Skip to main content
Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Elsevier - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2020 Sep 10;150:105190. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105190

Maternal influences on fetal brain development: The role of nutrition, infection and stress, and the potential for intergenerational consequences

Eamon Fitzgerald 1, Kahyee Hor 1, Amanda J Drake 1,
PMCID: PMC7481314  PMID: 32948364

Abstract

An optimal early life environment is crucial for ensuring ideal neurodevelopmental outcomes. Brain development consists of a finely tuned series of spatially and temporally constrained events, which may be affected by exposure to a sub-optimal intra-uterine environment. Evidence suggests brain development may be particularly vulnerable to factors such as maternal nutrition, infection and stress during pregnancy. In this review, we discuss how maternal factors such as these can affect brain development and outcome in offspring, and we also identify evidence which suggests that the outcome can, in many cases, be stratified by socio-economic status (SES), with individuals in lower brackets typically having a worse outcome. We consider the relevant epidemiological evidence and draw parallels to mechanisms suggested by preclinical work where appropriate. We also discuss possible transgenerational effects of these maternal factors and the potential mechanisms involved. We conclude that modifiable factors such as maternal nutrition, infection and stress are important contributors to atypical brain development and that SES also likely has a key role.

Keywords: Early life programming, Brain development, Neurodevelopmental outcome, Transgenerational effects


The Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (DOHaD) hypothesis posits that the antenatal period is a particularly vulnerable period of development in which exposure to adverse environments, such as malnutrition, infection or stress can have long-lasting or permanent impacts on the health trajectory of the offspring – a process that is termed “developmental programming” [1,2]. Much research has focussed on the role of the early life environment in the programming of cardiometabolic disease, including type 2 diabetes and hypertension [3,4]. However, a growing number of studies have shown that exposure to an adverse early life environment is also associated with long-term consequences for offspring neurodevelopment including effects on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis [5] and an increased risk of mental health disorders [6]. The developing brain may be particularly vulnerable to adverse environmental exposures in the antenatal period because of the dramatic developmental trajectory which occurs during this time. Optimal brain development relies upon a highly temporally constrained series of transcriptional and epigenetic states [7,8]; indeed this is underscored by the results from large genetic studies showing that the peak expression of common gene variants involved in the aetiology of several neurodevelopmental disorders occurs during pregnancy [9]. Disruption of these expression networks may mediate some of the risk for atypical neurodevelopment.

An adverse intra-uterine environment can affect fetal neurodevelopment either through direct effects and/or maternal signals, or indirectly as a consequence of preterm birth, which is independently associated with poor neurodevelopmental outcome [10]. In this short review we will focus on maternal-fetal communication in the domains of nutrition, infection and stress, all of which can affect fetal neurodevelopment.

1. Nutrition

Both maternal under- and overnutrition may have consequences for fetal neurodevelopment. The detrimental effects of maternal undernutrition have been characterised in studies of individuals who were exposed to severe undernutrition in utero during the Dutch Hunger Winter. In early 1944, the western part of The Netherlands was under German occupation and endured severe food shortages. During this time the population, including pregnant women, had an estimated caloric intake of 400–800 per day for a 5–6 month period [11]. The increased incidence of chronic heart disease and type 2 diabetes in the offspring of these pregnancies is well documented [12]. Although in an early follow-up study, general cognition was not found to be affected in individuals at 19 years of age [13], a subsequent study of individuals at the age of 56–59 years showed that those exposed during early gestation performed more poorly on a selective attention task, which associates with accelerated age-related cognitive decline [14]. Although this degree of undernutrition is uncommon, at least in developed nations, fetal exposure to an unbalanced maternal diet, which may associate with deficiencies in specific nutrients, has also been associated with effects on the response to stress in young adulthood [15]. Studies in animal models aimed at delineating the long-term consequences of in utero undernutrition have demonstrated adverse effects on neurodevelopment. For example in mice, exposure to a low protein diet during gestation affects neural progenitor populations at several points during embryogenesis in both the cortex and the ganglionic eminence, and in adulthood these mice display deficiencies in short term memory [16].

In 2017 62% of women in England were overweight and of these, 30% were obese [17] – these figures include women of childbearing age. Maternal obesity during pregnancy is associated with increased adiposity and cardiometabolic risk factors in adult offspring [18]. Maternal obesity is also associated with impaired neurodevelopment and executive functioning [19,20] and with adverse neuropsychiatric outcomes in children [20], including Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) [21] and Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) [22,23]. Studies in mouse models of maternal obesity suggest that there are adverse effects on brain development and behaviour [24,25] and in non-human primates, exposure to a high fat diet during pregnancy is associated with an increase in offspring anxiety-like and repetitive behaviours [26]. Some of these effects may be mediated by epigenetic alterations, for example in mice, exposure to excess dietary ω-6 poly-unsaturated fatty acids (used to model a western high fat diet) has been associated with altered DNA methylation and chromatin architecture in the offspring cortex [27].

Population data from England and Wales in 2014 indicated that 5% of pregnant women had diabetes [28], of which 87.5% had gestational diabetes (GDM). GDM is a condition which arises during pregnancy, typically resolves postnatally and is closely associated with BMI [28,29]. Diabetes during pregnancy is associated with a range of fetal effects across various organ systems, including an increased incidence of neural tube defects [30,31]. In mice, hyperglycaemia during pregnancy is associated with disrupted neurogenesis in the embryo [32] and with apoptosis in epithelial cells within the neural tube [33], and additionally affects social behaviours in adulthood [34].

Some of the effects of maternal obesity on the offspring may be mediated by the associated increase in inflammation. Higher levels of maternal inflammation during pregnancy are associated with an increased risk of neurodevelopmental delay during childhood and mediate the effect of prenatal environmental adversity on child neurodevelopmental delay [35]. Exposure to maternal obesity during the antenatal period may result in the fetus being more susceptible to other insults such as infection or inflammation; for example, in mice, high fat diet-induced diabetes during pregnancy can potentiate the transcriptional response to a subsequent inflammatory stimulus in the fetal brain [36]. This is in line with the multi-hit hypothesis which is commonly cited for schizophrenia but may have relevance to other neurological disorders [37].

2. Infection

Pregnant women are more susceptible to infection and display an increased inflammatory response to some pathogens, but the mechanisms behind this are largely unknown [38,39]. Maternal viral infections during pregnancy may increase the risk of psychiatric disease in the offspring, with the time of infection being particularly important. For instance, a large Danish study found a significant association between maternal hospitalisation for influenza during the first trimester and ASD in her offspring [40]. Maternal exposure to measles [41], rubella [42] and polio [43] have all been associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia in offspring. Other maternal infections (for example untreated genital herpes [44]) may affect neurodevelopment indirectly by increasing the risk for preterm birth, which is independently associated with adverse neurodevelopment [10]. Early reports from the current COVID-19 pandemic have suggested an increased preterm birth rate in pregnant women with confirmed infections, without convincing evidence of vertical transmission [45,46]. This may to some extent reflect medical intervention in infected mothers and any longer term consequences for the offspring of COVID-19 infection in pregnant women remains to be seen. Vertical transmission of a viral pathogen to the fetus can be associated with serious neurodevelopmental consequences: Zika virus and cytomegalovirus infection in the first trimester have been associated with microcephaly [47]. Bacterial infections during pregnancy have also been associated with poor neurodevelopmental outcome in offspring. The Danish study reported a moderate association between bacterial infection during the second trimester and ASD in the offspring [40] and in a large American study, there was a marked increase in psychiatric disorders in the adult offspring of mothers who experienced a bacterial infection during pregnancy [48]. This was dependent on the severity of infection and a higher incidence was seen in males [48].

Inflammation induced by infection can be modelled in a number of ways, notably the use of agonists of the Toll-like receptor (TLR) family. TLR3 and TLR4 are the most commonly studied receptors with respect to maternal infection and are stimulated by the agonists Poly I:C (used to model viral infection) and lipopolysaccharide (LPS, a component of the bacterial cell wall which models bacterial infections) [49]. Studies of non-human primates during pregnancy have provided important insights into the mechanisms by which maternal infection impact on fetal neurodevelopment. In rhesus macaques, the offspring of mothers infected with influenza virus during pregnancy showed decreased white matter volume/density [50], while LPS exposure was associated with an increased white matter volume [51]. Again in rhesus macaques, exposure to the viral mimetic Poly I:C during pregnancy results in an altered inflammatory profile and stereotyped, repetitive behaviours during childhood [52,53]. In rodents, maternal immune activation with Poly I:C results in altered social interaction, communication and repetitive behaviours in adult offspring [54]. One potential mechanism which might mediate this is through maternal cytokine release; in mouse models, maternal interleukin (IL-)6 has been shown to mediate the effects of Poly I:C on fetal neurodevelopment [55]. A separate study indicated the maternal production of IL-17A in response to Poly I:C was involved in the pathogenesis of offspring neurodevelopmental disorders [56]. This leads to a hypothesised model in which maternal IL-6 is stimulated by a viral infection, which in turn activates TH17 cells to produce IL17A, which crosses the placenta to affect offspring neurodevelopment. Models of bacterial infection during pregnancy in rodents have also given important insights into atypical neurodevelopment induced by infection. Prenatal LPS in rodents results in an increase in repetitive behaviours [57], reduced social interaction [58], anxiety like behaviour [59] and spatial memory defects [60] in adult offspring. In rats, prenatal LPS is associated with an altered lipid profile in 8 week old offspring [61] and alterations in dopaminergic signalling [62], which has been hypothesised to be central in the aetiology of schizophrenia [63].

3. Maternal stress

Stress can be broadly described as an imbalance between the environment and an individual's perception of their resources to cope with that environment. Maternal stress can occur as a result of a diverse range of environmental perturbations and crucially, the experience of stress is highly individually specific. Therefore there are many domains of stress which can be associated with pregnancy [64]. While stress during pregnancy affects women worldwide, pregnancy-related stress in lower and middle income countries appears to be associated with a greater risk of adverse outcome in offspring [65].

Offspring exposed to high levels of prenatal maternal stress or anxiety are at a higher risk of developing depression [66], ASD [67], schizophrenia [68] and ADHD [69] as well as various emotional and behavioural problems [70]; with the timing of stress and the sex of the fetus playing important roles in the outcome of these studies. There is also evidence to suggest that maternal antenatal depression and socioeconomic status can interact with a polygenic score for major depressive disorder to modulate risk [71]. Moreover, good maternal mental health during pregnancy is positively associated with cognitive abilities in the offspring at 2 years of age [72]. There are many factors that need to be accounted for in these studies. For instance some traditional metrics to assess stress may not be validated for use in pregnancy [64]. Further, studies typically focus on periods of increased stress and neglect, but fail to measure positive or uplifting experiences which may counterbalance these [73]. The questionnaires used to measure stress during pregnancy may also be sensitive to underlying personality traits, perhaps underscored by the substantial heritable component of stress as measured by these methods [74]. Moreover, stress experienced during pregnancy tends to continue during postnatal life, making assigning the importance of pre- and postnatal stress in mediating outcomes difficult [75,76]. To address this, some studies have focussed on the effects of short-lasting large-scale environmental perturbations during pregnancy. For example, stress associated with experiencing an earthquake during the first trimester has been associated with a shorter length of gestation [77]. There is also evidence for effects on neurodevelopmental outcome, as offspring born to mothers who were pregnant during the 1998 ice storm crisis in Quebec had lower cognitive and language abilities at 5.5 years of age when compared to control infants from pregnancies out with this period [78].

There are a number of rodent models of maternal stress during pregnancy including exposure to a lactating female, forced swimming, noise and restraint stress [79] and the first rodent experiments describing altered behaviour in the offspring of stressed mothers was published more than 60 years ago [80]. In this study the adult offspring showed increased activity in the open field (a preclinical proxy of anxiety like behaviour), a finding which has been replicated in many subsequent studies (see Weinstock 2017 for a full review [79]). Prenatal stress in rodent models has also been shown to affect other cognitive domains in offspring, such as spatial memory [81,82] and social behaviour [83]. To account for the effects of pre- and postnatal stress, offspring cross-fostering studies have been performed. Cross-fostering the offspring of stressed mothers onto a control non-stressed mother at birth results in no differences in anxiety or social related behaviours in stress-exposed offspring and controls, suggesting that the postnatal environment is important for some effects [84,85], but differences in learning memory, novel object recognition, long-term potentiation and risk of depression remain [[86], [87], [88]].

Many of the effects of maternal stress on the fetus may be attributable to fetal glucocorticoid overexposure [89]. The enzyme 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 is present within the placenta and is responsible for converting active glucocorticoids in the maternal circulation (cortisol in humans, corticosterone in rodents) into an inactive form (cortisone or 11-dehydrocorticosterone). However, the enzyme has finite capacity and some active glucocorticoid may cross to the fetus, particularly in the presence of the increased maternal circulating glucocorticoid concentrations which may occur in stressful situations [90]. The administration of synthetic glucocorticoids, which bypass this enzyme, is commonplace in the management of women at risk of preterm birth and significantly improves morbidity and mortality in preterm babies [91]. However, there have been concerns about the deleterious effects of exposure to excess glucocorticoids (endogenous or exogenous) on the developing brain, with clinical studies reporting long-term effects on the function of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and behaviour in exposed children [[92], [93], [94], [95], [96]]. This is supported by data from a number of animal studies (reviewed by McGowan and Matthews [95]). Although evidence from both human and animal studies suggests that excess fetal exposure to glucocorticoids can have adverse consequences on the brain, the effects of pregnancy related stress cannot be entirely attributed to glucocorticoid exposure [97]. Maternal stress in rodents is also associated with an accumulation of catecholamines in the fetal circulation [98], an increase in kynurenic acid in the placenta and fetal brain [99], changes in oxytocin signalling [83], and an altered maternal microbiome and associated in utero cytokine levels [100]. Finally, maternal stress may interact with other factors e.g. infection to modulate offspring neurodevelopmental outcome. For example concurrent infection and stress during the second trimester result in a higher adolescent depression score in offspring then either alone [101].

4. Transgenerational effects

A growing number of studies suggest that ‘programmed’ effects might not be limited to the first generation offspring i.e. those directly exposed in utero, but may be transmitted to a number of subsequent generations. Human epidemiological studies have demonstrated evidence for such ‘transgenerational’ effects on birth weight and cardiovascular risk [[102], [103], [104], [105]]. Animal studies have confirmed that diverse prenatal insults can indeed influence the health of future generations, including effects on neurodevelopment and behaviour [106,107]. The transmission of programmed effects on neurodevelopment through the maternal line could occur through a number of mechanisms including i) continued exposure to an adverse environment; ii) re-exposure via programmed alterations in maternal physiology which impact on the development of the next generation e.g. through increased maternal glucocorticoid levels; or iii) changes in maternal behaviour leading to the development of similar behavioural phenotypes in her own children.

Many studies have suggested that the early life environment influences the health of the offspring through induced changes in epigenetic marks and there has been much recent interest in the possibility that the transmission of effects (from both mother and father) could occur through epigenetic effects that are transmitted through the germline. Although there is no universal definition of the term ‘epigenetic’ [108,109], in the DOHaD field the term has often been used to include DNA methylation, histone modifications and/or non-coding RNA. Many animal and human studies reporting associations between the early life environment and disease risk have reported differences in such epigenetic marks in the offspring [110] although there is, as yet, little evidence that these differences are causative of disease. Although studies suggest that similar alterations occur in the germline epigenome and that these changes may be responsible for the transmission of effects across generations, the data in support of such transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in mammals remain limited [111].

5. Conclusions

Factors such as poor nutrition, stress and infection during pregnancy have all been associated with adverse effects on fetal neurodevelopment. One important consideration is that the long-term risks associated with exposure to an adverse intra-uterine environment are not evenly distributed across society. Low socio-economic status (SES) is associated with an array of adverse pregnancy outcomes even in countries with established antenatal care programs [112,113] which may in part be due to a reduced awareness of service provision [114]. Women of lower SES have an increased likelihood of exposure to risk factors during pregnancy [115] and SES is independently associated with future cognition [115,116]. In higher [117] as well as lower- and middle-income countries [118] lower SES is associated with measures of unhealthy behaviour for example poor nutrition, smoking and low levels of physical activity, including amongst women of reproductive age [119]. Increased levels of infections have been seen in pregnant women with a lower SES. For instance, an increased presence of the infectious bacterium H. pylori [120] (which has been linked with hyperemesis gravidarum [121]) and an increase in urinary tract infections [122]. This is important for public health policy since many of the behaviours associated with lower SES are potentially amenable to change. For example, income supplementation, independent of a changing family dynamic or characteristic can reduce the prevalence of clinically relevant externalising behaviours in a low income population [123]. Increasing the provision of support for families in lower socio-economic brackets may be one crucial step in mitigating some of the antenatal risk factors associated with atypical brain development. Such interventions also have the potential to improve help in future generations.

Acknowledgements

Eamon Fitzgerald was supported by a PhD studentship from Medical Research Scotland (PhD-878-2015), in collaboration with Aquila BioMedical, Edinburgh, UK. Kahyee Hor was supported by a Wellcome Trust PhD studentship (203913/Z/16/Z) as part of the Edinburgh Clinical Academic Track. Amanda Drake was supported by the British Heart Foundation Centre of Research Excellence, University of Edinburgh.

References

  • 1.Barker D.J.P., Osmond C. Infant mortality, childhood nutrition, and ischaemic heart disease in England and Wales. Lancet. 1986;327:1077–1081. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(86)91340-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Almond D., Currie J. Killing me softly: the fetal origins hypothesis. J. Econ. Perspect. 2011;25:153. doi: 10.1257/jep.25.3.153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Jiang X., Ma H., Wang Y., Liu Y. Early life factors and type 2 diabetes mellitus. J. Diabetes Res. 2013;2013(485082) doi: 10.1155/2013/485082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Alexander B.T., Dasinger J.H., Intapad S. Fetal programming and cardiovascular pathology. Compr. Physiol. 2015;5:997–1025. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c140036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Maniam J., Antoniadis C., Morris M.J. Early-life stress, HPA Axis adaptation, and mechanisms contributing to later health outcomes. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne). 2014;5(73) doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kim D.R., Bale T.L., Epperson C.N. Prenatal programming of mental illness: current understanding of relationship and mechanisms. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015;17:5. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0546-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Miller J.A. Transcriptional landscape of the prenatal human brain. Nature. 2014;508:199. doi: 10.1038/nature13185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Spiers H. Methylomic trajectories across human fetal brain development. Genome Res. 2015;25:338–352. doi: 10.1101/gr.180273.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Consortium, C.-D. G. of the P. G Genomic relationships, novel loci, and pleiotropic mechanisms across eight psychiatric disorders. Cell. 2019;179:1469–1482.e11. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fitzgerald E., Boardman J.P., Drake A.J. Preterm birth and the risk of neurodevelopmental disorders - is there a role for epigenetic dysregulation? Curr. Genomics. 2018;19:507–521. doi: 10.2174/1389202919666171229144807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schulz L.C. The Dutch hunger winter and the developmental origins of health and disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107:16757. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012911107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Roseboom T., de Rooij S., Painter R. The Dutch famine and its long-term consequences for adult health. Early Hum. Dev. 2006;82:485–491. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Stein Z., Susser M., Saenger G., Marolla F. Nutrition and Mental Performance: Prenatal exposure to the Dutch famine of 1944–1945 seems not related to mental performance at age 19. Science (80-.) 1972;178:708–713. doi: 10.1126/science.178.4062.708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.de Rooij S.R., Wouters H., Yonker J.E., Painter R.C., Roseboom T.J. Prenatal undernutrition and cognitive function in late adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2010;107:16881–16886. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009459107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Reynolds R.M., Godfrey K.M., Barker M., Osmond C., Phillips D.I.W. Stress responsiveness in adult life: influence of mother’s diet in late pregnancy. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2007;92:2208–2210. doi: 10.1210/jc.2007-0071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Gould J.M. Mouse maternal protein restriction during preimplantation alone permanently alters brain neuron proportion and adult short-term memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2018;115:E7398–E7407. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1721876115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.NHS Digital Health survey for England 2017. 2018. https://digital.nhs.uk/data-and-information/publications/statistical/health-survey-for-england/2017 at.
  • 18.Hochner H. Associations of maternal prepregnancy body mass index and gestational weight gain with adult offspring cardiometabolic risk factors. Circulation. 2012;125:1381–1389. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.111.070060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Girchenko P. Maternal early pregnancy obesity and related pregnancy and pre-pregnancy disorders: associations with child developmental milestones in the prospective PREDO study. Int. J. Obes. 2018;42(995–1007) doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0061-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mina T.H. Prenatal exposure to maternal very severe obesity is associated with impaired neurodevelopment and executive functioning in children. Pediatr. Res. 2017;82:47–54. doi: 10.1038/pr.2017.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rodriguez A. Maternal adiposity prior to pregnancy is associated with ADHD symptoms in offspring: evidence from three prospective pregnancy cohorts. Int. J. Obes. 2008;32:550–557. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Reynolds L.C., Inder T.E., Neil J.J., Pineda R.G., Rogers C.E. Maternal obesity and increased risk for autism and developmental delay among very preterm infants. J. Perinatol. 2014;34:688–692. doi: 10.1038/jp.2014.80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Li M. The association of maternal obesity and diabetes with autism and other developmental disabilities. Pediatrics. 2016;137 doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Graf A.E. Maternal high fat diet exposure is associated with increased hepcidin levels, decreased myelination, and neurobehavioral changes in male offspring. Brain Behav. Immun. 2016;58:369–378. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kim D.W., Glendining K.A., Grattan D.R., Jasoni C.L. Maternal obesity leads to increased proliferation and numbers of astrocytes in the developing fetal and neonatal mouse hypothalamus. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2016;53:18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2016.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Thompson J.R. Exposure to a high-fat diet during early development programs behavior and impairs the central serotonergic system in juvenile non-human primates. Front. Endocrinol. (Lausanne). 2017;8(164) doi: 10.3389/fendo.2017.00164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cinquina V. Life-long epigenetic programming of cortical architecture by maternal ‘Western’ diet during pregnancy. Mol. Psychiatry. 2020;25:22–36. doi: 10.1038/s41380-019-0580-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.NICE Guidelines . NICE; 2015. Diabetes in Pregnancy: Management From Preconception to the Postnatal Period. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.McIntyre H.D. Gestational diabetes mellitus. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2019;5:47. doi: 10.1038/s41572-019-0098-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Schaefer-Graf U.M. Patterns of congenital anomalies and relationship to initial maternal fasting glucose levels in pregnancies complicated by type 2 and gestational diabetes. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2000;182:313–320. doi: 10.1016/s0002-9378(00)70217-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Loeken M.R. Current perspectives on the causes of neural tube defects resulting from diabetic pregnancy. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part C Semin. Med. Genet. 2005;135C:77–87. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ji S. Maternal hyperglycemia disturbs neocortical neurogenesis via epigenetic regulation in C57BL/6J mice. Cell Death Dis. 2019;10:211. doi: 10.1038/s41419-019-1438-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Yang, P. et al. Maternal hyperglycemia activates an ASK1–FoxO3a–caspase 8 pathway that leads to embryonic neural tube defects. Sci. Signal. 6, ra74-ra74 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 34.Wang X. Maternal diabetes induces autism-like behavior by hyperglycemia-mediated persistent oxidative stress and suppression of superoxide dismutase 2. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2019;116:23743–23752. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1912625116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Girchenko P. Persistently high levels of maternal antenatal inflammation are associated with and mediate the effect of prenatal environmental adversities on neurodevelopmental delay in the offspring. Biol. Psychiatry. 2020;87:898–907. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Money K.M. Gestational diabetes exacerbates maternal immune activation effects in the developing brain. Mol. Psychiatry. 2018;23:1920–1928. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Maynard T.M., Sikich L., Lieberman J.A., LaMantia A.-S. Neural development, cell-cell signaling, and the "two-hit&quot. Hypothesis of Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2001;27:457–476. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a006887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sappenfield E., Jamieson D.J., Kourtis A.P. Pregnancy and susceptibility to infectious diseases. Infect. Dis. Obstet. Gynecol. 2013;2013(752852) doi: 10.1155/2013/752852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kourtis A.P., Read J.S., Jamieson D.J. Pregnancy and infection. N. Engl. J. Med. 2014;370:2211. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1213566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Atladóttir H.Ó. Maternal infection requiring hospitalization during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2010;40:1423–1430. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1006-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fuller Torrey E., Rawlings R., Waldman I.N. Schizophrenic births and viral diseases in two states. Schizophr. Res. 1988;1:73–77. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(88)90043-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brown A.S. Prenatal rubella, premorbid abnormalities, and adult schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2001;49:473–486. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01068-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Suvisaari J., Haukka J., Tanskanen A., Hovi T., Lönnqvist J. Association between prenatal exposure to poliovirus infection and adult schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1999;156:1100–1102. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.7.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Li D.-K. Genital herpes and its treatment in relation to preterm delivery. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2014;180:1109–1117. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwu242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Di Mascio D. Outcome of coronavirus spectrum infections (SARS, MERS, COVID-19) during pregnancy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. MFM. 2020;100107 doi: 10.1016/J.AJOGMF.2020.100107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Knight M. Characteristics and outcomes of pregnant women hospitalised with confirmed SARS-CoV-2 infection in the UK: a national cohort study using the UK Obstetric Surveillance System (UKOSS) medRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.05.08.20089268. 2020.05.08.20089268. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cauchemez S. Association between Zika virus and microcephaly in French Polynesia, 2013–15: a retrospective study. Lancet. 2016;387:2125–2132. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)00651-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lee Y.H. Maternal bacterial infection during pregnancy and offspring risk of psychotic disorders: variation by severity of infection and offspring sex. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2020;177:66–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18101206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kawai T., Akira S. The role of pattern-recognition receptors in innate immunity: update on toll-like receptors. Nat. Immunol. 2010;11:373–384. doi: 10.1038/ni.1863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Short S.J. Maternal influenza infection during pregnancy impacts postnatal brain development in the rhesus monkey. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;67:965–973. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Willette A.A. Brain enlargement and increased behavioral and cytokine reactivity in infant monkeys following acute prenatal endotoxemia. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;219:108–115. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rose D.R. Long-term altered immune responses following fetal priming in a non-human primate model of maternal immune activation. Brain Behav. Immun. 2017;63:60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.11.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Machado C.J., Whitaker A.M., Smith S.E.P., Patterson P.H., Bauman M.D. Maternal immune activation in nonhuman primates alters social attention in juvenile offspring. Biol. Psychiatry. 2015;77:823–832. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Meyer U. Prenatal poly(I:C) exposure and other developmental immune activation models in rodent systems. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014;75:307–315. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Smith S.E.P., Li J., Garbett K., Mirnics K., Patterson P.H. Maternal immune activation alters fetal brain development through interleukin-6. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:10695–10702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2178-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Choi G.B. The maternal interleukin-17a pathway in mice promotes autism-like phenotypes in offspring. Science (80-.) 2016;351:933–939. doi: 10.1126/science.aad0314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kirsten T.B., Bernardi M.M. Prenatal lipopolysaccharide induces hypothalamic dopaminergic hypoactivity and autistic-like behaviors: repetitive self-grooming and stereotypies. Behav. Brain Res. 2017;331:25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hava G., Vered L., Yael M., Mordechai H., Mahoud H. Alterations in behavior in adult offspring mice following maternal inflammation during pregnancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 2006;48:162–168. doi: 10.1002/dev.20116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lin Y.-L., Wang S. Prenatal lipopolysaccharide exposure increases depression-like behaviors and reduces hippocampal neurogenesis in adult rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2014;259:24–34. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.10.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Lanté F. LateN-acetylcysteine treatment prevents the deficits induced in the offspring of dams exposed to an immune stress during gestation. Hippocampus. 2008;18:602–609. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Yu S., Wen Y., Li J., Zhang H., Liu Y. Prenatal lipopolysaccharide exposure promotes dyslipidemia in the male offspring rats. Front. Physiol. 2018;9:542. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kirsten T.B. Hypoactivity of the central dopaminergic system and autistic-like behavior induced by a single early prenatal exposure to lipopolysaccharide. J. Neurosci. Res. 2012;90:1903–1912. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Fatemi S.H., Folsom T.D. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia, revisited. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:528–548. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Nast I., Bolten M., Meinlschmidt G., Hellhammer D.H. How to measure prenatal stress? A Systematic Review of Psychometric Instruments to Assess Psychosocial Stress during Pregnancy. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2013;27:313–322. doi: 10.1111/ppe.12051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Buffa G. Prenatal stress and child development: a scoping review of research in low- and middle-income countries. PLoS One. 2018;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0207235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Van den Bergh B.R.H., Van Calster B., Smits T., Van Huffel S., Lagae L. Antenatal maternal anxiety is related to HPA-Axis dysregulation and self-reported depressive symptoms in adolescence: a prospective study on the fetal origins of depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:536–545. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kinney D.K., Munir K.M., Crowley D.J., Miller A.M. Prenatal stress and risk for autism. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008;32:1519–1532. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fineberg A.M. Fetal exposure to maternal stress and risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders among offspring: differential influences of fetal sex. Psychiatry Res. 2016;236:91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Grizenko N. Maternal stress during pregnancy. ADHD Symptomatology in Children and Genotype: Gene-Environment Interaction. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;21:9–15. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.O’Connor T.G., Heron J., Golding J., Glover V., ALSPAC Study Team Maternal antenatal anxiety and behavioural/emotional problems in children: a test of a programming hypothesis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2003;44:1025–1036. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Qiu A. Effects of antenatal maternal depressive symptoms and socio-economic status on neonatal brain development are modulated by genetic risk. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:3080–3092. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Phua D.Y. Positive maternal mental health during pregnancy associated with specific forms of adaptive development in early childhood: evidence from a longitudinal study. Dev. Psychopathol. 2017;29:1573–1587. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417001249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.DiPietro J., Ghera M., Costigan K., Hawkins M. Measuring the ups and downs of pregnancy stress. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 2004;25:189–201. doi: 10.1080/01674820400017830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Federenko I.S. The heritability of perceived stress. Psychol. Med. 2006;36:375–385. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Dipietro J.A., Costigan K.A., Sipsma H.L. Continuity in self-report measures of maternal anxiety, stress, and depressive symptoms from pregnancy through two years postpartum. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 2008;29:115–124. doi: 10.1080/01674820701701546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Luoma I. Longitudinal study of maternal depressive symptoms and child well-being. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2001;40:1367–1374. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200112000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Glynn L.M., Wadhwa P.D., Dunkel-Schetter C., Chicz-DeMet A., Sandman C.A. When stress happens matters: effects of earthquake timing on stress responsivity in pregnancy. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 2001;184:637–642. doi: 10.1067/mob.2001.111066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Laplante D.P., Brunet A., Schmitz N., Ciampi A., King S. Project ice storm: prenatal maternal stress affects cognitive and linguistic functioning in 5½-year-old children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2008;47:1063–1072. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31817eec80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Weinstock M. Prenatal stressors in rodents: effects on behavior. Neurobiol. Stress. 2017;6:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Thompson W.R. Influence of prenatal maternal anxiety on emotionality in young rats. Science. 1957;125:698–699. doi: 10.1126/science.125.3250.698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Barzegar M., Sajjadi F.S., Talaei S.A., Hamidi G., Salami M. Prenatal exposure to noise stress: anxiety, impaired spatial memory, and deteriorated hippocampal plasticity in postnatal life. Hippocampus. 2015;25:187–196. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Benoit J.D., Rakic P., Frick K.M. Prenatal stress induces spatial memory deficits and epigenetic changes in the hippocampus indicative of heterochromatin formation and reduced gene expression. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;281:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Lee P.R., Brady D.L., Shapiro R.A., Dorsa D.M., Koenig J.I. Prenatal stress generates deficits in rat social behavior: reversal by oxytocin. Brain Res. 2007;1156:152. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Barros V.G. Prenatal stress and early adoption effects on benzodiazepine receptors and anxiogenic behavior in the adult rat brain. Synapse. 2006;60:609–618. doi: 10.1002/syn.20336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.de Souza M.A. Prenatal stress produces social behavior deficits and alters the number of oxytocin and vasopressin neurons in adult rats. Neurochem. Res. 2013;38:1479–1489. doi: 10.1007/s11064-013-1049-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Frye, C. A., Paris, J. J. Gestational exposure to variable stressors produces decrements in cognitive and neural development of juvenile male and female rats. Curr. Top. Med. Chem. 2011;11:1706–1713. doi: 10.2174/156802611796117649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Yang J., Han H., Cao J., Li L., Xu L. Prenatal stress modifies hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning in young rat offspring. Hippocampus. 2006;16:431–436. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Yeh C.-M., Huang C.-C., Hsu K.-S. Prenatal stress alters hippocampal synaptic plasticity in young rat offspring through preventing the proteolytic conversion of pro-brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) to mature BDNF. J. Physiol. 2012;590:991–1010. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.222042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Khulan B., Drake A.J. Glucocorticoids as mediators of developmental programming effects. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012;26:689–700. doi: 10.1016/j.beem.2012.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Benediktsson R., Calder A.A., Edwards C.R.W., Seckl J.R. Placental 11β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase: a key regulator of fetal glucocorticoid exposure. Clin. Endocrinol. 1997;46(161–166) doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1997.1230939.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Crowther C.A. Maternal intramuscular dexamethasone versus betamethasone before preterm birth (ASTEROID): a multicentre, double-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet. Child Adolesc. Heal. 2019;3:769–780. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30292-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Alexander N. Impact of antenatal synthetic glucocorticoid exposure on endocrine stress reactivity in term-born children. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012;97:3538–3544. doi: 10.1210/jc.2012-1970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Painter R.C., Roseboom T.J., de Rooij S.R. Long-term effects of prenatal stress and glucocorticoid exposure. Birth Defects Res. C. Embryo Today. 2012;96:315–324. doi: 10.1002/bdrc.21021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Ilg L., Klados M., Alexander N., Kirschbaum C., Li S.-C. Long-term impacts of prenatal synthetic glucocorticoids exposure on functional brain correlates of cognitive monitoring in adolescence. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:7715. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26067-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.McGowan P.O., Matthews S.G. Prenatal stress, glucocorticoids, and developmental programming of the stress response. Endocrinology. 2018;159:69–82. doi: 10.1210/en.2017-00896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Räikkönen K. Maternal licorice consumption during pregnancy and pubertal, cognitive, and psychiatric outcomes in children. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2017;185:317–328. doi: 10.1093/aje/kww172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Salomon S., Bejar C., Schorer-Apelbaum D., Weinstock M. Corticosterone mediates some but not other behavioural changes induced by prenatal stress in rats. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2011;23:118–128. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2010.02097.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Rohde W. Acute effects of maternal stress on fetal blood catecholamines and hypothalamic LH-RH content. Exp. Clin. Endocrinol. Diabetes. 1983;82:268–274. doi: 10.1055/s-0029-1210286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Notarangelo F.M., Schwarcz R. Restraint stress during pregnancy rapidly raises kynurenic acid levels in mouse placenta and fetal brain. Dev. Neurosci. 2016;38:458–468. doi: 10.1159/000455228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Gur T.L. Prenatal stress affects placental cytokines and neurotrophins, commensal microbes, and anxiety-like behavior in adult female offspring. Brain Behav. Immun. 2017;64:50–58. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.12.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Murphy S.K. Maternal infection and stress during pregnancy and depressive symptoms in adolescent offspring. Psychiatry Res. 2017;257:102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Baird D. Changing problems and priorities in obstetrics. BJOG An Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 1985;92:115–121. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.1985.tb01062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Emanuel I., Filakti H., Alberman E., Evans S.J. Intergenerational studies of human birthweight from the 1958 birth cohort. 1. Evidence for a multigenerational effect. Br. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 1992;99:67–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.1992.tb14396.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Kaati G., Bygren L.O., Pembrey M., Sjöström M. Transgenerational response to nutrition, early life circumstances and longevity. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2007;15:784–790. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201832. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kaati G., Bygren L.O., Edvinsson S. Cardiovascular and diabetes mortality determined by nutrition during parents’ and grandparents’ slow growth period. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2002;10:682–688. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5200859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Ronovsky M. Maternal immune activation transgenerationally modulates maternal care and offspring depression-like behavior. Brain Behav. Immun. 2017;63:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2016.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Moisiadis V.G., Constantinof A., Kostaki A., Szyf M., Matthews S.G. Prenatal glucocorticoid exposure modifies endocrine function and behaviour for 3 generations following maternal and paternal transmission. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:11814. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11635-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Arimondo P.B., Barberousse A., Pontarotti G. The many faces of epigenetics Oxford, December 2017. Epigenetics. 2019;14:623–631. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2019.1595298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Lappalainen T., Greally J.M. Associating cellular epigenetic models with human phenotypes. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2017;18:441–451. doi: 10.1038/nrg.2017.32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Godfrey K.M., Costello P.M., Lillycrop K.A. 2016. Development, Epigenetics and Metabolic Programming; pp. 71–80. (Nestle Nutr. Inst. Workshop Ser. 85). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Baxter F.A., Drake A.J. Non-genetic inheritance via the male germline in mammals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2019;374:20180118. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Kim M.K. Socioeconomic status can affect pregnancy outcomes and complications, even with a universal healthcare system. Int. J. Equity Health. 2018;17 doi: 10.1186/s12939-017-0715-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Matijasevich A. Association of socioeconomic position with maternal pregnancy and infant health outcomes in birth cohort studies from Brazil and the UK. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2012;66:127–135. doi: 10.1136/jech.2010.108605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Darling E.K. Access to midwifery care for people of low socio-economic status: a qualitative descriptive study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2019;19:416. doi: 10.1186/s12884-019-2577-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Hackman D.A., Farah M.J., Meaney M.J. Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Capron C., Duyme M. Assessment of effects of socio-economic status on IQ in a full cross-fostering study. Nature. 1989;340:552–554. [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Stringhini S. Association of socioeconomic position with health behaviors and mortality. JAMA. 2010;303:1159. doi: 10.1001/jama.2010.297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Allen L. Socioeconomic status and non-communicable disease behavioural risk factors in low-income and lower-middle-income countries: a systematic review. Lancet Glob. Heal. 2017;5:e277–e289. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(17)30058-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ng S.-K., Cameron C.M., Hills A.P., McClure R.J., Scuffham P.A. Socioeconomic disparities in prepregnancy BMI and impact on maternal and neonatal outcomes and postpartum weight retention: the EFHL longitudinal birth cohort study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2014;14:314. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-14-314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.C K. Is lower socio-economic status a risk factor for Helicobacter pylori infection in pregnant women with hyperemesis gravidarum? Turk. J. Gastroenterol. 2004;15:86–89. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Li L. Helicobacter pylori infection is associated with an increased risk of hyperemesis gravidarum: a meta-analysis. Gastroenterol. Res. Pract. 2015;2015 doi: 10.1155/2015/278905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Le J., Briggs G.G., McKeown A., Bustillo G. Urinary tract infections during pregnancy. Ann. Pharmacother. 2004;38:1692–1701. doi: 10.1345/aph.1D630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Costello E.J., Compton S.N., Keeler G., Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology. JAMA. 2003;290:2023. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Early Human Development are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES