Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 Jun 8;59(3):384–402. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12252

Randomized controlled trial of social cognition and interaction training compared to befriending group

Frances Dark 1,, James G Scott 2,3,4, Andrea Baker 4, Stephen Parker 5, Anne Gordon 3, Ellie Newman 6, Victoria Gore‐Jones 1, Carmen C W Lim 4,7, Lyndall Jones 8, David L Penn 9,10
PMCID: PMC7496415  PMID: 32515058

Abstract

Background

Deficits in social cognition are common in people with schizophrenia and are associated with impaired functioning. Finding effective interventions to address these deficits is a priority. Social Cognition Interaction Training (SCIT) is a psychosocial intervention that has demonstrated acceptability and feasibility in various health care settings. Larger, well‐designed randomized controlled trials are needed to examine the effectiveness of this intervention.

Design

A randomized controlled trial.

Methods

One hundred and twenty adults diagnosed with schizophrenia spectrum disorder were randomized to receive SCIT (n = 61) or Befriending Therapy (BT) (n = 59). Both intervention groups were delivered weekly for 2 hr over 12 weeks. Neurocognitive assessment was completed at baseline. Participants completed assessments of social cognition, social functioning, and meta‐cognition at baseline, post‐intervention, and 3‐month follow‐up.

Results

There were no clinically significant differences between group outcomes on any measure of social cognition or social functioning. There was a trend for both groups to improve over time but not at a level of statistical significance.

Conclusions

SCIT did not show any additional benefits on measures of social cognition compared to Befriending Therapy for people with schizophrenia spectrum disorder. The findings are discussed in terms of potential improvements to the programme.

Practitioner points

  • Effective interventions for the social cognitive deficits of schizophrenia spectrum disorders are still being refined.

  • Social Cognition Interaction Training is a promising therapy but requires further modifications to improve its effectiveness.

Keywords: social cognition, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, randomized controlled trial

Background

Deficits in social and community functioning are core features of schizophrenia (Bowie & Harvey, 2005; Green, Kern, Baff, & Mintz, 2000). Pharmacotherapy is not effective for the social cognitive and neurocognitive impairments associated with these poor functional outcomes (Bellack et al., 2004; Hogarty, Flesher, Ulrich, Carter, et al., 2004; Sergi, Rassovsky, Nuechterlein, & Green, 2006). Thus, psychosocial treatments have been developed to address these impairments. Interventions, such as cognitive remediation, focus on the processes that support or underlie social functioning (i.e., neurocognitive deficits). Although these interventions improve specific domains, their benefits do not automatically generalize to broader psychosocial outcomes (McGurk, Twamley, Sitzer, McHugo, & Mueser, 2007; Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk, & Czobor, 2011). This has led to the investigation of additional factors that contribute to functional impairments such as social cognition. Social cognition encapsulates cognitive processes involved in the recognition, understanding, accurate processing, and effective use of social cues in real‐world situations (Green et al., 2008). Social cognition is considered to have four domains: emotional and social perception, theory of mind (ToM), and attributional style (Pinkham et al., 2014). There is growing evidence that social cognition is an independent predictor of function but also mediates the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcomes in schizophrenia (Fett et al., 2011).

A number of therapies have arisen to address the multiple domains of social cognition. The first meta‐analysis of therapeutic approaches found large effect sizes for emotional recognition (=0.71), moderate effect sizes for theory of mind (d = 0.46), and large effect sizes for observed improvements in social functioning (d = 0.78) (Kurtz & Richardson, 2012). A more recent meta‐analysis screened for study methodology and largely supported these findings but with notable caveats especially about suboptimal study methodology. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) (Roberts, Penn, & Combs, 2016) was the most studied multidomain intervention in these meta‐analyses (Grant, Lawrence, Preti, Wykes, & Cella, 2017; Kurtz & Richardson, 2012).

SCIT is a group‐based therapy that comprises three phases (‘Introduction & Emotions’, ‘Figuring out Situations’, and ‘Checking it out’). A series of open trials have demonstrated that SCIT can be implemented in a variety of settings (inpatient and outpatient), is generally well‐received by clients and therapists, and is associated with improvements in social cognition and social functioning among individuals with schizophrenia (Combs et al., 2007; Roberts & Penn, 2009). To date, there have been only a small number of randomized controlled trials of SCIT (Gordon et al., 2017; Hasson‐Ohayon, Mashiach‐Eizenberg, Avidan, Roberts, & Roe, 2014; Roberts et al., 2014; Tas, Danaci, Cubukcuoglu, & Brune, 2012; Taylor et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2013). The results from these trials have been less uniform with Gordon et.al in a waitlist controlled design (n = 36) reporting no significant improvement in any measure but there was high acceptability of SCIT with around 86% of participants completing the programme. Roberts et al. in a medium‐sized (n = 66) randomized controlled trial of SCIT compared with treatment as usual found no significant improvement in the intervention group on measures of social cognition; however, there was some suggestion that those receiving SCIT did better on measures of functioning and negative symptoms. Larger trials addressing some of the methodological issues raised in the preceding studies are needed to further evaluate the effectiveness of SCIT. (Grant et al., 2017; Kurtz, Gagen, Rocha, Machado, & Penn, 2016).

Objectives

This randomized, controlled trial aimed to compare the effectiveness of SCIT for individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders with an active control group, Befriending Therapy (BT). It was hypothesized that individuals who receive SCIT would show greater improvement in social cognition as measured by the Bell Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task (BLERT) (Bryson, Bell, & Lysaker, 1997); Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ) (Kinderman & Bentall, 1996); Hinting Task (HT) (Corcoran, Mercer, & Frith, 1995); and social functioning as measured by the Social Functioning Scale (SFS) (Birchwood, Smith, Cochrane, Wetton, & Copestake, 1990) and the Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA) (Patterson, Moscona, McKibbin, Davidson, & Jeste, 2001) compared to individuals receiving BT at endpoint (week 12).

Method

Participants

One hundred and forty‐two people attending community mental health clinics in Brisbane, Australia, were screened and twenty‐two excluded from the study (Figure 1). One hundred and twenty (120) eligible participants provided written consent and were randomized to either SCIT or BT. Participants met the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th Edition (DSM 5) criteria for schizophrenia spectrum disorders based on the Diagnostic Interview for Psychosis (DIP) (Castle et al., 2006). They were between 18 and 65 years of age and had interpersonal communication difficulties based on a score of < 105 on the Interpersonal Communication Subscale Score of the Social Functioning Scale (Birchwood et al., 1990).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

CONSORT flow diagram. [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Intervention delivery

SCIT is a manualized group therapy of 20–24 hourly sessions (Roberts et al., 2016). In this study, a weekly session covered the equivalent of two sessions from the manual. Each session lasted 2 hr. The total programme duration was 12 weeks. Delivery in this format was intended to address attrition issues observed in an earlier pilot study (Parker et al., 2013) and is consistent with alternate delivery formats described in the SCIT manual (Roberts et al., 2016). Groups were based on a maximum of 8 participants per group with 2 facilitators. All clinicians had received 1‐day face‐to‐face training by the principal investigator (FD) who was endorsed by the programme developers (DP and DR) to train in SCIT. Less experienced staff co‐facilitated with experienced staff, and discussion about adherence to the manual occurred at the end of each session.

BT was developed as a control intervention for clinical trials of psychosocial interventions (Bendall et al., 2006). BT has been found to have a small effect in reducing symptoms in people with psychosis (Sensky et al., 2000). It is matched in many respects to SCIT (e.g., group format, duration, frequency), but does not include social cognitive skills development. It is comprised of a series of group conversations focusing on neutral, non‐clinical topics of interest to the participants. The facilitators of BT were trained by the principal investigator (FD). They were predominately peer workers employed by the mental health services who had experience conducting group work. One group was co‐facilitated by a social worker.

All sessions, of both interventions, were audio‐recorded, and these recordings were randomly assessed for adherence and fidelity by one of the investigators. The sessions were reviewed by experienced therapists who had not conducted the groups they were assessing (FD and SP) with feedback given to group facilitators to minimize any deviation from either manual (SCIT or BT).

Study assessments and procedures

The study commenced in June 2016, and recruitment was completed by November 2018. An independent member of the research team not involved in the delivery of the intervention or outcome assessment was emailed the list of participants for randomization. A computer‐generated randomization list of random numbers was created using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, NC, USA) with a 1:1 allocation using block sizes of 4.

All participants in the study continued to receive standard clinical care (i.e., there were no restrictions on medication or psychosocial interventions, apart from participants receiving SCIT or BT in addition to the standard treatments).

Trained research assistants, who were blind to group allocation, consented participants and conducted assessments at baseline, 12 weeks, and 12 weeks from programme completion.

To minimize dropout rates, participants were given reminder calls the day before group; followed up immediately with a phone call if the participant did not attend the group; and providing transportation (if necessary) to group.

Participants were reimbursed for out‐of‐pocket expenses, inconvenience, and time involved by the provision of $50 prepaid gift cards on completion of the baseline assessment, end‐of‐study assessment, and follow‐up assessment 3 months later (total reimbursement $150).

Measures

Social cognition

Emotional perception was assessed using the Bell Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task (BLERT), which measures the ability to correctly identify seven emotional states: happiness, sadness, fear, disgust, surprise, anger, or no emotion (Bryson et al., 1997). The BLERT is scored out of 21 with higher scores indicating better social cognition.

Theory of mind (ToM) was assessed using the Hinting Task (HT) (Corcoran et al., 1995). The HT consists of ten short stories that involve an interaction between two characters, one of which drops a hint at the end of the story. The participants task is to infer what the character really means by their hint. Higher scores indicate better ToM with the maximum score being 20.

Attribution bias was assessed using the Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ) (Kinderman & Bentall, 1996). The IPSAQ is used to assess attribution style. This measure consists of 32 items that describe in second person 16 positive and negative social situations. Two derivative scores are produced: ‘externalizing bias’ and ‘personalizing bias’. For both domains, scores range from 0 to 1.

Social functioning

Social functioning was measured using the Social Functioning Scale (SFS) (Birchwood et al., 1990) and the Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA) (Patterson et al., 2001). The SFS is a 79‐item questionnaire with seven domains. The raw score was transformed using a standardized distribution with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. The SFS has been found to have good reliability (test–retest, and informant/self‐report reliability, item‐total correlation) and validity (construct, concurrent, and criterion group validity) (Birchwood et al., 1990). A high score indicates a high level of functioning. The SSPA assesses social functioning in which the subject participates in three role‐plays presenting selected social problem situations. All role‐plays were videotaped to enable inter‐rater reliability analysis. Scores were rated on a Likert‐type scale from 1 to 5 where 5 indicates little or no deficiency. There are 17 categories in total. Scores from each role‐plays were summed with total scores ranging from 17 to 85. The SSPA has good reliability (test–retest of 0.92) and inter‐rater reliability (intraclass correlation of .91) (Patterson et al., 2001).

Neurocognition

Neurocognition was assessed using the CogState Brief Battery (CBB), which is a computer‐administered cognitive test battery that consists of cognitive tasks that measure psychomotor function, attention, and working memory and has good psychometric properties (Maruff et al., 2009). This was assessed at baseline to enable assessment of whether neurocognitive abilities impacted outcomes.

Meta‐cognition

Meta‐cognition, which has been hypothesized as a mechanism of skills transfer in psychosocial interventions, was measured using the Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ‐30), which consists of 30 items measuring an individuals meta‐cognitive beliefs, judgements, and monitoring (Wells & Cartwright‐Hatton, 2004). The MCQ‐30 has 30 items with 5 factors reflecting the dimensions of meta‐cognition. The maximum score is 24, and the total score for the task is 120.

Endpoints

The primary outcome was improvement in Bell Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task (BLERT). Secondary outcomes were improvement in SFS, SSPA, HT, and IPSAQ at endpoint (12 weeks). Co‐secondary outcomes were the persistence of effects in SFS, SSPA, HT, BLERT, and IPSAQ at the 24‐week follow‐up assessment (12 weeks post‐end of SCIT).

Sample size

We calculated our sample size based on our primary outcome, emotional recognition assessed using BLERT. The choice of primary outcome measure was based on (1) the first meta‐analysis of social cognitive interventions revealing large effect sizes for emotional recognition (Kurtz et al., 2015) and the finding that emotional recognition is associated with social functioning (Javed & Charles, 2018). We estimated sample size based on two sources: (1) previous research that has investigated the effects of psychosocial treatment on emotional perception (Silver, Goodman, Knoll, & Isakov, 2004) and (2) the results of open‐trial pilot studies (effect sizes ranging from 0.6 to 1.6) (Roberts, Penn, Labate, Margolis, & Sterne, 2010). Based on a conservative effect size of 0.84, and a baseline–post‐intervention correlation of 0.5, a sample of 28 participants per group (n = 56) was required to achieve 80% power with the alpha level set at .05. Assuming a dropout rate of 20%, a minimum sample of approximately 70 participants was required, thus 35 participants in each group. Due to practicalities of group size and opportunities for running the groups over the study time frame, we randomized 120 participants.

Statistical method

Analysis on the primary and secondary outcomes was performed based on the intention‐to‐treat (ITT) sample (i.e., participants analysed according to randomized treatment assignment ignoring non‐compliance and dropouts). The primary outcome was analysed using linear mixed models to estimate the effect of the intervention on emotional recognition from baseline to post‐intervention (endpoint) and from baseline to 3‐month follow‐up. Treatment group (a binary indicator for the intervention group (SCIT/BT)) and baseline BLERT scores were included as fixed effects, while site was included as random effects in the mixed model. Since participants were recruited across small and large sites with potential variation between sites in the severity of the illness of people recruited, it was desirable to control for site‐effects in the RCT analysis (Kahan, 2014). A similar modelling approach was undertaken on all secondary outcomes (MCQ scores, HT scores, etc.). Least squares mean and standard error along with difference in least squares means between groups were reported. Approximately two thirds of the participants completed at least six sessions. Post‐hoc analyses were carried out on participants who attended at least 12 out of 20 sessions with no major protocol violations and in the subsample of people with schizophrenia. Effect sizes based on Cohen’s d for all the outcomes were also calculated. Significance level was evaluated at the .05 level using 2‐sided test. All analysis was conducted using SAS software, version 9.4.

Results

The flow diagram detailing recruitment and withdrawal from treatment throughout the study is presented in Figure 1.

The groups did not significantly differ in any demographic variables or baseline neurocognitive variables (Table 1) or on baseline measures of social cognition and social functioning (Table 2). Ninety‐five (82%) of participants had a diagnosis of schizophrenia. All patients were prescribed antipsychotic medication; the majority were on clozapine, aripiprazole, olanzapine, and paliperidone (see Appendix 1). SCIT participants attended an average of 6.3 (SD = 3.8) out of the twelve available sessions. The BT participants attended a mean of 6.6 (SD = 4.1) sessions.

Table 1.

Baseline demographic and cognitive characteristics by the intervention group

Characteristics BT (n = 59) SCIT (n = 61) Total (n = 120) Test statistic, p‐value
Age in years – Mean (SD) 37.5 (10.1) 36.1 (10.7) 36.8 (10.4) t = 0.71, p = .477
Males – No. (%) 40 (67.8) 46 (75.4) 86 (71.7) χ2 = 0.86, p = .354
Aboriginal/Torres Strait Islander – No. (%) 3 (5.1) 5 (8.2) 8 (6.7) χ2 = 0.47, p = .494
Country of Birth – No. (%)
Australia 44 (74.6) 45 (73.8) 89 (74.2) χ2 = 0.01, p = .920
Employment – No. (%)
Part‐time 5 (8.4) 1 (1.6) 6 (5.0) χ2 = 3.25, p = .354
Casual 2 (3.4) 2 (3.3) 4 (3.3)
Unemployed 4 (6.8) 3 (4.9) 7 (5.8)
Government benefit 48 (81.4) 55 (90.2) 103 (85.8)
Living situation – No. (%)
Independent (alone) 17 (28.8) 18 (29.5) 35 (29.2) χ2 = 2.56, p = .279
Family 9 (15.2) 16 (26.2) 25 (20.8)
Supported housing 33 (55.9) 27 (44.3) 60 (50.0)
Cognitive state battery a – mean (SD)
Detection task (DET) b 2.5 (0.4) 2.6 (0.1) 2.5 (0.3) t = −0.9, p = .394
Identification task (IDN) c 2.7 (0.1) 2.7 (0.1) 2.7 (0.1) t = −0.5, p = .591
One Card Learning (OCL) d 0.9 (0.2) 0.9 (0.1) 0.9 (0.1) t = −0.6, p = .547
One Back task (ONB) e 2.9 (0.1) 2.9 (0.1) 2.9 (0.1) t = −1.3, p = .182

Higher score indicates better performance.

a

The cognitive state battery is a computerized series of tasks which uses playing cards to test aspects of cognition. The tasks included in this study were DET, IDN, OCL, and ONB

b

DET assesses processing speed in log10 milliseconds. Lower score indicates better performance

c

IDN assesses visual learning and memory in log10 milliseconds. Lower score indicates better performance

d

OCL assesses the accuracy of the performance in the form of arcsine square root of the proportion of correct responses

e

ONB assesses attention/working memory in log10 milliseconds. Lower score indicates better performance.

Table 2.

Baseline measures of social cognition by the intervention group

Outcomes Mean (SD) Test statistic, p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
BT (n = 59) SCIT (n = 61) Total (n = 120)
I. BLERT
Total score 14.1 (4.0) 14.7 (3.1) 14.4 (3.6) t = −0.95, p = .343 0.17
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Social withdrawal 98.6 (7.8) 99.4 (9.4) 99.0 (8.6) t = −0.53, p = .600 0.09
Interpersonal communication 94.3 (11.3) 96.8 (9.9) 95.6 (10.6) t = −1.27, p = .206 0.24
Independence performance 108.7 (10.2) 105.1 (11.4) 106.9 (10.9) t = 1.81, p = .073 0.34
Recreation activities 108.8 (13.8) 104.9 (15.3) 106.8 (14.6) t = 1.45, p = .151 0.27
Pro‐social activities 100.1 (12.9) 100.7 (12.8) 100.4 (12.8) t = −0.26, p = .797 0.05
Independence competence 97.8 (13.8) 94.0 (14.4) 95.8 (14.2) t = 1.48, p = .141 0.27
Occupational/employment 101.1 (9.4) 99.3 (10.4) 100.2 (9.9) t = 0.96, p = .337 0.18
Total score 101.3 (7.4) 100.0 (7.5) 100.7 (7.5) t = 0.95, p = .344 0.18
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 68.0 (16.6) 67.4 (12.8) 67.7 (14.7) t = 0.23, p = .818 0.04
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 15.6 (3.4) 15.8 (3.3) 15.7 (3.4) t = −0.39, p = .694 0.06
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 1.4 (3.4) 1.2 (3.7) 1.3 (3.5) t = 0.35, p = .726 0.06
Personalizing bias 1.0 (0.8) 0.9 (0.6) 0.9 (0.7) t = 0.80, p = .428 0.14
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Scenario 1 30.6 (5.5) 29.9 (5.2) 30.2 (5.4) t = 0.73, p = .469 0.13
Scenario 2 35.6 (5.4) 35.0 (6.0) 35.3 (5.8) t = 0.60, p = .550 0.11
Total score 66.2 (10.2) 64.9 (10.4) 65.5 (10.3) t = 0.71, p = .478 0.13

Social cognition and social functioning outcomes

The main effects analyses showed at endpoint (week 12), and SCIT participants did not differ significantly from BT participants in terms of emotional recognition (p = .185) although there was a small effect size in favour of SCIT (ES = −0.18). SCIT participants did not differ significantly from BT participants on any other measures of social cognition or social functioning (Table 3). Similarly, at 3‐month post‐intervention follow‐up, there were no significant differences between both groups of participants across all outcomes (Table 4).

Table 3.

Estimated treatment effect at post‐treatment (week 12)

Outcomes Post‐intervention, least squares means (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
I. BLERT
Total score 14.7 (0.7) 15.5 (0.6) 0.8 (0.6) .185 −0.18
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Social withdrawal 101.0 (1.3) 101.5 (1.1) 0.5 (1.7) .776 −0.06
Interpersonal communication 109.3 (2.8) 105.3 (2.5) −4.0 (3.7) .294 0.22
Independence performance 107.2 (1.5) 108.9 (1.3) 1.8 (2.0) .384 −0.18
Recreation activities 103.6 (2.0) 104.2 (1.8) 0.6 (2.6) .807 −0.05
Pro‐social activities 100.9 (1.7) 103.3 (1.5) 2.4 (2.3) .308 −0.22
Independence competence 96.6 (2.4) 97.9 (2.2) 1.3 (2.8) .649 −0.08
Occupational/employment 99.8 (1.3) 100.3 (1.2) 0.5 (1.7) .753 −0.06
Total score 102.7 (1.2) 102.9 (1.1) 0.3 (1.6) .852 −0.03
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 66.7 (1.7) 64.2 (1.6) −2.5 (2.3) .295 0.22
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 17.0 (0.4) 16.9 (0.4) −0.1 (0.5) .844 0.04
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 1.8 (0.4) 1.9 (0.4) 0.1 (0.3) .993 −0.04
Personalizing bias 0.9 (0.1) 0.7 (0.1) −0.1 (0.1) .182 0.29
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Scenario 1 32.5 (0.8) 32.1 (0.8) −0.4 (1.1) .703 0.07
Scenario 2 36.5 (0.8) 35.9 (0.7) −0.6 (1.1) .595 0.12
Total score 68.9 (1.5) 67.9 (1.5) −0.9 (2.0) .647 0.10

BT = Befriending Therapy; SCIT = Social Cognition Interaction Training; SE = standard error.

a

The reference group is BT.

Table 4.

Estimated treatment effect at 3‐month follow‐up

Outcomes 3‐month post‐intervention, least squares mean (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
I. BLERT
Total score 15.1 (0.5) 15.3 (0.5) 0.2 (0.6) .185 −0.06
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Social withdrawal 101.0 (1.4) 103.2 (1.5) 2.2 (2.0) .289 −0.23
Interpersonal communication 112.8 (2.7) 110.4 (2.8) −2.4 (3.6) .506 0.13
Independence performance 108.7 (1.5) 110.7 (1.6) 1.9 (1.8) .307 −0.20
Recreation activities 109.2 (2.2) 108.2 (2.3) −1.0 (2.9) .747 0.07
Pro‐social activities 105.6 (2.1) 103.5 (2.2) −2.1 (2.5) .418 0.15
Independence competence 98.1 (2.7) 99.7 (2.8) 1.6 (2.8) .573 −0.09
Occupational/employment 100.6 (1.2) 102.0 (1.3) 1.4 (1.7) .426 −0.17
Total score 105.0 (1.5) 105.4 (1.5) 0.4 (1.6) .809 −0.04
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 65.2 (1.7) 62.1(1.8) −3.1 (2.3) .203 0.27
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 17.6 (0.4) 17.1 (0.4) −0.5 (0.5) .352 0.19
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 1.0 (0.6) 1.1 (0.5) 0.1 (0.7) .905 −0.03
Personalizing bias 0.9 (0.1) 0.9 (0.1) 0.0 (0.1) .967 0.00
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Scenario 1 32.9 (0.6) 33.2 (0.6) 0.3 (0.8) .689 −0.08
Scenario 2 37.2 (0.6) 37.1 (0.7) −0.1 (0.9) .896 0.02
Total score 70.1 (1.2) 70.3 (1.2) 0.2 (1.5) .896 −0.03

BT = Befriending Therapy; SCIT = Social Cognition Interaction Training; SE = standard error.

a

The reference group is BT.

To observe whether the effect of intervention on emotional recognition was maintained at 3 months post‐intervention, we examined a group x time interaction model where time was treated as a categorical variable assuming an unstructured covariance. There was no significant time x treatment group interaction (F = 1.39, p = .252), which indicates that there was no difference in patterns of change between both groups over time. The time effect was significant (F = 3.07, p = .049) with both treatments showing some evidence of improvement over time from baseline to endpoint, and the effect was maintained at the 3‐month follow‐up.

Post‐hoc analysis of participants completing the programmes

We conducted a post‐hoc analysis on a subset of participants who completed at least 6 weeks (12 sessions) of the programme (n = 73). There were 34 SCIT and 39 BT participants. This subgroup of participants did not differ from the broader sample in terms of demographic, cognitive state and primary outcome (emotional recognition). The results of the post‐hoc analyses showed no significant difference in social cognition or functional outcomes between these groups at any time period in the subgroup of participants who completed at least 6 weeks of the programme (see Appendix 1).

Post‐hoc analysis of people with schizophrenia

Our study has recruited participants with schizophrenia spectrum disorders (a cohort which is more heterogeneous in terms of the schizophrenia diagnosis). We also conducted a post‐hoc analysis on a subset of people with schizophrenia. Again, this subgroup of participants did not differ in terms of the primary outcome, total BLERT scores at endpoint (week 12), and at 3‐month post‐intervention follow‐up (week 24) (see Appendices 3 and 4).

Meta‐cognition

There was no significant difference between both groups on total MCQ score.

Discussion

This large randomized controlled trial found no significant difference between SCIT and BT group participants on social cognition and functional outcome measures. This finding is not consistent with a recent meta‐analysis of approaches to remediate social cognition which found these interventions to be associated with improvement in affect recognition and theory of mind in studies with active and passive controls (Grant et al., 2017). There are a number of possible explanations for the lack of a significant improvement in social cognition in the SCIT participants compared to the BT participants. The participants were consumers of an Australian public mental health service. Most participants had severe and long‐standing schizophrenia, which was refractory to other interventions. Two hours per week for 12 weeks may not be enough to result in any meaningful change in this population. Session adherence was on average around 50%, which is lower than that from trials in Israel recruiting participants from rehabilitation services where the mean session attendance was 71% ± 24%. In our previous pilot studies, we found lower adherence for participants recruited from community settings (Parker, Foley, Walker, & Dark, 2013). In addition, despite a power analysis being conducted, the study may still not have been powered to show small effects. The developers of SCIT have previously suggested a threshold of 50% of sessions attended as an estimate of adequate treatment (Roberts et al., 2014; Roberts & Penn, 2009). Roberts et al. suggest a possible dose–response effect may exist in SCIT therapy and recommends more frequent and intense sessions may be useful (Roberts et al., 2014).

Consistent with our pilot work (Parker et al., 2013), attrition remained a significant concern especially for participants residing independently in the community. Even with attention to ensuring participant retention, one quarter of participants in both groups did not receive the allocated intervention. In providing psychosocial interventions to people living with schizophrenia, it is important to consider whether the participants can commit to the length of the programme. Further consistent efforts are required by staff to keep participant motivation high and to address any potential barriers to attendance. In the early stages of therapy, assistance to get to sessions can aid in encoding the value of attendance.

The heterogeneity of the population diagnosed with schizophrenia spectrum disorders can confound results. In this study, we did screen to ensure participants recruited did have social functioning deficits. Roberts et al. have also suggested that SCIT may be particularly suited to the subset of people diagnosed with schizophrenia who have dysfunctional attribution biases (Roberts et al., 2014).

Interventions to address social cognition are in their infancy. SCIT targets multiple social cognition domains with the associated challenge of ensuring each domain receives the correct ‘dose’ of treatment. Focusing on a single social cognitive domain may enable more precise selection of participants with that deficit. This would be consistent with current modular approaches to cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis (Addington & Gleeson, 2018; Kowalski, Pankowski, Lew‐Starowicz, & Gawęda, 2017; Steel, 2008). Roberts has recently extracted parts of the ‘figuring it out’ phase of SCIT to deliver as a stand‐alone module to target attribution bias (Roberts, Kleinlein, & Stevens, 2012). Further refinement is required to the SCIT intervention when delivered to this population in order to confer a benefit. This may involve modifying the selection of participants, the dose of intervention, or the actual intervention.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this study include the successful completion of an adequately powered intervention to people with severe illness attending a community mental health service delivered within existing resources.

Limitations included recruiting from residential rehabilitation centres where the participants may have discussed the interventions with each other potentially diluting any difference in the programmes. No standardized symptom ratings were conducted, which may have limited the ability to detect benefits on non‐targeted symptom domains. It is known that most psychosocial interventions, including SCIT, are recommended to be delivered when patients are in a relatively stable phase of their illness (Hogarty, Flesher, Ulrich, & et al., 2004; Medalia & Richardson, 2005), with no standardized measures of symptomatology and the assumption that living in the community equated with relative mental stability may have resulted in participants attending at a time when they were too symptomatic to optimally benefit.

Additionally, participant programme satisfaction ratings were not conducted. These data could have informed future programme iterations.

Recommendations for future research

Ongoing research to develop interventions to address the social cognitive impairments associated with schizophrenia should be a priority given the relationship between these symptoms and functional outcomes. It is important that these findings and recommendations from RCTs are incorporated into the implementation of SCIT in mental health services (Dark et al., 2016). Future research needs to be conducted on the effectiveness of SCIT in patients who are assessed to have a high level of paranoia (Kowalski et al., 2017). Roberts has suggested increasing the frequency of the sessions to twice weekly (Roberts et al., 2014). Motivational principles such as linking session topics to personal goals, and frequent reinforcement for participation should be used to maximize attendance to increase the dose received by participants. Sex differences have been reported in social cognition (Zhang et al., 2017). Future studies with larger sample sizes should examine whether there are sex differences in the response to SCIT. Given the stepped nature of the SCIT programme, a study where participants only progress through modules after achieving minimal competency in each phase may ensure that the intervention is delivered at the correct dose for each individual.

Conclusion

There is a very high level of disability experienced by many living with schizophrenia. To date, pharmacotherapy has been ineffective for social cognitive and neurocognitive impairments. It is essential that researchers and clinicians continue to collaborate in order to develop and evaluate psychosocial interventions, which aim to improve the lives of people living with schizophrenia.

Author contributions

Frances Dark (Conceptualization; Funding acquisition; Methodology; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing) James Scott (Formal analysis; Supervision; Writing – review & editing) Andrea Baker (Data curation; Project administration; Supervision) Stephen Parker (Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Writing – review & editing) Anne Gordon (Conceptualization; Data curation; Writing – review & editing) Ellie Newman (Data curation; Methodology; Project administration; Writing – review & editing) Victoria Gore‐Jones (Investigation; Writing – review & editing) Carmen Lim (Formal analysis; Methodology; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing) Lyndall Jones (Conceptualization; Project administration; Writing – review & editing) David Penn (Conceptualization; Formal analysis; Methodology; Project administration; Supervision; Writing – original draft; Writing – review & editing).

Conflicts of interest

DP is a co‐developer of SCIT. The other authors have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

The Metro South Human Research Ethics Committee provided ethical clearance for this study (HREC/16/QPAH/98).

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the Executive Directors of Metro South and Metro North Addictions and Mental Health Service (Associate Professor David Crompton and Dr Brett Emerson). We acknowledge funding support from the Queensland Health Department via the Queensland Centre for Mental Health Research.

Table A1.

Estimated treatment effect at post‐treatment (week 12) among those that completed at least 6 sessions (n = 70)

Outcomes Post‐intervention outcome, least squares mean (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
I. BLERT
Total score 14.4 (1.0) 15.2 (1.0) 0.8 (0.6) .199 −0.07
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Social withdrawal 100.8 (1.4) 101.9 (1.5) 1.1 (2.0) .582 −0.13
Interpersonal communication 111.3 (3.0) 103.1 (3.0) −8.1 (4.3) .068 0.47
Independence performance 106.7 (1.5) 110.6 (1.6) 3.9 (2.3) .112 −0.43
Recreation activities 103.7 (2.1) 105.8 (2.2) 2.1 (3.0) .480 −0.17
Pro‐social activities 100.2 (2.1) 103.2 (2.2) 2.9 (2.6) .269 −0.24
Independence competence 96.2 (2.4) 100.6 (2.4) 4.5 (3.3) .188 −0.31
Occupational/employment 100.1 (1.3) 100.9 (1.4) 0.8 (1.9) .683 −0.10
Total score 102.7 (1.3) 103.7 (1.3) 1.0 (1.8) .575 −0.13
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 63.7 (1.8) 61.3 (1.8) −2.3 (2.6) .376 0.23
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 17.0 (0.5) 16.7 (0.5) −0.3 (0.7) .645 0.10
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 1.8 (0.5) 2.2 (0.6) 0.4 (0.3) .575 −0.12
Personalizing bias 0.9 (0.1) 0.8 (0.1) −0.1 (0.1) .321 0.17
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Scenario 1 32.4 (0.9) 32.3 (0.9) −0.1 (1.3) .917 0.02
Scenario 2 36.5 (0.9) 35.6 (0.9) −0.9 (1.3) .498 0.17
Total score 68.9 (1.7) 67.9 (1.8) −1.0 (2.4) .687 0.10

BT = Befriending Therapy; SCIT = Social Cognition Interaction Training; SE = standard error.

a

The reference group is BT.

Table A2.

Antipsychotic medications by the intervention group

Type of antipsychotic BT SCIT Total
n % n % n %
Amisulpride 6 6.7 5 6.0 11 6.3
Aripiprazole 15 16.7 15 18.1 30 17.3
Asenapine 1 1.1 0 0.0 1 0.6
Clozapine 25 27.8 24 28.9 49 28.3
Flupenthixol 6 6.7 2 2.4 8 4.6
Fluphenazine (Modecate) 0 0.0 1 1.2 1 0.6
Lurasidone 1 1.1 0 0.0 1 0.6
Olanzapine 11 12.2 9 10.8 20 11.6
Paliperidone 6 6.7 16 19.3 22 12.7
Quetiapine 4 4.4 4 4.8 8 4.6
Risperidone 10 11.1 5 6.0 15 8.7
Zuclopenthixol (Clopixol) 4 4.4 0 0.0 4 2.3
Ziprasidone 1 1.1 2 2.4 3 1.7
Total 90 100.0 83 100.0 173 100.0

Table A3.

Estimated treatment effect at post‐treatment (week 12) in the subsample of people with schizophrenia (n = 74)

Outcomes Post‐intervention, least squares means (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
I. BLERT
Total score 14.3 (0.6) 15.3 (0.6) 0.9 (0.8) .251 −0.28
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Total score 100.4 (1.3) 103.2 (1.3) 2.8 (1.8) .141 −0.36
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 66.0 (2.1) 64.4 (2.1) −1.6 (3.0) .593 0.13
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 16.8 (0.6) 16.7 (0.6) −0.1 (0.8) .941 0.03
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 1.8 (0.5) 2.0 (0.5) 0.2 (0.7) .808 −0.07
Personalizing bias 0.7 (0.1) 0.7 (0.1) 0.0 (0.1) .903 0.00
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Total score 68.0 (1.8) 68.2 (1.9) 0.2 (2.5) .943 −0.02

BT = Befriending Therapy; SCIT = Social Cognition Interaction Training; SE = standard error.

a

The reference group is BT.

Table A4.

Estimated treatment effect at post‐treatment (week 24) in the subsample of people with schizophrenia (n = 68)

Outcomes Post‐intervention, least squares means (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value Effect size, Cohen’s d
I. BLERT
Total score 14.7 (0.5) 15.2 (0.6) 0.4 (0.7) .544 −0.16
II. Social Functioning Scale (SFS)
Total score 104.4 (1.7) 106.2 (1.7) 1.7 (2.0) .402 −0.18
III. Meta‐Cognition Questionnaire (MCQ)
Total score 65.1 (2.2) 62.6 (2.2) −2.5 (2.9) .403 0.20
IV. Hinting Task (HT)
Total score 17.2 (0.4) 17.0 (0.4) −0.2 (0.7) .782 0.09
V. Internal, Personal, and Situational Attributions Questionnaire (IPSAQ)
Externalizing bias 0.7 (0.7) 1.3 (0.6) 0.6 (0.9) .537 −0.16
Personalizing bias 0.8 (0.1) 0.9 (0.1) 0.1 (0.1) .433 −0.17
VI. Social Skills Performance Assessment (SSPA)
Total score 68.6 (1.3) 70.2 (1.4) 0.7 (1.9) .730 −0.21

BT = Befriending Therapy; SCIT = Social Cognition Interaction Training; SE = standard error.

a

The reference group is BT.

Table A5.

Estimated treatment effect by number of sessions attended at post‐treatment (week 12)

Number of sessions attended BLERT total scores at post‐treatment (week 12)
Post‐intervention, least squares means (SE) Between‐condition differences
BT SCIT Differences of least squares means (SE) a p‐value
1–3 14.5 (1.1) 16.0 (1.0) 1.5 (1.4) .309
4–6 15.3 (1.4) 14.6 (1.2) −0.7 (1.9) .733
7–9 14.2 (1.0) 14.7 (1.0) 0.5 (0.8) .527
10–12 15.0 (0.8) 16.2 (0.9) 1.3 (1.1) .272
a

The reference group is BT.

Trial registration number: Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry ACTRN12616000866404.

Data availability statement

De‐identified data are available from the corresponding author.

References

  1. Addington, J. , & Gleeson, J. (2018). Implementing cognitive‐behavioural therapy for first‐episode psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 187(S48), s72–s76. 10.1192/bjp.187.48.s72 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bellack, A. S. , Schooler, N. R. , Marder, S. R. , Kane, J. M. , Brown, C. H. , & Yang, Y. (2004). Do clozapine and risperidone affect social competence and problem solving? American Journal of Psychiatry, 161(2), 364–7. 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.2.364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bendall, S. , Jackson, H. J. , Killackey, E. , Allott, K. , Johnson, T. , Harrigan, S. , … McGorry, P. D. (2006). The credibility and acceptability of befriending as a control therapy in a randomized controlled trial of cognitive behaviour therapy for acute first episode psychosis. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 34(3), 277–91. 10.1017/S1352465806002815 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Birchwood, M. , Smith, J. , Cochrane, R. , Wetton, S. , & Copestake, S. (1990). The social functioning scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 853–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowie, C. R. , & Harvey, P. D. (2005). Cognition in schizophrenia: Impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatric Clinics, 28(3), 613–33. 10.1016/j.psc.2005.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bryson, G. , Bell, M. , & Lysaker, P. (1997). Affect recognition in schizophrenia: a function of global impairment or a specific cognitive deficit. Psychiatry Research, 71(2), 105–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Castle, D. J. , Jablensky, A. , McGrath, J. J. , Carr, V. , Morgan, V. , Waterreus, A. , … Farmer, A. (2006). The diagnostic interview for psychoses (DIP): Development, reliability and applications. Psychological Medicine, 36(1), 69–80. 10.1017/s0033291705005969 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Combs, D. R. , Adams, S. D. , Penn, D. L. , Roberts, D. , Tiegreen, J. , & Stem, P. (2007). Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for inpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: Preliminary findings. Schizophrenia Research, 91(1), 112–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Corcoran, R. , Mercer, G. , & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17(1), 5–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dark, F. , Newman, E. , Harris, M. , Cairns, A. , Simpson, M. , Gore‐Jones, V. , … Crompton, D. (2016). Implementing cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) in a mental health service: Staff training. Australas Psychiatry, 24(2), 185–9. 10.1177/1039856215604486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Fett, A. K. , Viechtbauer, W. , Dominguez, M. D. , Penn, D. L. , van Os, J. , & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta‐analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573–88. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gordon, A. , Davis, P. J. , Patterson, S. , Pepping, C. A. , Scott, J. G. , Salter, K. , & Connell, M. (2017). A randomized waitlist control community study of social cognition and interaction training for people with schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(1), 116–30. 10.1111/bjc.12161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Grant, N. , Lawrence, M. , Preti, A. , Wykes, T. , & Cella, M. (2017). Social cognition interventions for people with schizophrenia: A systematic review focussing on methodological quality and intervention modality. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 55–64. 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Green, M. F. , Kern, R. S. , Baff, D. L. , & Mintz, J. (2000). Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: Are we measuring the “right stuff?” Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 119–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Green, M. F. , Penn, D. L. , Bentall, R. , Carpenter, W. T. , Gaebel, W. , Gur, R. C. , … Heinssen, R. (2008). Social cognition in schizophrenia: An NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 1211–20. 10.1093/schbul/sbm145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hasson‐Ohayon, I. , Mashiach‐Eizenberg, M. , Avidan, M. , Roberts, D. L. , & Roe, D. (2014). Social cognition and interaction training: preliminary results of an RCT in a community setting in Israel. Psychiatric Services, 65(4), 555–8. 10.1176/appi.ps.201300146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hogarty, G. E. , Flesher, S. , Ulrich, R. , Carter, M. , Greenwald, D. , Pogue‐Geile, M. , … Zoretich, R. (2004a). Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: effects of a 2‐year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(9), 866–76. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hogarty, G. E. , Flesher, S. , Ulrich, R. , Carter, M. , Greenwald, D. , Pogue‐Geile, M. , … Zoretich, R. (2004b). Cognitive enhancement therapy for schizophrenia: Effects of a 2‐year randomized trial on cognition and behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(9), 866–76. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Javed, A. , & Charles, A. (2018). The importance of social cognition in improving functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 157 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kahan, B. C. (2014). Accounting for centre‐effects in multicentre trials with a binary outcome ‐ when, why, and how? BMC Medical Research Methodology, 14, 20–20. 10.1186/1471-2288-14-20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kinderman, P. , & Bentall, R. P. (1996). A new measure of causal locus: The internal, personal and situational attributions questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 20(2), 261–4. 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00186-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kowalski, J. , Pankowski, D. , Lew‐Starowicz, M. , & Gawęda, Ł. (2017). Do specific metacognitive training modules lead to specific cognitive changes among patients diagnosed with schizophrenia? A single module effectiveness pilot study. Psychosis, 9(3), 254–9. 10.1080/17522439.2017.1300186 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kurtz, M. M. , Gagen, E. , Rocha, N. B. F. , Machado, S. , & Penn, D. L. (2015). Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A critical review and effect‐size analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 43, 80–9. 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kurtz, M. M. , Gagen, E. , Rocha, N. B. , Machado, S. , & Penn, D. L. (2016). Comprehensive treatments for social cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A critical review and effect‐size analysis of controlled studies. Clin Psychol Rev, 43, 80–9. 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kurtz, M. M. , & Richardson, C. L. (2012). Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: A meta‐analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(5), 1092–104. 10.1093/schbul/sbr036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Maruff, P. , Thomas, E. , Cysique, L. , Brew, B. , Collie, A. , Snyder, P. , & Pietrzak, R. H. (2009). Validity of the CogState brief battery: relationship to standardized tests and sensitivity to cognitive impairment in mild traumatic brain injury, schizophrenia, and AIDS dementia complex. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(2), 165–178178. 10.1093/arclin/acp010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. McGurk, S. R. , Twamley, E. W. , Sitzer, D. I. , McHugo, G. J. , & Mueser, K. T. (2007). A meta‐analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(12), 1791–802. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07060906 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Medalia, A. , & Richardson, R. (2005). What predicts a good response to cognitive remediation interventions? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(4), 942–53. 10.1093/schbul/sbi045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Parker, S. , Foley, S. , Walker, P. , & Dark, F. (2013). Improving the social cognitive deficits of schizophrenia: A community trial of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT). Australas Psychiatry, 21(4), 346–51. 10.1177/1039856213486305 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Patterson, T. L. , Moscona, S. , McKibbin, C. L. , Davidson, K. , & Jeste, D. V. (2001). Social skills performance assessment among older patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 48(2‐3), 351–60. 10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00109-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pinkham, A. E. , Penn, D. L. , Green, M. F. , Buck, B. , Healey, K. , & Harvey, P. D. (2014). The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: Results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 40(4), 813–23. 10.1093/schbul/sbt081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Roberts, D. L. , Combs, D. R. , Willoughby, M. , Mintz, J. , Gibson, C. , Rupp, B. , & Penn, D. L. (2014). A randomized, controlled trial of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 53(3), 281–98. 10.1111/bjc.12044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Roberts, D. L. , Kleinlein, P. , & Stevens, B. (2012). An alternative to generating alternative interpretations in social cognitive therapy for psychosis. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40, 491–5. 10.1017/S1352465812000082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Roberts, D. L. , & Penn, D. L. (2009). Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia: A preliminary study. Psychiatry Research, 166(2–3), 141–7. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Roberts, D. L. , Penn, D. L. , & Combs, D. R. (2016). Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT)Group Psychotherapy for Schizophrenia and Other Psychotic Disorders, Clinician Guide. [Google Scholar]
  36. Roberts, D. L. , Penn, D. L. , Labate, D. , Margolis, S. A. , & Sterne, A. (2010). Transportability and feasibility of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) in community settings. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 38(1), 35–47. 10.1017/s1352465809990464 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Sensky, T. , Turkington, D. , Kingdon, D. , Scott, J. L. , Scott, J. , Siddle, R. , … Barnes, T. R. (2000). A randomized controlled trial of cognitive‐behavioral therapy for persistent symptoms in schizophrenia resistant to medication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(2), 165–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Sergi, M. J. , Rassovsky, Y. , Nuechterlein, K. H. , & Green, M. F. (2006). Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(3), 448–54. 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Silver, H. , Goodman, C. , Knoll, G. , & Isakov, V. (2004). Brief emotion training improves recognition of facial emotions in chronic schizophrenia. A pilot study. Psychiatry Research, 128(2), 147–54. 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Steel, C. (2008). Cognitive behaviour therapy for psychosis: Current evidence and future directions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 705–12. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tas, C. , Danaci, A. E. , Cubukcuoglu, Z. , & Brune, M. (2012). Impact of family involvement on social cognition training in clinically stable outpatients with schizophrenia – a randomized pilot study. Psychiatry Research, 195(1–2), 32–8. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Taylor, R. , Cella, M. , Csipke, E. , Heriot‐Maitland, C. , Gibbs, C. , & Wykes, T. (2016). Tackling social cognition in schizophrenia: a randomized feasibility trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 44, 306–17. 10.1017/s1352465815000284 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Wang, Y. , Roberts, D. L. , Xu, B. , Cao, R. , Yan, M. , & Jiang, Q. (2013). Social cognition and interaction training for patients with stable schizophrenia in Chinese community settings. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 751–5. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Wells, A. , & Cartwright‐Hatton, S. (2004). A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: properties of the MCQ‐30. Behavior Research and Therapy, 42, 385–96. 10.1016/s0005-7967(03)00147-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wykes, T. , Huddy, V. , Cellard, C. , McGurk, R. , & Czobor, P. (2011). A meta‐analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: Methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5), 472–85. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

De‐identified data are available from the corresponding author.


Articles from The British Journal of Clinical Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES