Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2020 Sep 16;15(9):e0233942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233942

Age-related slowing down in the motor initiation in elderly adults

Nikita S Frolov 1,*,#, Elena N Pitsik 1,#, Vladimir A Maksimenko 1,#, Vadim V Grubov 1,2, Anton R Kiselev 2, Zhen Wang 3, Alexander E Hramov 1,2,#
Editor: Mukesh Dhamala4
PMCID: PMC7494367  PMID: 32937652

Abstract

Age-related changes in the human brain functioning crucially affect the motor system, causing increased reaction time, low ability to control and execute movements, difficulties in learning new motor skills. The lifestyle and lowered daily activity of elderly adults, along with the deficit of motor and cognitive brain functions, might lead to the developed ambidexterity, i.e., the loss of dominant limb advances. Despite the broad knowledge about the changes in cortical activity directly related to the motor execution, less is known about age-related differences in the motor initiation phase. We hypothesize that the latter strongly influences the behavioral characteristics, such as reaction time, the accuracy of motor performance, etc. Here, we compare the neuronal processes underlying the motor initiation phase preceding fine motor task execution between elderly and young subjects. Based on the results of the whole-scalp sensor-level electroencephalography (EEG) analysis, we demonstrate that the age-related slowing down in the motor initiation before the dominant hand movements is accompanied by the increased theta activation within sensorimotor area and reconfiguration of the theta-band functional connectivity in elderly adults.

Introduction

Healthy aging affects neural processes by changing the neurochemical and structural properties of the brain [1]. It determines the cognitive and motor performance decline during a daily activity of elderly adults and negatively influences the quality of their life. The markers of age-related neural impairments are observed at the behavioral level as slowing of the reaction time (RT), reduced motor control and coordination, etc. [2, 3].

Upper limbs represent the most active part of the human motor system; thus, the degradation of its functioning with age is the most prominent [4]. Plenty of studies report difficulties in accomplishing complex motor tasks related to the deficit of hand movement coordination, ability to control force, execute sequential actions, learn new motor skills, etc. [2, 5, 6]. The motor performance decline while executing fine motor tasks is also well-documented [7, 8]. Several studies report that the level of beta-band (15-30 Hz) oscillations is a relevant marker of a decreased motor performance in healthy aging and disease [915]. Particularly, an increased movement-related beta desynchronization (MBRT) was linked with a greater GABAergic inhibitory activity in the primary motor cortex and suggested to influence the motor plasticity of elderly subjects [911]. A tentative relation between peri-movement beta-band desynchronization and motor performance was shown in [12, 13]. Also, a decreased post-movement beta rebound (PMBR) within the medial prefrontal cortex of elderly adults indicated an impaired cognitive control of stimulus-induced motor tasks [14].

Besides, a broad literature link the age-related motor performance decline with an over-activation of the motor and prefrontal area of the human brain, which control the motor execution process [1619]. Specifically, the recruitment of additional ipsilateral motor regions in elderly adults is supposed to provide a compensatory mechanism that supports overcoming the age-related structural changes in the human brain [20, 21]. On the one hand, it helps to maintain the performance of executed motor actions. On the other hand, this mechanism demands more neuronal resources and, therefore, slows the motor response. Also, several studies relate the over-activation of cortical areas to the ‘use-dependent plasticity’ [22], which is supposed to underlie dedifferentiation of brain functions in advanced age. In the context of the motor system, it is manifested as a developed ambidexterity, i.e., a loss of the dominant limb advances [8, 23].

While the age-related differences cortical activation directly related to motor execution and control is extensively studied, less is known about the effect of healthy aging on the motor planning phase and its influence on RT. Exploring these mechanisms is crucial to deeper understand motor control in humans. Motor planning is also subjected to the age-related changes due to the following: (i) motor initiation process involves many higher cognitive functions such as sensory processing, working memory, motor embodiment, and sensorimotor integration [2427], which are known to decline strongly with age; (ii) the theta activity underlying the majority of these processes exhibits significant age-related changes—abnormally increased theta activity in elderly people indicates subjective cognitive dysfunction and suspected dementia [28, 29].

Based on the above, we hypothesize that the age-related changes in the motor planning mechanism also affect the slowing of the motor initiation phase in elderly adults. To address the issue, we considered the differences in cortical activity during the controlled execution of fine motor tasks between elderly adults and young adults using electroencephalography (EEG). Consistent with the dedifferentiation theory [8, 23], we found that the motor cortex of younger adults activated much faster during the dominant hand task, while in elderly adults, the time required for motor activation was equal for both hands and approached the level of the non-dominant hand of younger adults. Further, as expected, we found the significant differences in cortical activation during the time interval preceding the motor action. In elderly adults, as well as in young adults performing the non-dominant hand task, we observed the increased theta-band power in the frontal, central, and central-parietal EEG sensor rows, whereas theta-activation was insignificant in young adults during the dominant hand task. Finally, based on the results of between-subject functional connectivity analysis, we revealed that motor planning involves different types of cortical interactions in young adults and elderly adults.

Materials and methods

Participants

Two groups of healthy volunteers, including 10 elderly adult subjects (EA group; age: 65±5.69 (MEAN±SD); range: 55-72; 4 males, 6 females) and 10 young adult subjects (YA group; age: 26.1±5.15 (MEAN±SD); range: 19-33; 7 males, 3 females), participated in this study. All subjects were right-handed and had no history of brain tumors, trauma or stroke-related medical conditions. The experimental protocol was approved by the local research Ethics Committee of Innopolis University. The experimental study was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants were pre-informed about the goals and design of the experiment and signed a written informed consent.

Task

All participants were instructed to sit on the chair with their hands lying comfortably on the table desk in front of them, palms up. The timeline of the experimental session is presented in Fig 1A. First, we recorded Eyes Open Resting state (5 minutes). Further experiment included sequential repetitions of the fine motor task (squeezing one of the hands into a fist after the audio signal and holding it until the second signal) using either left or right hand (30 repetitions per hand, 60 in total). The duration of the signal determined the type of movement: short beep (0.3 s) was given to perform a non-dominant hand (left hand, LH) movement and long beep (0.75 s) was given to perform a dominant hand (right hand, RH) movement. Thus, we conducted a mixed-design experimental study with the Movement Type (LH and RH conditions) as within-subject factor and the Age (EA and YA groups) as between-subject factor.

Fig 1. Experimental paradigm.

Fig 1

Timelines of the experimental session (A) and a single motor task (B). Here, tb is the duration of the beep, which is 0.3 s for the LH movement command, and 0.75 s for the RH movement command.

The timeline of a single motor task is presented in Fig 1B. The time interval between the signals during the task and the pause between the repetitions were chosen randomly in the range 4–5 s and 6–8 s, respectively. The types of movements (LH or RH) were mixed in the course of the session and given randomly to exclude possible training or motor-preparation effects caused by the sequential execution of the same tasks. The overall experimental session lasted approximately 16 minutes, including the background cortical activity recording and series of movement executions.

EEG data acquisition and preprocessing

We acquired EEG signals using the monopolar registration method (a 10—10 system proposed by the American Electroencephalographic Society [30]). According to this, we recorded EEG signals with 31 sensors (O2, O1, P4, P3, C4, C3, F4, F3, Fp2, Fp1, P8, P7, T8, T7, F8, F7, Oz, Pz, Cz, Fz, Fpz, FT7, FC3, FCz, FC4, FT8, TP7, CP3, CPz, CP4, TP8) and two reference electrodes A1 and A2 on the earlobes and a ground electrode N just above the forehead. We used the cup adhesive Ag/AgCl electrodes placed on the “Tien–20” paste (Weaver and Company, Colorado, USA). Immediately before the experiments started, we performed all necessary procedures to increase skin conductivity and reduce its resistance using the abrasive “NuPrep” gel (Weaver and Company, Colorado, USA). We controlled the variation of impedance within a range of 2–5 kΩ during the experiment. The electroencephalograph “Encephalan-EEG-19/26” (Medicom MTD company, Taganrog, Russian Federation) with multiple EEG and two EMG channels performed amplification and analog-to-digital conversion of the recorded signals. The EMG signals were acquired to verify the correctness of the epochs segmentation. This device possessed the registration certificate of the Federal Service for Supervision in Health Care No. FCP 2007/00124 of 07.11.2014 and the European Certificate CE 538571 of the British Standards Institute (BSI).

The raw EEG and EMG signals were sampled at 250 Hz and filtered by a 50–Hz notch filter by embedded hardware-software data acquisition complex. Additionally, raw EEG signals were filtered by the 5th-order Butterworth filter with cut-off points at 1 Hz and 100 Hz. Eyes blinking and heartbeat artifact removal was performed by the Independent Component Analysis (ICA) [31]. The recorded EEG and EMG signals presented in proper physical units (millivolts) were segmented into four sets of epochs according to the (group [YA, EA], condition [LH, RH]) combinations: YA LH, YA RH, EA LH, and EA RH. Each epoch was 10 s long, including 2s baseline activity and 8s motor-related activity. Data was then inspected manually and corrected for remaining artifacts. Epochs which we failed to correct manually mostly due to the strong muscle artifacts were rejected. Finally, each set contained 15 corrected epochs, which was equal to the minimal number of the artifact-free epochs over all participants.

All preprocessing steps including filtering, artifact removal and epoching were performed using MNE package (ver. 0.20.0) for Python 3.7 [32]. The analyzed EEG data is available online [33].

Time-frequency analysis in sensor space

For each (group, condition)–set of epochs, we estimated spectral power in theta (4-8 Hz), alpha/mu (8-14 Hz) and beta (15-30 Hz) frequency bands using time-frequency analysis implemented in MNE. Particularly, time-frequency representation of the EEG epochs was obtained via Morlet complex-valued wavelet in the range 4-30 Hz and contrasted with 2s baseline period using ‘percent’ mode, i.e., subtracting the mean of baseline values followed by dividing by the mean of baseline values. The number of cycles in the wavelet transform was set for each frequency f as f/2. Then, obtained time-frequency representations were averaged over epochs for each subject.

Estimation of the motor brain response time

A priory knowledge about the cortical activation during actual movements execution implies that motor brain response is determined as a pronounced event-related desynchronization (ERD) of mu and beta oscillations in the contralateral area of the motor cortex [3439]. Here, we used mu- and beta-band event-related spectral power (ERSPμ,β) at C4 sensor in LH condition and C3 sensor in RH condition to estimate motor brain response time in corresponding frequency bands (MBRTμ,β) for each subject of both groups. We manually inspected each ERSPμ,β time-series and defined MBRTμ,β as the first minimum of the spectral power below the 2.5th baseline level (an exemplary illustration of the MBRTμ estimation is presented in Fig 2A). Thus, we collected eight sets of MBRT corresponding to each (group, condition, frequency band)-set. Statistical comparison of the MBRT was performed using a two-way mixed-design ANOVA test implemented in JASP open-source statistical software [40].

Fig 2. Motor brain response time.

Fig 2

A An exemplary illustration of the MBRTμ estimation. The blue curve shows single-subject ERSPμ at the C4 sensor averaged over 15 LH epochs. Black solid and red dashed horizontal lines indicate mean and 2.5th percentile level of the baseline ERSPμ, respectively. Black solid and black dashed vertical lines show the beginning of the audio command and estimated motor brain response, respectively. B Distribution of MBRTμ,β across subjects in each (group,condition)-set. Here, ‘*’ indicates p < 0.05 and ‘***’ indicates p < 0.001. C Scatterplots of paired observations: (top) MBRTμ(RH) versus MBRTμ(LH) and (bottom) MBRTβ(RH) versus MBRTβ(LH) for each subject. Here, the diagonal line is MBRTμ,β(RH) = MBRTμ,β(LH).

Within-subject time-frequency analyses

We performed within-subject spatio-temporal clustering analyses to reveal arrays of sensors associated with the motor-related brain activity separately in each frequency band of interest for each age group and experimental condition. Pairwise comparison of (time,sensor)-pairs was performed via one-tailed one-sampled t-test (dF = 9, ppairwise = 0.005, tcritical = ±3.2498) and spatio-temporal clustering was assessed using non-parametric permutation test with r = 2000 random permutations (pcluster = 0.05) following Maris and Oostenveld [41].

Between-subject time-frequency analyses

During between-subject analyses, we compared brain activity of the age groups in the same experimental conditions. Again, we considered baseline-corrected topographic maps averaged in the frequency bands of interest. Effect of interest was evaluated at each (time,sensor)–pair using one-tailed unpaired F-test for independent samples (dF1 = 1, dF2 = 18, p = 0.025, Fcritical = 10.218) and spatio-temporal clustering was assessed using non-parametric permutation test with r = 2000 random permutations (pcluster = 0.05) [41].

Mixed-design analyses

Based on the results of within- and between-subject spatio-temporal clustering analyses, we localized the effect of significant spectral power change in the spatio-temporal domain. Further, for each (group, condition)-set we averaged spectral power over the corresponding spatio-temporal clusters and compared it using mixed-design ANOVA.

Functional connectivity analysis

Functional connectivity measures the similarity of activation in the different brain regions based on the recorded signals of brain activity. According to the review papers [42, 43], there exists a variety of functional connectivity metrics that evaluate this similarity in the different aspects. Moreover, functional connectivity analysis based on EEG or MEG recordings suffers from such problems as volume conduction/field spread effect, signal-to-noise ratio, common input, etc. due to the nature of these neuroimaging techniques [44]. Thus, the choice of the particular functional connectivity measure requires both a prior knowledge about the analyzed neuronal processes and an understanding of possible problems that may potentially interfere with the adequate interpretation of functional connectivity results.

Functional connectivity measure

In accordance with the prior knowledge that motor-related activity is associated with certain frequency bands, first of all we expect the similarity of oscillatory behavior in remote brain regions in terms of phase-locking. Among the variety of FC measures based on the phase-synchronization, phase lag index (PLI) seems to be an appropriate metric [45]. PLI is robust to the common source problem as it ignores simultaneous phase similarity, less sensitive to the intrinsic EEG noise and allows reasonable interpretation of the obtained results. PLI is traditionally defined as:

PLIi,j=|sign(ϕi(tk)-ϕj(tk))|, (1)

where ϕi,j(t) are phases of signals at ith and jth EEG sensors introduced via Hilbert transform and operator 〈•〉 averaging over time points k. It clearly follows from Eq (2), that PLI lies between 0 and 1, where PLI = 1 corresponds to perfect phase-locking and PLI = 0 implies a complete lack of synchrony.

PLI is also formulated in the frequency domain. In this case, the definition of PLI given in Eq (2) is rewritten as:

PLIi,j=|sign(Im[Si,j(f)])|, (2)

where Si,j is a complex-valued Fourier-based cross-spectrum of ith and jth time-series and f covers the frequency band of interest. Frequency-domain definition of PLI is implemented in MNE package and has been used in this study to reveal motor-related functional connectivity.

Adjacency matrix

The functional connectivity structure in each of the frequency bands of interest was presented by symmetric adjacency matrix sized (31 × 31). For each of n = 20 participants we calculated k = 15 connectivity matrices in both experimental conditions (LH and RH) during the premotor interval 0÷1.25 s contrasted by the baseline connectivity (−1.25÷0 s). Baseline contrast was applied to exclude false links, which could potentially arise due to the age-related changes in the resting-state functional connectivity. Then, for each subject, we computed mean connectivity matrices, averaged over k = 15 matrices, in both experimental conditions. To highlight statistically significant changes in functional connectivity related to the factor of age, we provided a between-subject analysis of the mean functional connectivity matrices. To address this issue we performed element-wise comparison of mean connectivity matrices for each type of movements between age groups using one-tailed unpaired t-test with ppairwise = 0.025 (dF = 9, tcritical = ±2.262). Multiple comparison problem (MCP) was addressed via Network-Based Statistic (NBS) approach with r = 2000 random permutations and pcluster = 0.05 [46].

Results

Motor brain response time analysis

First, we evaluated the effect of aging on the MBRT, i.e., the duration of the time interval required for the brain to activate a corresponding motor area for both groups. We estimated MBRT for each subject in mu and beta bands in both experimental conditions (Fig 2B) and compared the results taking into account Age, Movement Type and Frequency Band factors together (see Tables 1, 2 and 3). The mixed-design ANOVA test revealed a significant effect of Age (F(1, 18) = 22.793, p < 0.001), Band (F(1, 18) = 19.226, p < 0.001) and Movement Type (F(1, 18) = 4.752, p = 0.043) on MBRT. Post-hoc comparison via unpaired t-test indicated that the mean MBRT in EA group (M = 0.932, SD = 0.384) was significantly higher than mean MBRT in YA group (M = 0.66, SD = 0.234). Regarding the Frequency Band, post-hoc comparison via paired t-test demonstrated that the mean mu-band MBRT (M = 0.932, SD = 0.388) was significantly higher than mean beta-band MBRT (M = 0.648, SD = 0.211). Finally, post-hoc comparison via paired t-test showed that the mean MBRT in LH condition (M = 0.843, SD = 0.29) was significantly higher than mean MBRT in RH condition (M = 0.737, SD = 0.383).

Table 1. Motor brain response time, s (Two-way mixed-design ANOVA summary).

Cases dF1 dF2 Mean Square F p
Age (between-subject) 1 18 1.359 22.793 <.001***
Band (within-subject) 1 18 1.612 19.226 <.001***
Band * Age 1 18 0.981 11.703 0.003**
Movement Type (within-subject) 1 18 0.222 4.752 0.043*
Movement Type * Age 1 18 0.548 11.739 0.003**
Band * Movement Type 1 18 0.025 0.446 0.513
Band * Movement Type * Age 1 18 0.036 0.627 0.439

Table 2. Motor brain response time, s (Post hoc comparisons Band—Age).

Mean Difference SE t pholm
EA, mu YA, mu 0.482 0.085 5.693 <.001***
EA, beta 0.505 0.092 5.519 <.001***
YA, beta 0.545 0.085 6.430 <.001***
YA, mu EA, beta 0.023 0.085 0.274 1.000
YA, beta 0.062 0.092 0.681 1.000
EA, beta YA, beta 0.039 0.085 0.463 1.000

Table 3. Motor brain response time, s (Post hoc comparisons Movement Type—Age).

Mean Difference SE t pholm
EA, RH YA, RH 0.426 0.073 5.846 <.001***
EA, LH 0.060 0.068 0.881 0.400
YA, LH 0.155 0.073 2.132 0.120
YA, RH EA, LH -0.366 0.073 -5.020 <.001***
YA, LH -0.271 0.068 -3.964 0.004**
EA, LH YA, LH 0.095 0.073 1.306 0.400

Moreover, there was a significant interaction between the Band and Age of the participants (F(1, 18) = 11.703, p = 0.003). We could interpret this interaction as meaning that the different frequency bands activated differently in EA and YA groups. Particularly, mu-band MBRT w shown to be higher in EA group (EA, mu-band: M = 1.173, SD = 0.35) compared with the other (group,band)-pairs: (EA, beta-band: M = 0.668, SD = 0.213; YA, mu-band: M = 0.69, SD = 0.255; EA, beta-band: M = 0.628, SD = 0.213).

Finally, there was a significant interaction between the Movement Type and Age of the participants (F(1, 18) = 11.739, p = 0.003). We could interpret this interaction as meaning that the Movement Type influenced MBRT differently in EA and YA groups. Particularly, YA group reacted significantly faster in RH condition (YA, RH: M = 0.524, SD = 0.119) compared with the other (group,condition)-pairs: (YA, LH: M = 0.795, SD = 0.244; EA, RH: M = 0.95, SD = 0.44; EA, RH: M = 0.89, SD = 0.328). According to the results of paired observation (Fig 2C), 9 of 10 subjects in YA group demonstrated that MBRTμ(LH)>MBRTμ(RH) and 4 of 10 subjects in EA group had the same effect. Regarding the estimations of MBRTβ, 8 of 10 subjects in YA group showed that MBRTβ(LH)>MBRTβ(RH), while in EA group the same effect was demonstrated in 2 of 10 subjects.

Within-subject time-frequency analysis

Based on the above MBRT analysis, we assumed that age-related changes affecting the speed of brain motor activation should be found in the motor initiation period. With this aim, we performed within-subject spatio-temporal clustering analysis of the spectral power in the theta, alpha/mu and beta frequency bands for each (group, condition)-set during the motor initiation (0÷1.5 s). Fig 3 shows the results of within-subject clustering analysis in the LH condition for both groups of subjects. It is seen that in the LH condition (non-dominant hand movement), brain activation in both YA and EA groups proceeds similarly. Specifically, the suppression of beta-rhythm in the motor cortex at 472 ms (YA group) and 308 ms (EA group) was followed by the mu-band ERD at 604 ms (YA group) and 548 ms (EA group) and was related to the motor execution control. Desynchronization of the beta and mu oscillations was preceded by the theta-band activation from 308 to 540 ms (YA group) and from 156 to 512 ms (EA group). In the YA group, this theta-band cluster involved the left frontal (Fp1, F3), frontal-central (FC3) and temporal (FT7, T7, TP7) EEG sensors. In the EA group, strong theta-band synchronization spanned widely across the frontal, central and occipito-parietal EEG sensors. Also, a spatio-temporal cluster showing a significant activation in theta band appeared almost simultaneously with a mu-band desynchronization: from 720 to 920 ms (YA group) and from 648 to 904 ms (EA group). Here, a significant theta-band activation was shown in frontal (Fz, F4), central (FC3, FCz, FC4, C3, C4, CP3, CPz, Cp4) and left temporal (TP7) EEG sensors in YA group, while in EA group midline (Fz, Fcz, Cz) and bilateral central (C4, CP3) EEG sensors indicated increased theta-band ERSP. Thus, in LH condition, both groups shared a similar activation mechanism and timing of the motor initiation process.

Fig 3. Sensor-level within-subject time-frequency analyses during motor initiation (LH condition).

Fig 3

Baseline-corrected spatio-temporal clusters (left) and mean ERSP of the corresponding clusters (right): (A, B, E, F) theta clusters; (C, G) alpha/mu clusters; (D, H) beta clusters. White dots indicate sensors exhibiting significant spectral power changes. Pairwise comparison is performed via one-tailed one-sampled t-test with ppairwise = 0.005 (dF = 9, tcritical = ±3.2498) and cluster-based analysis is performed via non-parametric permutation test with pcluster = 0.05.

On the contrary, the way of cortical activation during the motor initiation period in the RH condition (dominant hand movement) was different in considered age groups (Fig 4). In both groups, beta- and mu-band ERD in RH condition started earlier compared with LH condition: from 424 ms (YA group) and 252 ms (EA group) for beta-band ERD; from 484 ms (YA group) and 328 ms (EA group) for mu-band ERD. However, in the YA group, the theta-band spectral power did not change significantly during the pre-movement period. At the same time, the theta-band activation in the RH condition similar to LH condition was observed in the EA group (248-746 ms) involving frontal (Fz), central (FC line, C line, CP line), parietal (P4) and right temporal (T8) EEG sensors.

Fig 4. Sensor-level within-subject time-frequency analyses during motor initiation (RH condition).

Fig 4

Baseline-corrected spatio-temporal clusters (left) and mean ERSP of the corresponding clusters (right): (A, D) theta clusters; (B, E) alpha/mu clusters; (C, F) beta clusters. White dots indicate sensors exhibiting significant spectral power changes. Pairwise comparison is performed via one-tailed one-sampled t-test with ppairwise = 0.005 (dF = 9, tcritical = ±3.2498) and cluster-based analysis is performed via non-parametric permutation test with pcluster = 0.05.

Between-subject time-frequency analysis

To address the age-related changes in the pre-movement theta-band activation in detail, we provided a between-subject spatio-temporal clustering analysis of ERSP separately in each experimental condition. In LH condition, the significant between-subject difference in the theta-band activation was not observed. On the contrary, the between-subject differences were found in RH condition from 364 to 512 ms: the theta-band spatio-temporal cluster included C3, C4, Cp3, Cpz, Cp4 and P4 EEG sensors (Fig 5A).

Fig 5. Between-subject analyses of the theta-band activation during motor initiation.

Fig 5

A Baseline-corrected spatio-temporal clusters (left) and mean theta-band ERSP of the corresponding clusters (right) preceding RH movements execution. White circles indicate cluster of sensors with significant differences via non-parametric test. Pairwise comparison is performed via one-tailed unpaired F-test with ppairwise = 0.005 (dF1 = 1 and dF2 = 18, Fcritical = 10.218) and cluster-based analysis is performed via non-parametric permutation test with pcluster = 0.05. B Distribution of the event-related theta-band spectral power of the uncovered spatio-temporal cluster (A) across subjects in each (group, condition)–set. Here, ‘*’ indicates p < 0.05. C Scatterplot of paired observations within each group.

To estimate age-related differences in theta-band activation taking into account both Age and Movement Type factors, we compared mean theta-band spectral power over the evaluated spatio-temporal cluster via mixed-designed ANOVA (Fig 5B and the summary is presented in detail in Tables 4 and 5). The mixed-design ANOVA test revealed no significant effect of both Age (F(1, 18) = 2.189, p = 0.156) and Movement Type (F(1, 18) = 3.151, p = 0.093) on the pre-movement theta-band spectral power. However, there was a significant interaction between these factors (F(1, 18) = 5.085, p = 0.037). We could interpret this interaction as follows: pre-movement theta-band power was similar for LH condition the EA and YA groups (EA, LH: M = 1.464, SD = 1.171; YA, LH: M = 1.169, SD = 1.038), while the YA group demonstrated lower pre-movement theta-band power in RH condition (EA, RH: M = 1.532, SD = 1.054; YA, RH: M = 0.601, SD = 0.520). According to the paired observations (Fig 5C), 8 of 10 subjects in YA group demonstrate the effect.

Table 4. Pre-movement theta-band spectral power (Two-way mixed-design ANOVA summary).

Cases dF1 dF2 Mean Square F p
Age (between-subject) 1 18 3.757 2.189 0.156
Movement Type (within-subject) 1 18 0.626 3.151 0.093
Movement Type * Age 1 18 1.010 5.085 0.037*

Table 5. Pre-movement theta-band spectral power (Post hoc comparisons—Age-Movement Type).

Mean Difference SE stat p
EA, LH YA, LH 0.295 0.438 42.0 (U) 0.28
EA, RH -0.068 0.199 21.0 (W) 0.51
YA, RH 0.863 0.438 2.503 (t) 0.022*
YA, LH EA, RH -0.363 0.438 25.0 (U) 0.032*
YA, RH 0.568 0.199 7.0 (W) 0.037*
EA, RH YA, RH 0.931 0.438 25.0 (U) 0.032*

According to the results of Shapiro-Wilk normality test, the (EA,RH)-set with p = 0.029 and (YA,LH)-set with p = 0.034 did not come from the normally distributed population. Therefore, along with the unpaired t-test (t) we used non-parametric Mann-Whitney U-test (U) and Wilcoxon signed-rank test (W) to the provide the post hoc comparisons.

Functional connectivity analysis

To support and extend our observations of the cortical activation during motor initiation, we explored age-related changes in terms of the underlying functional interactions between remote brain regions. Due to previously uncovered between-subject difference in the theta-band activity, we provided a between-subject comparison of the sensor-level theta-band functional connectivity estimated during the pre-movement stage in the RH condition (0÷1.25 s). As seen in Fig 6A, the distributed functional network with strong hubs in occipito-parietal (O1, O2, P3, P7), frontal (F7) and midline (Oz, Pz, CPz, FCz) EEG sensors was highly coupled in YA group compared to EA subjects. At the same time, we found the significant bilateral coupling increase between central (Cz, C3, C4, Cp3, Fc4), temporal (TP7, TP8, T7, FT7), and frontal (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4) EEG sensors in EA participants (Fig 6B). Here, Cz sensor being a strong hub of the functional network provided a large-scale neuronal communication via coupling with the bilateral cortical sensorimotor circuits (C3–TP7 and C4–TP8), along with temporal (Cz–FT7, Cz–T7) and frontal (Cz–F3, Cz–F4, Cz–Fc4) EEG sensors.

Fig 6. Between-subject analysis of the theta-band functional connectivity during motor initiation in the RH condition.

Fig 6

A Significantly stronger coupling in YA compared to the EA. B Significantly stronger coupling in EA compared to the YA. Here, ΔPLI defines the difference between group-level mean functional connectivity (EA versus YA). Element-wise comparison of mean connectivity matrices between Age groups was performed via one-tailed unpaired t-test with ppairwise = 0.025 (dF = 9, tcritical = ±2.262).

Discussion

We considered the effect of healthy aging on the cortical activation in the motor initiation phase during the controlled repetition of fine motor tasks—squeezing one of the hands into a fist paced by the audio command. We found that the time required for motor-related mu- and beta-band desynchronization, which we referred to as a motor brain response time (MBRT), was increased in the elderly subjects compared to the younger control group during the dominant hand task. Based on the results of time-frequency and functional connectivity analyses, we found that the prolonged motor response was preceded by the increased theta-band activation in central, central-parietal, and parietal EEG sensors, along with a stronger coupling between central, bilateral temporal, and frontal sensors. Further, we discuss our results in the context of possible mechanisms supporting the motor initiation slowdown.

We observed the significant age-related differences in the MBRTs, which demonstrated a higher speed of the motor initiation in the case of the dominant (right) hand task in younger participants compared to the elderly adults. At the same time, motor initiation was equally slow during the non-dominant (left) hand task in both age groups. Moreover, MBRTs of elderly adults in both conditions approached the level of the non-dominant hand in younger subjects. Based on these findings, we suggested that the neuronal mechanisms supporting right-hand dominance are impaired under healthy aging. Despite the conflicting evidence in the literature, our results are consistent with several studies showing a similar effect. First, T. Kalisch et al. [8] demonstrated the behavioral decline in the dominant hand performance leading to ambidexterity in elderly adults. The authors argued that their findings could be explained by the mechanism of use-dependent plasticity [22], causing the degradation of well-trained motor functions due to the reduced activity and sedentary lifestyle of elderly individuals. Also, J. Langan et al. [17] supported these results and showed less-lateralized task-related motor activity in elderly adults compared to the younger control group. They found that longer reaction time in elderly adults was correlated with greater activation of the ipsilateral primary motor cortex during the motor task performance and weaker resting-state interhemispheric coupling, which was also observed in Refs. [16, 47, 48]. Described changes provided the compensatory mechanism to maintain the level of motor performance consisting in the reorganization of functional networks aimed at overcoming the age-related chemical and structural changes [20, 21]. Our results also evidence the motor-related over-activation of the brain areas in elderly adults as a large cluster of mu- and beta-band desynchronization covering additional frontal, central, parietal, occipital, and temporal EEG sensors (see Figs 3 and 4).

However, the aforementioned mechanisms are not the only ones that support the brain’s motor response slowdown. Our results showed that the prevalent theta-band activation in central, central-parietal, and parietal EEG sensors preceded the motor-related mu-band desynchronization during the non-dominant hand movements in both groups and the dominant hand movements in elderly adults. The mechanism of motor initiation related to the increased theta activity is explained by the Bland’s sensorimotor integration model. In their early works on rodents [49, 50], B.H. Bland with colleagues treated the hippocampal formation theta activity as a communication channel between the sensory processing and movement initiation. Further, the Bland’s model was extended to the human brain in a series of works by J.B. Caplan et al. [25, 51]. In their studies, they concluded that while mu-band suppression (a traditional hallmark of the motor-related brain activity) reflected cortical activation directly during the motor task execution, the increased theta power between stimuli presentation and motor execution was associated with sensorimotor integration similarly to rodents. Along with this, several EEG-studies reported the increase of theta-band power during the planning phase in the choice-related, catching, and imagery motor tasks [5254]. Specifically, M. Tambini et al. [53] demonstrated a positive correlation between theta-power and task performance. On the contrary, we found that increased theta-band power was associated with prolonged motor initiation. It should be noted that the significant increase of the theta-band power related to the dominant hand decline in elderly adults was observed in the central, central-parietal, and parietal EEG sensors covering the sensorimotor area. Following the recent study by J. Dushanova et al. [55], such result should be explained by the different strategies of the motor task initiation between age groups. While the degraded plasticity in elderly adults requires higher cortical activation for motor planning, younger subjects optimize their cognitive resources for the familiar and well-trained motor task accomplishment. The latter was represented as a lower theta-band activation. Therefore, less effective use of cognitive resources slowed the motor planning phase in elderly adults compared to the younger control group during the dominant hand tasks.

These conclusions were also supported and extended by the results of functional connectivity analysis during the pre-movement phase. The differences in theta-band functional connectivity between two groups could be interpreted as a meaning of the different mechanisms of cortical interaction that subserve motor planning in elderly adults and young subjects. First, we showed that in young adults, pre-movement theta-band functional connectivity strongly involves midline EEG sensors. According to the previous studies [5659], strong midline coupling could be interpreted as increased perceptual-motor facility and motor working memory. Thus, we suppose that in young adults, initiation of the familiar motor activity emphasizes motor working memory and enables the formation and processing of the motor memories, i.e., the stored information about the motor action obtained from prior experience, for accurate motor performance [60]. On the contrary, in elderly adults, we observed a completely different structure of the sensor-level functional connectivity, i.e., a stronger coupling between the frontal, central-parietal, and bilateral temporal EEG sensors with the most influential node located in the central EEG row (Cz sensor). As the working memory decline with age is well-documented [6163], we conclude that memory representation of motor actions is less accessible in elderly adults. Based on our findings and the existing literature, we suggest that higher coupling within the sensorimotor area during a pre-movement phase in the elderly group indicates the prevalence of sensorimotor integration mechanisms relative to the resource-demanding Bland’s Type 1 motor-related theta activation [25]. We conclude that the uncovered differences in the cortical activation, related with an increased theta-band power, taken together with age-related changes in neural interactions reflect non-optimal utilization of the cognitive brain resources in elderly adults causing the significantly delayed motor initiation process.

Conclusion

Elderly adults exhibited the approach to ambidexterity in term of the slowdown in cortical activation related to the execution of the dominant hand task. We showed that motor-related mu- and beta-band desynchronization appeared faster in young subjects during dominant hand movement, while in elderly adults it appeared equally slow in both hands. We demonstrated that the observed age-related loss of the dominant hand advance was accompanied by the increased theta-band activation similar to Bland’s Type 1 sensorimotor integration model. At the same time, age-related changes affected the structure of sensor-level functional connectivity during motor initiation: younger subjects demonstrated stronger interaction between frontal, parietal, and midline EEG sensors, while elderly adults demonstrated higher coupling between central, temporal, frontal sensors. Taken together, our results on cortical activation and underlying neuronal interactions suggest the utilization of more demanding pre-movement processes in elderly adults causing a significant slowing down in the motor initiation.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the anonymous reviewer for a detailed feedback and valuable comments.

Data Availability

The data underlying the results presented in the study is available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12301181.

Funding Statement

NF received funding for this work from the Council on grants of the President of the Russian Federation (Grant No. MK-2080.2020.2). AH received funding for this work from the Russian Foundation for Basic Research (Grant No. 19-52-55001) and the Council on grants of the President of the Russian Federation (Grant No. NSh-2594.2020.2). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1. Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, et al. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(5):721–733. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Sorond FA, Cruz-Almeida Y, Clark DJ, Viswanathan A, Scherzer CR, De Jager P, et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility in older adults: neural mechanisms of mobility impairment. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2015;70(12):1526–1532. 10.1093/gerona/glv130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Maes C, Gooijers J, de Xivry JJO, Swinnen SP, Boisgontier MP. Two hands, one brain, and aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;75:234–256. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Carmeli E, Patish H, Coleman R. The aging hand. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2003;58(2):M146–M152. 10.1093/gerona/58.2.M146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Papegaaij S, Taube W, Baudry S, Otten E, Hortobágyi T. Aging causes a reorganization of cortical and spinal control of posture. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2014;6:28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Voelcker-Rehage C. Motor-skill learning in older adults—a review of studies on age-related differences. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity. 2008;5(1):5–16. 10.1007/s11556-008-0030-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Smith CD, Umberger G, Manning E, Slevin J, Wekstein D, Schmitt F, et al. Critical decline in fine motor hand movements in human aging. Neurology. 1999;53(7):1458–1458. 10.1212/WNL.53.7.1458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kalisch T, Wilimzig C, Kleibel N, Tegenthoff M, Dinse HR. Age-related attenuation of dominant hand superiority. PLoS One. 2006;1(1). 10.1371/journal.pone.0000090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Hall SD, Barnes GR, Furlong PL, Seri S, Hillebrand A. Neuronal network pharmacodynamics of GABAergic modulation in the human cortex determined using pharmaco-magnetoencephalography. Human brain mapping. 2010;31(4):581–594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Hall SD, Stanford IM, Yamawaki N, McAllister C, Rönnqvist K, Woodhall GL, et al. The role of GABAergic modulation in motor function related neuronal network activity. Neuroimage. 2011;56(3):1506–1510. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Rossiter HE, Davis EM, Clark EV, Boudrias MH, Ward NS. Beta oscillations reflect changes in motor cortex inhibition in healthy ageing. Neuroimage. 2014;91:360–365. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW. Is an absolute level of cortical beta suppression required for proper movement? Magnetoencephalographic evidence from healthy aging. Neuroimage. 2016;134:514–521. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Heinrichs-Graham E, Arpin DJ, Wilson TW. Cue-related temporal factors modulate movement-related beta oscillatory activity in the human motor circuit. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2016;28(7):1039–1051. 10.1162/jocn_a_00948 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Liu L, Rosjat N, Popovych S, Wang BA, Yeldesbay A, Toth TI, et al. Age-related changes in oscillatory power affect motor action. PloS one. 2017;12(11):e0187911 10.1371/journal.pone.0187911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Xifra-Porxas A, Niso G, Larivière S, Kassinopoulos M, Baillet S, Mitsis GD, et al. Older adults exhibit a more pronounced modulation of beta oscillations when performing sustained and dynamic handgrips. Neuroimage. 2019;201:116037 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. Journal of neuroscience. 2008;28(1):91–99. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Langan J, Peltier S, Bo J, Fling BW, Welsh RC, Seidler RD. Functional implications of age differences in motor system connectivity. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2010;4:17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Berchicci M, Lucci G, Pesce C, Spinelli D, Di Russo F. Prefrontal hyperactivity in older people during motor planning. Neuroimage. 2012;62(3):1750–1760. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Fernandez-Ruiz J, Peltsch A, Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Garcia A, et al. Age related prefrontal compensatory mechanisms for inhibitory control in the antisaccade task. Neuroimage. 2018;165:92–101. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Ward NS. Compensatory mechanisms in the aging motor system. Ageing research reviews. 2006;5(3):239–254. 10.1016/j.arr.2006.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. How does it STAC up? Revisiting the scaffolding theory of aging and cognition. Neuropsychology review. 2014;24(3):355–370. 10.1007/s11065-014-9270-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Bütefisch CM, Davis BC, Wise SP, Sawaki L, Kopylev L, Classen J, et al. Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2000;97(7):3661–3665. 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Carp J, Park J, Hebrank A, Park DC, Polk TA. Age-related neural dedifferentiation in the motor system. PloS one. 2011;6(12). 10.1371/journal.pone.0029411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Ikkai A, Curtis CE. Common neural mechanisms supporting spatial working memory, attention and motor intention. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49(6):1428–1434. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Cruikshank LC, Singhal A, Hueppelsheuser M, Caplan JB. Theta oscillations reflect a putative neural mechanism for human sensorimotor integration. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;107(1):65–77. 10.1152/jn.00893.2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Leisman G, Moustafa AA, Shafir T. Thinking, walking, talking: integratory motor and cognitive brain function. Frontiers in public health. 2016;4:94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Sepp S, Howard SJ, Tindall-Ford S, Agostinho S, Paas F. Cognitive load theory and human movement: Towards an integrated model of working memory. Educational Psychology Review. 2019; p. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Prichep L, John E, Ferris SH, Reisberg B, Almas M, Alper K, et al. Quantitative EEG correlates of cognitive deterioration in the elderly. Neurobiology of Aging. 1994;15(1):85–90. 10.1016/0197-4580(94)90147-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Stomrud E, Hansson O, Minthon L, Blennow K, Rosén I, Londos E. Slowing of EEG correlates with CSF biomarkers and reduced cognitive speed in elderly with normal cognition over 4 years. Neurobiology of aging. 2010;31(2):215–223. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Nuwer MR, Comi G, Emerson R, Fuglsang-Frederiksen A, Guérit JM, Hinrichs H, et al. IFCN standards for digital recording of clinical EEG. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology. 1998;106(3):259–261. 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00106-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hyvärinen A, Oja E. Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural networks. 2000;13(4-5):411–430. 10.1016/S0893-6080(00)00026-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Gramfort A, Luessi M, Larson E, Engemann DA, Strohmeier D, Brodbeck C, et al. MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Frontiers in neuroscience. 2013;7:267 10.3389/fnins.2013.00267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Frolov N, Pitsik E, Grubov V, Kiselev A, Maksimenko V, Hramov A. EEG dataset for the analysis of age-related changes in motor-related cortical activity during a series of fine motor tasks performance;. Available from: https://figshare.com/articles/EEG_dataset_for_the_analysis_of_age-related_changes_in_motor-related_cortical_activity_during_a_series_of_fine_motor_tasks_performance/12301181/1.
  • 34. Pfurtscheller G, Brunner C, Schlögl A, Da Silva FL. Mu rhythm (de) synchronization and EEG single-trial classification of different motor imagery tasks. NeuroImage. 2006;31(1):153–159. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Gao L, Wang J, Chen L. Event-related desynchronization and synchronization quantification in motor-related EEG by Kolmogorov entropy. Journal of neural engineering. 2013;10(3):036023 10.1088/1741-2560/10/3/036023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Maksimenko VA, Pavlov A, Runnova AE, Nedaivozov V, Grubov V, Koronovslii A, et al. Nonlinear analysis of brain activity, associated with motor action and motor imaginary in untrained subjects. Nonlinear Dynamics. 2018;91(4):2803–2817. 10.1007/s11071-018-4047-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Pitsik E, Frolov N, Hauke Kraemer K, Grubov V, Maksimenko V, Kurths J, et al. Motor execution reduces EEG signals complexity: Recurrence quantification analysis study. Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science. 2020;30(2):023111 10.1063/1.5136246 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. McFarland DJ, Miner LA, Vaughan TM, Wolpaw JR. Mu and beta rhythm topographies during motor imagery and actual movements. Brain topography. 2000;12(3):177–186. 10.1023/A:1023437823106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Kühn AA, Williams D, Kupsch A, Limousin P, Hariz M, Schneider GH, et al. Event-related beta desynchronization in human subthalamic nucleus correlates with motor performance. Brain. 2004;127(4):735–746. 10.1093/brain/awh106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.JASP Team. JASP (Version 0.12)[Computer software]; 2020. Available from: https://jasp-stats.org/.
  • 41. Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG-and MEG-data. Journal of neuroscience methods. 2007;164(1):177–190. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Bastos AM, Schoffelen JM. A tutorial review of functional connectivity analysis methods and their interpretational pitfalls. Frontiers in systems neuroscience. 2016;9:175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Hamedi M, Salleh SH, Noor AM. Electroencephalographic motor imagery brain connectivity analysis for BCI: a review. Neural computation. 2016;28(6):999–1041. 10.1162/NECO_a_00838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Nunez PL, Srinivasan R, et al. Electric fields of the brain: the neurophysics of EEG. Oxford University Press, USA; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Stam CJ, Nolte G, Daffertshofer A. Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Human brain mapping. 2007;28(11):1178–1193. 10.1002/hbm.20346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage. 2010;53(4):1197–1207. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Mattay VS, Fera F, Tessitore A, Hariri A, Das S, Callicott J, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology. 2002;58(4):630–635. 10.1212/WNL.58.4.630 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Riecker A, Gröschel K, Ackermann H, Steinbrink C, Witte O, Kastrup A. Functional significance of age-related differences in motor activation patterns. Neuroimage. 2006;32(3):1345–1354. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Oddie SD, Bland BH. Hippocampal formation theta activity and movement selection. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 1998;22(2):221–231. 10.1016/S0149-7634(97)00003-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Bland BH, Oddie SD. Theta band oscillation and synchrony in the hippocampal formation and associated structures: the case for its role in sensorimotor integration. Behavioural brain research. 2001;127(1-2):119–136. 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00358-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Caplan JB, Madsen JR, Schulze-Bonhage A, Aschenbrenner-Scheibe R, Newman EL, Kahana MJ. Human θ oscillations related to sensorimotor integration and spatial learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23(11):4726–4736. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-11-04726.2003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Perfetti B, Moisello C, Landsness EC, Kvint S, Pruski A, Onofrj M, et al. Temporal evolution of oscillatory activity predicts performance in a choice-reaction time reaching task. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105(1):18–27. 10.1152/jn.00778.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Tombini M, Zappasodi F, Zollo L, Pellegrino G, Cavallo G, Tecchio F, et al. Brain activity preceding a 2D manual catching task. Neuroimage. 2009;47(4):1735–1746. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Tomassini A, Ambrogioni L, Medendorp WP, Maris E. Theta oscillations locked to intended actions rhythmically modulate perception. Elife. 2017;6:e25618 10.7554/eLife.25618 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Dushanova J, Christov M. The effect of aging on EEG brain oscillations related to sensory and sensorimotor functions. Advances in medical sciences. 2014;59(1):61–67. 10.1016/j.advms.2013.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Gruzelier J. A theory of alpha/theta neurofeedback, creative performance enhancement, long distance functional connectivity and psychological integration. Cognitive processing. 2009;10(1):101–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Reiner M, Rozengurt R, Barnea A. Better than sleep: theta neurofeedback training accelerates memory consolidation. Biological psychology. 2014;95:45–53. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Kardos Z, Tóth B, Boha R, File B, Molnár M. Age-related changes of frontal-midline theta is predictive of efficient memory maintenance. Neuroscience. 2014;273:152–162. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.04.071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Reiner M, Lev DD, Rosen A. Theta neurofeedback effects on motor memory consolidation and performance accuracy: an apparent paradox? Neuroscience. 2018;378:198–210. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Dahan A, Bennet R, Reiner M. How Long Is Too Long: An Individual Time-Window for Motor Planning. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2019;. 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00238 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Quentin R, Cohen LG. Reversing working memory decline in the elderly. Nature neuroscience. 2019;22(5):686–688. 10.1038/s41593-019-0386-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Steiger TK, Herweg NA, Menz MM, Bunzeck N. Working memory performance in the elderly relates to theta-alpha oscillations and is predicted by parahippocampal and striatal integrity. Scientific reports. 2019;9(1):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Matysiak O, Kroemeke A, Brzezicka A. Working memory capacity as a predictor of cognitive training efficacy in the elderly population. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2019;11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Mukesh Dhamala

25 Jun 2020

PONE-D-20-14466

Age-related changes in the motor planning strategy slow down motor initiation in elderly adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Frolov,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by July 24, 2020. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Mukesh Dhamala, Ph. D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is a particularly well designed and interesting study by Frolov and colleagues to understand the age associated changes in the motor initiation. However, please find below my general criticisms and suggestions about your approach to elucidate the mechanisms underlying age related differences in motor initiation.

Without digressing much, I do not necessarily agree that you could call any of findings of this study as potential mechanisms of motor planning and execution. Neuroscience has long been hailing some critical observations and phenomenon as mechanisms. I don’t cast necessarily a doubt on the overall findings and observations based on this study but hesitant to infer them as underlying mechanisms of motor planning and control during aging.

The findings in this paper at best in my opinion what we should call as empirical observations grounded on certain hypothesis backed up by extant literature on Cognitive Neuroscience of healthy and pathological Aging such as Compensation related Utilization of Resources, Dedifferentiation etc. applied for interpreting observations during finer motor task performances. On the same token, Mu band ERD signatures and functional connectivity based on PLI is not the mechanisms but qualitative and quantitative observations associated with the aging process. May I also ask you what exactly is the underlying mechanism associated with aging that causally explain these observations? I don’t think a clear answer would emerge unless you do some computational modelling or DCM etc. Since, You have a clear hypothesis why didn’t you think of it on the first place to do a systematic model validation based on your EEG observations. I would suggest that this would be of great interest to test out the alternate hypothesis and mechanisms to explain these observations based on change in ambidexterity with aging in the elderly group. Yes agreed that this may be related to slowing down and also recruitment of large scale brain networks as we know there is more integration with aging at the expense of specialization. But then again, these competitive mechanisms has not really been tested in my opinion.

Having said this, I do think these observations are necessary in understanding the motor planning and control strategies with healthy and pathological aging process. In this regard, authors have made some interesting observations similar to what is somewhat known in the existing literature.

They found that the motor cortex of younger adults activated much faster during the dominant hand task compared to elderly adults and the time required for motor activation in elderly group was equal for both hands and approached the level of the non-dominant hand of younger adults. Furthermore, they found significant differences in cortical activation during the time interval preceding the motor action (during motor initiation phase). In elderly adults, as well as in young adults performing the non-dominant hand task, they observed the increased theta-band power in sensorimotor and frontal areas, whereas theta-activation was insignificant in young adults during the dominant hand task. Finally, based on the results of between-subject functional connectivity analysis, they revealed that motor planning involves different types of cortical interactions in young adults and elderly adults, which allows concluding about age-related changes in motor planning mechanisms.

The article is very well written with no ambiguity in understanding the key message and the narrative. The materials & methods are also clearly presented. The introduction and discussion carefully construed backed up by appropriate and relevant literature review and summary. Below I outline my suggestions, questions and corrections for all the sections.

Abstract and title

Perhaps it would be better if you change the article title and the abstract a bit. My reading of your article gives me an impression that this article mainly concerns about movement initiation phase rather than the entire motor planning strategy and contingency. You are interested in understanding behavioural characteristics RT, Response Accuracy etc. is affected due to differences ensuing in the motor initiation phase. In my opinion, a more appropriate title replacement would be “Age-related slowing down in the motor initiation in elderly adults”.

Materials and methods

In methods section, you talk about background recording before recording active phase. You could simply say 5 minutes Eyes Open Resting state. They don’t appear to be any different to me. In the active phase you say you have 60 fine motor tasks per participant and 30 tasks per hand. The duration of the beep short or long provides cue which hand (dominant vs. non-dominant) to use. This is fine, but how different are these 60 tasks actually from each other. Are they really all different or similar? Could you provide a statistical summary or similarity measure to point out the differences between categories of finer motor task categories. If the task categories are dissimilar then the motor signal or changes in motor signals would be more enhanced irrespective of the age category. In this regard, just a clarification will suffice. I am a bit confused as the Experimental paradigm presented in figure 1 clearly shows a single motor task (I guess squeezing wrist of one hand with the other)

In line 124, authors suggest that a priory knowledge about the cortical activation during movements execution implies that motor brain response is determined as a pronounced event-related desynchronization (ERD) of mu-oscillations in the contralateral area of the motor cortex. Therefore, they recorded and analysed activity from symmetric sensors C3 and C4 respectively to record mu band response time (MBRT).

I am wondering based on the recent literature (which is by the way not referenced) Transient human movement is served by a specific pattern of neural oscillatory activity, particularly in the beta band (14–30 Hz). Briefly, prior to and during movement, there is a strong decrease in beta activity relative to baseline levels, known as the peri-movement beta event-related desynchronization (ERD), which begins about 1.0 s before movement onset and dissipates shortly after movement concludes. Actually, roughly 500 ms following stimulus offset or the completion of an actual voluntary or passive movement, or imagined movement, the beta rhythm increases in magnitude with respect to a baseline period preceding the event. This period of ERS is often termed the post-movement beta rebound (PMBR). Could you please comment why are you not looking at this Beta ERD. Is not that relevant? I guess post-movement you should also check PMBR for frequency and amplitude. May be something interesting out there.

Based on my understanding of the aging literature on motor signals, older adults exhibited an almost threefold increase in spontaneous beta power in the primary motor cortices, as well as significantly stronger beta ERD in the same regions compared to younger adults. Furthermore, it has been shown that during simple movements, these beta-band oscillations reliably peak in the precentral gyri bilaterally with stronger activity contralateral to movement, while more complex movements (and some simple movements) also induce activity in the supplementary motor area and bilateral premotor cortices, postcentral gyri, parietal cortices, and cerebellum. Perhaps, it would be necessary to discuss about this in the intro and discussion section.

Results

On the contrary, the between-subject differences were found in the spatial cluster, which included Cp3, Cpz, and Cp4 sensors (dorsal stream region of the sensorimotor area). Did you carry out a source analysis or else how are you actually talking about Ventral and Dorsal regions? I thought you have restricted your analysis at the sensor level. I wouldn’t talk about network based on sensor based functional connectivity in the time or frequency domain. Spatial resolution is too poor to argue for this even if you observed spectro-temporal signatures. For example, you mention distributed frontoparietal network. If I ask you how many areas do you think FPN comprise of then how would you answer? Please stick to sensor level analysis of your brain signals and connectivity patterns among sensor groups to discern changes. I don’t agree that talking about brain areas, ventral and dorsal streams etc. and above all networks makes much sense. You would probably admit networks in this case is loosely defined. This is also not a MEG Study.

Discussion

I enjoyed reading the discussion section. Having said that I think the authors need to seriously look at some of their assertions. One thing is to see a change and other is to speculate about those change. Many of the formulations and interpretations are at this point remain unverified and speculative. Not a clear demonstration yet. However the good thing is that the authors have cited and covered references which are most relevant to their findings based on mainly two types of analysis time-frequency and functional connectivity. Further, they discussed various mechanisms which render support to their empirical observations and relates to existing literature. In particular, this is one of the key reason why instead of listing a series of plausible mechanisms, the authors could identify and evaluate quantitatively one or two key mechanisms highlighted in the discussion section. For example, the observation of increase in theta band activity/power in the Frontal sensors and in the sensors near sensorimotor areas could be sensorimotor integration (provided by Bland’s model) mechanisms or this could be long range connectivity change between distal brain areas leading to an elevation in the theta band power. Also, I don’t like the fact that you keep mentioning throughout the article about brain areas while you actually carry out all of your analysis primarily at the EEG sensor level. They clearly do not commensurate with each other. Please, see my previous comments on this issue persistent in this manuscript.

Please see below an example of what I mean.

You write in line number 340-342 “that It should be noted that the significant increase of the theta-band power related to the dominant hand decline in elderly adults was observed in the dorsal stream region of the sensorimotor cortex and associated with the motor 342 planning but not with audio command processing”. How do you establish that? You don’t look at structure/anatomy neither you acquire MEG/fMRI data nor do any source estimation. Then I fail to understand what’s actually the basis for making the above statement. The above may be possible but to pinpoint that precisely you do need concurrent EEG-MRI evidence in my opinion.

Minor

Line number 190 may have a typo. Please check. Same pattern of typo recurring throughout the manuscript. Actually spotted them in several other places in this manuscript. Please revisit those sections where these typos are present and rectify.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Sep 16;15(9):e0233942. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0233942.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


5 Aug 2020

Response to the Reviewer #1: First, we would like to thank the Reviewer for a careful reading of our paper, pointing the major and minor issues, and a detailed feedback, which has been extremely helpful for improving the quality of the revised manuscript. We are also delighted to know that the Reviewer has found our study interesting and well-designed. We have done our best to address all the issues raised by the Reviewer. Below, please find our response to the Reviewer’s comments.

Reviewer #1 Comments

Abstract and title

Comment: Perhaps it would be better if you change the article title and the abstract a bit. My reading of your article gives me an impression that this article mainly concerns about movement initiation phase rather than the entire motor planning strategy and contingency. You are interested in understanding behavioural characteristics RT, Response Accuracy etc. is affected due to differences ensuing in the motor initiation phase. In my opinion, a more appropriate title replacement would be “Age-related slowing down in the motor initiation in elderly adults”.

Answer: We thank the reviewer for the criticism. We found that the referee’s suggestions about the title fits better to the main results presented in our study, so we corrected it accordingly. We also modified the abstract putting more emphasis on our experimental observation than on our conclusions.

Materials and methods

Comment: In methods section, you talk about background recording before recording active phase. You could simply say 5 minutes Eyes Open Resting state. They don’t appear to be any different to me. In the active phase you say you have 60 fine motor tasks per participant and 30 tasks per hand. The duration of the beep short or long provides cue which hand (dominant vs. non-dominant) to use. This is fine, but how different are these 60 tasks actually from each other. Are they really all different or similar? Could you provide a statistical summary or similarity measure to point out the differences between categories of finer motor task categories. If the task categories are dissimilar then the motor signal or changes in motor signals would be more enhanced irrespective of the age category. In this regard, just a clarification will suffice. I am a bit confused as the Experimental paradigm presented in figure 1 clearly shows a single motor task (I guess squeezing wrist of one hand with the other)

Answer: We thank the reviewer for this comment. Indeed, the experimental task in the original manuscript was described in misleading form. We should have used a term ‘repetitions’ instead of ‘tasks’, since each participant was asked to perform multiple repetitions of the same fine motor task (squeezing a hand into a wrist after the audio signal and holding it until the second signal) using either left or right hand (30 repetitions per hand, 60 in total). We clarified this point in the revised version of the manuscript. We also corrected the description of the background recordings accordingly.

Comment: In line 124, authors suggest that a priory knowledge about the cortical activation during movements execution implies that motor brain response is determined as a pronounced event-related desynchronization (ERD) of mu-oscillations in the contralateral area of the motor cortex. Therefore, they recorded and analysed activity from symmetric sensors C3 and C4 respectively to record mu band response time (MBRT).

I am wondering based on the recent literature (which is by the way not referenced) Transient human movement is served by a specific pattern of neural oscillatory activity, particularly in the beta band (14–30 Hz). Briefly, prior to and during movement, there is a strong decrease in beta activity relative to baseline levels, known as the peri-movement beta event-related desynchronization (ERD), which begins about 1.0 s before movement onset and dissipates shortly after movement concludes. Actually, roughly 500 ms following stimulus offset or the completion of an actual voluntary or passive movement, or imagined movement, the beta rhythm increases in magnitude with respect to a baseline period preceding the event. This period of ERS is often termed the post-movement beta rebound (PMBR). Could you please comment why are you not looking at this Beta ERD. Is not that relevant? I guess post-movement you should also check PMBR for frequency and amplitude. May be something interesting out there.

Answer: We thank the reviewer for this valuable remark. Indeed, the level of neocortical beta-band oscillations is considered as relevant marker of declined motor performance in healthy ageing and disease. Also, a peri-movement beta-band ERD emerging slightly before the motor action is known to be associated with motor planning [Heinrichs-Graham E., et al. (2016). Journal of cognitive neuroscience]. According to the reviewer’s comment we have discussed this topic in the intro section.

We have also modified the analysis of MBRT by additional consideration of MBRT in the beta-band (see Fig.2 and subsection ‘Motor brain response time analysis’ in the Results section). Specifically, during this analysis we have found that beta-band MBRT reflects the same properties as a mu-band MBRT (the fastest brain response has been observed in RH condition in YA group). Also, we observed that beta-band MBRT is significantly lower than mu-band MBRT, that is consistent with the existing literature and reviewer’s comment.

We suppose, that the provided extended analysis of MBRT has gained the relevance of our conclusions.

We also thank the reviewer for an interesting idea’s for the continuation of a current research.

Comment: Based on my understanding of the aging literature on motor signals, older adults exhibited an almost threefold increase in spontaneous beta power in the primary motor cortices, as well as significantly stronger beta ERD in the same regions compared to younger adults. Furthermore, it has been shown that during simple movements, these beta-band oscillations reliably peak in the precentral gyri bilaterally with stronger activity contralateral to movement, while more complex movements (and some simple movements) also induce activity in the supplementary motor area and bilateral premotor cortices, postcentral gyri, parietal cortices, and cerebellum. Perhaps, it would be necessary to discuss about this in the intro and discussion section.

Answer: We agree with the reviewer’s comment. We have added a discussion about a significance of peri-and post-movement beta oscillations in the intro section. However, in the current study we were mostly focused on the pre-movement neuronal activity (after the audio cue and before mu- and beta-band ERD) and the provided statistical analysis in spatio-temporal domain did not reveal any significant age-related changes in cortical activation besides the increased theta-band activity in central-parietal sensors within this time frame. Maybe, this could be a consequence of experimental design and, particularly, a quite simple motor task, which execution may not strongly involve complex motor planning operations usually associated with early beta-band ERD.

Results

Comment: On the contrary, the between-subject differences were found in the spatial cluster, which included Cp3, Cpz, and Cp4 sensors (dorsal stream region of the sensorimotor area). Did you carry out a source analysis or else how are you actually talking about Ventral and Dorsal regions? I thought you have restricted your analysis at the sensor level. I wouldn’t talk about network based on sensor based functional connectivity in the time or frequency domain. Spatial resolution is too poor to argue for this even if you observed spectro-temporal signatures. For example, you mention distributed frontoparietal network. If I ask you how many areas do you think FPN comprise of then how would you answer? Please stick to sensor level analysis of your brain signals and connectivity patterns among sensor groups to discern changes. I don’t agree that talking about brain areas, ventral and dorsal streams etc. and above all networks makes much sense. You would probably admit networks in this case is loosely defined. This is also not a MEG Study.

Answer: We agree with the reviewer’s opinion. We have modified the Results section by sticking to the sensor-level description of the obtained results and excluding misleading formulations.

Discussion

Comment: I enjoyed reading the discussion section. Having said that I think the authors need to seriously look at some of their assertions. One thing is to see a change and other is to speculate about those change. Many of the formulations and interpretations are at this point remain unverified and speculative. Not a clear demonstration yet. However the good thing is that the authors have cited and covered references which are most relevant to their findings based on mainly two types of analysis time-frequency and functional connectivity. Further, they discussed various mechanisms which render support to their empirical observations and relates to existing literature. In particular, this is one of the key reason why instead of listing a series of plausible mechanisms, the authors could identify and evaluate quantitatively one or two key mechanisms highlighted in the discussion section. For example, the observation of increase in theta band activity/power in the Frontal sensors and in the sensors near sensorimotor areas could be sensorimotor integration (provided by Bland’s model) mechanisms or this could be long range connectivity change between distal brain areas leading to an elevation in the theta band power. Also, I don’t like the fact that you keep mentioning throughout the article about brain areas while you actually carry out all of your analysis primarily at the EEG sensor level. They clearly do not commensurate with each other. Please, see my previous comments on this issue persistent in this manuscript.

Answer: First, we would like to thank the reviewer for a positive feedback to our discussion section. Here, we also agree with the reviewer’s opinion that many interpretations and formulations are rather speculative. In the revised version of the Manuscript, we have tried to clarify and modify the most unsuccessful conclusions and formulation, to be less speculative and mostly associated with our observations.

Minor

Comment: Line number 190 may have a typo. Please check. Same pattern of typo recurring throughout the manuscript. Actually spotted them in several other places in this manuscript. Please revisit those sections where these typos are present and rectify.

Answer: We guess, that this typo is a ‘pre-motor phase’. We have corrected this typo by replacing it with a ‘pre-movement phase’ or ‘motor initiation phase’.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response_Letter.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Mukesh Dhamala

2 Sep 2020

Age-related slowing down in the motor initiation in elderly adults

PONE-D-20-14466R1

Dear Dr. Frolov,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Mukesh Dhamala, Ph. D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Mukesh Dhamala

4 Sep 2020

PONE-D-20-14466R1

Age-related slowing down in the motor initiation in elderly adults

Dear Dr. Frolov:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Mukesh Dhamala

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_Letter.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    The data underlying the results presented in the study is available at https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12301181.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES