Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 Jul 6;74(8):1851–1855. doi: 10.1111/evo.14033

Sexually selected male weapon is associated with lower inbreeding load but higher sex load in the bulb mite

Aleksandra Łukasiewicz 1, Małgorzata Niśkiewicz 1, Jacek Radwan 1,
PMCID: PMC7496443  PMID: 32519389

Abstract

Elaborate sexually selected ornaments and armaments are costly but increase the reproductive success of their bearers (usually males). It has been postulated that high‐quality males can invest disproportionately more in such traits, making those traits honest signals of genetic quality. However, genes associated with such traits may have sexually antagonistic effects on fitness. Here, using a bulb mite Rhizoglyphus robini, a species in which a distinct dimorphism exists between males in the expression of a sexually selected weapon, we compare inbreeding and gender load between lines derived from armed fighters and unarmed scramblers. After four generations of sib‐mating, inbreeding depression for female fitness was significantly lower in fighter‐derived lines compared to scrambler‐derived lines, suggesting that fighter males had significantly higher genetic quality. However, outbred females from fighter‐derived lines had significantly lower fitness compared to outbred females from scrambler‐derived lines, demonstrating significant gender load associated with the presence of a sexually selected male weapon. Our results imply that under outbreeding, genetic benefits of mating with bearers of elaborate sexually selected traits might be swamped by the costs of decreased fitness of female progeny due to sexually antagonistic effects.

Keywords: Alternative reproductive tactics, good genes, male weapons, sexual conflict, sexual selection, sexually antagonistic selection


Darwin developed his theory of sexual selection to explain the evolution of elaborate traits, such as deer antlers and peacock trains, that are likely to decrease survival but increase reproductive success (Darwin 1871), helping in male–male combat and in attracting the opposite sex (Andersson 1994; Andersson and Simmons 2006). Energetic costs of developing and maintaining these sexually selected traits in males imply that they can act as an honest signal of male genetic quality. Thus, female preferences for such traits can accrue indirect fitness benefits due to increased fitness of their progeny (Zahavi 1975; Andersson 1986; Grafen 1990; Houle and Kondrashov 2002). However, support for this “good genes” hypothesis is equivocal. While some studies that manipulated deleterious mutation load directly (Herdegen and Radwan 2015), exposed it via inbreeding (van Oosterhout et al. 2003) or inferred it from genomic data (Dugand et al. 2019) find support for sexually attractive males carrying lower mutation load (but see Prokop et al. 2010), a meta‐analysis found no significant association between male attractiveness and progeny fitness, except for increased sexual attractiveness of male progeny (Prokop et al. 2012). These apparently conflicting results may be reconciled if genes associated with the expression of sexually selected traits in males also have negative pleiotropic effects on other male fitness components, or on female fitness (Lande 1980; Rice and Chippindale 2001; Radwan et al. 2015; Zajitschek and Connallon 2018).

Indeed there is some evidence that sexual weapons or ornaments show negative genetic correlations with female fitness (Harano et al. 2010; Plesnar‐Bielak et al. 2014), as well as juvenile (Brooks 2000) and adult male survival (Johnston et al. 2013). Thus, even if males expressing the more elaborate sexual traits carry a lower load of deleterious mutations, their progeny, particularly of the opposite sex, may not necessarily have higher fitness. Empirical studies simultaneously quantifying both mutation load and negative pleiotropy are needed to better appreciate the relative contribution of each to male and female fitness. Here, we report such a study using inbred lines derived from males differing in the expression of a heritable sexually selected weapon.

Our model species for this study was the bulb mite Rhizoglypus robini, a male‐dimorphic species in which aggressive fighter males express a sexually selected weapon in the form of thickened and sharply terminated third pair of legs, which in scrambler males and in females are unmodified and resemble the other pairs. This weapon is costly to produce and condition‐dependent (Smallegange 2011b; Radwan 1995), therefore its significant heritability (Radwan 1995, 2003a; Smallegange and Coulson 2011) could potentially result from differences in male genetic quality, with the weapon expressed mostly by males with a lower load of deleterious mutations. Indeed, proportions of morphs among progeny do not conform to standard Mendelian segregation, although the contribution of major effect genes cannot be excluded (Radwan 1995). Consistent with this "good genes" mechanism, fighters were reported to have better survival and higher reproductive success than scramblers (Radwan and Klimas 2001). By contrast, there is some evidence that genes associated with fighter morph expression are detrimental when expressed in females. Females from artificial selection lines nearly fixed for fighter males were less fecund and shorter‐lived compared to females from lines nearly fixed for scrambler males (Plesnar‐Bielak et al. 2014). One explanation for this phenomenon would be that fighter males carry a higher load of deleterious mutations across their genome, but this would be inconsistent with condition‐dependent expression of the weapon. Alternatively, during mass selection on morph frequencies, natural and sexual selection could simultaneously act within selection lines. The resulting lower female fecundity in fighter‐selected lines could be due to sexually antagonistic alleles being favored by sexual or natural selection. Finally, if expression of the weapon is directly affected by genes unrelated to condition, such genes might be negatively associated with female fitness. For example, the genes for weapon expression could be located on inversions also carrying female‐harm genes. Here, we estimated both the load of deleterious mutations and gender load using inbred lines derived from females mated with either fighter or scrambler males (henceforth F‐ and S‐lines, respectively).

Because deleterious mutations segregating in populations are typically partially recessive, their deleterious effects are easiest to detect under inbreeding (Tomkins et al. 2004). Inbreeding increases genome‐wide homozygosity leading to inbreeding depression attributable mainly to the expression of a large number of small effect, partially recessive mutations (Charlesworth and Willis 2009). To test whether expression of the weapon is associated with a lower load of deleterious recessives, we compared inbreeding depression for female fecundity between F‐ and S‐lines. We chose female fecundity as our focal trait because, being typically highly polygenic, it can capture the load of deleterious mutations across a large proportion of the genome (Houle 1998; Radwan 2003b). To test for gender load associated with genes underlying fighter expression, we compared female fecundity between outbreds derived from F‐ and S‐lines.

Methods

We collected mites from onions gathered in a field in Mosina, Wielkopolskie, Poland, in October 2017, and maintained them under laboratory conditions of 24°C at >95% humidity, on a diet of ad libitum powdered yeast. We established inbred lines from the first generation that hatched in the laboratory, and used the remaining individuals to found a stock culture. To obtain virgin females to start our inbred lines, we placed 100 larvae in individual vials (0.8 cm diameter glass vials with plaster base soaked with water) and after they reached adulthood, we set up males and females in single‐pair crosses. The proportion of both male morphs among our inbred lines reflected those found in the original population: we established 26 F‐lines and 15 S‐lines. In the following generations, we maintained these inbred lines by sib–sib mating as described below. Individuals were selected randomly for the next generation, except that we used males of the same morph as the founder males for each line, to help maintain differences in morph proportions between F‐ and S‐lines (Fig. S1). To do this, we reared 20 larvae through to adulthood for each line, and mated each male of the appropriate morph with a randomly selected virgin female, giving typically two to six replicate families per generation. One of these families was randomly selected each time to found the next generation. If the first selected family failed to produce offspring, another was randomly selected to replace it; this occurred on five separate occasions at generation 2 (one in S lines and four in F lines), three cases at generation 3 (all three in F lines), and seven at generation 4 (three in S lines and four in F lines). After four generations of sib–sib mating (inbreeding coefficient F = 0. 594) we estimated inbreeding depression and gender load in F‐ and S‐lines by comparing female fecundity between F4 inbred and outbred lines. We used 14 of the 15 available S‐lines (one line did not produce enough individuals) and a subset of 19 randomly selected F‐lines. We produced outbred females by setting up crosses between different inbred lines founded by the same male morph type. Through this method we generated 18 different combinations of S‐lines, and 17 combinations of F‐lines, with each inbred line used in no more than two such combinations. After females laid eggs, we randomly selected 10 larvae from each inbred and outbred family and from this selected two to four virgin females at maturity to mate with males from the stock culture. This allowed us to estimate inbreeding depression for female fecundity independently from male fertility. Males of both morphs were used as mates. We counted the number of eggs laid in a period of 7 days, during which females had continuous access to a male, in order to avoid confounding effects due to sperm limitation. Pairs were transferred to fresh vials once at 4 days after male introduction. Because female reproductive output remains high and changes little over the first 3 weeks (Tilszer et al. 2005), eggs laid over 7 days can be used as a proxy for female lifetime fecundity.

Data on female fecundity were analyzed by using a linear mixed model (LMM). Normality of error distributions was verified with diagnostic plots of residuals and Shapiro–Wilk tests. Treatment (inbred or outbred), founder male morph, their interaction, and morph of a male with which the focal female was paired, were entered as fixed factors. A significant founder × treatment interaction was expected if inbreeding load differed between lines founded by scramblers and fighters. Maternal and paternal line identities were entered as random factors. Random slopes were modeled to allow for lines to respond differentially to inbreeding. LMM was implemented by the lmer function in the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2015), with P‐values calculated based on Satterthwate's approximations using the package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2014), implemented in R version 3.5.1 (R Core Team 2018).

Results and Discussion

We found that female fecundity was significantly influenced by the interaction between founder morph and inbreeding (LMM: t = −3.096, P = 0.003). Female fecundity in inbreds dropped, compared to outbreds, more markedly in S‐lines than in F‐lines (Fig. 1, Table 1). This indicates that a higher load of deleterious recessives segregated in S‐lines compared to F‐lines. In addition to a lower mutation load in fighter founders, this result could be due to more effective purging of deleterious recessives in F‐lines. This could occur because fighter males selected as sires in each generation of inbreeding in these lines were likely in better condition compared to scrambler males (Radwan 1995; Smallegange 2011b). In any case, the apparent lower load of deleterious recessives in F‐lines did not result in higher fitness for outbred females. On the contrary, inspection of Figure 1 and the model summary (Table 1) shows that fecundity of outbred females derived from S‐lines was significantly higher than in F‐lines (the effect of scrambler founder compared to outbred fighter in intercept, t = 2.557, P = 0.019). This result may appear paradoxical, as inbreeding depression is thought to be caused mainly by the accumulation of mildly deleterious mutations (Charlesworth and Willis 2009), which are often only partially recessive (Crow and Simmons 1983; Barrett and Charlesworth 1991). Likewise in the bulb‐mites, there is evidence for partially recessive alleles of small effect having a major contribution to inbreeding depression, as estimated from female fecundity effects (Radwan 2003b; Jarzebowska and Radwan 2010). If this is true then the accumulation of recessive alleles across multiple genetic loci should have negatively affected female fecundity more in S‐lines that demonstrated greater evidence of inbreeding depression. The opposite pattern that we detected indicates that genes associated with expression of a costly weapon have a negative effect on female fitness, that is, cause gender load.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Effects of inbreeding and inbred lines founder morph on female fecundity. The box encloses values between the first and third quartiles of the data (the inter‐quartile range, IQR), while the horizontal bar within the box indicates the median. Whiskers extend from the box to the largest/smallest values that are within 1.5× the IQR of the box.

Table 1.

Results of linear mixed model with female fecundity as a dependent variable, and the morph of inbred line founder (Founder morph), treatment (inbreeding vs. outbreeding) and morph of a male the female mated with (Mate morph) as predictors. The identities of inbred lines that the female and male came from were entered as crossed random factors

Parameter estimate Standard error df T P‐value
Fixed effects (Intercept) 75.44 9.67 26.34 7.80 <0.001
Founder morph ‐ scrambler 32.69 12.79 20.28 2.557 0.019
Treatment ‐ inbred −13.12 9.93 51.75 −1.322 0.192
Mate morph ‐ scrambler −10.82 5.77 152.89 −1.874 0.063
Founder morph* Treatment state −40.95 13.23 53.47 −3.096 0.003
Variance Standard deviation
Maternal line:
Random effects (Intercept) 512.96 22.65
Treatment ‐ inbred 44.80 6.69
Paternal line:
(Intercept) 72.06 8.49
Treatment ‐ inbred 31.37 5.60
Residual 1556.67 39.46

Our results are consistent with another report of decreased female fecundity in bulb‐mites following near‐fixation of the male weapon phenotype in mass‐selection lines (Plesnar‐Bielak et al. 2014) and with results of a similar experiment on horned beetles (Harano et al. 2010). Interestingly, the effect sizes reported in these studies are highly comparable with those found here (15‐25%; here: 22% ± 8% decline in fecundity). While female harm associated with a sexually selected weapon in males appears to be a general phenomenon in R. robini, the results of the experiment by Plesnar‐Bielak et al. (2014) could be also explained by inadvertent selection for fighter‐ or scrambler‐beneficial alleles, with the former possibly having sexually antagonistic effects. Here, natural and sexual selection were minimized by using inbred lines established by sib‐mating of randomly selected, monogamous pairs, such that inadvertent selection on life histories was much reduced. Nevertheless, by selecting fighter males in F‐lines, we could have selected alleles that increased the probability of weapon expression, for example, those contributing to male phenotypic condition. This would imply that such genes have negative pleiotropic effects on female fitness. Alternatively, genes underlying morph expression could have pleiotropic effects on other sexually antagonistic traits expressed in both sexes (Hosken 2011), or be located in regions of low recombination (e.g., Küpper et al. 2016) containing linked genes that negatively impact female fitness.

Our results indicate that both mutation load and negative pleiotropy can contribute to the maintenance of genetic variance for male weapon expression in the bulb mite. Because morph expression in this species is condition‐dependent (Smallegange 2011a; Radwan 1995), deleterious mutation load should be negatively correlated to expression of sexual weapons, which is consistent with our evidence for a higher inbreeding load in S‐lines. The genetic variance may be further augmented by balancing selection arising from sexual antagonism and other forms of negative pleiotropy (Zajitschek and Connallon 2018). Our results highlight sexually antagonistic effects associated with elaboration of traits under sexual selection, and imply that such effects can swamp any genetic benefits of mating with high‐condition males. If our results reflect the general property of sexually selected traits, then sexual antagonism associated with their evolution may explain the absence of good genes benefits of mating with their bearers despite substantial heritability of these traits (Prokop et al. 2012; Prokuda and Roff 2014). Comparatively high gender load can also limit the potential of good genes sexual selection to improve population fitness, and interspecific differences in this load may help explain why elaborate sexual traits are associated with increased risk of extinction in some taxa (Martins et al. 2018) but with decreased risk in others (Parrett et al. 2019).

DATA ARCHIVING

Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.ghx3ffbkc

Associate Editor: L. Spurgin

Handling Editor: D. W. Hall

Supporting information

Fig. S1. Proportions of fighter morph in fighter (black) and scrambler (grey) inbred lines across 4 generations of brother‐sister mating.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Jon Parrett for his advice on statistics and for his comments on the manuscript, which also benefited from comments by Philip Leftwich, Tracey Chapman, three anonymous reviewers and Editors, Drs David Hall and Lewis Spurgin. We are grateful to Agnieszka Szubert‐Kruszyńska for help during experiments. This work was supported by National Science Centre grant 2017/27/B/NZ8/00077 to JR and PhD scholarships to AŁ (founded by National Science Centre 2019/32/T/NZ8/00112 and European Social Funds POWER.03.02.00–00–I006/17).

LITERATURE CITED

  1. Andersson, M. 1986. Evolution of condition dependent sex ornaments and mating preferences: sexual selection based on viability differences. Evolution 40:804–816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersson, M. , and Simmons L. W.. 2006. Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends Ecol. Evol 21:296–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersson, M. B. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ. [Google Scholar]
  4. Barrett, S. C. , and Charlesworth D.. 1991. Effects of a change in the level of inbreeding on the genetic load. Nature 352:522–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates, D. , Mächler M., Bolker B., and Walker S.. 2015. Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
  6. Brooks, R. 2000. Negative genetic correlation between male sexual attractiveness and survival. Nature 406:67–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Charlesworth, D. , and Willis J. H.. 2009. The genetics of inbreeding depression. Nat. Rev. Genet 10:783–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crow, J. F. , and Simmons M. J.. 1983. The mutation load in Drosophila Pp 1–35 in Ashburner M., Carson H. L., and Thompson J. N., editors. The genetics and biology of Drosophila. Academic Press, London, U.K. [Google Scholar]
  9. Darwin, C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray, London, U.K. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dugand, R. J. , Tomkins J. L., and Kennington W. J.. 2019. Molecular evidence supports a genic capture resolution of the lek paradox. Nat. Commun 10:1359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Grafen, A. 1990. Biological signals as handicaps. J. Theor. Biol 144:517–546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Harano, T. , Okada K., Nakayama S., Miyatake T., and Hosken D. J.. 2010. Intralocus sexual conflict unresolved by sex‐limited trait expression. Curr. Biol 20:2036–2039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Herdegen, M. , and Radwan J.. 2015. Effect of induced mutations on sexually selected traits in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata . Anim. Behav. 110:105–111. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hosken, D. J. 2011. Gene duplication might not resolve intralocus sexual conflict. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26:556–557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Houle, D. 1998. How should we explain variation in the genetic variance of traits? Genetica 103:241–253. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Houle, D. , and Kondrashov A. S.. 2002. Coevolution of costly mate choice and condition‐dependent display of good genes. Proc. Royal Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 269:97–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jarzebowska, M. , and Radwan J.. 2010. Sexual selection counteracts extinction of small populations of the bulb mites. Evolution 64:1283–1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Johnston, S. E. , Gratten J., Berenos C., Pilkington J. G., Clutton‐Brock T. H., Pemberton J. M., and Slate J.. 2013. Life history trade‐offs at a single locus maintain sexually selected genetic variation. Nature 502:93–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kuznetsova, A. , Brockhoff P. B., and Christensen R. H. B.. 2014. lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effectmodels (lmer objects of lme4package). R package version 2.0‐6. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package5lmerTest
  20. Küpper, C. , Stocks M., Risse J. E., dos Remedios N., Farrell L. L., McRae S. B., Morgan T. C., Karlionova N., Pinchuk P., Verkuil Y. I., et al. 2016. A supergene determines highly divergent male reproductive morphs in the ruff. Nat. Genet. 48:79–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lande, R. 1980. Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection and adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34:292–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Martins, M. J. F. , Puckett T. M., Lockwood R., Swaddle J. P., and Hunt G.. 2018. High male sexual investment as a driver of extinction in fossil ostracods. Nature 556:366–369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Parrett, J. M. , Mann D. J., Chung A. Y. C., Slade E. M., and Knell R. J.. 2019. Sexual selection predicts the persistence of populations within altered environments. Ecol. Lett. 22:1629–1637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Plesnar‐Bielak, A. , Skrzynecka A. M., Miler K., and Radwan J.. 2014. Selection for alternative male reproductive tactics alters intralocus sexual conflict. Evolution 68:2137–2144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Prokop, Z. M. , Les J. E., Banas P. K., Koteja P., and Radwan J.. 2010. Low inbreeding depression in a sexual trait in the stalk‐eyed fly Teleopsis dalmanni . Evol. Ecol. 24:827–837. [Google Scholar]
  26. Prokop, Z. M. , Michalczyk L., Drobniak S. M., Herdegen M., and Radwan J.. 2012. Meta‐analysis suggests choosy females get sexy sons more than “good genes”. Evolution 66:2665–2673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Prokuda, A. Y. , and Roff D. A.. 2014. The quantitative genetics of sexually selected traits, preferred traits and preference: a review and analysis of the data. J. Evol. Biol. 27:2283–2296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Radwan, J. 1995. Male morph determination in two species of acarid mites. Heredity 74:669–673. [Google Scholar]
  29. Radwan, J. 2003a. Heritability of male morph in the bulb mite, Rhizoglyphus robini (Astigmata, Acaridae). Exp. Appl. Acarol. 29:109–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Radwan, J. 2003b. Inbreeding depression in fecundity and inbred line extinction in the bulb mite, Rhizoglyphus robini . Heredity 90:371–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Radwan, J. , Engqvist L., and Reinhold K.. 2015. A paradox of genetic variance in epigamic traits: beyond “good genes” view of sexual selection. Evol. Biol. 43:267–275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Radwan, J. , and Klimas M.. 2001. Male dimorphism in the bulb mite, Rhizoglyphus robini: fighters survive better. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 13:69–79. [Google Scholar]
  33. R Core Team . 2018. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria: http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
  34. Rice, W. R. , and Chippindale A. K.. 2001. Intersexual ontogenetic conflict. J. Evol. Biol. 14:685–693. [Google Scholar]
  35. Smallegange, I. M. 2011a. Complex environmental effects on the expression of alternative reproductive phenotypes in the bulb mite. Evol. Ecol. 25:857–873. [Google Scholar]
  36. Smallegange, I. M. 2011b. Effects of paternal phenotype and environmental variability on age and size at maturity in a male dimorphic mite. Naturwissenschaften 98:339–346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Smallegange, I. M. , and Coulson T.. 2011. The stochastic demography of two coexisting male morphs. Ecology 92:755–764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tilszer, M. , Antoszczyk K., Salek N., Zajac E., and Radwan J.. 2005. Evolution under relaxed sexual conflict in the bulb mite Rhizoglyphus robini. Evolution 60:1868 https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2006.tb00530.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Tomkins, J. L. , Radwan J., Kotiaho J. S., and Tregenza T.. 2004. Genic capture and resolving the lek paradox. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19:323–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. van Oosterhout, C. , Trigg R. E., Carvalho G. R., Magurran A. E., Hauser L., and Shaw P. W.. 2003. Inbreeding depression and genetic load of sexually selected traits: how the guppy lost its spots. J. Evol. Biol. 16:273–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Zahavi, A. 1975. Mate selection ‐ a selection for a handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53:205–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Zajitschek, F. , and Connallon T.. 2018. Antagonistic pleiotropy in species with separate sexes, and the maintenance of genetic variation in life‐history traits and fitness. Evolution 72:1306–1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Fig. S1. Proportions of fighter morph in fighter (black) and scrambler (grey) inbred lines across 4 generations of brother‐sister mating.

Data Availability Statement

Data available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.ghx3ffbkc


Articles from Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES