Abstract
Olfaction is in addition to touch the most ancient of our senses, developing already in the womb it decays progressively from 65 years of age with a more pronounced impairment associated with dementia. Despite its clinical relevance and testing accessibility, smell remains an overlooked biomarker, which is rarely used by neurologists in the early screening phase. In this perspective article, we outline the reasons underlying the lack of awareness for this sense. In an attempt to put olfaction forward as an early biomarker for pathological brain aging, we draw a comparison with vision and hearing, regarded as more relevant for general health. This perspective article wants to encourage further studies aimed at understanding the mechanisms responsible for the early smell dysfunction in individuals a decade or more before the onset of cognitive symptoms.
Keywords: smell, Alzheimer’s disease, vision, hearing, biomarkers brain alterations
Smell – An Invisible but Vital Sense
Olfaction, from an evolutionary aspect, is the oldest of our senses. Across different species, it modulates the interactions between an organism and the surrounding environment even before birth (Brai and Alberi, 2018). From early postnatal life into adulthood, the sense of smell regulates many of our behaviors, from nutrition to social interaction (Schaal et al., 2020). For example, human neonates are attracted by a pad impregnated with the mother’s breast odor (promoting locomotor activity) compared to an empty control pad, supporting an odor memory developed already during the fetal stage (Han and Hummel, 2019). Nevertheless, the majority of the studies on chemo-sensation have been developed in rodents, with a less rich literature in humans (Zhou et al., 2019). The incomplete understanding of human olfaction may relate to the complexity of studying the multiple olfactory centers distributed in several brain regions comprising the cortical and the subcortical pathways, e.g., olfactory bulb, piriform and entorhinal cortex, amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex and hypothalamus (Shepherd, 2004; Atanasova et al., 2008; Fagundo et al., 2015; Hummel et al., 2017). This anatomical heterogeneity implies an extensive connection among the olfactory sensory areas which constitute a complex network essential to associate the olfactory stimulus with other cerebral regions, such as those involved in the processing of memories and emotions and multisensory integration with other senses (Shepherd, 2004; McGann, 2017).
Another challenge facing smell research in humans relates to its minor clinical implication as compared to impairment of vision and hearing: the occurrence of blindness or deafness produces a massive personal and social deficit which severely disrupts someone’s life. In line with these observations, the different attention paid to these three senses has been also described by Murphy et al. (2002) who reported that older adults in the US received assistance for vision and hearing deficits, whereas no testing for olfactory dysfunction was performed. While vision and hearing have been treated as primary senses for general health, olfaction is gaining increasing importance in clinical settings since its impairment represents an overarching non-invasive biomarker in predicting dementia during aging (MacDonald et al., 2018). With the frequent decline in smell acuity, mostly attributed to the reduced turnover of the olfactory neuroepithelium with aging (Doty et al., 2011), the early and pronounced olfactory deficit described in different neurodegenerative diseases, ranging from Alzheimer’s to Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases remains yet poorly understood.
In daily life, smell is an “invisible” sense influencing much of our daily existence from social interactions to our most ancestral memories and healthy aging. Without the sense of smell, there is no flavor perception during eating and drinking, so that everything tastes bland (Croy et al., 2014). Without a functioning sense of smell, many people develop mood and anxiety disorders. This body of evidence can be partially explained from an anatomical perspective since several brain regions, such as the limbic system and the orbitofrontal cortex, are involved in olfactory processing and also in the pathophysiology of these psychiatric disturbances (Atanasova et al., 2008; Stevenson, 2010; Croy et al., 2014; Takahashi et al., 2015; Kohli et al., 2017; Kamath et al., 2018; Rochet et al., 2018).
For instance, Croy and colleagues have shown that depressed patients display a decreased olfactory acuity. In particular, a decrement in odor discrimination and activation of the olfactory areas was observed in these individuals compared to healthy subjects. However, upon anti-depressive intervention, the olfactory parameters were comparable in the two groups (Croy et al., 2014). Additional data supporting the interconnection between olfaction and depression report that a dysfunction in odor identification can also be experienced by patients affected by this disorder (Kamath et al., 2018).
Among the studies suggesting a potential link between anxiety and olfactory impairment, Takahashi and coworkers explored whether anxiety traits could influence olfactory tasks in healthy subjects. Specifically, the authors observed that individuals suffering from states of anxiety presented remarkable odor detection and identification deficits (Takahashi et al., 2015).
Anxiety and mood disorders may impinge the cognitive function in older adults as indicated by neuroimaging data, showing that this condition may be possibly considered as an early marker or an independent risk component for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Becker et al., 2018). Furthermore, subjects affected by mild cognitive impairment or dementia show a higher probability to develop anxiety (Lyketsos et al., 2002). Therefore, accounting for olfactory impairment and co-symptomatic depression may represent a multifactorial risk factor for the development of dementia with aging.
Altogether these observations suggest that understanding more deeply how olfactory processing modulates odor perception across the lifespan will add considerable value for our well-being and health status along the age continuum.
Olfaction, Vision, and Hearing as Non-Invasive Biomarkers in Dementia
In an effort to halt the progressive spread of Alzheimer’s disease worldwide, more studies in the past decade have focused on the senses, vision, hearing, and smell, as sources of stable non-invasive biomarkers (MacDonald et al., 2018; Murphy, 2019; Lin, 2020), which may enable a preventive treatment during its preclinical phase (Figure 1). In this section we summarize some of the advances in sensory biomarkers offering a predictive value for dementia conversion/progression.
Olfactory Biomarkers
Olfactory performance reaches its peak at the age of 40 years (Buschhüter et al., 2008) and then progressively declines with aging (Eibenstein et al., 2005; Doty and Kamath, 2014; Palmquist et al., 2020; Parvand and Rankin, 2020; Van Regemorter et al., 2020). In addition, in different neurological pathologies, like Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease, dysosmia is exacerbated compared to the physiological decrease and occurs prior to motor and cognitive disabilities (Doty, 2012; Masala et al., 2018; Bathini et al., 2019; Cecchini et al., 2019; Dintica et al., 2019; Murphy, 2019). Many studies sustain that the alteration of the olfactory system may be used as an early predictor for detecting AD because several brain olfactory regions are impaired in the asymptomatic phase of this disorder due to the deposition of pathological hallmarks (Braak and Braak, 1991). Consequently, monitoring the olfactory sensitivity through different tests offers a supplementary approach which, together with other medical evaluations, can help to forecast the risk of developing memory decline, favoring the implementation of prevention strategies and promotion of clinical trials development (Table 1).
TABLE 1.
There is a battery of tests which can be adopted to gauge olfactory loss (Doty and Kamath, 2014), but the most used in determining early-stage AD include odor identification, discrimination and threshold (Lötsch et al., 2008; Hummel et al., 2010; Doty, 2015).
In particular, the first two olfactory tasks provide a more accurate evaluation in diagnosing prodromal dementia (Rahayel et al., 2012; Sohrabi et al., 2012; Quarmley et al., 2017). This can be explained by the fact that odor discrimination and identification rely, to a greater extent than odor threshold, on higher brain centers, such as the piriform, entorhinal, orbitofrontal cortices and the amygdala (Rahayel et al., 2012; Sohrabi et al., 2012; Quarmley et al., 2017), therefore indicating a more complex processing for their execution. On the contrary, odor threshold performance is mainly associated with peripheral olfactory stimuli as perceived by the olfactory receptors placed in the nasal neuroepithelium (Doty et al., 1994; Atanasova et al., 2008) and is susceptible to changes in the anatomy of the nasal cavity and clogging of the cribriform plate which occurs naturally with aging (Damm et al., 2002; Doty and Kamath, 2014; Masala et al., 2019).
Olfactory function can also be influenced by other factors, e.g., smoking or chronic rhinosinusitis may also trigger olfactory loss and should be taken into consideration (Devanand, 2016). Psychophysical tests of olfactory function are non-invasive, fast, inexpensive, and may be self-administered. Moreover, they can spare some patients from undergoing costly examinations, such as positron emission tomography (PET) scans, especially when administered together with other analysis (Devanand, 2016). In order to obtain reliable results, an important aspect which is taken into account when performing odor identification tasks is that people from different regions are familiar with distinct odors related to their cultural and traditional background (Konstantinidis et al., 2008; Neumann et al., 2012; Tekeli et al., 2013; Cavazzana et al., 2017). In addition to olfactory tests, nasal biopsies and swabs represent a supplementary procedure which can be adopted to evaluate the integrity of the olfactory system and its possible impairment in neurodegenerative diseases (Brozzetti et al., 2020). This multivariate approach on studying the olfactory system may provide complementary results useful for the diagnosis of neurological pathologies during their preclinical phase, therefore further elucidating whether a person is “silently” developing a pathology.
In conclusion, fully characterizing olfaction in its diverse aspects, as done with the other senses, will provide an added value in understanding the development of cognitive deficits and their prevention, offering new horizons for therapeutic applications during the preclinical phase of AD and possibly tackling its progression before it becomes irreversible.
Visual Biomarkers
Additionally to olfactory decline, AD patients can experience visual disturbances before displaying memory impairment (Harding et al., 1981; Coben et al., 1983; Schlotterer et al., 1984). Poor vision, spatial disorientation, and visual agnosia were characterized in patients with presumable AD (Cogan, 1985). Therefore, detecting these deficits in the asymptomatic stage of the pathology might play an important role in its early diagnosis (Javaid et al., 2016; Lim et al., 2016; Colligris et al., 2018; Alber et al., 2020). There is a number of retinal and non-retinal biomarkers which may be used to predict the risk of dementia in its prodromal phase (Javaid et al., 2016; Lim et al., 2016; Colligris et al., 2018; Dehghani et al., 2018; Alber et al., 2020; Table 1). For instance, some reports show that eye tests conducted using PET and optical coherence tomographic angiography could detect retinal microvascular abnormalities in cognitively normal subjects with preclinical AD (O’Bryhim et al., 2018). Furthermore, PET analysis revealed that intraretinal aggregation of β-amyloid and Tau occurs before the onset of AD clinical profile (Pike et al., 2007; Rowe et al., 2007). In particular, the structural variation observed in the eye components along with the accumulation of amyloid deposits cause visual deficits in the early stages of AD, and such variations could also alter the composition and the production of tears. Notably, it has been shown that the protein level in this ocular fluid is increased in AD subjects compared to that in healthy controls (Kalló et al., 2016). This data is in line with a previous study describing the use of tears to detect other neurodegenerative pathologies, like glaucoma (Tezel, 2013). While the detection of structural and functional changes through retinal scans may be informative of early neurodegenerative processes, it requires medical assistance and will be less accessible and compliant than olfactory testing to aging adults. On the other hand, point-of-care tear biomarker diagnostics may have a wider acceptance and usability across the population, but studies in this area remain few to date and there is so far no approved or marketed product.
Auditory Biomarkers
During aging the prevalence of hearing reduction becomes predominant and it is estimated that about 9–14% of people aged >65 years show auditory processing disorders (Rosenhall, 2015). With age-dependent hearing damage communication is affected, contributing to social isolation and loneliness which may alter the integrity of cognitive processes (Bennett et al., 2006; Wilson et al., 2007a). In addition, increasing studies suggest the association between hearing loss and cognitive deficit and dementia-related disorders (Gates et al., 2008; Lin, 2011; Lin et al., 2011a, 2014). Furthermore, such deficits appear to be stronger in concurrent multi-sensory impairments, like in auditory and vision domains (Yamada et al., 2016). Auditory performance can be tested through several measures, such as pure tone threshold, word recognition and central auditory testing (Gates et al., 2008; Lin, 2011, 2020; Lin et al., 2011a, b; Merten et al., 2020) (Table 1). Overall, hearing loss alters the input and therefore the information from external stimuli, leading to reduction in general awareness of the person with respect to time and space. Taken altogether, these observations sustain a potential involvement of hearing loss in dementia and to the late AD pathobiology. However, further studies need to target the key mechanisms underlying the link between auditory dysfunction and dementia/AD condition and therefore promote hearing monitoring as a reliable early predictor of cognitive decline.
Conclusion
In the challenge for finding relevant diagnostic markers to target dementia in its early stage, sensory modalities are offering themselves as promising non-invasive predictors. Different studies provide increasing evidence that monitoring olfaction acuity and, to some extent, vision and hearing could contribute to the early detection of neuronal changes linked to mental decline, therefore promoting a more immediate intervention long before the onset of the cognitive symptoms (Table 1).
As shown by several studies, a reduced olfactory activity is a valuable predictor for cognitive decline in non-demented older adults (Wilson et al., 2007b; Sohrabi et al., 2012). The sequential association between olfactory dysfunction and cognitive deficit is not completely resolved. Nevertheless, the spreading rostral to caudal tauopathy in the limbic brain areas may influence olfactory transmission first and cognitive processing later (Doty and Kamath, 2014; Dintica et al., 2019). These observations further support the association between the sense of smell and cognition and promote olfactory testing to identify those subjects with a higher risk of developing dementia.
Since visual symptoms are experienced by many AD patients, increasing studies are investigating whether this sensory system may represent another potential source of early biomarkers to diagnose cognitive decline and dementia. Among the features corroborating the pathophysiological link between visual/ocular impairments and cognitive dysfunction, it can be acknowledged that the eye and the central nervous system have some commonalities, such as the same embryologic origin and the presence of physiological barriers (Alber et al., 2020). In addition, advanced imaging techniques revealed the accumulation of AD hallmarks in visual structures during the prodromal stage of dementia (Lim et al., 2016; Colligris et al., 2018; Alber et al., 2020).
Many research projects foster the use of hearing assessment as a supplemental diagnostic measure to target cognitive decline and dementia during their preclinical stage. For instance, Lin et al. (2011a, b) explored whether hearing impairment could be mirrored by decreased cognitive tests scoring. Interestingly, they observed that hearing decrement was related with lower cognitive scores. Further evidence proposing this interplay have been correlated with cerebral atrophy in regions implicated in speech processing, common degenerative mechanisms, social isolation and altered communication (Lin et al., 2011a, 2014; Lin and Albert, 2014).
Overall, if the use of sensory functions would be considered as a reliable tool to evaluate the risk of developing dementia, this would enrich the array of multi-step screening protocols that can be adopted in clinical practice. As with other chronic pathologies, such as diabetes and hypertension, where a medical evaluation is regularly recommended in many countries, the monitoring of olfactory performance during aging through simple smell testing could inform about a possible neuropathological risk. A strong informative campaign about the potential benefits which could be obtained by administering olfactory tests might lead to a broader consensus in providing a first screening phase in classifying individuals with a higher risk of developing cognitive impairment and then fostering the development of novel clinical trials. Obviously, supplementary examinations like cognitive tests and brain imaging techniques would be required to confirm this evidence.
In conclusion, we see olfactory testing as a potentially first-line non-invasive diagnostic measure with great compliance and flexibility that may inform the patient and the family and aid in planning healthcare interventions. This primary monitoring would be of substantial value for clinical studies where the selection and the recruitment of subjects in prodromal stages remain challenging. Furthermore, a first diagnostic screening based on olfactory testing will allow the swift implementation of preventive approaches aimed at improving cognition and brain health. In the future, we see the integration of analog tests with IT interfaces expanding the capabilities of olfactory monitoring as part of the personalized health program aimed at improving the quality of life for a long term.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Author Contributions
EB wrote the manuscript. TH reviewed and gave the feedback. LA advised and wrote part of the text. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Funding. This study was supported by the Schweizerischer Nationalfonds zur Förderung der Wissenschaftlichen Forschung (163470).
References
- Alber J., Goldfarb D., Thompson L. I., Arthur E., Hernandez K., Cheng D., et al. (2020). Developing retinal biomarkers for the earliest stages of Alzheimer’s disease: what we know, what we don’t, and how to move forward. Alzheimers Dement. J. Alzheimers Assoc. 16 229–243. 10.1002/alz.12006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atanasova B., Graux J., El Hage W., Hommet C., Camus V., Belzung C. (2008). Olfaction: a potential cognitive marker of psychiatric disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32 1315–1325. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bathini P., Brai E., Auber L. A. (2019). Olfactory dysfunction in the pathophysiological continuum of dementia. Ageing Res. Rev. 55:100956. 10.1016/j.arr.2019.100956 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker E., Orellana Rios C. L., Lahmann C., Rücker G., Bauer J., Boeker M. (2018). Anxiety as a risk factor of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry J. Ment. Sci. 213 654–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett D. A., Schneider J. A., Tang Y., Arnold S. E., Wilson R. S. (2006). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurol. 5 406–412. 10.1016/s1474-4422(06)70417-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braak H., Braak E. (1991). Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82 239–259. 10.1007/bf00308809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brai E., Alberi L. (2018). Olfaction, Among the First Senses to Develop and Decline Sensory Nervous System. Thomas Heinbockel: IntechOpen. [Google Scholar]
- Brozzetti L., Sacchetto L., Cecchini M. P., Avesani A., Perra D., Bongianni M., et al. (2020). Neurodegeneration-associated proteins in human olfactory neurons collected by nasal brushing. Front. Neurosci. 14:145. 10.3389/fnins.2020.00145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschhüter D., Smitka M., Puschmann S., Gerber J. C., Witt M., Abolmaali N. D., et al. (2008). Correlation between olfactory bulb volume and olfactory function. NeuroImage 42 498–502. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavazzana A., Wesarg C., Schriever V. A., Hummel T., Lundström J. N., Parma V. (2017). A cross-cultural adaptation of the sniffin’ sticks olfactory identification test for US children. Chem. Senses 42 133–140. 10.1093/chemse/bjw113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cecchini M. P., Federico A., Zanini A., Mantovani E., Masala C., Tinazzi M., et al. (2019). Olfaction and taste in Parkinson’s disease: the association with mild cognitive impairment and the single cognitive domain dysfunction. J. Neural Transm. 1996 585–595. 10.1007/s00702-019-01996-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coben L. A., Danziger W. L., Hughes C. P. (1983). Visual evoked potentials in mild senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 55 121–130. 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90178-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cogan D. G. (1985). Visual disturbances with focal progressive dementing disease. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 100 68–72. 10.1016/s0002-9394(14)74985-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colligris P., Perez de Lara M. J., Colligris B., Pintor J. (2018). Ocular manifestations of Alzheimer’s and other neurodegenerative diseases: the prospect of the eye as a tool for the early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Ophthalmol. 2018 1–12. 10.1155/2018/8538573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croy I., Nordin S., Hummel T. (2014). Olfactory disorders and quality of life–an updated review. Chem. Senses 39 185–194. 10.1093/chemse/bjt072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damm M., Vent J., Schmidt M., Theissen P., Eckel H. E., Lötsch J., et al. (2002). Intranasal volume and olfactory function. Chem. Senses 27 831–839. 10.1093/chemse/27.9.831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehghani C., Frost S., Jayasena R., Masters C. L., Kanagasingam Y. (2018). Ocular biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease: the role of anterior eye and potential future directions. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 59 3554–3563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devanand D. P. (2016). Olfactory identification deficits, cognitive decline, and dementia in older adults. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24 1151–1157. 10.1016/j.jagp.2016.08.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dintica C. S., Marseglia A., Rizzuto D., Wang R., Seubert J., Arfanakis K., et al. (2019). Impaired olfaction is associated with cognitive decline and neurodegeneration in the brain. Neurology 92 e700–e709. 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L. (2012). Olfaction in Parkinson’s disease and related disorders. Neurobiol. Dis. 46 527–552. 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.10.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L. (2015). Olfactory dysfunction and its measurement in the clinic. World J. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 1 28–33. 10.1016/j.wjorl.2015.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L., Kamath V. (2014). The influences of age on olfaction: a review. Front. Psychol. 5:20. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L., Petersen I., Mensah N., Christensen K. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on odor identification ability in the very old. Psychol. Aging 26 864–871. 10.1037/a0023263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty R. L., Smith R., McKeown D. A., Raj J. (1994). Tests of human olfactory function: principal components analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept. Psychophys. 56 701–707. 10.3758/bf03208363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eibenstein A., Fioretti A., Lena C., Rosati N., Ottaviano I., Fusetti M. (2005). Olfactory screening test: experience in 102 Italian subjects. Acta Otorhinolaryngol. Ital. 25 18–22. 10.1001/archderm.98.1.18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fagundo A. B., Jiménez-Murcia S., Giner-Bartolomé C., Islam M. A., de la Torre R., Pastor A., et al. (2015). Modulation of higher-order olfaction components on executive functions in humans. PLoS One 10:e0130319. 10.1371/journal.pone.0130319 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gates G. A., Anderson M. L., Feeney M. P., McCurry S. M., Larson E. B. (2008). Central auditory dysfunction in older persons with memory impairment or Alzheimer dementia. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 134 771–777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han P., Hummel T. (2019). “Orthonasal and retronasal olfaction,” in Food Aroma Evolution, 1st Edn, eds Bordiga M., Nollet L. M. L. (Boca Raton: CRC Press; ), 99–120. 10.1201/9780429441837-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Harding G. F. A., Doggett C. E., Orwin A., Smith E. J. (1981). “Visual evoked potentials in pre-senile dementia,” in Visual Pathways: Electrophysiology and Pathology, eds Spekreijse H., Apkarian P. A. (Dordrecht: Springer; ), 193–202. 10.1007/978-94-009-8656-5_20 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel T., Pfetzing U., Lötsch J. (2010). A short olfactory test based on the identification of three odors. J. Neurol. 257 1316–1321. 10.1007/s00415-010-5516-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel T., Whitcroft K. L., Andrews P., Altundag A., Cinghi C., Costanzo R. M., et al. (2017). Position paper on olfactory dysfunction. Rhinol. Suppl. 54 1–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Javaid F. Z., Brenton J., Guo L., Cordeiro M. F. (2016). Visual and ocular manifestations of Alzheimer’s disease and their use as biomarkers for diagnosis and progression. Front. Neurol. 7:55. 10.3389/fneur.2016.00055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalló G., Emri M., Varga Z., Ujhelyi B., Tőzsér J., Csutak A., et al. (2016). Changes in the chemical barrier composition of tears in Alzheimer’s Disease reveal potential tear diagnostic biomarkers. PLoS One 11:e0158000. 10.1371/journal.pone.0158000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamath V., Paksarian D., Cui L., Moberg P. J., Turetsky B. I., Merikangas K. R. (2018). Olfactory processing in bipolar disorder, major depression, and anxiety. Bipolar Disord. 20 547–555. 10.1111/bdi.12625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohli P., Naik A. N., Harruff E. E., Nguyen S. A., Schlosser R. J., Soler Z. M. (2017). The prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in chronic rhinosinusitis. Laryngoscope 127 309–320. 10.1002/lary.26316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konstantinidis I., Printza A., Genetzaki S., Mamali K., Kekes G., Constantinidis J. (2008). Cultural adaptation of an olfactory identification test: the Greek version of Sniffin’. Sticks Rhinol. 46 292–296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim J. K. H., Li Q.-X., He Z., Vingrys A. J., Wong V. H. Y., Currier N., et al. (2016). The eye as a biomarker for Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neurosci. 10:536. 10.3389/fnins.2016.00536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R. (2011). Hearing loss and cognition among older adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66 1131–1136. 10.1093/gerona/glr115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R. (2020). Making sense of the senses in aging research. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 75 529–530. 10.1093/gerona/glaa028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Albert M. (2014). Hearing loss and dementia – who’s listening? Aging Ment. Health 18 671–673. 10.1080/13607863.2014.915924 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Ferrucci L., An Y., Goh J. O., Doshi J., Metter E. J., et al. (2014). Association of hearing impairment with brain volume changes in older adults. NeuroImage 90 84–92. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.059 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Ferrucci L., Metter E. J., An Y., Zonderman A. B., Resnick S. M. (2011a). Hearing loss and cognition in the baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Neuropsychology 25 763–770. 10.1037/a0024238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin F. R., Metter E. J., O’Brien R. J., Resnick S. M., Zonderman A. B., Ferrucci L. (2011b). Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch. Neurol. 68 214–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lötsch J., Reichmann H., Hummel T. (2008). Different odor tests contribute differently to the evaluation of olfactory loss. Chem. Senses 33 17–21. 10.1093/chemse/bjm058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyketsos C. G., Lopez O., Jones B., Fitzpatrick A. L., Breitner J., DeKosky S. (2002). Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia and mild cognitive impairment: results from the cardiovascular health study. JAMA 288 1475–1483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald S. W. S., Keller C. J. C., Brewster P. W. H., Dixon R. A. (2018). Contrasting olfaction, vision, and audition as predictors of cognitive change and impairment in non-demented older adults. Neuropsychology 32 450–460. 10.1037/neu0000439 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masala C., Käehling C., Fall F., Hummel T. (2019). Correlation between olfactory function, trigeminal sensitivity, and nasal anatomy in healthy subjects. Eur. Arch. Oto-Rhino-Laryngol. 276 1649–1654. 10.1007/s00405-019-05367-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masala C., Solla P., Liscia A., Defazio G., Saba L., Cannas A., et al. (2018). Correlation among olfactory function, motors’ symptoms, cognitive impairment, apathy, and fatigue in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. 265 1764–1771. 10.1007/s00415-018-8913-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGann J. P. (2017). Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science 356:eaam7263. 10.1126/science.aam7263 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merten N., Fischer M. E., Tweed T. S., Breteler M. M. B., Cruickshanks K. J. (2020). Associations of hearing sensitivity, higher-order auditory processing, and cognition over time in middle-aged adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 75 545–551. 10.1093/gerona/glz189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy C. (2019). Olfactory and other sensory impairments in Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 15 11–24. 10.1038/s41582-018-0097-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy C., Schubert C. R., Cruickshanks K. J., Klein B. E. K., Klein R., Nondahl D. M. (2002). Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA 288 2307–2312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neumann C., Tsioulos K., Merkonidis C., Salam M., Clark A., Philpott C. (2012). Validation study of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” olfactory test in a British population: a preliminary communication. Clin. Otolaryngol. 37 23–27. 10.1111/j.1749-4486.2012.02431.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Bryhim B. E., Apte R. S., Kung N., Coble D., Van Stavern G. P. (2018). Association of preclinical Alzheimer disease with optical coherence tomographic angiography findings. JAMA Ophthalmol. 136 1242–1248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmquist E., Larsson M., Olofsson J. K., Seubert J., Bäckman L., Laukka E. J. (2020). A prospective study on risk factors for olfactory dysfunction in aging. J. Gerontol. Ser. A 75 603–610. 10.1093/gerona/glz265 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parvand M., Rankin C. H. (2020). Is there a shared etiology of olfactory impairments in normal aging and neurodegenerative disease? J. Alzheimers Dis. 73 1–21. 10.3233/jad-190636 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pike K. E., Savage G., Villemagne V. L., Ng S., Moss S. A., Maruff P., et al. (2007). Beta-amyloid imaging and memory in non-demented individuals: evidence for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain J. Neurol. 130 2837–2844. 10.1093/brain/awm238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quarmley M., Moberg P. J., Mechanic-Hamilton D., Kabadi S., Arnold S. E., Wolk D. A., et al. (2017). Odor identification screening improves diagnostic classification in incipient Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55 1497–1507. 10.3233/jad-160842 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rahayel S., Frasnelli J., Joubert S. (2012). The effect of Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease on olfaction: a meta-analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 231 60–74. 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rochet M., El-Hage W., Richa S., Kazour F., Atanasova B. (2018). Depression, olfaction, and quality of life: a mutual relationship. Brain Sci. 8:80. 10.3390/brainsci8050080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenhall U. (2015). Epidemiology of age related hearing loss. Hear. Bal. Commun. 13 46–50. 10.3109/21695717.2015.1013775 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe C. C., Ng S., Ackermann U., Gong S. J., Pike K., Savage G., et al. (2007). Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia. Neurology 68 1718–1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaal B., Saxton T. K., Loos H., Soussignan R., Durand K. (2020). Olfaction scaffolds the developing human from neonate to adolescent and beyond. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 375:20190261. 10.1098/rstb.2019.0261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlotterer G., Moscovitch M., Crapper-McLachlan D. (1984). Visual processing deficits as assessed by spatial frequency contrast sensitivity and backward masking in normal ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain J. Neurol. 107(Pt 1), 309–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shepherd G. M. (2004). The human sense of smell: are we better than we think? PLoS Biol. 2:e146. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sohrabi H. R., Bates K. A., Weinborn M. G., Johnston A. N. B., Bahramian A., Taddei K., et al. (2012). Olfactory discrimination predicts cognitive decline among community-dwelling older adults. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e118. 10.1038/tp.2012.43 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson R. J. (2010). An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chem. Senses 35 3–20. 10.1093/chemse/bjp083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi T., Itoh H., Nishikawa Y., Higuchi Y., Nakamura M., Sasabayashi D., et al. (2015). Possible relation between olfaction and anxiety in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 69 431–438. 10.1111/pcn.12277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tekeli H., Altundað A., Salihoðlu M., Çayönü M., Kendirli M. T. (2013). The applicability of the “Sniffin’ Sticks” olfactory test in a Turkish population. Med. Sci. Monit. Int. Med. J. Exp. Clin. Res. 19 1221–1226. 10.12659/msm.889838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tezel G. (2013). A proteomics view of the molecular mechanisms and biomarkers of glaucomatous neurodegeneration. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 35 18–43. 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2013.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Regemorter V., Hummel T., Rosenzweig F., Mouraux A., Rombaux P., Huart C. (2020). Mechanisms linking olfactory impairment and risk of mortality. Front. Neurosci. 14:140. 10.3389/fnins.2020.00140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson R. S., Krueger K. R., Arnold S. E., Schneider J. A., Kelly J. F., Barnes L. L., et al. (2007a). Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64 234–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson R. S., Schneider J. A., Arnold S. E., Tang Y., Boyle P. A., Bennett D. A. (2007b). Olfactory identification and incidence of mild cognitive impairment in older age. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64 802–808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamada Y., Denkinger M. D., Onder G., Henrard J.-C., van der Roest H. G., Finne-Soveri H., et al. (2016). Dual sensory impairment and cognitive decline: the results from the shelter study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 71 117–123. 10.1093/gerona/glv036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou G., Lane G., Cooper S. L., Kahnt T., Zelano C. (2019). Characterizing functional pathways of the human olfactory system. eLife 8:e47177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.