Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2020 Sep 16;15(9):e0238702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238702

Deconstructing celebratory acts following goal scoring among elite professional football players

Assaf Lev 1,2,3,*,#, Yair Galily 3,4,#, Omer Eldadi 3,4, Gershon Tenenbaum 3,4
Editor: Christian Stamov Roßnagel5
PMCID: PMC7494092  PMID: 32936807

Abstract

Goal celebration in sport is mostly spontaneous and is manifested via vocal expressions and bodily gestures aimed at communicating emotions. The aim of this study is to deconstruct the celebratory acts among elite professional football players in the European Champions League following scoring a goal, and to capture the multiple acts and functions of the celebrations. In examining the 2018/19 season of the European Champions League tournament, we draw attention to the players’ celebrations and their corresponding social and individual functions. All goals/celebrations (K = 366) were used for the analyses. To analyze the goal celebration acts, a socio-psychological model was established which is comprised of several theories. To describe the goal celebration acts across the competition stages (e.g., preliminary and final), match location (i.e., home or away), time phase (0–15, 15–45, 45–75, 75–90, 90+ minutes), scoring mode (i.e., prior to the goal, after the goal), and players’ continent origin (Europe, Africa, Asia, South/Central, and North America), the number and percent of all the celebratory acts were counted and presented in their respective mode (i.e., single, double, and team). The main findings indicate that (a) most of the goal celebration acts were performed interactively by the scoring player and his teammates, (b) the interactive modes of celebration lasted longer than the modes which were performed non-interactively, (c) the celebration lasted longer following goal scoring in the final stage than in the preliminary stage, (d) the celebration duration lasted the longest time when the goal was scored during the overtime phase (90+ min) of the final but not the preliminary stage, and (e) players from Africa and South America demonstrated religious acts more than their European counterparts. We assert that our conceptual model enables the categorization of a variety of personal and social meanings to the celebrations on the field during the most thrilling moments of the game.

Introduction

Goal celebration is the act of rejoicing at the scoring of a goal. Customarily performed by the scorer, the celebration may include his/her teammates, the team manager, the coaching staff, and/or the followers of the team. The term can refer to the celebration of a goal in general, as well as to specific actions such as a player removing his shirt, thanking God, or performing a somersault.

Frequently, the moments following the scoring of a goal in professional football (soccer) are filled with enthusiastic bodily gestures and unbridled joy. In 1982 FIFA (The Fédération Internationale de Football Association) issued guidelines that the ‘exuberant outbursts of several players at once jumping on top of each other, kissing and embracing, should be banned from the football pitch’. However, according to Burnton [1] this suggestion didn’t work. Players liked kissing each other so much that no amount of official declarations were going to stop them. In 1996 FIFA was forced to back down, revising their guidelines to allow ‘reasonable celebration’ but warning that ‘choreographed celebrations’ were not to be encouraged.

Immediately after scoring a goal the scorer becomes the center of the media’s and spectators’ attention. Utilizing the focus of the camera, the scoring player and his/her teammates celebrate their desired outcome. These precious seconds must be wisely capitalized upon to convey a message aimed at leveraging the player’s career. The object of this study is to capture the multiple acts and functions of the celebrations following goal scoring. By analyzing the 2018/19 season of European Champions League games, we draw attention to the players’ celebrations and the corresponding social and individual functions of these acts.

Celebration after scoring a goal is context dependent. It is usually expressed spontaneously and impulsively but has both individual and social-cultural roles [2]. Celebrations emphasize the loyalty and solidarity of the scoring player to the club for which he/she plays [3]. Several examples of behavior exhibiting solidarity and loyalty are hugging teammates, running to celebrate with fans, provoking the fans of the opposing team, kissing the team shirt’s logo, and hugging the coach and/or the player’s bench teammates. Celebrations are also aimed at promoting personal and social motives, such as strengthening one’s personal elements by promoting bodily capital (e.g., exposing a muscular body by taking off the jersey), revealing an undershirt that contains a message, a religious ritual that praises and/or thanks God, and utilizing screen time for social protest [2]. Some celebrations comprise components of both themes while others focus on one single theme.

The study of post-performance behaviors in soccer is rare [3]. Among the few that do exist, Bornstein and Goldschmidt [4] imply that post-performance behaviors reflect the team’s cohesion. Athletes in different sporting realms reported feeling basic emotions such as cheerfulness, enjoyment, and pride as a result of successful performances [57]. Expression of mood and emotions is transferable among teammates through a contagion process [3, 8, 9], which intensifies following goal scoring, leading to a celebration. The display of emotions following goal scoring affects the player through internal feedback loops [10], and results in cultivating and/or intensifying this emotional state [11]. The aim of this study is to sort out the post goal-scoring celebrations and provide an interpretation of these acts by referring to values such as collectivity and individualism.

To capture the fundamental underlying mechanism of the post-goal celebration and its consequences, we integrate ideas from several cognitive and emotion conceptual frameworks. In general, people interact with the environment via two systems, which Kahneman [12] terms Fast (system 1) and Slow (system 2). The fast system (system 1) relies on perception-action coupling and is intuitive–operating automatically and quickly with little or no perceptual-cognitive effort and without intentional or voluntary control. System 2, (slow), relies on deliberative processes which allocate attention to the effortful mental activities that demand it. The two systems entail cognitive, emotional, and motor expressions in an interactive manner, which is illustrated graphically in Fig 1. These systems interact immediately and consequently to an event which triggers such a response, but is delayed when the response calls for an action or alteration. Celebration in sport is mostly spontaneous and repetitious, and is manifested via vocal and bodily gestures to express emotions. There are a number of studies on nonverbal behaviors in sport (e.g., [3, 1321]), and for the most part (with the exception of [17] and [21]) not in the context of goal celebration. The celebration satisfies internal and personal needs and is rooted in cultural and contextual constraints. Specifically, scoring a goal is a stimulus which triggers the fast system to celebrate spontaneously by using vocal, facial, and bodily expressions (i.e., responses) to satisfy internal needs, and in cases, emphasize cultural roots [2]. However, one cannot ignore the role of the slow system in the celebration process. System 2 is very much related to specific appraisals of various emotions [22]. The significance of the goal depends on the player’s interpretive process, but possibly is mediated by social interactions. In this vein, the celebration is a consequence of expected interactions with the player’s teammates, coaching staff, bench-players, and spectators.

Fig 1. A model depicting the goal celebration process, role, and expression.

Fig 1

The celebration is perceived as an overt expression of emotions mediated by environmental constraints and targeted toward interpersonal and intrapersonal roles.

Goffman’s [23] dramaturgical perspective is considered a critical cultural analysis [24, 25], and it provides a solid conceptual framework for players’ goal celebrations. According to Goffman, social interaction, such as goal celebration, is a stage where the player strives to promote his/her honest and unbiased social identity. In this reality, the performer is a ‘fabricator of impressions involved in the all-too-human task of staging a performance’ [23, p. 252]. Specifically, the player is an actor who has Front and Back regions of behavior. The ‘Front region of behavior’ refers to the place the performance is given, where the actor engages in and performs his/her role for the audience, while the ‘Back region of behavior’ is considered a place where the performer can step out of character and cease to ‘play a part’ for an audience. Moreover, according to Goffman, to construct and maintain a trustworthy and credible impression as part of the Front region, one must be persistent in conveying expression in both the ‘gives’ and ‘gives-off’ channels. The ‘gives’ channel involves, for the most part, the verbal symbols used and conveyed purposefully, while the ‘gives-off’ channel involves a range of various actions transmitted via nonverbal symbols such as bodily gestures and props. The current paper centers on the players’ celebrations (’gives-off’) in front of the fans and cameras (i.e., the ‘front region of behavior’). Detecting the players’ performances on the football field via Goffman’s theoretical perspective is useful, given the fact that:

‘On the dramaturgical dimension, sport competition is defined as a play/spectacle, all actors acting towards putting the competition on stage in front of the public. The focus is on the performance act and on the way sport actors manage to respond to the social constraints and expectations attached to their role’

[26, p. 508].

Though the back and front stage metaphors may illuminate the goal celebration phenomenon, it must also be considered in the context of personal and social psychological perspectives. Most theories of emotions consider the emotion response as one that aims at coping, overcoming and achieving personal objectives [e.g., 2729]. However, emotions are determined largely by culturally supplied aims (see [30] for concrete examples). Specifically, most of the causes of emotional expressions are interpersonally, institutionally, and culturally defined.

Emotions provide information to the self [31] and influence others’ cognition, attitudes, and behaviors [32]. The expression of emotions has consequences for other people, and serves interpersonal and cultural functions along with being internal and reactive phenomena (see [3, 33] for review). According to Parkinson, besides the pre-wired neurological-physiological expressions of emotions, emotional expressions are supplemented and supplanted by social and psychological interpretations, elaborations, and consequences. Moreover, cultures and institutions promote implicit and explicit expectations about emotional expressions. Thus, societal, cultural, and institutional constraints feed directly into the process of determining emotions in the first place [33].

Moreover, relying on Van Kleef’s Emotions as a Social Information (EASI) model [2], Van Kleef, Van Doorn, Heerdink, and Koning [32] claimed that the primary function of emotions at the personal level is engendered by social influence. Accordingly, emotional expressions following the goal scoring produce interpersonal effects by triggering affective reactions (e.g., joy, happiness, pride) and inferential processes in ‘targets’ (e.g., spectators, teammates, rivals, media watchers, etc.). Within the context of soccer playing, the extent of the target’s reaction depends on the information processing depth and the perceived appropriateness of the emotional expressions. Thus, according to the EASI model, emotional expressions maintain personal functions required for adaptation and action readiness [28, 34] but also share a social function which engenders social influence. Van Kleef et al. [32] consider the interpersonal effect of emotions in social encounters such as negotiations, leadership, attitude change, compliance with requests, and conformity. In this context, we view goal celebration as an event of social-cultural function beyond the personal expression of joy, happiness, and the exhibition of superiority over the rival team. In line with Kahneman’s [12] information processing systems, the expression of joy and happiness is spontaneous and fast, while the social functions are deliberate and, in most cases, pre-planned. The two systems feed on each other within the celebration event (see model in Fig 1).

The expression of emotions by players immediately after scoring a goal is a form of communication, in which evaluative representations are made to other people. The response is reflected by physiological-neurological and cognitive outcomes, but the syndrome is driven by social considerations [33]. For example, the majority (three-fourths) of the emotional reactions of people were attributed to the persons’ relationship with other people [35], thus it is conceivable to believe that goal celebration is a personal expression of joy that is also driven by and aims at expressing social messages to others (e.g., spectators, teammates, rivals, non-attendees, etc.). This paper is designed to observe and analyze goal scoring celebration acts (see Fig 1) in 125 matches played in the European Premier League (EPL) during the 2019 season, and to measure their duration, categorize their meaning, nature, and message (i.e., interpersonally, institutionally, and culturally), and contrast these characteristics in the preliminary and final stages of the competition. Moreover, we also consider the effect of time phase during the competitive match, score status before and after the goal, the scoring team (home and away), and the continent origin of the scorer in describing the goal celebration. Overall, the aim is to operationally define the goal celebration characteristics within a more general value system which incorporates collectivity and individualism.

Method

The Champions League (CL) framework

The European Football Cup (now the Champions League) is a seasonal football tournament, established in 1956. The league is open to all 55 UEFA (the Union of European Football Associations) Champions Leagues, as well as teams that finished second through fourth in the Senior Leagues. Prior to the 1992/93 season, the tournament was called the ‘European Cup’. In 1997, teams that finished in second place were also allowed to take part in the competition. The 1999/2000 season ushered in a less stringency system under the terms of the Champions League entry, with the first three UEFA leagues (Spanish League, Italian Serie A, and German Bundesliga) sending four teams each, while the next three in the rankings (Premier League, French League 1, and the Dutch Premier League) sent three delegates each.

According to Frick [36], since 1960, increasing numbers of football players from Eastern Europe, South America, Africa, and Asia have migrated to the top leagues in Western Europe (England, France, Germany, Italy, and Spain). This phenomenon has been massively fostered by the Bosman ruling. This ruling, or Bosman Law, is an act that changed the face of professional football in Europe (and in many respects the world), stating that players may leave their teams at the end of the contract without compensation, and invalidated the restrictions that prevented teams from sharing more than a certain number of non-local players. As a result of the ruling, issued in late 1995, there were a number of significant changes in European football, most notably the many players from the EU who played outside their country in one of the other EU countries. There has also been a sharp increase in player salaries and contract extensions.

The UEFA Champions League final is the most watched annual sporting event in the world. The final of the 2012/13 season, for example, which featured Borussia Dortmund vs Bayern Munich, was aired in 203 countries and had the highest TV rating of the year worldwide with 360 million viewers. According to UEFA.com, the 2018/19 Final (Liverpool vs Tottenham) attracted 358 million viewers with a season that included players from 66 different nationalities (e.g., 62 from Brazil, 61 from Spain, 55 from France, and 47 from Germany).

Scenarios’ selection

In this study, we observed matches played from late June 2018 till May 2019, with 79 teams (from 54 European associations) taking part in the opening round, three qualifying rounds, and a play-off round; all qualifying rounds (except the preliminary round) are played under a two-legged knockout system (home and away basis).

The six remaining teams entered the group stage, joining 26 teams that qualified in advance. The 32 teams were drawn into eight groups of four teams and played each other in a double round-robin system. Subsequently, 32 teams participated in the UEFA Champions League. To represent the Champions League goal celebration phenomenon, all the 125 matches played in the Premier League during the 2018/19 season were recorded and all the goals/celebrations (K = 366) were used for the analyses. As stated, we sampled all 366 goals scored at the Champions League (exclusive of qualification sequences) from 125 games played. There were no selection criteria, and thus no selection bias: Every UEFA Champions League begins with a double (round-robin) group stage of 32 teams, which since the 2009/10 season is preceded by two qualification sequences for teams (N = 79 in the 2018/9 season) that do not receive direct entry to the tournament. The celebration acts in several analyses were broken down by competition stage–preliminary (group) stage (91 matches) and final (knockout–top 16) stage (27 matches), along with time phase in the competitive match, scoring status before and after the goal, and the goal scorer’s origin.

Coding celebratory behaviors, quality, and trustworthiness

According to Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer [37] there are several methods of coding body movement in nonverbal behavior research, but they claim there is no consensus on a reliable coding system that can be used for the study of emotion expression. They proposed a new integrative method, which they termed ‘the body action and posture coding system’, which comprehensively demonstrated intercoder reliability at three different levels (occurrence, temporal precision, segmentation). Of the three dimensions they proposed for the body action and posture coding system (anatomical, form, and function), we relied mainly on observing the form of the goal celebration acts, but assigned their functions to individual and social roles driven from a sound theoretical foundation, which led to a newly established model that guided our study.

Video clips of the celebration acts were titled immediately after each observation by one of the authors, who has expertise in body expression. This author, who performed the coding, has extensive formal education and experience in teaching, coding, and interpreting body motions of artists, politicians, and instructors. To finalize the coding scheme, a negotiated approach was taken wherein the main coder and the other authors actively discussed the identified codes, while aiming to form a final version with the entire coded celebratory actions [38]. The discussions resulted in a final full agreement about the coded behaviors. Nevertheless, it should be acknowledged that a limitation of the study was that it utilized an ad-hoc coding procedure and there may not be consensus as to its reliability. All the coded behaviors expressed positive emotions of joy, pride, and happiness. However, the coding system did not allow for distinguishing among the designated emotions. The coder observed each act and assigned to it a code and a title. Following the observation of 366 goals, the final celebration acts were condensed by all the authors into coherent categories. The actions were divided into three modes: (1) single mode, which consisted of 14 possible actions performed by the scoring player, (2) double mode, where two players celebrated the goal together, which consisted of four possible actions, and (3) team mode, where more than two players joined the celebration, which consisted of four possible actions. The total number of celebration acts (N = 1190) was divided into 14 single acts (N = 731, 61.4%), four double acts (N = 114, 9.6%) and four team acts (N = 345, 29.0%). The number and percentage of goal celebrations in 366 games and possible 22 celebration modes are presented in Table 1. (All graphics in this paper are original and drawn by the artist, Yehuda Hillman, for this study to demonstrate the celebration acts described above and do not represent any specific individual player or team. The artist waives all rights of artistic ownership and/or royalties for the graphics included in this paper).

Table 1. Number and % of goal celebrations in 366 games and 22 celebration modes.

Celebration Mode/Actions Single Double Team Total
Total # of actions 14 4 4 22
Actions performed M = 1.99 M = 0.31 M = 0.94 M = 3.25
SD = 1.55 SD = 0.61 SD = 0.36 SD = 1.63
61.23% 9.53% 28.92% 100%

To ensure quality and trustworthiness, we followed Tracy’s [39] conceptualization of quality. Our study focused on a new topic with meaningful implications for society, given the significant role soccer plays in society worldwide. It also holds significant value for understanding acts which express joy, pride, and happiness, which have both personal and social roles. A unique model was developed to represent these expressions of emotions in the form of body language. The data obtained herein clarify the relationship between the celebratory action and the individual and social facilitation of these acts. Additionally, to gain a meaningful coherence within our data, the stated study’s objectives were followed, and the selected exploratory research design and data analysis procedures aligned with these objectives. We established a solid theoretical model which was associated with the study’s objectives, methodology, and findings. In the process of data collection, appropriate behavioral coding practices were conducted with full agreement among the main coder and his affiliates.

Statistical analyses

Following data collection, the number of single, double, team, and total celebration actions were summarized. Also, the time (in seconds) of each goal celebration was considered. Descriptive (M and SD) statistics were subjected to one- and two-way factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) considering competitive stage (preliminary vs. final), time phase (1–15 min, 15–45 min, 45–75 min, 75–90 min, 95+ min), scoring state before and after the goal (behind, draw, lead), scoring team (home vs. away), and scorer’s continent of origin (Europe, South America, or Africa) as factors in the analyses. Cohen’s d coefficients were computed to estimate the standardized differences between means when effects were significant (p < .05).

Results

Descriptive data of celebration acts and their duration

To describe the goal celebration acts across the competition stage (e.g., preliminary and final), match location (e.g., home and away), time-phase (0–15, 15–45, 45–75, 75–90, 90+ minutes), scoring mode (e.g., prior to goal, after goal), and players’ continent origin (Europe, Africa, Asia, South/Central and North America), the number and percent of all the celebration acts were counted and presented in their respective mode (e.g., single, double, team). The distribution of single acts is presented in Fig 2.

Fig 2. Percent of single player’s acts (K = 14; # of actions = 731) in 366 goal celebrations.

Fig 2

Index: H–Outstretched hands; K–Glitch; J–Air jump; F–Hand fist; S–Vocal scream; KISS–Emotion expression (kissing/heart); C–Approaching camera; ICA–Pre-determined movement; CM–Spontaneous movement; A–Appealing to audience; R–Religious virtue; ME–Self-aggrandizement; FB–Ball catching; SS–Tapping shirt symbol.

The most frequent dual celebration acts following a goal were: outreaching hands (N = 123, 33.6%), hand fist (N = 109, 29.8%), appealing to audience (N = 96, 26.2%), predetermined movement (N = 91, 24.9%) and religious virtue (N = 64, 17.5%). Similar distribution for the double celebration acts is presented in Fig 3.

Fig 3. Percent of double players’ celebration acts (K = 4; # of actions = 114) in 366 goal celebrations.

Fig 3

Index: PM–Pair movements; RT–Running together; PDM–Predetermined movements; PTP–Pointing toward the passer.

The most frequent double celebration act was pair movements (N = 71, 19.4%), followed by pointing toward the passer (N = 31, 8.5%). Finally, the team celebration acts’ distribution is shown in Fig 4.

Fig 4. Percent of team players’ celebration actions (k = 4; # of actions = 345) in 366 goal celebrations.

Fig 4

Index: TM–Team movements; UTM–Unified team movements; MR–Players run toward audience; JBP–Join bench players.

The most frequent and dominant team celebration act following a goal was team movements (N = 331, 90.4%); in few cases players running toward the audience (N = 9, 2.5%), and joining bench players (N = 4, 1.1%). Since some of the goal celebrations consisted of acts which comprised single, double, and team acts interactively, the distribution of all celebration acts was computed and is presented in Table 2. Along with the celebration occurrence, the mean time of the celebration acts was also computed and presented.

Table 2. Means and SD’s for goal celebration time by celebration mode.

Celebration Mode N Mt SDt
Single 20 22.95 10.59
Double 1 20.00 _
Team 28 19.25 6.63
Single + Double 2 17.50 3.54
Single + Team 196 25.81 10.82
Double + Team 24 23.71 8.25
Single + Double + Team 47 26.34 9.24
Total 318 24.90 10.21

Of the 366 goal celebrations, the video clips of 318 (86.88%) could be used for time duration measurement. Most of the goal celebration acts (n = 196, 61.63%) were performed interactively by single and team modes. The double or single + double celebration modes were very rare. Single and team modes of celebration were also rare (6.29% and 8.8%, respectively). The single + double + team interactive mode of celebration lasted the longest (26.34 s), followed by single + team mode (25.81 s). All the interactive modes of celebration except one lasted longer (23.71 s– 26.34 s) than the modes which were performed non-interactively (19.25 s– 22.95 s). The two cases wherein the acts were performed by a single + double mode lasted the shortest amount of time (17.50 s).

Celebration actions and durations in the preliminary and final competition stages

The number of goal celebration acts, along with their time durations, were calculated and contrasted to each other by considering the competition stage (e.g., preliminary vs. final).

A two-way ANOVA followed by LSD post-hoc mean difference tests were performed to detect significant differences. The descriptive distribution of the goal celebration acts by celebration mode for the two competition stages is presented in Fig 5.

Fig 5. Means and SD’s for number of actions by celebration mode and competition stage (preliminary, final).

Fig 5

The ANOVA revealed non-significant (p > .05) differences between the preliminary and final stages on all celebration modes. The time of the celebration is presented in Fig 6.

Fig 6. Means and SD’s for goal celebration time in the preliminary and final competition stages.

Fig 6

The analysis revealed a significant competition stage effect, F(1,319) = 193.40, p < .001. The celebration lasted longer following goal scoring in the final stage than in the preliminary stage (M = 38.69, SD = 13.88 vs M = 22.09, SD = 6.36, Cohen’s d = 1.54).

Goal celebration acts and duration as a function of time phase and competition stage

Goal celebration duration was measured in 6-time phases during the matches, as follows: 1–15 min, 15–45 min, 45–75 min, 75–90 min, and 90+ min. The 45+ min time phase was deleted due to the small number of goal celebrations occurring in this time phase.

To examine the main and interaction effect of time phase (5 time intervals) and competition stage (i,e, preliminary and final), two-way factorial ANOVAs were performed for celebration duration and each of the celebration acts (e.g., single, double, team, total) separately.

The two-way ANOVA applied to the celebration duration revealed significant competition stage effect, F(1,316) = 180.58, p < .001, time phase effect, F(4,316) = 7.53, p < .01, and competition stage by time phase interaction, F(4,316) = 7.43, p = .01. Celebration duration across all time phases was longer at the final stage than at the preliminary stage (M = 38.69, SD = 13.88 vs. M = 22.09, SD = 6.36, Cohen’s d = 1.54). The time-phase effect revealed that celebration duration increased gradually throughout the game phases, from M = 27.24 s, SD = 9.37 during the 1-15-time phase to M = 45.24 s, SD = 13.47 during the 90+ time phase (see Fig 7). Moreover, the time phase by competitive stage interaction (see Fig 8) revealed that the celebration duration lasted the longest time when the goal was scored during the overtime phase (90+ min) of the final but not the preliminary stage (M90+f = 67.50, SD90+f = 10.61 vs. M90+p = 22.95, SD90+p = 8.09, Cohen’s d = 4.72). The time phase by competition stage interaction effect on celebration time is attributed to the drastic change in celebration time during the overtime (90+ min) phase in the final competition stage.

Fig 7. Means and SDs of celebration duration through 5-time phases of the competitive matches.

Fig 7

Fig 8. Means of goal celebration duration by time phase and competition stage.

Fig 8

The two-way ANOVAS pertaining to the number of acts revealed a non-significant (p > .05) effect for either time phase, competition stage, or interaction. The total number of acts by the two factors is shown in Fig 9. Though non-significant, the number of celebration actions in the final stage seems to increase from the game outset to the final moments of the game but decrease during the overtime phase. In contrast, the number of celebration acts tends to decrease monotonically from the outset to end and the overtime phases of the game during the preliminary stage.

Fig 9. Means of total actions celebration by competition stage (preliminary and final) and time phase.

Fig 9

Goal celebration acts and duration by competitive stage and scoring team (home vs. away)

Two-way ANOVA applied to the celebration time by the scoring team (e.g., home vs. away) and competitive stage (e.g., preliminary vs. final) revealed only a significant (p < .05) competitive stage effect. This effect is presented in Fig 6 and revealed that during the final stage the goal celebration duration is longer than in the preliminary stage.

Similar analyses were performed for the single, double, team, and total number of celebrations acts separately. All the main and interaction effects resulted in a non-significant effect (p > .05), except the two-way scoring team by competitive stage interaction effect on the number of team acts, F(1,362) = 3.70, p < .05. This interaction is presented in Fig 10. In the preliminary stage the away team players exhibited more team celebration acts than the home team players (M = .99, SD = .34 vs M = .92, SD = .37, Cohen’s d = 0.20). In contrast, in the final stage the home team players exhibited more team celebration acts than the away team players (M = .96, SD = .39 vs M = .87, SD = .34, Cohen’s d = 0.25).

Fig 10. Means and SDs for team celebration actions by competition stage (preliminary and final) and scoring team (home vs. away).

Fig 10

Goal celebration duration and game score status

Celebration duration was measured after scoring when the team was behind, in a draw, or leading in scoring position. One-way ANOVA considering the scoring factor revealed a significant effect for scoring status, F(2,318) = 4.09, p < .02. When scoring from a behind state, the celebration lasted a shorter time than when leading or in drawing states (M = 22.03 s, SD = 9.47 vs. M = 25,56 s, SD = 10.93 and M = 26.27 s, SD = 10.18, respectively).

Similar analysis was performed when considering the score status after the goal. The ANOVA revealed significant scoring status effect, F(2,318) = 6.91, p < .001. When the scoring team was behind, the celebration time was much shorter than leading or drawing (M = 18.08 s, SD = 6.51 vs. M = 25.94 s, SD = 10.52 and M = 24.22 s, SD = 10.18. respectively).

Goal celebration acts and game score status

Single celebration acts were measured after scoring when the team was behind, in a draw, or leading. One-way ANOVA considering the scoring factor revealed a significant effect for scoring status, F(2,365) = 3.19, p < .04. The number of single celebration acts was higher when the score prior to the goal was a draw than when the team was behind or in lead positions (M = 2.24, SD = 1.63 vs. M = 1.79, SD = 1.43 and M = 1.85, SD = 1.50, respectively). Similar analysis pertaining to the double celebration acts revealed non-significant effect (p > .05). However, a significant score status was revealed for the team acts, F(2,365) = 4.73, p < .01. The number of team celebration acts was lower when the score of the team before the goal occurred was behind than when the team was in a draw or a lead position (M = .84, SD = .46 vs. M = .95, SD = .39 and M = .99, SD = .23, respectively). Consequently, the total number of celebration acts was different with respect to score status, F(2,365) = 3.76, p < .02. The number of celebration acts was the lowest when the team was behind than when it was in a draw or in a lead position (M = 2.88, SD = 1.54 vs. M = 3.48, SD = 1.66 and M = 3.22, SD = 1.60, respectively).

Considering the scoring status after the goal, the analyses pertaining to the single and the double celebration acts were non-significant (p > .05). However, the analysis for the teams acts was significant, F(2,365) = 7.01, p < .001. When the goal resulted in a lead position, the number of the team celebration acts was greater than when drawing or staying behind (M = .97, SD = .32 vs. M = .91, SD = .40 and M = .71, SD = .53, respectively). A similar analysis for the total number of celebration acts revealed a significant scoring status effect, F(2,365) = 4.195, p < .02. When the goal resulted in a lead position the number of the celebration acts was greater than when drawing or staying behind (M = 3.36, SD = 1.64 vs. M = 3.09, SD = 1.66 and M = .2.46, SD = 1.23, respectively).

Celebration actions by players of various continent origins

To designate the goal celebration acts to the players’ original culture, all the single, double, and team acts were divided by the players’ continent origin. These data are shown in Fig 11. Celebration acts which occurred in less than 20% of the cases were designated in white. Players were assigned to one of three continents: Europe, Africa, or South America. The number of goal celebration acts for single, double, and team modes were expressed in counts and percentage. The acts which were used marginally by all the players were deleted and were not included in Fig 11. As one may notice, players of all continent origins were quite similar in celebrating in pairs (PM) and together with their teammates (TM). Pertaining to single actions, African players celebrate the goal via single acts such as outstretched hands, appealing to the audience, and religious virtue. South American players prefer single acts such as hand fists, outstretched hands, predetermined movements, appealing to the audience, and religious virtue. European players’ most-used single actions are outstretched hands, hand fists, predetermined movements, and appealing to the audience. Thus, outstretched hands and appealing to the audience are common, frequently used single celebration acts cross-culturally, while African players perform fewer air jumps, hand fists, vocal screams, expressions of emotion (kissing/heart fingers), and predetermined movements, and South Americans use fewer air jumps, vocal screams, and emotion expressions (kissing/heart fingers). Of note, Europeans refrain from religious virtue more than their African and South American counterparts.

Fig 11. Celebration actions’ frequency by players’ continent origin.

Fig 11

Discussion

The aim of this paper was to deconstruct the celebratory acts among elite professional football players in the European Champions League following scoring a goal. By analyzing the celebration from a socio-psychological perspective, we established an interactive model which is comprised of several theories. Considering the model as context dependent (e.g., preliminary and final football matches), the general contribution of the model is centered on its capacity to dismantle and to comprehend mainly the overt performances of goal celebration among football players in different stages and timing.

It is commonly agreed that goal celebration in sport is mostly spontaneous, repetitious, and manifested via vocal and bodily gestures aimed at expressing emotions. Expression of emotion is an act which signifies achieving personal and social objectives [2729], which are largely determined by culturally driven motives [30]. We examined the goal scoring festivity in a multi-layered background by looking into single, dual, and team celebrations.

The findings revealed that the most frequent single celebration is outstretching hands followed by a fist display and appealing to the audience, suggesting there are both personal and collective festivities which correspond to the notion of personal and collective roles that emotions share (e.g., [3, 16, 20]). While the first two acts emphasize the individual aspect (‘I scored the goal, look at me!’), the latter stresses that the scorer is an integral part of a much bigger entity (‘I did it for you people and the club!’). Similarly, the most frequent dual celebration act was expressed as pair players moving together as one, followed by pointing toward the passer indicating ‘it isn’t just me’. Both cases correspond to the newly created model (Fig 1) which guides our observations wherein overt behavior such as body gestures result from goal scoring (i.e., the stimulus). The celebration which followed evoked basic intrapersonal feelings such as cheerfulness, joy, and pride, along with interpersonal messages of loyalty, solidarity, and team cohesion, which serve cultural and interpersonal functions [2, 4, 32].

Drawing on Goffman’s [23] theoretical perspective, such goal celebration (‘gives-off’) is a stage where the player strives to manage his impression as part of his ‘front region’, and performs his role for the audience. Yet such behavior is also part of a collective action performed not just by individuals or pairs. As football is a team sport, the collective–both in sporting and social acts–plays a critical role in any festivity/celebration. As Goffman states:

‘Individuals may be bound together formally or informally into an action group in order to further like or collective ends by any means available to them. In so far as they co-operate in maintaining a given impression, using this device as a means of achieving their ends, they constitute what has here been called a team.’

[23, p. 84]

Our findings support the notion of the social role associated with expressions of emotions, in that team movements were by far more evident following goal scoring. In this respect, goal celebration emphasizes the loyalty and solidarity of both the scoring player and his teammates to the club for whom he plays. To a lesser extent, team celebrations were also joined by bench/staff members. However, it must be considered that scoring a goal is usually an act that was pre-planned by several players, and thus the celebration is an immediate response mediated by social interactions with the scorer’s teammates, coaching staff, bench players, and spectators. Moreover, most of the goal celebration acts (n = 196, 61.63%) were performed interactively by singles and teammates. As Moll et al. [3] shows, and in accordance with emotional contagion postulation [8], our findings revealed that the post-performance emotions expressed by an individual were transferred spontaneously to the scorer’s teammates through the intended induction of emotional states and associated behavioral attitudes [40]. A social context, such as scoring a goal, encourages the expression of emotions in people witnessing these emotions [33], who share a strong identification with the club. The expression of an emotional state in one person transfers to the experience of a comparable emotion in persons perceiving the expression, especially in spectators who identify with the action and emotions of joy, happiness, and pride [5, 6]. Within this process, emotions influence other peoples’ emotions, manners, and conduct in a similar manner [3, 33].

While our analysis revealed non-significant (p > .05) differences between the preliminary and final stages of competition on all celebration modes, the goal celebration lasted significantly longer following goal scoring in the final stage of the tournament than in the preliminary stage. Corresponding to our model, during the final stages, both intrapersonal (pride, joy) and interpersonal (situational, contextual, and media watchers) aspects are comprised, since at this stage the knockout phase (16 teams/quarterfinal) in the tournament is decisive. In the 2018/19 season, for example, the VAR (video assisting referee) was used for the first time from that stage onwards. Additionally, our findings revealed that the celebration duration lasted the longest (by 50%) when the goal was scored during the overtime phase (90+ min) of the final competition stage, but not during the preliminary stage. The goal scored is a decisive and imperative one because the chances are slim for the score to be overturned, due to time constraints. This makes the celebration ‘sweeter’ and considered to be a conclusive act.

Where home and away celebrations were considered in the current study, it was revealed that in the preliminary stage the away team players displayed more team celebration acts than the home team players. In contrast, in the final competition stage the home team players exhibited more team celebration acts than the away team players. Moreover, the number of celebration acts was the lowest when the team was behind rather than when it was in a draw or lead position. Players in a behind position are eager to continue the game, in order to score additional goals and not ‘waste’ time for celebration. Thus, goal celebration is expressed more frequently under a game condition where superiority over the rivals is evident, but not under a condition where superiority is not evident. Football matches must be considered as special social contexts where the spontaneous and pre-planned celebrations (fast and slow systems [12]) and expression of emotions are unique and context dependent. As such, environmental, social, and cultural issues must be taken into account when including celebrations and their related emotional expressions within this context [32].

Considering the cultural perspective, players of all continent origins were quite alike, but also unique in celebrating in pairs and together with their teammates (TM). This suggests that there are cultural differences and functions at work [2, 33]. Concerning single actions, African players celebrated the goal via single acts such as outstretched hands, appealing to the audience, and religious virtue. South American players preferred other single acts such as hand fist, outstretched hands, predetermined movement, appealing to the audience, and religious virtue, and European players mostly used single acts such as outstretched hands, hand fist, predetermined movement, and appealing to audience. Hence, outstretching hands and appealing to the audience are communal and habitually used single celebration acts cross-culturally, while African players used fewer air jump, hand fist, vocal scream, emotion expression (kissing/ heart fingers), and pre-determined movement, and South Americans used less air jump, vocal scream, and motion expression (kissing/heart fingers). Additionally, European players refrained from religious virtues more than their African and South American counterparts. The act of celebration on the field brings the players’ cultural background to the ‘front stage’ [23]. Playing in the Champions League molds players from different continents into more of a homogenized group, where they shed some of their native customs; however, during times of celebration, players tend to maintain their cultural backgrounds. An example of the homogenization of football players can be found in Acheampong’s [41] study regarding the behavioral changes in African players post migration to European professional football. In Acheampong’s study, he notes that African players in Europe distance themselves from their native locales after integrating into the European high-class group. However, in the context of maintaining their culture, during times of celebration players from Africa and South America demonstrate performances of religion far more than their European counterparts. This may reflect the fact that the nonreligious population of Europe is far greater than that of Africa or South America (see [42]). After all, sports cannot be disentangled from larger social practices such as religion and politics. In both Africa and South America, sport is intertwined with control of the body and space, and has become closely associated with social identity and the expression of a people’s social character [43, 44]. For example, in Africa sports serve to teach moral values such as religion, and ‘the maintenance and transformation of identities can be examined beautifully through sports’ [44, p. 337].

The conceptual model presented in this study enables the categorization of a variety of personal and social meanings to the celebrations on the field during the most thrilling moments of the game. Such moments are habitually taken for granted by fans and scholars. As Sekerka and Fredrickson [45] assert, establishing a positive emotional climate in organizations adds to growth and eventually better performance. Our findings illuminate how, in a team sports setting–namely football, through the process of emotional contagion, the cohesion of teammates may directly benefit from expressed positive emotions following scoring a goal.

As a final point, we strongly believe that our model can also be utilized in other team sports, such as hockey and American football. Nevertheless, there were several limitations that must be acknowledged. First, our model does not cover covert behaviors that may help shed light on both individual and team festivity. Examining the pulse, blood pressure, brain activity, and level of sweat, and respiration level are examples that may help us better understand the celebration act. Since the present data were collected during a single tournament (i.e., Champions League 2018–19), its generalization to other leagues matches (English Premier League, Bundesliga, etc.) or other tournaments (Football World Cup) must be further explored.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Art Director Yehuda Hillman for his graphic art; two anonymous reviewers for their invaluable comments, and Shaul Kashtan plus Ori Pnini from SportsMatrix for their expert support with the data collection process and building of the data set.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Burnton S. Explain … goal celebrations. The Guardian online. Retrieved Nov, 19th, 2019 from https://www.theguardian.com/football/2000/feb/13/newsstory.sport5
  • 2.Van Kleef GA. How emotions regulate social life: The emotions as social information (EASI) model. Curr. Directions Psychol Sci. 2009; 18(3): 184–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Moll T, Jordet G, Pepping GJ. Emotional contagion in soccer penalty shootouts: Celebration of individual success is associated with ultimate team success. J Sports Sci. 2010; 28(9): 983–992. 10.1080/02640414.2010.484068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bornstein G, Goldschmidt C. Post-scoring behavior and team success in football In Myths and facts about football: The economics and psychology of the world’s greatest sport. Newcastle upon Tyne, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing; 2008, pp.113–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cerin E, Barnett A. A processual analysis of basic emotions and sources of concerns as they are lived before and after a competition. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2006; 7(3): 287–307. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ruiz MC, Hanin YL. Metaphoric description and individualized emotion profiling of performance states in top karate athletes. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2004; 16(3): 258–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Szabo A, Bak M. Exercise-induced affect during training and competition in collegiate soccer players. Eur Yearbook Sport Psychol. 1999; 3: 91–104. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hatfield E, Cacioppo JT, Rapson RL. Emotional contagion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kelly JR, Barsade SG. Mood and emotions in small groups and work teams. Organiz Behav Hum Decis Processes. 2001; 86(1): 99–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Price TF, Peterson CK, Harmon-Jones E. The emotive neuroscience of embodiment. Motivat Emot. 2012; 36(1): 27–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Moesch K, Kenttä G, Kleinert J, Quignon-Fleuret C, Cecil S, Bertollo M. FEPSAC position statement: Mental health disorders in elite athletes and models of service provision. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2018; 38, 61–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Macmillan; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bijlstra G, Furley P, Nieuwenhuys A. The power of nonverbal behavior: Penalty-takers’ body language influences impression formation and anticipation performance in goalkeepers in a simulated soccer penalty task. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2020; 21(2): 137–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Brimmell J, Parker JK, Furley P, Moore LJ. Nonverbal behavior accompanying challenge and threat states under pressure. 2018; Psychol Sport Exerc. 39: 90–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Furley P, Dicks M, Memmert D. Nonverbal behavior in soccer: The influence of dominant and submissive body language on the impression formation and expectancy of success of soccer players. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2015; 34(1): 61–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Furley P, Moll T, Memmert D. “Put your hands up in the air”? The interpersonal effects of pride and shame expressions on opponents and teammates. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015; 6: 1361 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Furley P, Schweizer G. The expression of victory and loss: Estimating who’s leading or trailing from nonverbal cues in sports. J Nonverbal Behav. 2014a; 38(1): 13–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Furley P, Schweizer G. “I’m pretty sure that we will win!”: The influence of score-related nonverbal behavioral changes on the confidence in winning a basketball game. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2014b; 36(3): 315–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Furley P, Schweizer G. Nonverbal communication of confidence in soccer referees: An experimental test of Darwin’s leakage hypothesis. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016; 38(6): 590–597. 10.1123/jsep.2016-0192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Greenlees I, Bradley A, Holder T, Thelwell R. The impact of opponents’ non-verbal behaviour on the first impressions and outcome expectations of table-tennis players. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2005; 6(1): 103–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Moesch K, Kenttä G, Bäckström M, Mattsson CM. Exploring nonverbal behaviors in elite handball: How and when do players celebrate? J Appl Sport Psychol. 2015; 27(1): 94–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Smith CA, Lazarus RS. Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognit Emot. 1993; 7(3–4): 233–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goffman E. The presentation of self in everyday life. London: Penguin; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Birrell S, Donnelly P. Reclaiming Goffman: Erving Goffman’s influence on the sociology of sport In: Giulianotti R, editor. Sport and modern social theorists. London: Macmillan; 2004, pp. 49–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Lev A. Distance runners in a dys-appearance state–Revising the perception of pain and suffering in times of bodily distress. Qual Res Sport Exerc Health. 2020: 1–15. 10.1080/2159676X.2020.1734647 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dumitriu DL. The social dramaturgy of sport: Towards an integrative Goffmanian model. Revista Romana de Sociologie. 2014; 25(5/6): 493–511. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Carver CS, Scheier MF. Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: a control-process view. Psychol Rev. 1990; 97(1): 19–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Frijda NH. The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mandler G. Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: W.W. Norton; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Markus HR, Kitayama S. A collective fear of the collective: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality Soc Psychol Bull. 1994; 20(5): 568–579. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Schwarz N, Clore GL. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive functions of affective states. J Personal Social Psychol. 1983; 45(3): 513. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Van Kleef GA, Van Doorn EA, Heerdink MW, Koning LF. Emotion is for influence. Eur Rev Soc Psychol. 2011; 22(1): 114–163. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Parkinson B. Emotions are social. Brit J Psychol. 1996; 87(4): 663–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lazarus RS. Cognition and motivation in emotion. Amer Psychologist. 1991; 46(4): 352–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Shaver PR, Wu S, Schwartz JC. Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation In Clark MS, editor. Review of personality and social psychology, No. 13, Emotion. London: Sage; 1992; pp. 175–212. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Frick B. Globalization and factor mobility: The impact of the “Bosman-Ruling” on player migration in professional soccer. J Sports Economics. 2009; 10(1): 88–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dael N, Mortillaro M, Scherer KR. The Body Action and Posture Coding System (BAP): Development and reliability. J Nonverbal Behav. 2012. 36(2), 97–121. 10.1007/s10919-012-0130-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Garrison DR, Cleveland-Innes M, Koole M, Kappelman J. Revisiting methodological issues in transcript analysis: Negotiated coding and reliability. The Internet and Higher Educ. 2006; 9(1): 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Tracy SJ Qualitative quality: Eight “big-tent” criteria for excellent qualitative research. Qualitative Inquiry. 2010; 6(10): 837–851. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Schoenewolf G. Emotional contagion: Behavioral induction in individuals and groups. Mod Psychoanal. 1990; 15(1): 49–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Acheampong EY. How does professional football status challenge African players’ behaviour? Soccer & Society. 2018; February 17: 1–14, 10.1080/14660970.2018.1541797 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.World Economic Forum website. 2019. https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2019/03/this-is-the-best-and-simplest-world-map-of-religions.
  • 43.Arbena LJ. Latin America In: Coakley J, Dunning E, editors. Handbook of sports studies. London: Sage; 2012, pp. 548–551. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Vidacs B. Through the prism of sports: Why should Africanists study sports? Africa Spectrum. 2006; 41(3): 331–349. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sekerka LE, Fredrickson BL. Establishing positive emotional climates to advance organizational transformation In: Ashkanasy NM, Cooper CL, editors. New horizons in management. Research companion to emotion in organizations. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing; 2008, pp. 531–545. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

14 Apr 2020

PONE-D-20-00271

Deconstructing celebratory acts following goal scoring among elite professional football players

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lev,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by May 23 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

Dear Dr Lev,

I truly hope all is well with you!

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE.

Your paper certainly has merit, as you can see from the attached review. Similar to the reviewer, however, I think that information is currently missing that is essential for an informed decision on your manuscript. Before we can proceed with your manuscript, I therefore ask you to address the most critical questions.

Specifically, please give more information on

1) the sample: were all matches of an entire Champions League season analysed? More information on this is needed to convince readers that there was no selection bias and that the sample is representative.

2) the coding of the celebratory behaviour. As the reviewer notes, coding the kind of whole body movements that you studied is highly challenging. From the manuscript, it cannot be judged if the person who did the coding had had sufficient training, even more so as there is a vast literature on behavioural coding that you do not refer to. Related to this, please explain why you chose not to refer to much of the relevant literature in setting up this research (see review for details).

Please respond at your next convenience, preferably before 20th April. Based on your information, we will decide without further delay on the next steps in the review process.

Happy to answer all your questions.

Best regards,

Christian.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that Figures 2, 3, 4 includes an image of a patient / participant / in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

3. Please ensure that all your conclusions are sufficiently supported by data and/or references. For example, you have stated that:

“However, in the context of maintaining their culture, during times of celebration, players from Africa and South America demonstrate performances of religion far more than their European counterparts. This is given the fact that the nonreligious population of Europe is far greater than that of Africa or South America (see World Economic Forum website, 2019).” But have not proven this link in the context of your research; thus, this should be presented as a hypothesis not as an statement.

4. Thank you for including your ethics statement:  "It was approved by the University’s ethical committee and sent only to the PLOS ONE

journal."

Please amend your current ethics statement to include the full name of the ethics committee/institutional review board(s) that approved your specific study.

Once you have amended this/these statement(s) in the Methods section of the manuscript, please add the same text to the “Ethics Statement” field of the submission form (via “Edit Submission”).

For additional information about PLOS ONE ethical requirements for human subjects research, please refer to http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research

5. Please include your tables as part of your main manuscript and remove the individual files. Please note that supplementary tables (should remain/ be uploaded) as separate "supporting information" files

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The present research attempts to deconstruct the celebratory behavior of soccer players based on televised recordings of the 2018-2019 Champions League season.

I found several things to like about the present report. Most importantly, this is an understudied topic and I believe the immense documentation of sports events is well suited to advance the empirical study of e.g. emotions and nonverbal behavior in an ecologically valid way. However, I do have some substantial reservations regarding the report at present. Potentially some of these issues could be addressed in a revision.

Major comments

1) The introduction could be more focused. Please make sure to be explicit about the rationale of the paper and the aim(s) of this research. Also, if you do decide to conduct Null-Hypothesis-Significance-Testing, make sure to report hypotheses based on sound theory.

2) An important issue in this paper regards sampling celebratory behavior in soccer. Where all matches of an entire season of the Champions League analyzed? More information on this is needed to convince readers that there was no selection bias in the study and that the sample studied is representative considering the claims the authors would like to make.

3) The most important problem with this paper at present is the coding of the celebratory behavior. This is not a trivial issue as coding whole body movements in non-standardized real-life contexts that were filmed for different durations and from different perspectives is highly challenging. Hence, it is insufficient to state that one person was trained to code the body language. There is a vast literature base on behavioral coding in psychology. But the authors do not refer to any of this literature and I am skeptical if the method used in this research is reliable. The authors need to substantially elaborate on this. This information is essential for me to make an informed recommendation concerning publication of this research.

3b) A related point. The authors seem to have missed a lot of relevant literature in setting up this research, especially on nonverbal behavior coding and nonverbal behavior in sports.

Minor comments:

1) At the end of the introduction you state that you analyzed 125 matches in the European Premier League during 2019. Champions League seems the less confusing description here.

2) It’s Kahneman not Khanman. By the way, dual-process theorizing is quite common in sports and the authors might want to incorporate some of the dual-process work in sports into their manuscript.

3) Is the historical information in the section “the Champions League framework” necessary? This section seems not very important towards the papers aims.

4) This is not a very important point, but is it correct that “The UEFA Champions League final is the most watched annual sporting event in the world”? I thought this was the Super Bowl.

5) I don’t understand how 79 teams (from 54 European associations) can be correct

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Sep 16;15(9):e0238702. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238702.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


14 May 2020

Given the fact that the response letter to reviewers is formatted in tables, the system does not allow it to be pasted in this box. Response letter to reviewers is attached as file in this submission.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Letter for reviewers (1).docx

Decision Letter 1

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

22 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-00271R1

Deconstructing celebratory acts following goal scoring among elite professional football players

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lev,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

As one minor revision, to avoid a potential misunderstanding, please acknowledge in the manuscript that a limitation of the study was that it utilized an ad-hoc coding procedure and we do not know how reliable the coding was. On p. 10 you write "According to Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer (2012) ... there is no consensus on a reliable coding system", which may be taken to imply that intercoder reliability may not be determined; however, Dael and colleagues did just that in the quoted paper. The second minor change concerns the original data that should be in English. Please change this.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 04 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Sara Fuentes Perez, PhD

Staff editor

On behalf of:

Christian Stamov Roβnagel 

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I reviewed the initial submission of this manuscript and found much to like about the study. I think the authors have responded well and professionally to my comments. This has resulted in an improved manuscript. As I consider this to be interesting and novel data I think this manuscript should be published. I do have some minor suggestions.

My only more substantial concern still regards the coding of the video material. As I have done this a lot (or tried to do this on videos from sports recordings) I know that this is a problematic issue. I think the authors should acknowledge in the manuscript that a limitation of the study was that it utilized an ad-hoc coding procedure and we do not know how reliable the coding was. Also, the authors could give the video clips to a second coder and asses intercoder reliability with the first coder. Would this be possible?

Two additional minor suggestions:

1) It would be helpful if the original data file would be in English. Please change this.

2) Would it be possible to upload the videos of the celbrations (e.g. OSF page)? This would help other researchers to build on this work. Or do additional coding of the material.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

24 Aug 2020

Deconstructing celebratory acts following goal scoring among elite professional football players

PONE-D-20-00271R2

Dear Dr. Lev,

Thank you for your revised version. I've really enjoyed working with you during the review process!

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Christian Stamov Roßnagel

27 Aug 2020

PONE-D-20-00271R2

Deconstructing celebratory acts following goal scoring among elite professional football players

Dear Dr. Lev:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Mr Christian Stamov Roßnagel

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Letter for reviewers (1).docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES