Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is frequently associated with anxiety and hyperarousal. While the pathological changes in the noradrenergic system in ASD are not entirely clear, a number of functional indices of the sympathetic/parasympathetic balance are altered in individuals with ASD, often with a high degree of inter-individual variability. The neuropsychopharmacological effects of α2 agonists and β-adrenergic antagonists make agents targeting these receptors of particular interest. α2 agonists have shown beneficial effects for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and in other domains in individuals with ASD, but effects on core ASD symptoms are less clear. Case series and single dose psychopharmacological challenges suggest that treatment with β-adrenergic antagonists has beneficial effects on language and social domains. Additionally, psychophysiological markers and premorbid anxiety may predict response to these medications. As a result, β-adrenergic antagonists are currently being utilized in a clinical trial for improving core symptoms as well as anxiety in individuals with ASD.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social communication impairments and restricted, repetitive behaviors.1 Pharmacological intervention can be an important component of treatment for ASD symptoms. However, most currently available agents target psychiatric symptoms associated with ASD. For example, antipsychotic medications target irritability and agitation,2 and are the only agents approved by the Food and Drug Adminstration for ASD. New pharmacological approaches that can impact core symptoms of ASD would represent a significant advance. We will start by reviewing what is known about the impact of the noradrenergic system on cognition, focusing on the effects of targeting α2 and β-adrenergic receptors and then turn to ASD, reviewing what is known about the noradrenergic system in ASD and then discussing how this might relate to pharmacotherapy in ASD.
Effects in non-ASD Individuals
Effects of noradrenergic agents on cognition
Norepinephrine is a critical component of the arousal mechanism.3–5 The locus coeruleus contains a majority of the noradrenergic neurons in the central nervous system and sends extensive efferents throughout the brain.6 The prefrontal cortex, which is believed to be important for various types of cognitive flexibility,7–11 has afferent projections to the locus coeruleus in primates.12 A range of other cognitive effects have also been described with noradrenergic agents, including effects on motor learning,13 response inhibition,14 working memory and emotional memory.15
α2 adrenergic agonist effects in non-ASD individuals
In the periphery, α2 adrenergic agonists inhibit the release of norepinephrine presynaptically, which suggests that they would have a similar effect as the postsynaptic β-adrenergic antagonists. However, α2 agonists appear to have distinct cognitive effects. High-dose clonidine, an α2 agonist, has been shown to improve immediate spatial memory in aged monkeys,16,17 an effect also found in younger monkeys,18 and believed to be mediated by action in the prefrontal cortex.19 Lower doses of clonidine, those that are typically utilized clinically in humans, demonstrate varying results at varying doses, resulting in impaired visual working memory, increased impulsive responses on planning tasks, and varying effects on spatial working memory.20,21 Pharmacological stimulation of the postsynaptic α2A subtype of adrenoreceptors enhances the ‘signal’ in the signal-to-noise ratio, resulting in beneficial effects for attention deficit disorder patients.22 However, α2 agonists do not appear to have the same effect on creative verbal problem-solving as β-adrenergic antagonists,23 as will be described below.
β-adrenersic antagonist effects in non-ASD individuals
Research involving adolescents with stress-induced cognitive impairment without neurodevelopmental diagnoses demonstrated that treatment with the β-adrenergic antagonist propranolol significantly improved scores on the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT).24 This finding suggests a role of the noradrenergic system in stress-related modulation of performance in some types of problem solving in certain individuals. Propranolol has also demonstrated efficacy in stress-induced impairment in performance on other tasks including public speaking in anxiety prone individuals.25,26
Several early explorations into the mechanism of action of this effect utilized verbal problem solving tasks. One of these, the anagram task, had been widely used in studies of anxiety, demonstrating a decrement in performance in anxious subjects,27–29 and has furthermore been proposed as a marker of anxiety,30 suggesting its utility in research on the influence of stress. An increase in activity of the noradrenergic system is known to occur in the setting of stress,31,32 and situational stressors have been shown to impair performance on other tests of cognitive flexibility.33 Therefore the anagram task was utilized in studies investigating the effects of noradrenergic agents on network flexibility in verbal problem solving in individuals without a history of neurodevelopmental disorders. Anagram performance was found to be significantly better after administration of β-adrenergic antagonists (propranolol) than after noradrenergic agonists (ephedrine).34
Follow-up studies demonstrated that noradrenergic modulation of these aspects of cognitive flexibility appears to be mediated by a central mechanism rather than a peripheral mechanism, since performance is significantly better after propranolol (central and peripheral β-adrenergic antagonist) than after nadolol (peripheral-only β-adrenergic antagonist).35 A central-only mechanism would be predicted by the modulatory effect of norepinephrine on the signal-to-noise ratio of neuronal activity within the cortex,36 and the correlation between electronic coupling of noradrenergic neurons in the monkey with proportions of goal-directed versus exploratory behaviors.37
However, in each of these studies involving anagrams, whereas performance on propranolol was significantly better than on ephedrine or nadolol, it did not significantly differ from placebo.34,35 Subsequent research demonstrated that propranolol is beneficial for network flexibility in problem solving particularly when the subject is struggling with the problem38 This might be expected since greater flexibility would be required for such situations where a greater network search is needed, and can actually impair performance when subjects are solving problems with ease38 However, in patients where noradrenergic activity is upregulated, such as in cocaine withdrawal, propranolol benefits performance on the simplest problems39,40
Further research that examined the interaction between propranolol and the cognitive effect of stress in individuals without any history of anxiety-related disorders revealed that propranolol reverses the impairment in anagram performance as well as other tasks involving semantic network flexibility under conditions of stress.41 Propranolol can also benefit performance on verbal problem-solving for the easiest problems in situations where there is a physiological or anatomical alteration of the language network that results in a loss of flexibility (e.g. Broca’s aphasia due to stroke).42 Therefore, this pharmacology-stress interaction effect on cognition may represent a fundamental aspect of cognition in typical individuals and does not require the presence of an anxiety-related disorder or noradrenergic dysregulation.43
The relationship between noradrenergic tone and performance on creative verbal problem-solving tasks can also be observed in the performance impacts of alterations in noradrenergic tone induced by changes in posture,44 arousal from different sleep phases associated with differing noradrenergic tone,45 and vagal nerve stimulation.46 Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, a state associated with decreased noradrenergic activity, enhances integration of weakly associated information for creative problem-solving,45,47 and is associated with high levels of spontaneous thought akin to those experienced in waking restful states.48 These effects appear to be specific to the noradrenergic system and is not due to general anti-anxiety effects, since such cognitive effects do not appear to occur with non-adrenergic anxiolytics.49
While tasks such as anagrams involve a search through a wide network in order to identify a solution (“unconstrained flexibility”), other cognitive flexibility tasks such as the Wisconsin Cart Sort Test50 involve set-shifting between a limited range of options (“constrained flexibility”). These two aspects of flexibility may not be modulated by the noradrenergic system in the same manner, with constrained flexibility possibly benefiting from increased noradrenergic activity.37,51 Specifically, as described above, decreased noradrenergic activity appears to benefit tasks such as anagrams when subjects are struggling or challenged by stressors,38,41 whereas increased set switching on a two alternative forced choice task is associated with increased noradrenergic tone in primate studies.37,51
‘Constrained’ flexibility can be further divided into intradimensional and extradimensional set-shifting.11 The dopaminergic system appears to affect intradimensional set-shifting,11 while the noradrenergic system, specifically by action on the α1 receptor, appears to modulate performance on extradimensional set-shifting.11,52 β adrenergic receptors in the noradrenergic system appear to modulate the ‘unconstrained’ flexibility.34,35,41 A systematic exploration of the effects of the noradrenergic system on intradimensional and extradimensional set-shifting as well as creative problem-solving is needed to better characterize these contrasting effects. Exploration of such comparisons has been initiated in an animal model, revealing no effects of β-adrenergic antagonists on reversal learning, intradimensional set-shifting, or extradimensional set-shifting. However, β-adrenergic show a significant benefit for the rodent required to shift to a highly novel solution in order to obtain reward.53
Whereas the anatomical pathways by which the brain utilizes network flexibility in problem solving, such as in anagrams, are not yet understood, the frontal lobes appear to play a crucial role.7–10 In general, frontal brain regions, possibly localized to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, may guide the search by selective engagement of the posterior brain regions relevant to the type of problem being solved. Electroencephalographic data is supportive of a strong frontal-posterior network as evidenced by the strong coherence of tracings of such regions during “creative” tasks in various modalities.54 Further support for this comes from evidence that right frontal lesions impair strategy shifting ability in patients whereas parietal lesions result in a general visuospatial information processing impairment in patients using a spatial task derived from an unconstrained visuospatial flexibility task, the Matchstick Test of Cognitive Flexibility.55,56 Furthermore, the recently reported effects of transcranial direct-current stimulation on frontal regions have demonstrated effects on creativity task performance,57–59 which relates to critical underlying regional neuropharmacological mechanisms. However, regional effects within the frontal lobes are important, since relative deactivation is observed in the dorsolateral prefrontal and lateral orbital regions with activation of frontal polar regions during jazz improvisation,60 suggesting that it is actually disengagement by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that is important. Propranolol might be expected to have a modulatory effect the interactions between these anterior as well as posterior brain regions.
Less is known about the specific cognitive effects of β1 and β2 adrenergic receptors. However, in one animal study, endogenous β1 selective activation impaired working memory.61 A subsequent study demonstrated that β2 selective agonists enhance working memory in aging animals,62 suggesting opposing effects between β1 and β2 receptors on working memory, and explaining the lack of effect of the non-specific β-antagonist propranolol on working memory in previous research.63,64 Further research will be necessary to better understand the specific cognitive effects due to action at selective subtypes of β-adrenergic (β1 and β2) receptors.
Noradrenergic system, cognition, and autism
The norardrenergic system in ASD
Agents that decrease activity of the noradrenergic system have bee-+n used for anxiolytic and behavioral purposes in ASD. Early reports indicated benefits in language and social behaviors in a consecutive case series of individuals with ASD treated with β-adrenergic antagonists.65 Early reports also indicated benefits with other agents that act on the noradrenergic system. α2 adrenergic agonists, drugs which act to presynaptically inhibit norepinephrine release, improve hyperactivity, impulsivity, hyperarousal and social relationships in double-blinded placebo-controlled crossover trials in individuals with ASD. 66,67 In fact, marked improvement in behavior and verbal response has been reported in one case.68 A number of researchers have demonstrated findings suggestive of increased noradrenergic activity in ASD, including increased plasma epinephrine and norepineprine,69–70 and altered urinary excretion of various catecholaminergic metabolites.71,72 However, there may be a number of alternative explanations for these findings. Subsequent studies demonstrating no abnormalities in basal noradrenergic functioning have led to the suggestion that increased reactivity to clinical procedures such as blood drawing and urine collection in ASD may have led to the earlier atypical findings.73 Furthermore, pathology is not found in the volume, cell counts, or cell density in postmortem tissue from the locus coeruleus in ASD.74 Others, though, have proposed that the behavioral effects of fever in ASD75 may be related to normalization of a developmentally dysregulated noradrenergic system.76
While the nature of neuropathological changes of the noradrenergic system in ASD remain a question, there are a number of other lines of evidence that at least support autonomic dysregulation. For example, development of the autonomic nervous system function, as assessed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia, appears to follow a delayed trajectory in ASD.77 Also, autonomic nervous system responses have been proposed as markers for joint attention in ASD,78 and individuals with ASD have blunted responses to social evaluative stressors.79 Regardless of the ambient activity of the noradrenergic system in ASD, though, other evidence suggests a potential for benefit from noradrenergic blockade in ASD, as will be discussed below.
α2 adrenergic agonist treatment in ASD
As stated above, early reports suggested improvements in hyperactivity, impulsivity, hyperarousal, and social relationships in small double-blinded placebo-controlled crossover trials of clonidine,66–67 and with marked improvement in behavior and verbal response in one reported case.68 However, beneficial effects have been primarily observed for ADHD in ASD since these early reports. Subsequent small open label studies of clonidine, widely used for ADHD,80–81 demonstrated beneficial effects on sleep as well as attention deficits, hyperactivity, and aggression.82 Similar effects on ADHD were also observed for another α2 agonist, guanfacine, in another small double-blinded placebo-controlled crossover trial.83 The noradrenergic reuptake inhibitor atomoxetine, also widely used for ADHD, has also demonstrated benefit for ADHD in ASD.84 However, individuals with ASD and ADHD tend to be less responsive to ADHD treatment than individuals with ADHD without ASD.85
β-adrenersic agonist treatment in ASD
Hope for possible targeting of the core aspects of ASD may come from β-adrenergic antagonists. A range of theories have been proposed to account for the core features of autism, including inability to comprehend the perspectives of others (“theory of mind”),86 inability to utilize context in understanding the environment (“central coherence”),87,88 inability to process emotional information,89–92 impaired executive function,93–95 and global/local processing biases towards the local,96 among others. As a manifestation of decreased utilization of context, research has supported the hypothesis that individuals with ASD have a restriction of flexibility of access to the semantic network, including decreased semantic clustering in verbal memory,97 and a failure to utilize syntactic and semantic context when recalling words.98–100 False memories can be induced in typically developing individuals by providing semantic and associative relationship in word lists and visuospatial task. Individuals with ASD perform superior on such ‘false memory’ task because they don’t use context.101,92,102 Individuals with ASD do utilize semantic information to some extent in memory performance, as is demonstrated by previous research including other ‘false memory’ studies,106 but not to the same degree as typical subjects. Research using functional connectivity fMRI (fcMRI) has demonstrated a potential neural substrate for such decreased semantic network flexibility by showing a decrease in the interrelation between active brain regions in ASD. This was initially described in fMRI studies performed during sentence comprehension tasks,103 in addition to executive function tasks,104 believed to be related to the under-connectivity between distant cortical regions in ASD.105
Thus, an agent that could affect the semantic network and other information networks might be of benefit in ASD. Therefore, we examined the effects of pharmacological agents that might impact network flexibility among individuals with ASD. In our early work, we examined the effect of propranolol on network flexibility in high-functioning adults with ASD using simple verbal problem solving tasks. We did not expect individuals without neurodevelopmental diagnoses to benefit.38 Due to their decreased flexibility of access to networks, we expected that individuals with ASD would have a selective performance benefit from propranolol.
As expected, based on our previous examination of the interaction between task difficulty and the effects of propranolol on network access in verbal problem solving,38 our pilot study demonstrated an impairment in performance on these simple anagrams with propranolol in individuals without neurodevelopmental diagnoses. However, as indicated by a significant drug by group interaction, the effect of drug on performance in individuals with ASD was significantly different from controls, with ASD subjects performing better with propranolol on the same task.107 This finding was present despite no significant difference between groups in age, IQ or overall performance on the anagrams in the placebo condition. Early evidence suggested that propranolol increases functional connectivity in ASD. As performance in individuals with ASD is believed to be related to the restricted flexibility in the language network in ASD,109 these fMRI data lend some support to the proposed mechanism of action of propranolol on network access,108 However, in subsequent studies, propranolol was found to have diverging effects on resting state connectivity within various subregions of the default mode network.110
Subsequent small single dose, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover psychopharmacological challenge studies revealed improved performance on semantic fluency tasks,111 and working memory tasks.112 More recently, these benefits in ASD with propranolol have also been observed in the social domain, including decreased time spending looking at mouths (despite typical time spent looking at eyes in this high functioning population),113 and improved performance on a structured conversational task.114 Additionally, a subsequent study has revealed that baseline anxiety and psychophysiological markers predict response to propranolol on verbal problem solving in ASD.115 A recent review of the literature has suggested propranolol as a promising agent for emotional, behavioral, and autonomic dysregulation in ASD,116 but randomized controlled trials are still lacking.
As described above, previous work has demonstrated that whereas effects of propranolol on the networks in verbal problem solving can be difficult to detect in individuals without neurodevelopmental diagnoses,34,35,38 a beneficial effect of the drug occurs in the setting of a psychosocial stressor,41 and cocaine withdrawal,40 believed to be due to noradrenergic upregulation, also observed in individuals without such stressors when encountering more difficult problems.38 Whereas it is not certain whether norepinephrine is upregulated in ASD,69–74 or whether the restriction is more connectomic in nature,105 our preliminary findings begin to suggest that propranolol also has some benefit for performance of the hyper-restrictive networks proposed by network models of ASD, as is characterized in Figure 1.109,117,118 As a result, we are currently performing a double-blind, placebo controlled clinical trial to examine the effect of serial doses of propranolol in ASD, targeting the social domain, and secondarily language and anxiety, while determining whether baseline psychophysiological measures, fcMRI, or baseline anxiety symptomatology predict best responders (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02871349).
Future directions
While the current clinical trial will begin to answer whether propranolol has a possible role in the treatment of core symptoms of ASD, a number of questions will remain. Further research also will need to investigate related noradrenergic or anxiolytic agents for their role in ASD in this setting. Additionally, as the participants in the present trial were sufficiently high functioning to participate in the social and language assessments, it will remain to be determined the potential for benefit from such agents in lower functioning patients. Furthermore, as this agent is already widely used in infants to treat infantile hemangioma,119 this also facilitates the possibility of exploring its effect in a younger patient population during early brain development when neuroplasticity is high. The possibility of combining propranolol with early behavioral interventions may also be a very promising approach.
The potential effects of propranolol on comorbid conditions associated with ASD may also be of interest, given, for example, the alterations observed in sympathetic/parasympathetic balance and stress reactivity associated with gastrointestinal symptomatology in ASD.120,121 Finally, the role of this class of agents in managing aggressive behaviors warrants further exploration, for patients with whom this is an issue, as benefits have been suggested in other patient populations,122–125 and previous findings have suggested that this may extend to ASD patients.65,126 The potential for helping to manage behaviors while minimizing the adverse effects that are associated with other medications currently utilized in this setting is promising. A trial to investigate this aspect has recently been funded and is currently being planned by other investigators.
Acknowledgements:
This research was funded by NINDS (K23 NS43222-Beversdorf) and a pilot grant from NAAR (National Alliance for Autism Research), HRSA (1R40MC19926), DOD (AR150091)
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
REFERENCES
- 1.American Psychological Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-V) Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Myers SM, Johnson CP. Management of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 120:1162–1182; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Smith A, Nutt D. Noradrenaline and attention lapses. Nature 380:291; 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Coull JT, Frith CD, Dolan RSJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Grasby PM. The neural correlates of the noradrenergic modulation of human attention, arousal and learning. Eur J Neurosci 9:589–598; 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Coull JT, Jones MEP, Egan TD, Frith CD, Maze M. Attentional effects of noradrenaline vary with arousal level: selective activation of thalamic pulvinar in humans. NeuroImage 22:315–322; 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Barnes CA, Pompeiano M. Neurobiology of the locus coeruleus. Prog Brain Res 88:307–321; 1991.1687619 [Google Scholar]
- 7.Vikki J Cognitive flexibility and mental programming after closed head injuries and anterior and posterior cerebral excisions. Neuropsychologia 30:807–814; 1992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Karnath HO, Wallesch CW. Inflexibility of mental planning: a characteristic disorder with prefrontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 30:1011–1016; 1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Eslinger PJ, Grattan LM. Frontal lobe and frontal-striatal substrates for different forms of human cognitive flexibility. Neuropsychologia 31:17–28; 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Duncan J, Burgess P, Emslie H. Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 33:261–268; 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Robbins TW. Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Phil Trans Royal Soc London. Series B: Biol Sci 362:917–932; 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Selective prefrontal cortical projections to the region of the locus coeruleus and raphe nuclei in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 306:9–18; 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Foster DJ, Good DC, Fowlkes A, Sawaki L. Atomoxetine enhances a short-term model of plasticity in humans. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87:216–221; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Clark L, Robbins TW, Sahakian B. Neurochemical modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in humans. Science 311:861–863; 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian B. Noradrenergic modulation of working memory and emotional memory in humans. Psychopharmacology 188:397–407; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Arnsten AFT, Cai JX, Goldman-Rakic PS. The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: evidence for alpha-2 receptor subtypes. J Neurosci 8: 4287–4298; 1988 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Arnsten AFT, Leslie FM. Behavioral and receptor binding analysis of the alpha-2 adrenergic agonist, 5-bromo-6 [2-imidazoline-2-yl amino] quinoxaline (uk-14204): evidence for cognitive enhancement at an alpha-2-adrenoreceptor subtype. Neuropsychopharmacol 30: 1279–1289; 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Franowicz JS, Arnsten AFT. Treatment with the noradrenergic alpha-2 agonist clonidine, but not diazepam, improves spatial working memory in normal rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 21: 611–621; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Li BM, Mao ZM, Wang M, Mei ZT. Alpha-2 adrenergic modulation of prefrontal cortical neuronal activity related to spatial working memory in monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacol 21:601–610; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Coull JT, Middleton HC, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Contrasting effects of clonidine and diazepam on tests of working memory and planning. Psychopharmacol 120:311–321; 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jäkälä P, Riekkinen M, Sirvi J, Koivisto E, Kejonen K, Vanhanen M, Riekkinen P Jr. Guanfacine, but not clonidine, improves planning and working memory performance in humans. Neuropsychopharmacol 20: 460–470; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Brennan AR, Arnsten ADT. Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129:236–245; 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Choi Y, Novak J, Hillier A, Votolato NA, Beversdorf DQ. The effect of a-2 adrenergic agonist on memory and cognitive flexibility. Cogn Behav Neurol 19:204–207; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Faigel HC. The effect of beta blockade on stress-induced cognitive dysfunction in adolescents. Clin Pediatrics 30:441–445; 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lader M Beta-adrenergic antagonists in neuropsychiatry: an update. J Clin Psychiatry 49:213–223; 1988. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Laverdue B, Boulenger JP. Medications beta-bloquantes et anxiete. Un interet therapeutique certain. [Beta-blocking drugs and anxiety. A proven therapeutic value.] L’Encephale 17:481–492; 1991. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Harleston BW, Smith MG, Arey D. Test-anxiety level, heart rate, and anagram problem solving. J Person Soc Psychology 1:551–557; 1965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tomasini J Effect of peer-induced anxiety on a problem-solving task. Psychol Rep 33:355–358; 1973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dey MK. Anagram solution speed as a joint function of manifest anxiety and number of category sets. Am J Psycho logy 91:81–88; 1978. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Thyer BA, Papsdorf ID. Discriminant and concurrent validity of two commonly used measures of test anxiety. Educ Psychol Meas 42:1197–1204; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ward MM, Metford IN, Parker SD, Chesney MA, Taylor CB, Keegan DL, Barchas JD. Epinephrine and norepinephrine responses in continuously collected human plasma to a series of stressors. Psychosomat Med 45:471–486; 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kvetnansky R, Pacak K, Sabban EL, Kopin IJ, Goldstein DS. Stressor specificity of peripheral catecholaminergic activation. Adv Pharmacol 42:556–60; 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Martindale C Greenough J The differential effect of increased arousal on creative and intellectual performance. J Genet Psychol 123:329–335; 1975. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Beversdorf DQ, Hughes JH, Steinberg BA, Lewis LD, Heilman KM. Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility in problem solving. NeuroReport 10:2763–2767; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Beversdorf DQ, White DM, Cheever DC, Hughes JD, Bornstein RA. Central beta-adrenergic blockers modulation of cognitive flexibility. NeuroReport 13:2505–2507; 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hasselmo ME, Linster C, Patil M, Ma D, Cecik M. Noradrenergic suppression of synaptic transmission may influence cortical signal-to-noise ratio. J Neurophysiol 77 3326–3339; 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Usher M, Cohen ID, Servan-Schreiber D, Rajkowski J, Aston-Jones G. The role of locus coeruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance. Science 283:549–554; 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Campbell HL, Tivarus ME, Hillier A, Beversdorf DQ. Increased task difficulty results in greater impact of noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility. Pharmacol BiochemBehav 88:222–229; 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kelley BJ, Yeager KR, Pepper TH, Beversdorf. Cognitive impairment in acute cocaine withdrawal. Cogn Behav Neurol 18: 108–112; 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kelley BJ, Yeager KR, Pepper TH, Bornstein RA, Beversdorf DQ. The effect of propranolol on cognitive flexibility and memory in acute cocaine withdrawal. Neurocase 13:320–327; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Alexander JK, Hillier A, Smith RM, Tivarus ME, Beversdorf DQ. Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. J Cogn Neuroscience 19:468–478; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Beversdorf DQ, Sharma UK, Phillips NN, Notestine MA, Slivka AP, Friedman NM, Schneider SL, Nagaraja HN, Hillier A. Effect of propranolol on naming in chronic Broca’s aphasia with anomia. Neurocase 13:256–259; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Beversdorf DQ. Neuropsychopharmacological regulation of performance on creativity-related tasks. Curr Opin Behav Sci 27:55–63; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lipnicki DM, Byrne DG. Thinking on your back: solving anagrams faster when supine than when standing. Cogn Brain Res 24:719–722; 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Stickgold R, Hobson JA, Fosse R, Fosse M. Sleep, learning, and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing. Science 294:1052–1057; 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ghacibeh GA, Shenker JI, Shenal B, Uthman BM, Heilman KM. Effect of vagus nerve stimulation on creativity and cognitive flexibility. Epilepsy Behav 8:720–725; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cai DJ, Mednick SA, Harrison EM, Kanady JC, Mednick SC. REM, not incubation, improves creativity by priming associative networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 10130–10134; 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fox KC, Nijeboer S, Solomonova E, Domhoff GW, Christoff K. Dreaming as wandering: Evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports. Front Hum Neurosci 7:412; 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Silver JA, Hughes JD, Bornstein RA, Beversdorf DQ. Effect of anxiolytics on cognitive flexibility in problem solving. Cogn Behav Neurol 17:93–97; 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sort Test Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norpeinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Ann Rev Neurosci 28:403–450; 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lapiz MDS, Morilak DA. Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive function in rat medial prefrontal cortex as measured by attentional set shifting capability. Neuroscience 137:1039–1049; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hecht PM, Will MJ, Schachtman TR, Welby LM, Beversdorf DQ. Beta-adrenergic antagonistic effects on a novel cognitive flexibility task in rodents. Behav Brain Res 260: 148–154; 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Petsche H Approaches to verbal, visual and musical creativity by EEG coherence analysis. Int J Psychophysiol 24:145–159; 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Guilford JP. The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1967. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Miller LA, Tippett LJ. Effects of focal brain lesions on visual problem-solving. Neuropsychologia 34:387–398; 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Colombo B, Bartesaghi N, Simonelli L, Antonietti A. The combined effects of neurostimulation and priming on creative thinking: a preliminary tDCS study on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 9:403 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00403; 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Green AE, Spiegel KA, Giangrande EJ, Weinberger AB, Gallagher NM, Turkeltaub PE. Thinking cap plus thinking zaps: tDCS of frontopolar cortex improves creative analogical reasoning and facilitates conscious augmentation of state creativity and verbal expression. Cereb Cortex 27:2628–2639; 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Milano N, Goldman A, Woods A, Williamson J, Acosta L, Lamb D, Zhang H, Heilment K. The influence of right and left frontotemporal stimulation on visuospatial creativity. Neurology 78:P4–051.; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Limb CJ, Braun AR. Neural Substrates of Spontaneous Musical Performance: An fMRI Study of Jazz Improvisation. PLoS ONE 3(2): el679. doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001679; 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Ramos BP, Colgan LA, Nou E, Arnsten AFT. β2 adrenergic antagonist, clenbuterol, enhances working memory performance in aging animals. Neurobiol Aging 29: 1060–1069; 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ramos BP, Colgan L, Nou E, Ovaria S, Wilson SR, Arnsten AFT. The beta-1 adrenergic antagonist, betaxolol, improves working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 58: 894–900; 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Arnsten AF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Alpha-2 adrenergic mechanisms in prefrontal cortex associated with cognitive decline in aged non-human primates. Science 230: 1273–1276; 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Li BM, Mei ZT. Delayed response deficit induced by local injection of the alpha-2 adrenergic antagonist yohimbine into the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in young adult monkeys. Behav Neural Biol 62: 134–139; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ratey JJ, Bemporad J, Sorgi P, Bick P, Polakoff S, O’Driscoll G, Mikkelsen E. Brief report: open trial effects of beta-blockers on speech and social behaviors in 8 autistic adults. J Autism Devel Disord 17:439–446; 1987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jaselskis CA, Cook EH Jr, Fletcher KE, Leventhal BL. Clonidine treatment of hyperactive and impulsive children with autistic disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 12:322–327; 1992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fankhauser MP, Karumanchi VC, German ML, Yates A, Karumanchi SD. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of the efficacy of transdermal clonidine in autism. J Clin Psychiatry 53:77–82; 1992. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Koshes RJ, Rock NL. Use of clonidine for behavioral control in an adult patient with autism. Am J Psychiatry 151:1714; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lake CR, Ziegler MG, Murphy DL. Increased norepinephrine levels and decreased dopamine-beta-hydroxylase activity in primary autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 34:553–556; 1977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Launay J- M, Bursztejn C, Ferrari P, Dreux C, Braconnier A, Zarifian E, Lancrenon S, Fermanian J. Catecholamines metabolism in infantile autism: a controlled study of 22 autistic children. J Autism Devel Disord 17:333–347; 1987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Barthelemy C, Bruneau N, Cottet-Eymard JM, Domenech-Jouve J, Garreau B, Lelord G, Muh JP, Peyrin L. Urinary free and conjugated catecholamines and metabolites in autistic children. J Autism Devel Disord 18:583–591; 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Martineau J, Barthelemy C, Jouve J, Muh J- P, Lelord G. Monoamines (serotonin and catecholamines) and their derivatives in infantile autism: age-related changes and drug effects. Devel Med Child Neurol 34:593–603; 1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Minderaa RB, Anderson GM, Volkmar FR, Akkerhuis GW, Cohen DJ. Noradrenergic and adrenergic functioning in autism. Biol Psychiatry 36:237–241; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Martchek M, Thevarkunnel S, Bauman M, Blatt G, Kemper T. Lack of evidence of neuropathology in the locus coeruleus in autism. Acta Neuropathologica 111:497–499; 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Curran LK, Newschaffer CJ, Lee L, Crawford SO, Johnston MV, Zimmerman AW. Behaviors associated with fever in children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 120:e1386–e1392; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Mehler MF, Purpura DP. Autism, fever, epigenetics and the locus coeruleus. Brain Res Rev 59:388–392; 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Sheinkopf SJ, Levin TP, McCormick CEB, Puggioni G, Conradt E, Lagasse LL, Lester BM. Developmental trajectories of autonomic functioning in autism from birth to early childhood. Biol Psychol 142:13–19; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Billeci L, Tonacci A, Narzisi A, Manigrasso Z, Varanini M, Fulceri, Lattarulo C, Calderoni S, Muratori F. Heart rate variability during a joint attention task in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Front Physiol. 9:467. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00467; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Corbett BA, Muscatello RA, Baldinger C. Comparing stress and arousal systems in response to different social context in children with ASD. Biol Psychol 140:119–130; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Palumbo D, Salee FR, Pelham WE, Bukstein OG, Daviss WB, McDermott MP, CAT study team. Clonidine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Efficacy and tolerability outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:180–188; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Daviss WB, Patel NC, Robb AS, McDermott MP, Bukstein OG, Pelham WE, Palumbo D, Harris P, Salee FR, CAT study team. Clonidine for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. ECG changes and adverse events analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 47:189–198; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ming X, Gordon E, Kang N, Wagner GC. Use of clonidine in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Dev 30: 454–460; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Handen BL, Sahl R, Hardan AY. Guanfacine in children with autism and/or intellectual disabilities. J Dev Behav Pediatr 29: 303–308; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Harfterkamp M, van de Loo-Neus G, Minderaa RB, van der Gaag R-J, Escobar R, Schacht A, Pamulapati S, Buitelaar JK, Hoekstra PJ. A randomized double-blind study of atomoxetine versus placebo for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51:733–741; 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Handen BL, Taylor J, Tumuluru R. Psychopharmacological treatment of ADHD symptoms in children with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Adolesc Med Health 23:167–173; 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21:37–46; 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Frith U, Happe FGE. Autism: beyond theory of mind. Cognition 50:115–132; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Happe FGE. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Devel Disord 24:129–154; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Fotheringham JB. Autism: Its primary psychological and neurological deficit. Can J Psychiatry 36:686–692; 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Hobson RP. Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individuals’ perception and understanding of emotion. J Child Psycho l Psychiatry 32:1135–1158; 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Hobson RP. Understanding persons: the role of affect In Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (Eds), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press; pp. 208–227; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Beversdorf DQ, Anderson JM, Manning SE, Anderson SL, Nordgren RE, Felopulos GJ, Nadeau SE, Heilman KM, Bauman ML. The effect of semantic and emotional context on written recall for verbal language in high functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 65:685–692; 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Rumsey JM. Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, non-retarded autistic men. J Autism Devel Disord 15:23–36; 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD. Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism, residual state. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 10:201–221; 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD. Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. J Autism Devel Disord 20:155–168; 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Mottron L, Burack JA. Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism In Burack JA, Charman T, Yirmiya N, Zelazo PR (Eds), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 131–148; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 97.Minshew NJ, Goldstein G. The pattern of intact and impaired memory functions in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:1095–1101; 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.O’Connor N, Hermelin B. Auditory and visual memory in autistic and normal children. J Mental Deficiency 11:126–131; 1967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Hermelin B, O’Connor N. Psychological experiments with autistic children. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Hermelin B, Frith U. Psychological studies of childhood autism: can autistic children make sense of what they hear? Focus on Autistic Behavior 6:6–13; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 101.Beversdorf DQ, Smith BW, Crucian GP, Anderson JM, Keillor JM, Barrett AM, Hughes JD, Felopulos GJ, Bauman ML, Nadeau SE, Heilman KM. Increased discrimination of “false memories” in autism spectrum disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:8734–8737; 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hillier A, Campbell H, Keillor J, Phillips N, Beversdorf DQ. Decreased false memory for visually presented shapes and symbols among adults on the autism specrum. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 29:610–616; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 127:1811–1821; 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an fMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cerebral Cortex 17:951–961; 2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci 24:9228–9231; 2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Bowler DM, Gardiner JM, Grice SJ, Saavalainen P. Memory illusions: False recall and recognition in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. J Abnorm Psychol 10:663–672; 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Beversdorf DQ, Carpenter AL, Miller RF, Cios JS, Hillier A. Effect of propranolol on verbal problem solving in autism spectrum disorder. Neurocase 14:378–383; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Narayanan A, White CA, Saklayen S, Scaduto MJ, Carpenter AL, Abduljalil A, Schmalbrock P, Beversdorf DQ. Effect of propranolol on functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorder. Brain Imag Behav 4: 189–197; 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Beversdorf DQ, Narayanan A, Hillier A, Hughes JD. Network model of decreased context utilization in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Devel Disord 37:1040–1048; 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Hegarty JP II, Ferguson BJ, Zamzow RM, Rohowetz LJ, Johnson JD, Christ SE, Beversdorf DQ. Beta-adrenergic antagonism modulates functional connectivity in the default mode network of individuals with and without autism spectrum disorder. Brain Imag Behav 11: 1278–1289; 2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Beversdorf DQ, Saklayen S, Higgins KF, Bodner KE, Kanne SM, Christ SE. Effect of propranolol on word fluency in autism. Cogn Behav Neurol 24: 11–17; 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Bodner KE, Beversdorf DQ, Saklayen SS, Christ SE. Noradrenergic moderation of working memory impairments in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Intl Neuropsychol Soc 18: 556–564; 2012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Zamzow RM, Christ SE, Saklayen SS, Moffitt AJ, Bodner KE, Higgins KF, Beversdorf DQ. Effect of propranolol on facial scanning in autism spectrum disroder: a preliminary investigation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 36: 431–445; 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Zamzow RM, Ferguson BJ, Stichter JP, Porges EC, Ragsdale AS, Lewis ML, Beversdorf DQ. Effects of propranolol on conversational reciprocity in autism spectrum disroder: a pilot, double-blind, single-dose psychopharmacological challenge study. Psychopharmacol 233: 1171–1178; 2016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Zamzow RM, Ferguson BJ, Ragsdale AS, Lewis ML, Beversdorf DQ. Effects of acute beta-adrenergic antagonism on verbal problem solving in autism spectrum disorder and exploration of treatment response markers. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 39:596–606; 2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Sagar-Ouriaghli I, Lievesley K, Santosh PJ. Propranolol for treating emotional, behavioral, autonomic dysregulation in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Psychopharmacol 32:641–653; 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Cohen IL. An artificial neural network analogue of learning in autism. Biol Psychiatry 36:5–20; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.McClelland JL. The basis of hyperspecificity in autism: a preliminary suggestion based on properties of neural nets. J Autism Devel Disord 30:497–502; 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Theunissen CIJM, Sillevis Smitt JH, van der Horst CMAM. Propranolol versus corticosteroids: what should be the treatment of choice infantile hemangiomas? Ann Plast Surg 74:237–41; 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Ferguson BJ, Marler S, Altstein LL, Lee EB, Akers J, Sohl K, McLaughlin A, Hartnett K, Kille B, Mazurek M, Macklin EA, McDonnell E, Barstow M, Bauman ML, Margolis KG, Veenstra-VanderWeele J, Beversdorf DQ. Psycholphysiological associations with gastrointestinal symptomatology in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 10:276–288; 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Ferguson BJ, Marler S, Altstein LL, Lee EB, Mazurek M, McLaughlin A, Macklin EA, McDonnell E, Davis DJ, Belenchia AM, Gillespie CH, Peterson CA, Bauman ML, Margolis KG, Veenstra-VanderWeele J, Beversdorf DQ. Associations between cytokines, endocrine stress response, and gastrointestinal symptoms in autism spectrum disorder. Brain Behav Immun 2016;58:57–62; 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Greendyke RM, Kanter DR, Schuster DB, Verstreate S, Wootton J. Propranolol treatment of assaultive patients with organic brain disease: a double-blind crossover, placebo-controlled study. J Nerv Ment Dis 174:290–294; 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Thibaut F, Colonna L. Anti-aggressive effect of beta-blockers. Encephale 19:263–267; 1993. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Haspel T. Beta-blockers and the treatment of aggression. Harvard Rev Psychiatry 2:274–281; 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Newman WJ, McDermott BE. Beta-blockers for violence prophylaxis: case reports. J Clin Psychopharmacol 31:785–787; 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.London EB. High dose propranolol, a beta adrenergic blocker for the control of violence and self abuse in patients with autism spectrum disorder. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 45:785.04; 2015. [Google Scholar]