Abstract
Background:
Many studies have attempted to identify the sources of interindividual variability in response to theta-burst stimulation (TBS). However, these studies have been limited by small sample sizes, leading to conflicting results.
Objective/Hypothesis:
This study brought together over 60 TMS researchers to form the ‘Big TMS Data Collaboration’, and create the largest known sample of individual participant TBS data to date. The goal was to enable a more comprehensive evaluation of factors driving TBS response variability.
Methods:
118 corresponding authors of TMS studies were emailed and asked to provide deidentified individual TMS data. Mixed-effects regression investigated a range of individual and study level variables for their contribution to variability in response to iTBS and cTBS.
Results:
430 healthy participants’ TBS data was pooled across 22 studies (mean age = 41.9; range = 17 – 82; females = 217). Baseline MEP amplitude, age, target muscle, time of day, and TMS machine significantly predicted iTBS-induced plasticity. Baseline MEP amplitude, and timepoint after TBS significantly predicted cTBS-induced plasticity.
Conclusions:
This is the largest known study of interindividual variability in TBS. Our findings indicate that a significant portion of variability can be attributed to the methods used to measure the modulatory effects of TBS. We provide specific methodological recommendations in order to control and mitigate these sources of variability.
Introduction
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) protocols delivering trains of repetitive pulses (i.e. rTMS) are widely used to induce plasticity in the human brain [1]. These rTMS protocols are used experimentally, to assess the brain’s response to modulation [2–3], and also clinically, to treat neuropsychiatric disorders such as major depressive disorder [4–5] and obsessive compulsive disorder [6]. One form of rTMS, theta-burst stimulation (TBS) holds particular promise in the induction of brain plasticity given that it requires lower stimulus intensities and shorter stimulation times than previous rTMS protocols [7]. At present however, the effects of TBS are highly variable across participants [8–9], limiting its utility as a clinical and experimental tool. Several studies have investigated interindividual variability in response to TBS [8, 10–14]. However, these single site studies have been limited by small sample sizes, preventinga thorough evaluation of the sources of TBS variability. The current paper describes an approach that brings together over 60 TMS researchers to form the ‘Big TMS Data Collaboration’ (Supplementary file 1), where we share individual participant data across studies to increase sample size and statistical power. Similar to other neuroscience consortia, which have long been common in neuroimaging [15–16], we form this collaboration in the hope that large-scale analyses will allow us to answer questions that have not been able to be answered with single-site research. Here in the first instance, we investigate factors accounting for interindividual variability in response to TBS.
Methods
This project was deemed exempt from ethical review by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Committee because it involved only the use of preexisting, non-identifiable or re-identifiable data. All primary studies had been approved by local institutional review boards, and all participants had provided informed consent.
Article identification strategy
The present TBS analysis comes from a larger project also collecting single and paired-pulse TMS data and input-output (I/O) curve data. A systematic search for all of these data was conducted in PubMed in February 2017. A search for each TMS protocol was conducted separately using the combination of synonyms of the following terms: intermittent theta-burst stimulation; continuous theta-burst stimulation; short-interval intracortical inhibition; intracortical facilitation; input-output curve; stimulus-response curve; transcranial magnetic stimulation. The full search syntax is provided in Supplementary file 2. Given our aim of collecting individual participant data from corresponding authors, it was unrealistic to collect data from all previous TMS studies implementing these protocols. Rather, we set a goal of collecting at least 10 studies’ individual participant data per TMS protocol. The initial search was limited to studies published between 1/1/2014 and 31/12/2016, as we reasoned that there was a higher likelihood of obtaining data from more recent studies. For iTBS and I/O curve data, the search was expanded to include studies from 2013, and studies from 2012 and 2013 for cTBS, because we could not initially collect enough individual participant data to meet our goal.
Inclusion criteria were: studies using a figure-of-eight coil and studies measuring TMS responses from intrinsic hand muscles. Exclusion criteria were: non-human subjects, and TBS applied to a brain region other than the M1 representation from which motor evoked potentials (MEPs) were collected.
Titles of all articles identified by the search were exported to a spreadsheet for iTBS, cTBS, SICI, ICF, and I/O curve data separately. To avoid bias in study selection, we then ran a random number generator to identify the studies from which we would attempt to collect individual participant data. If an article met inclusion criteria, the corresponding author of the study was emailed and asked for participants’ age, gender, motor threshold (MT), and baseline and conditioned MEP amplitudes. We screened studies and emailed authors in blocks of 20 studies at a time until we reached our goal of at least 10 studies per protocol. Corresponding authors were asked to de-identify data prior to sending, and the template spreadsheet was sent with numbers pre-filled 1-x along a ‘Participant ID’ column. Formal collection ceased in August 2017. Unpublished data, and other TMS data not identified within our search, were also included through informal communication with colleagues or corresponding authors. These data were subject to inclusion/exclusion criteria, but were not constrained by publication date.
Variables of interest and data used for present TBS analyses
All participants’ data were collected from studies, yet only healthy participant data were analysed within the present paper. For each individual, conditioned MEP amplitudes (i.e. post-TBS MEPs) were normalised to the baseline MEP amplitude using the equation: (conditioned MEP amplitude / baseline MEP amplitude) × 100 [9, 13, 17]. A value of 100% represents no change, values <100% represent suppression, and values >100% indicate facilitation, of MEP amplitudes following TBS. This value is referred to as ‘normalised MEP’ henceforth, and was used as the dependent variable (DV) for all main analyses. Note that some previous studies have used a ‘percentage of change’ value to quantify TBS effects (conditioned MEP amplitude - baseline MEP amplitude) / (baseline MEP amplitude) × 100 [3, 14]. However, this value and normalised MEP give the exact same output for regression analysis (Supplementary file 3).
For the main analyses investigating IVs predicting iTBS and cTBS-induced plasticity, as per the results of Chung et al., [18], who demonstrated changes in MEP amplitudes at early (<5 minutes), mid (20–30 minutes), and late (50–60 minutes) post TBS, we analysed MEP amplitudes up to 60 minutes post-TBS. A grand average normalised MEP amplitude was then created for each participant by averaging normalised data across all (0–60 minute) timepoints [9, 14]. Subsequent analyses investigated TBS-induced plasticity at different timepoints (see ‘Additional analyses‘ section).
The following independent variables (IVs) were analysed for their influence on interindividual variability in TBS data: age, gender, baseline MEP amplitude, TMS machine, target muscle, M1 hemisphere, TBS intensity, test (TS) stimulus intensity, pulse waveform, the use/absence of neuronavigation, MT (monophasic RMT, monophasic AMT, biphasic RMT and biphasic AMT), time of day, and the number of baseline MEPs collected. For time of day, we grouped data into morning (7am – 11.59am); afternoon (12pm – 2.59pm); and evening (3pm – 7pm) groups. These times were defined based upon an effort to make the study/participant numbers approximately equal between groups. For the number of MEPs collected, the uneven distribution of the data did not allow this to be analysed as a continuous variable, therefore we split the dataset into a low number of MEPs collected (<20), or a high number of MEPs collected (> 20), based on the median value. We collected handedness data for nine studies, yet there were only four left handers, therefore this IV could not be analysed statistically.
Studies used either a Magstim Rapid TMS machine, a Nexstim NBS TMS, or a MagPro TMS machine. We could not determine the specific MagPro model used in all studies, therefore these machines were grouped based on the brand. We also controlled for pulse waveform in regression analyses to ensure that the effect of TMS machine was not due the differential use of monophasic or biphasic pulses. For the comparison of TS intensities, most studies either used 120% of RMT or a percentage of maximum stimulator output evoking a 1 mV MEP, thus a comparison was made between these intensities. Two studies used neither method [19–20] and were therefore excluded from this comparison.
Three studies used I/O curves to measure corticospinal excitability pre and post TBS [21–23]. Here, to maintain uniformity and increase statistical power, we included data elicited by the 120% of RMT stimulus intensity within the I/O curves. We were not able to obtain baseline MEP amplitude data from one study [24]. To ensure that this study’s data were not excluded from any regression analysis (i.e. listwise deletion) including baseline MEP amplitude as a IV, we imputed these missing values based on a Gaussian normal regression model in Stata 15.0 [25–26]. For studies that tested the effect of external interventions on TMS outcomes (e.g. exercise - McDonnell et al., [27]), only control/baseline data were analysed.
We verified the accuracy of the data sent to us by comparing the results to group mean data in the corresponding published paper. In cases of discrepancies, corresponding authors were contacted for clarification. In instances where accuracy could not be verified, the study was excluded. See Figure 1 for adapted PRISMA flowchart [28].
Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart.
Adapted from Liberati et al. (2009). Note that some studies employed more than one TMS protocol, resulting in more datasets than studies. IPD = individual participant data.
All statistical analyses were conducted using Stata 15.0 (StataCorp, USA). First, normalised MEP data were checked for outliers using histograms and descriptive statistics. A number of outliers were detected, therefore values falling outside of the 2nd and 98th percentiles (for iTBS and cTBS protocols separately) were winsorized [29–30]. See Supplementary file 4 for histograms prior to winsorization.
Variability analysis
Prior to our main analyses investigating IVs predicting interindivdual variability in response to TBS, we sought to characterise the variability of the data across our collected sample. As per the method of Brown et al., [31], we calculated intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), standard deviation (SD), and coefficient of variation (CV) [32] values to assess within participant, within study, and between study variability of TBS data. Within participant SDs and CVs were calculated using the normalised MEP value across TBS timepoints. Note that this differs to previous studies’ evaluations of within participant variability/reliability, which have used within-participant responses to TBS across separate sessions [33–34]. Within study values were calculated using the normalised MEP of participants within each study, and between study values were calculated using the mean normalised MEP value of each study [31]. ICC values < 0.50 were considered low; values 0.50 – 0.75 considered moderate; and > 0.75 considered high [35]. High ‘within participant’ ICC values reflect smaller normalised MEP variance within participants (i.e. across TBS timepoints) relative to larger variance within study (or between participants), while high ‘within study’ ICC values represent smaller within, relative to larger between, study variance [36].
Main regression analysis
Main analyses investigated IVs predicting iTBS and cTBS-induced plasticity. Here, we used mixed-effects linear regression using a ‘one-step’ model as described by Riley et al., [37], using ‘study ID’ as a random factor. This preserves the nesting of participants within studies, given that it is inappropriate to simply analyze individual-participant data as if they all came from a single study [37]. We used forward-stepwise regression in two stages for each TMS protocol [38]. We chose this method because we had a large set of potential predictors, and we wanted to identify IVs that had a remaining independent relationship with the dependent variable.
Stage 1 regressions analysed the variance in normalised MEP explained by each IV separately, while controlling for the age and gender of participants. Age and gender were included in all stage 1 models given that these are individual characteristics for which we had data for all participants. IVs with p-values < 0.10 were added to the regression model in stage 2, while IVs with p-values > 0.10 were dropped.
The stage 2 starting regression model comprised of all IVs that were p < 0.10 in stage 1. Consecutive regressions then iterated through IVs that were dropped in stage 1, to see whether these IVs now obtained a p-value < 0.10 controlling for IVs in the starting stage 2 model. Thus, the final regression model comprised of IVs that obtained a p-value < 0.10 in predicting TBS-induced plasticity in either stage 1 or 2 regressions.
IVs were omitted from regression analyses for three possible reasons. First, an IV was omitted if it was not comprised of at least three studies within each IV level, given that unreliable estimates may have resulted from a smaller number of studies per level. For example, the IV ‘M1 hemisphere’ was included only if both left and right hemisphere data were each collected in at least three separate studies. Second, an IV was omitted if its inclusion led to a substantial reduction in the overall sample size of the regression analysis for that DV, due to that IV only being measured in a subset of studies. We defined a ‘substantial reduction of the regression sample size’ as cases where two or more studies were excluded from the regression analysis. Third, an IV was omitted because of collinearity, which occurred if two types of MTs were included in the same regression model. To avoid this, if two or more types of MTs had a p-value < 0.10 in stage 1 regressions, for stage 2 we included only the MT that was the strongest predictor of normalised MEP.
Given the presence of non-linearity and non-normality, robust variance estimates were used for all regressions. Adjusted marginal means (just ‘marginal means’ henceforth) estimated the mean normalised MEP amplitude adjusted/controlled for all other variables in the regression model, allowing for an interpretable estimate of TBS-induced plasticity [39].
Post-hoc analyses
Where sufficient data, post-hoc analyses were run on IVs that were omitted from the main regression analyses for any of the three aforementioned reasons. Next, given the appearance of bimodal groupings of age (see Figures 4 and 7), we also analysed normalised MEP with age split into a younger and older group, based on median age (iTBS = 50 years; cTBS = 30 years). Lastly, scatterplots indicated possible non-linear relationships between normalised MEP and age and also baseline MEP amplitude. Therefore, we re-analysed these relationships using quadratic and cubic regression models [40]. All post-hoc analyses controlled for all other IVs in the final regression model.
Figure 4. Relationships between normalised MEP amplitude and continuous IVs in final iTBS regression model.
Our main analyses using linear regression showed that baseline MEP was a significant predictor of iTBS normalised MEP, while age did not reach significance (Table 3). However, post-hoc analyses demonstrated a significant cubic relationship for age, and a significant quadratic relationship for baseline MEP amplitude. These bivariate scatterplots are presented to give an indication of relationships only. See regression analyses for results controlling for all IVs in final model. Green line fits a smoothed ‘lowess’ curve through data (smoothing level = 0.8 - default).
Figure 7. Relationships between normalised MEP amplitude and continuous IVs in final cTBS regression model.
Our main linear regression analyses showed that baseline MEP, but not age was a significant predictor of cTBS normalised MEP (Table 4). Post-hoc analyses also demonstrated significant cubic and quadratic relationships for baseline MEP amplitude. Green line fits a smoothed ‘lowess’ curve through data.
Additional analyses
A number of additional analyses were performed to further explore the data. First, we sought to determine the effect of time on TBS-induced plasticity. To do this, we created the IV ‘timepoint’, and grouped data into 10 minutes intervals post-TBS (i.e. data collected 0–10 minutes post TBS; 10–20 minutes, etc.), up until the point at which fewer than three studies collected data in that time interval (as per our criteria for the necessary number of studies per level in the main regression analyses). For example, only one iTBS study [3] collected MEPs > 30 minutes post-TBS, thus our last time interval for iTBS was 20–30 minutes. There were two exceptions to this: first, we had sufficient data to split the first time interval into 0–5 minutes and 5–10 minutes post-TBS; and second, only two studies collected MEPs 40–50 minutes post cTBS, thus we grouped these data into a time interval of 40–60 minutes (4 studies). To analyse the effect of time, we re-ran our final iTBS and cTBS regression analyses while additionally including the ‘timepoint’ IV. Post-hoc analyses compared normalised MEP data between each of the timepoints, and also to 100 (no TBS-induced plasticity).
Next, given evidence that TBS-induced effects may be strongest at early timepoints [17–18], and the results of the above timepoint analysis demonstrating that plasticity was only present 0–10 minutes following cTBS, we sought to determine whether the results of our main analyses may have differed if data were analysed within this earlier time interval where plasticity was strongest. To do this, we repeated our main regression analyses whilst only including normalised MEP data from 0–10 minutes following TBS.
Lastly, we investigated whether 70% RMT and 80% AMT TS intensity methods might deliver TBS at different machine output intensities, given these methods have previously shown to result in differences in TBS-induced plasticity [41]. To do this, we used the marginal means of biphasic RMT and biphasic AMT as computed in our companion paper [42], and then multiplied these values by 0.7 (i.e., 70% of biphasic RMT) and 0.8 (i.e., 80% of biphasic AMT), respectively.
Results
Individual participant data from 22 TBS studies (Table 1) and 430 healthy participants (mean age = 41.9; SE = 1.02; range = 17 – 82; males = 213) were included in the present analysis (Figure 1).
Table 1.
Characteristics of included studies.
Study | Author/s | Participants | TMS protocols |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Barhoun (unp.) | 13 healthy (5F, 22.1 ± 3.0 y) | cTBS |
2 | Di Lazzaro et al. (2008) | 12 stroke patients (5F, 69.4 ± 9.5 y), 12 controls (2F, 63.2 ± 5.3 y) | iTBS & cTBS |
3 | Di Lazzaro et al. (2011) | 10 healthy (7F, 26.6 ± 4.1 y) | SICI, ICF, iTBS, cTBS |
4 | Dickins et al. (2015) | 20 younger (10F, 22.9 ± 2.5 y) and 20 older participants (10F, 70.2 ± 3.1 y) | iTBS |
5 | Dileone et al. (2016) | 16 healthy (10F, 23.2 ± 3.8 y) | iTBS |
6 | Do et al. (2018) | 20 healthy (14F, 26.5 ± 3.1 y) | cTBS |
7 | Fried et al. (2017) | 28 type 2 diabetes patients (12F, 65.8 ± 7.7 y), 22 AD patients (13F, 69.6 ± 7.4 y), 26 healthy (13F, 62.9 ± 8.9 y) | SICI, ICF, iTBS |
8 | Goldsworthy et al. (2016) | 18 healthy (10F, 22.1 ± 4.4 y) | iTBS |
9 | Gomes-Osman (unp.) | 17 healthy (10F, 30.0 ± 12.9 y) | SICI, ICF, iTBS |
10 | Jannati et al. (2017) | 30 healthy (3F, 36.0 ± 14.4 y) | cTBS |
11 | Koch et al. (2016) | 40 AD patients (17F, 71.0 ± 6.4 y) and 24 healthy (12F, 69.3 ± 2.3 y) | iTBS, cTBS |
12 | Lee et al. (2014) | 18 healthy (12F, 73.8 ± 5.1 y) | cTBS |
13 | McDonnell et al. (2013) | 25 healthy (9F, 26.8 ± 8.1 y) | cTBS |
14 | Morris (unp.) | 15 healthy (9F, 25 ± 2.7 y) | SICI, ICF, iTBS |
15 | Munneke et al. (2013) | 10 ALS patients (10M, 57.8 ± 1.8 y) and 10 controls (0F, 49.0 ± 3.6 y) | SICI, ICF, cTBS |
16 | Nettekoven et al. (2014) | 16 healthy (9F, 27.0 ± 3.0 y) | iTBS |
17 | Opie et al. (2013) | 13 sleep apnoea patients (2F, 42.6 ± 10.2 y), 11 controls (2F, 43.0 ± 10.3 y) | SICI, cTBS |
18 | Puri et al. (2016) | 33 healthy (21F, 66.0 ± 4.8 y) | iTBS |
19 | Singh et al. (2016) | 10 healthy (6F, 25.4 ± 4.0 y) | SICI, ICF, cTBS |
20 | Vallence et al. (2015) | 18 healthy (10F, 23.1 ± 4.0 y) | cTBS |
21 | Vernet et al. (2014) | 10 healthy (5F, 33.0 ± 18.0 y) | cTBS |
22 | Young-Bernier et al. (2014) | 20 younger (13F, 22.3 ± 3.2 y) and 18 older healthy (9F, 70.1 ± 5.6 y) | iTBS |
Note: age mean and standard deviation are shown. Abbreviations: F = females; y = years old; GAD = generalised anxiety disorder; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; ALS = amyotrophic lateral sclerosis; MDD = major depressive disorder; I/O = input/output; FDI = first dorsal interosseous; APB = abductor pollicis brevis.
Figure 2 shows the distribution of normalised MEP data for each protocol. 67.9% of participants (180 of 265) demonstrated iTBS-induced facilitation and 65.4% of participants (137 of 211) demonstrated cTBS-induced suppression of MEP amplitudes. There was no evidence of bimodal groupings for ‘responders’ and ‘non-responders’ to TBS, as per previous suggestions [13, 43].
Figure 2. Distribution plots.
Histograms show distribution of normalised MEP data for iTBS and cTBS protocols. Y-axis shows the percentage of responses within each normalised MEP bin of 10 along the X-axis.
Variability analysis
Table 2 shows variability for iTBS and cTBS data. ICCs demonstrate low to moderate reliability of normalised MEP within participants, reflecting some grouping of participant data across repeated TBS timepoints. However, within study reliability was almost non-existent for both iTBS and cTBS normalised MEP, demonstrating very little grouping of study data (relative to the entire sample of data). This is also reflected by CVs, showing that while variability is relatively low between studies, there is high variability within studies.
Table 2. Variability of iTBS and cTBS data.
ICC = intraclass correlation coefficient; SD = standard deviation; CV = coefficient of variation %.
ICC within participants | ICC within studies | SD within studies | SD between studies | CV within studies (%) | CV between studies (%) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
iTBS | 0.53 | 0.00 | 43.08 | 15.28 | 34.61 | 12.04 |
cTBS | 0.34 | 0.04 | 27.02 | 12.19 | 30.31 | 13.49 |
iTBS regression analysis
The final iTBS regression model demonstrated that baseline MEP amplitude and target muscle were significantly associated with changes in iTBS-induced plasticity (Table 3 and Figure 3). See Supplementary file 5 for results of all stage 1 and 2 iTBS regressions.
Table 3. Final iTBS regression model.
B-values for continuous IVs show the amount of increase in normalised MEP, for a one unit increase in the IV, after adjusting for all other variables in the model. i.e. a 1 unit increase in age (i.e. 1 year) resulted in a 0.18% reduction in iTBS-induced facilitation. For categorical IVs, the B-value shows the difference between the IV levels in normalised MEP. e.g. the APB muscle demonstrated 16.41% less iTBS-induced facilitation than the FDI muscle (see Figure 5 for all IV levels). Bold denotes significance (p < 0.05). Participants = 265; studies = 12.
IV | B | SE | 95% CIs | ß | P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | −0.18 | 0.10 | −0.38 | - | 0.02 | −0.09 | 0.071 |
Baseline MEP | −10.93 | 2.73 | −16.27 | - | −5.58 | −0.23 | <0.001 |
Machine | 5.63 | 5.91 | −5.95 | - | 17.20 | 0.13 | 0.341 |
Muscle | −16.41 | 4.61 | −25.44 | - | −7.38 | −0.37 | <0.001 |
TBS intensity | 4.94 | 6.09 | −6.98 | - | 16.87 | 0.11 | 0.417 |
Neuronavigation use | −9.41 | 5.93 | −2.21 | 21.02 | −0.21 | 0.113 |
Figure 3. Regression flowchart for iTBS.
Figure shows the method employed to arrive at the final regression model, demonstrating IVs accounting for interindividual variability in iTBS normalised MEP. IVs were omitted if they did not include at least three studies within each IV level. Stage 1 regressions analysed the variance in normalised MEP explained by each IV separately, while controlling for age and gender. IVs were dropped from the model if they did not obtain a p-value < 0.10. Step 2 regressions again iterated through IVs that were dropped in stage 1, to see whether these IVs now obtained a p-value < 0.10 controlling for IVs in the starting stage 2 model. Thus, the final regression model comprised of IVs that obtained a p-value < 0.10 in predicting TBS-induced plasticity in either stage 1 or 2 regressions. See Table 3 for results.
Note that two iTBS studies using a MagPro machine used biphasic pulses to collect pre and post TBS MEPs [3, 44], yet we could not control for pulse waveform in the iTBS regression model due to insufficient data (Figure 3). Thus, we re-calculated the effect of TMS machine with these studies excluded (Magstim Rapid, studies = 6; N = 169 vs. MagPro, studies = 4; N = 53). In this case, TMS machine was a significant predictor of iTBS normalised MEP: Magstim Rapid marginal mean = 119.43; SE = 1.14 vs. MagPro marginal mean = 129.97; SE = 3.72; p = 0.014.
Figure 4 presents bivariate relationships for continuous IVs age and baseline MEP, which were both included in the final iTBS regression model.
Figure 5 shows the iTBS normalised MEP marginal means for IVs included in the final regression model. Overall, there was significant iTBS-induced facilitation of MEP amplitudes across this sample (orange bar).
Figure 5. Marginal means for iTBS normalised MEP amplitude.
Marginal means provide an estimate of normalised MEP, adjusted for all variables in the final regression model. Orange bar shows the overall marginal mean for iTBS. Grey and white bars show marginal means for each level of the IVs TMS machine, target muscle, and TBS intensity, and neuronavigation (NN) use, which were included in the final model. * denotes a significant difference between levels (p < 0.05. All samples showed significant facilitation from 100 (all p < 0.001). Note that when excluding two MagPro machine studies that used biphasic pulses (for pre/post MEPs) there was significantly higher normalised MEP for MagPro machine (see Results). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals. Brackets show (studies/participants).
iTBS post-hoc analyses
With age split into younger and older groups based on the median value, there was slightly greater iTBS-induced facilitation for younger (marginal mean = 126.04; SE = 2.63), compared to older adults (marginal mean = 119.02; SE = 2.92), as in the main analysis. However, this was again non-significant (p = 0.115).
Although no significant linear relationship was found in the main regression (nor split by group, as above), there was a significant cubic relationship between iTBS normalised MEP and age (p = 0.038) (Figure 4). Further, in addition to a linear relationship (Table 3), there was a significant quadratic relationship between iTBS normalised MEP and baseline MEP amplitude (p < 0.001) (Figure 4).
Time of day significantly predicted iTBS-induced plasticity (χ2 = 7.46; df = 2; p = 0.024). Post-hoc pairwise comparisons showed that there was a significant difference between morning and afternoon groups (marginal mean morning = 132.96; SE 3.94, studies = 4; N = 32; marginal mean afternoon = 122.17; SE = 2.61, studies = 6; N = 50; p = 0.008), but no differences between these groups and the evening group (125.78; SE = 8.95, studies = 6; N = 24).
cTBS regression analysis
The final regression model (Figure 6) demonstrated that baseline MEP amplitude was significantly negatively associated with cTBS normalised MEP (Table 4). See Supplementary file 6 for results of all stage 1 and 2 cTBS regressions.
Figure 6. Regression flowchart for cTBS.
Figure shows the method employed to arrive at a final model, demonstrating IVs accounting for interindividual variability in cTBS normalised MEP. See Methods and Figure 3 caption for further explanation of method.
Table 4. Final cTBS regression model.
For every 1 mV increase in baseline MEP, there was on average a 5.32% reduction in cTBS normalised MEP (greater suppression). Bold denotes significance (p < 0.05). Participants = 211; studies = 13.
IV | B | SE | 95% CIs | ß | P | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 0.13 | 0.12 | −0.01 | - | 0.36 | 0.09 | 0.246 |
Baseline MEP | −5.32 | 1.59 | −8.44 | - | −2.20 | −0.14 | 0.001 |
Figure 7 shows bivariate relationships for continuous IVs baseline MEP and age, which were included in the final cTBS regression model.
Figure 8 (blue bar) shows that there was significant cTBS-induced suppression of MEPs across this sample (p = 0.018). With age split into younger and older groups based on the median value, there was slightly greater cTBS-induced suppression for younger adults, yet this was non-significant (p = 0.19) (Figure 8). Although TS intensity and pulse waveform did not reach our p-value threshold (< 0.10) for inclusion in the final regression model, we present these results given the debate on the influence of these IVs on TBS-induced plasticity [33, 45] (Figure 8). While there were moderate effect sizes for these IVs, neither were significant (pulse waveform = 0.174; TS intensity p = 0.186). Interestingly, our companion paper, investigating single and paired-pulse TMS, showed reduced MEP amplitudes and increased MEP variability for the 120% RMT TS intensity method in comparison to the 1 mV method [42]. There were insufficient data to evaluate whether these two IVs also showed the same trends in iTBS data.
Figure 8. Marginal means for cTBS normalised MEP amplitude.
Blue bar shows the overall marginal mean for cTBS. Grey and white bars show marginal means for each level of the IVs TS intensity, pulse waveform, and age group. These IVs were not included in the final regression model, but demonstrated moderate effect sizes between levels. * denotes a significant difference from 100 (suppression) (p < 0.05). Error bars show 95% confidence intervals. Brackets show (studies/participants). Slight discrepancy in age group N is due to a number of participants aged at the median value (30). These were placed into the ‘older’ group.
cTBS post-hoc analyses
Controlling for all variables included in the final model, biphasic AMT just fell short of out threshold for significance in predicting cTBS normalised MEP (B = −0.44; SE 0.23; ß = −0.17; p = 0.057. Time of day did not significantly predict cTBS-induced plasticity (χ2 = 1.44; df = 2; p = 0.488).
In addition to a linear relationship (Table 4), there were significant quadratic (p = 0.014) and cubic (p < 0.001) relationship between cTBS normalised MEP and baseline MEP amplitude (Figure 7).
Additional analyses
Figure 9 shows the effect of time on iTBS and cTBS effects. iTBS-induced plasticity was significant at all timepoints (up to 30 minutes post-iTBS), and there was no significant difference in plasticity across the timepoints (χ2 = 4.56; df = 3; p = 0.207). In contrast, cTBS-induced plasticity was only significant at 0–5 minute and 5–10 minute timepoints, and regression demonstrated a significant difference in plasticity across the timepoints (χ2 = 26.70; df = 5; p < 0.001) (Figure 9).
Figure 9. TBS-induced plasticity at different timepoints.
iTBS normalised MEP amplitude was significantly different from 100 at each timepoint (* denotes p < 0.05) and there were no significant differences between timepoints. cTBS plasticity was only significant at 0–5 and 5–10 minute timepoints. 0–5 minute and 5–10 minute timepoints demonstrated significantly greater cTBS plasticity than all other timepoints, except for the 30–40 minute timepoint (p = 0.10 and p = 0.08, respectively). No other pairwise comparisons were significant for cTBS. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals. Brackets show (studies/participants).
Our additional regression analyses including only ≤ 10 minutes post-TBS data demonstrated very similar results, with the exact same IVs being included in these iTBS and cTBS models as in the main regression analyses (due to obtaining a p-value < 0.10 in either stage 1 or 2 regressions) (Supplementary file 7).
Finally, there was only a small difference between the TMS machine output % used for the two TBS intensities: 70% of biphasic RMT = 33.93% versus 80% of biphasic AMT = 35.24%.
Discussion
This study pooled data from 22 studies to investigate factors contributing to interindividual variability in response to TBS. Our initial variability analysis largely agreed with previous research by demonstrating moderate levels of within participant reliability [14, 33–34, 46], yet much larger variability of between participant responses to TBS [11, 13, 46]. In attempting to identify the sources of this variability, we demonstrated that baseline MEP amplitude, target muscle, age, TMS machine, time of day, and TBS timepoint significantly predicted response to TBS. This indicates that a significant portion of variability can be attributed not only to TBS-induced plasticity itself, but also to the methods used to measure it, and highlights the need for greater standardisation in the methods used to measure TBS-induced plasticity.
Baseline MEP amplitude predicts TBS response
The clearest result from this study was the negative relationship between baseline MEP amplitude and normalised MEP, with smaller baseline MEP amplitudes related to larger relative MEPs amplitudes following TBS. However, post-hoc analyses demonstrated that this effect was not linear (also see Figures 4 and 7). We suggest a number of possible reasons for these patterns of response.
Regression to the mean
Regression to the mean is the statistical phenomenon by which an initial extreme measurement is more likely to be closer to the mean if measured for a second time, and should be considered when designing experimental studies [47–49]. For example, a large reduction in an extremely high blood pressure measurement in a drug trial at follow up does not necessarily demonstrate an effect of the intervention, because it is likely that any extreme initial measurement was partly attributable to chance [47–48]. High trial-to-trial MEP variability means that baseline MEP values are susceptible to chance, especially if relatively few MEPs are collected [32, 50–51]. This suggests that a portion of interindividual variability in TBS response might be attributable to regression to the mean after the collection of baseline MEPs that may not accurately reflect the individual’s typical level of corticospinal excitability.
Extreme baseline MEP values could also be partly attributable to an initial state of MEP hyperexcitability during TMS sessions [52–53]. Thus, depending on the time taken to carry out procedures such as finding motor hotspot and measuring MT, one participant’s baseline MEPs could be collected within a state of hyperexcitability, while another’s could be collected in a relatively steady state.
Floor and ceiling effects
Floor and ceiling effects occur when increases in TMS inputs at low or high intensities fail to produce changes in MEP amplitude due to a lack of activation (floor), or maximal activation (ceiling) of neurons that comprise the MEP [54–55]. While single-pulse (pre/post) TMS intensities are individualised, usually to 120% RMT or a 1 mV value, there can be substantial variability in relation to where these stimulus intensities occur in relation to each individual’s input/output curve [21, 56–57]. In other words, these individualised stimulus intensities can be relatively low or high between individuals. This can bias estimates of TBS plasticity between individuals, with a floor effect more likely in the former individuals, and a ceiling effect more likely in the latter individuals [21]. These effects could partly explain the non-linear relationships for baseline MEP in the present data, where TBS-induced plasticity first increased, and then decreased, in response to higher baseline MEP amplitudes (Figures 4 and 7). While speculative, this pattern could reflect a lower susceptibility to floor and ceiling effects for participants with baseline MEP amplitudes in the intermediate range (i.e. ~0.5 – 1.5 mV).
Interindividual variability in the neural circuits activated by the TMS pulse
The observed relationships to baseline MEP amplitude could also be explained by individual differences in the neural circuits activated by the TMS pulse assessing TBS plasticity. These differences may occur due to interindividual variability in the anatomy of the motor cortex, and variability in relative stimulus intensities between participants (as above) [58–59]. Studies using input/output curves have shown that iTBS-induced plasticity may be best detected at low stimulus intensities relative to an individual’s MT, whereas cTBS-induced plasticity may be best detected at high relative intensities, because these intensities might better assess the components of neural circuitry that are differentially acted upon by iTBS and cTBS [21–22]. If we assume that those with low baseline MEP amplitudes received TMS pulses at relatively low intensities, this would agree with the present data, where low baseline MEP amplitudes resulted in greater iTBS effects yet ameliorated cTBS effects (Figures 4 & 7). However, this is speculative, and we could not test this further because we did not possess sufficient I/O curve data, as in the aforementioned studies [21–22].
Target muscle
We demonstrated greater iTBS-induced facilitation for the FDI than the APB muscle. This result does not seem to be caused by significant differences in the size and location of the muscle representations within the M1 [60]. Thus, this may be due to technical reasons. Studies have shown that optimal current directions for evoking MEPs vary between different intrinsic hand muscles [60–61]. Specifically, Pascual-Leone et al., [60] showed that the optimal current direction for evoking MEPs from the FDI muscle was at a greater angle antero-laterally than for the APB. Thus, the use of a fixed coil orientation of 45° with respect to the mid-sagittal line (as done in all iTBS studies in present analysis) may better activate cortico-cortical circuits connected to FDI corticospinal neurons, than those connected to APB corticospinal neurons. Notably, this may apply to repetitive pulses, and also to the single pulses used to measure the response. This requires further investigation, ideally using a repeated-measures design, and neuronavigated TMS.
Effect of timepoint on TBS-induced plasticity
Our timepoint analysis on iTBS data agreed with a prior meta-analysis, with induced plasticity lasting at least 30 minutes [18]. Longer duration analyses were prevented by the small number of iTBS studies collecting data after this timepoint. In contrast, while Chung et al., [18] showed that cTBS suppression was significant at mid (20–30 minutes) and late (50–60 minutes) timepoints, our analyses demonstrated this effect for only 10 minutes. However it should be noted that when Chung et al., [18] only included studies that used the standard 600 pulse cTBS protocol (as with all studies in present analysis), cTBS suppression was non-significant at the late timepoint [18]. Thus, while our results also show the greatest cTBS effects at early timepoints [18, 62–63], our results differ to prior literature in that our effects do not last beyond 10 minutes. Although our study has the advantage of controlling for participant and study covariates, unlike Chung et al., [18], it is open to sampling bias due to only including a fraction of the cTBS literature. For instance, if we exclude one study from the present analysis that demonstrated the reversal of the typical cTBS effect [12], cTBS suppression becomes significant up to 20 minutes post-stimulation. Therefore, it is possible that sampling error can account for the aforementioned discrepancies in cTBS results. The collection of additional individual participant data in the future is required to answer this question.
Effect of age on TBS-induced plasticity
Previous TMS studies have demonstrated attenuated plasticity for older adults [64–66]. However, the influence of age on TBS plasticity has been mixed [67–68]. Linear and grouped analyses demonstrated small (non-significant) effects for attenuated iTBS and cTBS-induced plasticity with age (Tables 3 & 4). However, further analysis demonstrated a significant non-linear effect for iTBS. The curved pattern of response shows that very young participants exhibited increased iTBS-induced facilitation, which stabilised in middle age, before a steady drop-off in facilitation for older adults (Figure 4). However, there were few child/adolescent participants (minimum age = 17 years) and middle-aged participants included in the sample (just four participants aged 35 – 50). A more staged approach, testing across the lifespan (e.g. Freitas et al., [68]), appears to be justified in the future.
Limitations
A number of limitations should be acknowledged. First, we did not have data for various factors that have been demonstrated to contribute to variability in TBS responses, such as genotype [69], direct and indirect-wave recruitment profile [70], nor did we have participant’s MRI data [71]. Second, our approach pools data from different studies, and therefore does not have the precision of a repeated-measures design [72]. Next, the effects demonstrated are limited to the studies and participants within the present sample, and are therefore open to sampling error. Also, of the eight studies that used neuronavigation, none reported the coordinates of the motor hotspot, nor coil shift data from the motor hotspot. Thus, differences in coil position may have accounted for some unobserved intraindividual variability in TMS outcomes. Lastly, our findings may be influenced by publication bias, which has been previously demonstrated in a meta-analysis of TBS studies [18].
Recommendations
We first propose some steps to counter the significant relationships observed between baseline MEP amplitude and response to TBS. To avoid regression to the mean caused by chance occurrences of high or low MEP amplitudes, we recommend that investigators: 1) collect a sufficient number (20–30) of MEPs in their TMS blocks [50–51]; 2) avoid possible initial states of hyperexcitability within TMS sessions [52–53]; and 3) include baseline MEP amplitude as a covariate in statistical analyses. To avoid possible floor and ceiling effects, it has been suggested that researchers could use the machine intensity evoking an MEP amplitude of 50% of a participants’ maximum MEP value [21, 57]. However, Goldsworthy et al., [21] showed that this intensity also produced high levels of interindividual variability and did not demonstrate either iTBS or cTBS at the group level. Authors suggested that this may be due to the still substantial between-participant differences in the neural circuits activated by the TMS pulse (as discussed above). This is a complex issue, therefore we recommend focused research into the most appropriate stimulus intensity/intensities to probe TBS-induced plasticity, given that this may substantially reduce the high between-participant (versus between-study) variability observed in the present study. Until a more time-efficient solution, we recommend the collection of input-output curves to evaluate TBS-induced plasticity across a range of intensities [21, 46]. However, these can be more complex to calculate and interpret, thus we recommend a consensus on the specific input/output curve methods and analyses that should be employed to enable comparison of outcomes across studies [73]. In addition, we suggest that where neuronavigation can be used, researchers should report the coordinates of the motor hotspot, and report or analyse the impact of shifts from the motor hotspot for individual participants. Next, grouped (i.e. young vs. old), or linear, analyses may not be sufficient to uncover more fine-grained relationships between age and TBS plasticity, especially with very young or very old participants. Thus, non-linear relationships across the lifespan should be considered. Lastly, we recommend the use of the FDI muscle over the APB muscle to better detect iTBS-induced plasticity.
Conclusions
This study pooled data from 22 studies to demonstrate a number of readily controllable factors that can reduce interindividual variability in TBS-induced plasticity. Our findings demonstrate that a significant portion of variability may be attributable not only to brain plasticity, but also to the methods used to measure it. We make specific methodological recommendations that we hope will reduce interindividual variability and increase the standardisation of the methods used to evaluate TBS-induced plasticity. These findings justify the benefit of this collaborative approach to TMS research, which we are currently expanding through the construction of an individual participant TMS data repository at www.bigtmsdata.com. We welcome investigators to contribute to this database in order to solve these important questions that cannot be easily answered with single-site research.
Supplementary Material
430 healthy participants’ theta-burst stimulation data was pooled across 22 studies
Individual participant data was analysed using mixed-effects regression
Baseline MEP amplitude, muscle, time of day, and TMS machine predicted iTBS response
Baseline MEP amplitude and timepoint predicted cTBS response
Specific recommendations are proposed to reduce TBS variability
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all of the researchers who were kind enough to share the data that they worked so hard to collect.
Funding and disclosures
A.J. was supported by postdoctoral fellowships from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC 454617) and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR 41791). A.P.-L. was partly supported by the Sidney R. Baer Jr. Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, the National Science Foundation, and DARPA. A.P.-L. serves on the scientific advisory boards for Starlab Neuroscience, Neuroelectrics, Magstim Inc., Nexstim, and Cognito; and is listed as an inventor on several issued and pending patents on the real-time integration of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electroencephalography and magnetic resonance imaging. J.G.O. was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number KL2TR002737. P.B.F. is supported by a NHMRC Practitioner Fellowship (1078567). P.B.F. has received equipment for research from MagVenture A/S, Medtronic Ltd, Neuronetics and Brainsway Ltd. and funding for research from Neuronetics. He is on scientific advisory boards for Bionomics Ltd and LivaNova and is a founder of TMS Clinics Australia. P.G.E. is supported by a Future Fellowship from the Australian Research Council (FT160100077). N.C.R. was supported by a Discovery Early Career Researcher Award from the Australian Research Council (DE180100741).
Abbreviations and nomenclature
- TMS
Transcranial magnetic stimulation
- MEP
motor evoked potential
- iTBS
intermittent theta-burst stimulation
- cTBS
continuous theta-burst stimulation
- DV
dependent variable
- IV
independent variable
- Normalised MEP
DV for main regression analyses (conditioned MEP amplitude expressed as a percentage of the baseline MEP amplitude)
- TS
test stimulus (pulses used to collect pre/post MEPs for TBS protocols, i.e. 120% RMT or 1 mV method)
- RMT
resting motor threshold
- AMT
active motor threshold
- Pulse waveform
pulses used to collect pre/post MEPs (i.e. monophasic or biphasic)
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- [1].Hallett M Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the human brain. Nature 2000;406:147–150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Dileone M, Ranieri F, Florio L, Capone F, Musumeci G, Leoni C, et al. Differential Effects of HRAS Mutation on LTP-Like Activity Induced by Different Protocols of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation. Brain stimulation 2016;9:33–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Fried PJ, Schilberg L, Brem A-K, Saxena S, Wong B, Cypess AM, et al. Humans with type-2 diabetes show abnormal long-term potentiation-like cortical plasticity associated with verbal learning deficits. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 2017;55:89–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Blumberger DM, Vila-Rodriguez F, Thorpe KE, Feffer K, Noda Y, Giacobbe P, et al. Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. The Lancet 2018;391:1683–1692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Pascual-Leone A, Rubio B, Pallardó F, and Catalá MD. Rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in drug-resistant depression. The Lancet 1996;348:233–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Carmi L, Alyagon U, Barnea-Ygael N, Zohar J, Dar R, and Zangen A. Clinical and electrophysiological outcomes of deep TMS over the medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in OCD patients. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation 2018;11:158–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Suppa A, Huang YZ, Funke K, Ridding MC, Cheeran B, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Ten Years of Theta Burst Stimulation in Humans: Established Knowledge, Unknowns and Prospects. Brain Stimul 2016;9:323–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Hordacre B, Goldsworthy MR, Vallence A-M, Darvishi S, Moezzi B, Hamada M, et al. Variability in neural excitability and plasticity induction in the human cortex: a brain stimulation study. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation 2017;10:588–595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Hamada M, Murase N, Hasan A, Balaratnam M, and Rothwell JC. The role of interneuron networks in driving human motor cortical plasticity. Cerebral Cortex 2012;bhs147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Rocchi L, Ibanez Pereda J, Benussi A, Hannah R, Rawji V, Casula E, et al. Variability and predictors of response to continuous theta burst stimulation: a TMS-EEG study. Frontiers in Neuroscience 2018;12:400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Schilberg L, Schuhmann T, and Sack AT. Interindividual Variability and Intraindividual Reliability of Intermittent Theta Burst Stimulation-induced Neuroplasticity Mechanisms in the Healthy Brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2017; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Jannati A, Block G, Oberman LM, Rotenberg A, and Pascual-Leone A. Interindividual variability in response to continuous theta-burst stimulation in healthy adults. Clinical Neurophysiology 2017;128:2268–2278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Lopez-Alonso V, Cheeran B, Rio-Rodriguez D, and Fernandez-Del-Olmo M. Inter-individual variability in response to non-invasive brain stimulation paradigms. Brain Stimul 2014;7:372–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Hinder MR, Goss EL, Fujiyama H, Canty AJ, Garry MI, Rodger J, et al. Inter-and intra-individual variability following intermittent theta burst stimulation: implications for rehabilitation and recovery. Brain stimulation 2014;7:365–371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Milham MP, Fair D, Mennes M, and Mostofsky SH. The ADHD-200 consortium: a model to advance the translational potential of neuroimaging in clinical neuroscience. Frontiers in systems neuroscience 2012;6:62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Thompson PM, Stein JL, Medland SE, Hibar DP, Vasquez AA, Renteria ME, et al. The ENIGMA Consortium: large-scale collaborative analyses of neuroimaging and genetic data. Brain imaging and behavior 2014;8:153–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Huang Y-Z, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, and Rothwell JC. Theta Burst Stimulation of the Human Motor Cortex. Neuron 2005;45:201–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Chung SW, Hill AT, Rogasch NC, Hoy KE, and Fitzgerald PB. Use of theta-burst stimulation in changing excitability of motor cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 2016;63:43–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Puri R, Hinder MR, Canty AJ, and Summers JJ. Facilitatory non-invasive brain stimulation in older adults: the effect of stimulation type and duration on the induction of motor cortex plasticity. Experimental brain research 2016;234:3411–3423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Singh AM, Duncan RE, and Staines WR. Aerobic exercise abolishes cTBS-induced suppression of motor cortical excitability. Neurosci Lett 2016;633:215–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Goldsworthy MR, Vallence AM, Hodyl NA, Semmler JG, Pitcher JB, and Ridding MC. Probing changes in corticospinal excitability following theta burst stimulation of the human primary motor cortex. Clin Neurophysiol 2016;127:740–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Nettekoven C, Volz LJ, Kutscha M, Pool E-M, Rehme AK, Eickhoff SB, et al. Dose-dependent effects of theta burst rTMS on cortical excitability and resting-state connectivity of the human motor system. Journal of Neuroscience 2014;34:6849–6859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Vallence AM, Goldsworthy MR, Hodyl NA, Semmler JG, Pitcher JB, and Ridding MC. Inter- and intra-subject variability of motor cortex plasticity following continuous theta-burst stimulation. Neuroscience 2015;304:266–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Munneke MA, Rongen JJ, Overeem S, Schelhaas HJ, Zwarts MJ, and Stegeman DF. Cumulative effect of 5 daily sessions of theta burst stimulation on corticospinal excitability in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Muscle Nerve 2013;48:733–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Royston P Multiple imputation of missing values. The Stata Journal 2004;4:227–241. [Google Scholar]
- [26].Gelman A, Carlin JB, Stern HS, and Rubin DB. Bayesian data analysis (Vol. 2). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman; 2014; [Google Scholar]
- [27].McDonnell MN, Buckley JD, Opie GM, Ridding MC, and Semmler JG. A single bout of aerobic exercise promotes motor cortical neuroplasticity. J Appl Physiol (1985) 2013;114:1174–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. PLoS medicine 2009;6:e1000100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Field A (2009): Sage publications.
- [30].Tukey JW The future of data analysis. The annals of mathematical statistics 1962;33:1–67. [Google Scholar]
- [31].Brown KE, Lohse KR, Mayer I, Strigaro G, Desikan M, Casula EP, et al. The reliability of commonly used electrophysiology measures. Brain stimulation 2017;10:1102–1111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Brasil-Neto JP, McShane LM, Fuhr P, Hallett M, and Cohen LG. Topographic mapping of the human motor cortex with magnetic stimulation: factors affecting accuracy and reproducibility. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 1992;85:9–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Fried PJ, Jannati A, Davila-Pérez P, and Pascual-Leone A. Reproducibility of single-pulse, paired-pulse, and intermittent theta-burst TMS measures in healthy aging, type-2 diabetes, and Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in aging neuroscience 2017;9:263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Jannati A, Fried PJ, Block G, Oberman LM, Rotenberg A, and Pascual-Leone A. Test–Retest Reliability of the Effects of Continuous Theta-Burst Stimulation. Frontiers in neuroscience 2019;13:447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Portney LG and Watkins MP. (2009): Pearson/Prentice Hall.
- [36].Kline P (2000): Psychology Press.
- [37].Riley RD, Lambert PC, and Abo-Zaid G. Meta-analysis of individual participant data: rationale, conduct, and reporting. Bmj 2010;340:c221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Bendel RB and Afifi AA. Comparison of stopping rules in forward “stepwise” regression. Journal of the American Statistical association 1977;72:46–53. [Google Scholar]
- [39].Williams R Using the margins command to estimate and interpret adjusted predictions and marginal effects. The Stata Journal 2012;12:308–331. [Google Scholar]
- [40].Davidson R and MacKinnon JG. Estimation and inference in econometrics. OUP Catalogue 1993; [Google Scholar]
- [41].Goldsworthy MR, Müller-Dahlhaus F, Ridding MC, and Ziemann U. Inter-subject variability of LTD-like plasticity in human motor cortex: a matter of preceding motor activation. Brain Stimulation 2014;7:864–870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Corp DT, Bereznicki HGK, Clark GM, Youssef GJ, Fried PJ, Jannati A, et al. Large-scale analysis of interindividual variability in single and paired-pulse TMS data: results from the ‘Big TMS Data Collaboration’. submitted; [DOI] [PubMed]
- [43].McAllister CJ, Rönnqvist KC, Stanford IM, Woodhall GL, Furlong PL, and Hall S.D.J.J.o.N.. Oscillatory beta activity mediates neuroplastic effects of motor cortex stimulation in humans. 2013;33:7919–7927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Gomes-Osman J Unpublished;
- [45].Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Mazzone P, Insola A, Pilato F, Saturno E, et al. Comparison of descending volleys evoked by monophasic and biphasic magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex in conscious humans. Experimental brain research 2001;141:121–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Vallence A-M, Goldsworthy MR, Hodyl NA, Semmler JG, Pitcher JB, and Ridding MC. Inter-and intra-subject variability of motor cortex plasticity following continuous theta-burst stimulation. Neuroscience 2015;304:266–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Stigler SM Regression towards the mean, historically considered. Statistical methods in medical research 1997;6:103–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Bland JM and Altman DG. Statistics notes: some examples of regression towards the mean. Bmj 1994;309:780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Daskalakis ZJ, Möller B, Christensen BK, Fitzgerald PB, Gunraj C, and Chen R. The effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on cortical inhibition in healthy human subjects. Exp Brain Res 2006;174:403–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Chang WH, Fried PJ, Saxena S, Jannati A, Gomes-Osman J, Kim Y-H, et al. Optimal number of pulses as outcome measures of neuronavigated transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology 2016;127:2892–2897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Goldsworthy M, Hordacre B, and Ridding M. Minimum number of trials required for within-and between-session reliability of TMS measures of corticospinal excitability. Neuroscience 2016;320:205–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Brasil-Neto JP, Cohen LG, and Hallett M. Central fatigue as revealed by postexercise decrement of motor evoked potentials. Muscle & nerve 1994;17:713–719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Schmidt S, Cichy RM, Kraft A, Brocke J, Irlbacher K, and Brandt SA. An initial transient-state and reliable measures of corticospinal excitability in TMS studies. Clinical Neurophysiology 2009;120:987–993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Devanne H, Lavoie B, and Capaday C. Input-output properties and gain changes in the human corticospinal pathway. Experimental brain research 1997;114:329–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Amandusson Å, Flink R, and Axelson HW. Comparison between adaptive and fixed stimulus paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (ppTMS) in normal subjects. Clinical Neurophysiology Practice 2017;2:91–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Pitcher JB, Doeltgen SH, Goldsworthy MR, Schneider LA, Vallence A-M, Smith AE, et al. A comparison of two methods for estimating 50% of the maximal motor evoked potential. Clinical Neurophysiology 2015;126:2337–2341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Houdayer E, Degardin A, Cassim F, Bocquillon P, Derambure P, and Devanne H. The effects of low-and high-frequency repetitive TMS on the input/output properties of the human corticospinal pathway. Experimental Brain Research 2008;187:207–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Di Lazzaro V and Rothwell JC. Corticospinal activity evoked and modulated by non-invasive stimulation of the intact human motor cortex. The Journal of physiology 2014;592:4115–4128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Opitz A, Zafar N, Bockermann V, Rohde V, and Paulus W. Validating computationally predicted TMS stimulation areas using direct electrical stimulation in patients with brain tumors near precentral regions. NeuroImage: Clinical 2014;4:500–507. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Pascual-Leone A, Cohen L, Brasil-Neto J, and Hallett M. Non-invasive differentiation of motor cortical representation of hand muscles by mapping of optimal current directions. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 1994;93:42–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Raffin E, Pellegrino G, Di Lazzaro V, Thielscher A, and Siebner HR. Bringing transcranial mapping into shape: sulcus-aligned mapping captures motor somatotopy in human primary motor hand area. Neuroimage 2015;120:164–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Koch G, Di Lorenzo F, Bonnì S, Giacobbe V, Bozzali M, Caltagirone C, et al. Dopaminergic modulation of cortical plasticity in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Neuropsychopharmacology 2014;39:2654–2661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Conte A, Belvisi D, Bologna M, Ottaviani D, Fabbrini G, Colosimo C, et al. Abnormal cortical synaptic plasticity in primary motor area in progressive supranuclear palsy. Cerebral Cortex 2012;22:693–700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Müller-Dahlhaus JFM, Orekhov Y, Liu Y, and Ziemann U. Interindividual variability and age-dependency of motor cortical plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation. Experimental brain research 2008;187:467–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Fathi D, Ueki Y, Mima T, Koganemaru S, Nagamine T, Tawfik A, et al. Effects of aging on the human motor cortical plasticity studied by paired associative stimulation. Clinical neurophysiology 2010;121:90–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Tecchio F, Zappasodi F, Pasqualetti P, De Gennaro L, Pellicciari M, Ercolani M, et al. Age dependence of primary motor cortex plasticity induced by paired associative stimulation. Clinical Neurophysiology 2008;119:675–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Young-Bernier M, Tanguay AN, Davidson PS, and Tremblay F. Short-latency afferent inhibition is a poor predictor of individual susceptibility to rTMS-induced plasticity in the motor cortex of young and older adults. Frontiers in aging neuroscience 2014;6:182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Freitas C, Perez J, Knobel M, Tormos JM, Oberman L, Eldaief M, et al. Changes in cortical plasticity across the lifespan. Frontiers in aging neuroscience 2011;3: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Antal A, Chaieb L, Moliadze V, Monte-Silva K, Poreisz C, Thirugnanasambandam N, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene polymorphisms shape cortical plasticity in humans. 2010;3:230–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Huang G and Mouraux A. MEP latencies predict the neuromodulatory effect of cTBS delivered to the ipsilateral and contralateral sensorimotor cortex. PloS one 2015;10:e0133893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Cocchi L, Sale MV, Lord A, Zalesky A, Breakspear M, and Mattingley JB. Dissociable effects of local inhibitory and excitatory theta-burst stimulation on large-scale brain dynamics. Journal of neurophysiology 2015;113:3375–3385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Higgins JP and Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2011; 5.1.0:[Available from: www.cochrane-handbook.org.
- [73].Massie CL and Malcolm MP. Considerations for Stimulus–Response Curves in Stroke: An Investigation Comparing Collection and Analysis Methods. International Journal of Neuroscience 2013;123:175–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Do M, Kirkovski M, Davies CB, Bekkali S, Byrne LK, and Enticott PG. Intra-and Inter-Regional Priming of Ipsilateral Human Primary Motor Cortex With Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation Does Not Induce Consistent Neuroplastic Effects. Frontiers in human neuroscience 2018;12:123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Koch G, Di Lorenzo F, del Olmo MF, Bonni S, Ponzo V, Caltagirone C, et al. Reversal of LTP-like cortical plasticity in Alzheimer’s disease patients with tau-related faster clinical progression. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease 2016;50:605–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Lee NJ, Ahn HJ, Jung K-I, Ohn SH, Hong J, Kim YJ, et al. Reduction of continuous theta burst stimulation-induced motor plasticity in healthy elderly with COMT Val158Met polymorphism. Annals of rehabilitation medicine 2014;38:658–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].Opie GM, Catcheside PG, Usmani ZA, Ridding MC, and Semmler JG. Motor cortex plasticity induced by theta burst stimulation is impaired in patients with obstructive sleep apnoea. Eur J Neurosci 2013;37:1844–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Vernet M, Bashir S, Yoo WK, Oberman L, Mizrahi I, Ifert-Miller F, et al. Reproducibility of the effects of theta burst stimulation on motor cortical plasticity in healthy participants. Clin Neurophysiol 2014;125:320–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Di Lazzaro V, Pilato F, Dileone M, Profice P, Capone F, Ranieri F, et al. Modulating cortical excitability in acute stroke: a repetitive TMS study. Clinical Neurophysiology 2008;119:715–723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Di Lazzaro V, Dileone M, Pilato F, Capone F, Musumeci G, Ranieri F, et al. Modulation of motor cortex neuronal networks by rTMS: comparison of local and remote effects of six different protocols of stimulation. Journal of neurophysiology 2011;105:2150–2156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Dickins DS, Sale MV, and Kamke MR. Plasticity induced by intermittent theta burst stimulation in bilateral motor cortices is not altered in older adults. Neural plasticity 2015;2015: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Beynel L, Chauvin A, Guyader N, Harquel S, and Marendaz C. Age-related changes in intracortical inhibition are mental-cognitive state-dependent. Biol Psychol 2014;101:9–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Do M, Clark G, Fuelscher I, Kirkovski M, Cerins A, Corp DT, et al. Magstim 2002 and Bistim Mode maximum stimulus output values not equivalent: Configuration selection is critical. Brain Stimulation: Basic, Translational, and Clinical Research in Neuromodulation 2019; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.