Wolke, Page, & Horwood, 2006) found, for example, that for some boys (but not girls), obesity can lead to peer popularity, perhaps because it allows them to be physically dominant; that type of dominance may not be as valued (or even tolerated) among girls. Two recent studies suggest that overweight and obese girls’ body esteem (Shriver, Hubbs-Tait, Harrist, Topham, & Page, 2015) and psychosocial functioning (Davison, Schmalz, Young, & Birch, 2008) may be particularly negatively affected by even apparently benign parenting practices such as maternal monitoring. These findings emphasize the need for future research to test how weight-related teasing and other forms of peer exclusion interact with sex in influencing child negative affect.
Socioeconomic status and ethnicity might also interact with weight to predict different socio-emotional outcomes. Ethnic groups may view weight differently and this might impact the peer relations of overweight children. Given our finding of highest risk among severely obese children, it is also noteworthy that severe obesity is not increasing at equal rates among all ethnic groups; among women in the U.S., for example, severe obesity is increasing most rapidly among non-Hispanic blacks and Mexican Americans (Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2012). Similarly, family socioeconomic status is related to childhood obesity in multiple complex ways (e.g., Paeratakul, White, Williamson, Ryan, & Bray, 2002), and therefore might impact the associations among weight, social factors, and emotional experiences of children. Future studies with samples sizes large enough to analyze children with severe obesity as a separate weight classification are encouraged to examine such demographic factors.
When designing intervention programs targeting severely obese children, we encourage researchers and practitioners to start when children are young; behavioral treatments that have not worked for severely obese adolescents have been found to be effective for younger children (e.g., Danielsson, Kowalski, Ekblom, & Marcus, 2012). Also, we encourage interventionists to consider targeting the peer group. Social science experiments (e.g., Tang & Richardson, 2103) find that the “pain of exclusion” can be ameliorated by inclusion experiences if they follow the exclusion; teachers might be able to facilitate this process. Teasing of children with weight problems was noted in our study by teachers, peers, and self-reporting children. Some researchers (e.g., Allen, Byrne, Blair, & Davis, 2006) suggest that teasing about weight—not weight per se—is the cause of overweight children’s socio-emotional difficulties. Given the findings of our study, we endorse strongly the statement made in a recent American Psychological Association publication (Puhl & Peterson, 2014, p. 183), that weight stigma should be “a social justice issue and a public health priority.”
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by the National Institute of Food and Agriculture, U. S. Department of Agriculture, under Agreement No. 05545; Oklahoma Center for the Advancement of Science & Technology, Grant #HR07-044; Oklahoma Agricultural Experiment Station, Grant #2744; Oklahoma State University’s College of Human Sciences and College of Arts & Sciences; and the Bryan Close Professorship; and the Translational Research Institute (TRI), grants UL1TR000039 and KL2TR000063 through the NIH National Center for Research Resources and the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of NIFA or NIH.
Contributor Information
Amanda W. Harrist, Department of Human Development & Family Science, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK
Taren M. Swindle, Department of Family and Preventive Medicine, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, AK
Laura Hubbs-Tait, Department of Human Development & Family Science, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK
Glade L. Topham, Department of Human Development & Family Science, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK
Lenka H. Shriver, Department of Nutrition, University of North Carolina-Greensboro, Greensboro, NC
Melanie C. Page, Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morganton, WV
References
- Allen KL, Byrne SM, Blair EM, & Davis EA (2006). Why do some overweight children experience psychological problems? The role of weight and shape concern. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 1, 239–247. doi: 10.1080/17477160600913552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrade BF, Waschbusch DA, King S, Thurston C, McNutt L, & Terrio B (2005). Teacher-classified peer social status: Preliminary validation and associations with behavior ratings. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23, 279–290. doi: 10.1177/073428290502300306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Asher SR, Hymel S, & Renshaw PD (1984). Loneliness in children. Child Development, 55, 1456–1464. doi: 10.2307/1130015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Booth ML, King LA, Pagnini DL, Wilkenfeld RL, & Booth SL (2009). Parents of school students on childhood overweight: The Weight of Opinion study. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 45, 194–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.2008.01453.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boschemail J, Stradmeijer M, & Seidell J (2004). Psychosocial characteristics of obese children/youngsters and their families: Implications for preventive and curative interventions. Patient Education and Counseling, 55, 353–362. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2003.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braet C, Mervielde I, & Vandereycken W (1997). Psychological aspects of childhood obesity: A controlled study in a clinical and nonclinical sample. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22, 59–71. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/22.1.59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy J, & Asher SR (1992). Loneliness and peer relations in young children. Child Development, 63, 350–365. doi: 10.2307/1131484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Center for Disease Control and Prevention. Epi Info for Windows. [Computer software]. (2008). Available from http://wwwn.cdc.gov/epiinfo
- Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2007). Defining Childhood Overweight. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpa/obesity/childhood/defining.html
- Chen EY, Fettich KC, Tierney M, Cummings H, Berona J, Weissman J, … & Coccaro E (2012). Factors associated with suicide ideation in severely obese bariatric surgery-seeking individuals. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42, 541–549. doi: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00110.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen R, Klesges RC, Summerville M, & Meyers AW (1989). A developmental analysis of the influence of body weight on the sociometry of children. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 473–476. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(89)90036-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coie JD, Dodge KA, & Coppotelli H (1982). Dimensions and types of status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.18.4.557 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Daniels DY (2008). Examining attendance, academic performance, and behavior in obese adolescents. The Journal of School Nursing, 24, 379–387. doi: 10.1177/1059840508324246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danielsson P, Kowalski J, Ekblom Ö, & Marcus C (2012). Response of severely obese children and adolescents to behavioral treatment. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 166, 1103–1108. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison KK, & Birch LL (2002). Processes linking weight status and self-concept among girls from ages 5 to 7 years. Developmental Psychology, 38, 735–738. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.38.5.735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison KK, Schmalz DL, Young LM, & Birch LL (2008). Overweight girls who internalize fat stereotypes report low psychosocial well-being. Obesity, 16(S2), S30–S38. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkelstein EA, Graham WCK, & Malhotra R (2014). Lifetime direct medical costs of childhood obesity. Pediatrics, 133, 854–862. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-0063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freedman DS, Mei Z, Srinivasan SR, Berenson GS, & Dietz WH (2007). Cardiovascular risk factors and excess adiposity among overweight children and adolescents: The Bogalusa Heart Study. Journal of Pediatrics, 150, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2006.08.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gable S, Krull JL, & Chang Y (2009). Implications of overweight onset and persistence for social and behavioral development between kindergarten entry and third grade. Applied Development Science, 13, 88–103. doi: 10.1080/10888690902801970 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson LY, Byrne SM, Blair E, Davis EA, Jacoby P, & Zubrick SR (2008). Clustering of psychosocial symptoms in overweight children. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 42, 118–125. doi: 10.1080/00048670701787560 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gifford-Smith ME, & Brownell CA (2003). Childhood peer relationships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of School Psychology, 41, 235–284. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(03)00048-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gray WN, Kahhan NA, & Janicke DM (2009). Peer victimization and pediatric obesity: A review of the literature. Psychology in the Schools, 46, 720–727. doi: 10.1002/pits.20410 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths LJ, Wolke D, Page AS, & Horwood JP (2006). Obesity and bullying: Different effects for boys and girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 91, 121–125. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.072314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnarsdottir T, Njardvik U, Olafsdottir AS, Craighead LW, & Bjarnason R (2012). Teasing and social rejection among obese children enrolling in family-based behavioural treatment: Effects on psychological adjustment and academic competencies. International Journal of Obesity, 36, 35–44. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halfon N,. Larson K, & Slusser W (2013). Associations between obesity and comorbid mental health, developmental, and physical health conditions in a nationally representative sample of US children aged 10 to 17. Academic Pediatrics. 13, 6–13. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2012.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrington DM, Staiano AE, Broyles ST, Gupta AK, & Katzmarzyk PT (2013). BMI percentiles for the identification of abdominal obesity and metabolic risk in children and adolescents: evidence in support of the CDC 95th percentile. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 67, 218–22. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2012.203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrist AW, & Bradley KD (2003). “You can’t say you can’t play”: Intervening in the process of social exclusion in the kindergarten classroom. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 18, 185–205. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2006(03)00024-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Harrist AW, Topham GL, Hubbs-Tait L, Page MC, Kennedy TS, & Shriver LH (2012). What developmental science can contribute to a multidisciplinary understanding of childhood obesity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 445–465. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hayden-Wade HA, Stein RI, Ghaderi A, Saelens BE, Zabinski MF, & Wilfley DE (2005). Prevalence, characteristics, and correlates of teasing experiences among overweight children vs. non-overweight peers. Obesity Research, 13, 1381–1392. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helfert S, &Warschburger P (2013). The face of appearance-related social pressure: Gender, age and body mass variations in peer and parental pressure during adolescence. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 7, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-7-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. (2010). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 19.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine (2012). IOM report identifies key obesity-prevention strategies to scale back ‘Weight of the Nation’ (Press release). Retrieved from http://www8.nationalacademies.org/onpinews/newsitem.aspx?RecordID=13275
- Juvonen J, & Gross EF (2005). The rejected and the bullied: lessons about social misfits from developmental psychology In Williams KD, Forgas JP, & Von Hippel W (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 155–170). New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kellou N, Sandalinas F, Copin N, & Simon C (2014). Prevention of unhealthy weight in children by promoting physical activity using a socio-ecological approach: what can we learn from intervention studies? Diabetes & Metabolism, 40, 258–71. doi: 10.1016/j.diabet.2014.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly AS, Barlow SE, Rao G, Inge TH, Hayman LL, Steinberger J, … & Daniels SR (2013). Severe obesity in children and adolescents: Identification, associated health risks, and treatment approaches. Circulation, 128, 1689–1712. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e3182a5cfb3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovacs M (1985). The Children’s Depression Inventory. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995–998. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumanyika SK (1993). Special issues regarding obesity in minority populations. Annals of Internal Medicine, 119, 650–654. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-119-7_Part_2-199310011-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latner JD, Stunkard AJ, & Wilson GT (2005). Stigmatized students: age, sex, and ethnicity effects in the stigmatization of obesity. Obesity Research, 13, 1226–1231. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim CS, Graziano PA, Janicke DM, Gray WN, Ingerski LM, & Silverstein JH (2011). Peer victimization and depressive symptoms in obese youth: The role of perceived social support. Children’s Health Care, 40, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/02739615.2011.537929 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lynagh M, Cliff K, & Morgan PJ (2015), Attitudes and beliefs of nonspecialist and specialist trainee health and physical education teachers toward obese children: Evidence for “anti-fat” bias. Journal of School Health, 85, 595–603. doi: 10.1111/josh.12287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marder W, & Chang S (2006) Childhood obesity: Costs, treatment patterns, disparities in care, and prevalent medical conditions. Thomson Medstat Research Brief. Retrieved from www.medstat.com/pdfs/childhood_obesity.pdf
- McKown C, Gumbiner LM, & Johnson J (2011). Diagnostic efficiency of several methods of identifying socially rejected children and effect of participation rate on classification accuracy. Journal of School Psychology, 49, 573–595. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2011.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen JV, Robbins JM, Houck KL, Nobis EA, Inman KA, Khan KS, & Robbins SW (2014). Severe obesity and high blood pressure among children, Philadelphia health centers, 2010. Journal of Primary Care Community Health, 5, 152–155. doi: 10.1177/2150131913506437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Curtin LR, Lamb MM, & Flegal KM (2010). Prevalence of high body mass index in US children and adolescents, 2007–2008. Journal of the American Medical Association, 303, 242–249. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, & Flegal KM (2012). Prevalence of obesity and trends in body mass index among US children and adolescents, 1999–2010. Journal of the American Medical Association, 307, 483–490. doi: 10.1001/jama.2012.40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oklahoma State Department of Health. (2013, August). Chronic disease in Oklahoma data book. Chronic Disease Service, Oklahoma State Department of Health, Oklahoma City, OK: Retrieved from http://www.ok.gov/health2/documents/CDS-Chronic%20data%20book%20AUG2013.pdf [Google Scholar]
- Olson CL (1974). Comparative robustness of six tests in multivariate analysis of variance. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 69, 894–908. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1974.10480224 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Onyike, Crum RM, Lee HB, Lyketsos CG, & Eaton WW (2003). Is obesity associated with major depression? Results from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. American Journal of Epidemiology, 158, 1139–1147. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paeratakul S, White MA, Williamson DA, Ryan DH, & Bray GA (2002). Sex, race/ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and BMI in relation to self-perception of overweight. Obesity Research, 10, 345–350. doi: 10.1038/oby.2002.48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips RG, & Hill AJ (1998). Fat, plain, but not friendless: Self-esteem and peer acceptance of obese pre-adolescent girls. International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 22, 287–293. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0800582 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce JW, & Wardle J (1997). Cause and effect beliefs and self-esteem of overweight children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 645–650. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01691.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puhl RM, Latner JD, O’Brien K, Luedicke J, Forhan M, & Danielsdottir S (2015). Cross-national perspectives about weight-based bullying in youth: Nature, extent and remedies. Pediatric Obesity. (Online version before publication.) doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puhl RM, & Peterson JL (2014). The nature, consequences, and public health implications of obesity stigma In Corrigan PW (Ed), The stigma of disease and disability: Understanding causes and overcoming injustices (pp. 183–203). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/14297-010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pulgarón ER (2013). Childhood obesity: A review of increased risk for physical and psychological comorbidities. Clinical Therapeutics, 35, A18–A32. doi: 10.1016/j.clinthera.2012.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qualter P, Murphy SM, Abbott J, Gardner KJ, Japel C, Vitaro F, … & Tremblay RE (2015). Developmental associations between victimization and body mass index from 3 to 10 years in a population sample. Aggressive Behavior. doi: 10.1002/AB.21580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiter-Purtill J, Ridel S, Jordan R, & Zeller MH (2010). The benefits of reciprocated friendships for treatment-seeking obese youth. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 35, 905–914. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsp140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CR, & Kamphaus RW (1992). BASC: Behavior Assessment System for Children: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CR, & Kamphaus RW (2004). BASC-2 Behavioral Assessment System for Children Manual (2nd Edition). Bloomington, MN: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Saylor CF, Williams KD, Nida SA, McKenna ME, Twomey KE, & Macias MM (2013). Ostracism in pediatric populations: Review of theory and research. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 34, 279–287. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3182874127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shriver LH, Hubbs-Tait L, Harrist AW, Topham G, & Page M (2015). Child gender and weight status moderate the relation of maternal feeding practices to body esteem in 1st grade children. Appetite, 89, 62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skelton JA, Cook SR, Auinger P, Klein JD, & Barlow SE (2009). Prevalence and trends of severe obesity among U.S. children and adolescents. Academic Pediatrics, 9, 322–329. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2009.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele RG, Little TD, Ilardi SS, Forehand R, Brody GH, & Hunter HL (2006). A confirmatory comparison of the factor structure of the Children’s Depression Inventory between European American and African American youth. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 15, 773–788. doi: 10.1007/s10826-006-9054-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, & Ledley DR (2005). Peer victimization and psychosocial adjustment in children: Current knowledge and future directions. Clinical Pediatrics, 44, 29–38. doi: 10.1177/000992280504400103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strauss C, Smith K, Frame C, & Forehand R (1985). Personal and inter-personal characteristics associated with childhood obesity. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 10, 337–343. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/10.3.337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su W, & Aurelia DS (2012). Preschool children’s perceptions of overweight peers. Journal of Early Childhood Research, 10, 19–31. doi: 10.1177/1476718X11407411 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Swindle T, Harrist AW, Rutledge JM, Topham GL, Hubbs-Tait L, Shriver LH, & Page MC (2014). Classroom intervention to decrease peer rejection improves obese children’s BMI over time. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 46, S133. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2014.04.098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tang HHY, & Richardson R (2103). Reversing the negative psychological sequelae of exclusion: Inclusion is ameliorative but not protective against the aversive consequences of exclusion. Emotion, 13, 139–150. doi: 10.1037/a0029521 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SA, Garland BH, Sanchez-Fournier BE, Allen KF, Doak JS, & Wiemann CM (2013). A qualitative study of the day-to-day lives of obese Mexican-American adolescent females. Pediatrics, 131, 1132–1138. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson JK, Cattarin J, Fowler B, & Fisher E (1995). The Perception of Teasing Scale: A revision and extension of the Physical Appearance Related Teasing Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 65, 146–157. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6501_11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turer CB, Lin H, & Flores G (2013). Health status, emotional/behavioral problems, health care use, and expenditures in overweight/obese U.S. children/adolescents. Academic Pediatrics, 13, 251–258. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2013.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van den Berg P, Neumark-Sztainer D, Eisenberg M,E, & Haines J (2008). Racial/ethnic differences in weight-related teasing in adolescents. Obesity, 16(Suppl. 2): S3–S10. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Varni JW, Limbers CA, & Burwinkle TM (2007). How young can children reliably and validly self-report their health-related quality of life? Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5, 1 Retrieved from http://www.hqlo.com/content/5/1/1. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-5-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang SS, Houshyar S, & Prinstein MJ (2006). Adolescent girls’ and boys’ weight-related health behaviors and cognitions: Associations with reputation-and preference-based peer status. Health Psychology, 25, 658. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wentzel KR (2003). Sociometric status and adjustment in middle school: A longitudinal study. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 23, 5–28. doi: 10.1177/0272431602239128 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Williams NA, Fournier J, Coday M, Richey PA, Tylavsky FA, & Hare ME (2013). Body esteem, peer difficulties and perceptions of physical health in overweight and obese urban children aged 5 to 7 years. Child: Care, Health and Development, 39, 825–834. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2012.01401.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Hyman D, Tanofsky-Kraff M, Yanovski SZ, Keil M, Cohen ML, Peyrot M, & Yanovski JA (2006). Psychological status and weight-related distress in overweight or at-risk for overweight children. Obesity, 14, 2249–2258. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeller MH, Reiter-Purtill J, & Ramey C (2008). Negative peer perceptions of obese children in the classroom environment. Obesity, 16, 755–762. doi 10.1038/o [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]