Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Sep 21.
Published in final edited form as: Trends Neurosci. 2020 Apr 16;43(6):385–393. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.03.010

Impacts of Sleep Loss versus Waking Experience on Brain Plasticity: Parallel or Orthogonal?

Robbert Havekes 1,*, Sara J Aton 2,*
PMCID: PMC7505037  NIHMSID: NIHMS1629292  PMID: 32459991

Abstract

Recent studies on the effects of sleep deprivation on synaptic plasticity have yielded discrepant results. Sleep deprivation studies using novelty exposure as a means to keep animals awake suggests that sleep (compared with wake) leads to widespread reductions in net synaptic strength. By contrast, sleep deprivation studies using approaches avoiding novelty-induced arousal (i.e., gentle handling) suggest that sleep can promote synaptic growth and strengthening. How can these discrepant findings be reconciled? Here, we discuss how varying methodologies for the experimental disruption of sleep (with differential introduction of novel experiences) could fundamentally alter the experimental outcome with regard to synaptic plasticity. Thus, data from experiments aimed at assessing the relative impact of sleep versus wake on the brain may instead reflect the quality of the waking experience itself. The highlighted work suggests that brain plasticity resulting from novel experiences versus wake per se has unique and distinct features.

Resolving the Plasticity Functions of Sleep in the Brain

Sleep behavior is conserved across animal phyla, and sleep appears to be critical both for appropriate function in wake, and ultimately, for survival. Some features of sleep behavior, such as its prevalence during early life in many species, have suggested that the sleeping brain state promotes experience- dependent remodeling of the brain. A large body of research has shown that throughout the human lifespan, there are numerous cognitive benefits of sleep. What aspects of sleep mediate these effects on the nervous system? Work aimed at clarifying the physiological benefits of sleep, and why sleep loss is so detrimental, has profiled numerous effects of experimental sleep deprivation on the brain.

Studies examining the molecular and cellular impact of sleep deprivation have generally fallen into two categories: (i) those suggesting that sleep leads to synaptic downscaling throughout the brain (while spontaneous wake or experimental sleep deprivation promote net synaptic growth and strengthening); and (ii) those that indicate a role for sleep in the strengthening of synapses following learning (with experimental sleep deprivation having the opposite effect). In this opinion article, we aim to reconcile these seemingly contradictory findings. We first provide an overview of work suggesting these opposite effects of sleep deprivation on synaptic strength, focusing on implications for hippocampal and cortical plasticity. We then discuss issues of experimental methodology that could contribute to these divergent effects (Figure 1), as well as other methodological issues that may impact the consequences of sleep and sleep loss on the brain.

Figure 1. Different Methodologies for Induction of Sleep Deprivation Result in Divergent Effects on Synaptic Strength.

Figure 1.

In the past few years, studies have reported that prolonged wake either increases net spine numbers and synaptic strength or (conversely) decreases synaptic strength. These discrepant observations could be explained by examining the methodology used in these studies to enforce wake. We propose a hypothetical scenario in which the magnitude of novel sensory stimuli (associated with both sleep deprivation methods) and prolonged wake itself have opposite effects on net synaptic strength. These effects (relative to sleep) are shown in the graph at the left. Provision of novel sensory stimuli alone (broken red line) leads to maximal increases in net synaptic strength in the neocortex and hippocampus. This increase is partially mitigated by the effects of wake itself (broken blue line) during novel-stimulus-induced sleep deprivation (unbroken red line). This is because prolonged wake alone (broken blue line) tends to reduce synaptic strength. Gentle handling (mild sensory stimulation) leads to a more moderate increase in net synaptic strength in the neocortex and hippocampus (broken violet line), which is similarly mitigated by the effects of wake itself (broken blue line) during gentle-handling sleep deprivation (unbroken violet line).

Evidence for Net Synaptic Strengthening in Wake and Net Weakening in Sleep

A prominent hypothesis regarding the function of sleep for the brain is the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis, which proposes that sleep leads to a beneficial net weakening of (excitatory) synapses in circuits throughout the brain [1]. The hypothesis is supported by data from studies of synaptic structures in the neocortex and hippocampus using experimental sleep deprivation. The hypothesis is supported by several lines of data. The first indicates that spontaneous wake or experimentally prolonged sleep loss in rats causes firing rates among barrel cortex neurons to gradually increase [2]. While this change could be due to changes in intrinsic excitability, excitatory/inhibitory balance, or neuromodulation alterations during sleep deprivation, in light of additional evidence (described below) these changes in firing have been attributed to increases in excitatory synaptic strength. Second, 4–6-h prolonged wake is associated with increased phosphorylation of AMPA receptors (AMPARs), CaMKII, and GSK3beta in both hippocampal and neocortical synaptoneurosomes of mice and rats (all indicative of glutamatergic synapse potentiation) [3,4]. Using serial electron microscopy (EM), it was shown that 6–7 h of enforced wake leads to an increase in the axon–spine interface and spine head volume in mouse somatosensory and motor cortices [5]. Analyses of the hippocampus using a partially overlapping cohort of mice indicated that enforced wake lead to a redistribution to larger sizes of the axon–spine interface in perforated synapses only. There was no associated change in non-perforated synapses (although the authors reported a nonsignificant trend of increase) or perforated synapse density [6]. Similarly, in vivo imaging in mice indicated that 6–7 h of experimental sleep deprivation, or spontaneous wake at night, leads to relative increases in the rate of spines formed and decreases in the rate of spines lost in barrel cortex dendrites, relative to uninterrupted daytime sleep [7]. Together, these data have built a case for net synaptic strengthening across wake and, conversely, net synaptic weakening across periods of sleep.

Evidence for Net Synaptic Strengthening during Sleep and Net Weakening during Wake

Work examining plasticity in both the neocortex and the hippocampus have found evidence that experimental sleep deprivation disrupts, rather than promotes, synaptic potentiation. For example, increases in primary visual cortex (V1) neurons’ firing rate responses to visual stimuli are seen after a period of sleep in both mouse [810] and cat [11,12] following novel visual experiences. As discussed above, while firing rate changes alone could be due to many cellular or network processes, these effects are stimulus specific and linked to a Hebbian plasticity mechanism (NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation [LTP]; [13,14]). In both systems, this sleep-associated response potentiation is associated with biochemical hallmarks of synaptic potentiation and are dependent on pathways mediating LTP [11,15,16]. Both forms of plasticity and associated molecular events are disrupted by experimental sleep deprivation. Similarly, evoked responses are potentiated in an AMPAR- and NMDA receptor-dependent manner in the cat somatosensory cortex over a period of slow-wave sleep [17]. In line with these observations in the cortex, 5–6-h sleep deprivation attenuates long-lasting forms of hippocampal LTP in mice that require the cAMP, LIMK–cofilin, and mTOR pathways [1821]. Brief experimental sleep loss has also been shown to disrupt Arc expression [22] and AMPAR phosphorylation [23] in the mouse hippocampus.

There are also data suggesting that synaptic structures can be preferentially enhanced during sleep or disrupted by sleep loss. Sophisticated in vivo imaging studies have shown that following training on a motor task, new dendritic spines are formed and stabilized in motor cortex neurons in freely sleeping mice but not in those that are deprived of sleep [24,25]. In the dorsal adult mouse hippocampus, 5–6-h sleep deprivation leads to a reduction in dendritic spine numbers in CA1 pyramidal neurons (e.g., [18,20]) and dentate gyrus (DG) granule cells [26] (for a review, see [27]). Together, these electrophysiological, biochemical, and neuroanatomical studies support conclusions contradicting the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis – namely, that synaptic potentiation can occur across sleep and synaptic growth and strengthening can be disrupted by sleep deprivation.

How do neuroscientists reconcile the seemingly contradictory findings linking sleep to either synaptic weakening or synaptic strengthening? Does sleep loss itself lead to synaptic strengthening, weakening, or perhaps both? In the next section, we discuss how subtle differences in methodological approaches and experimental design may drastically influence the outcome of studies linking sleep and sleep loss to synaptic structure and function.

Effects of Brain State versus Experience on Synaptic Plasticity

Experimental methodology to enforce wake varies extensively between laboratories. To keep rodents awake for a short duration of time (i.e., a few hours), many laboratories have adopted ‘gentle handling’ procedures; however, the precise method of handling involved can vary. Some laboratories appear to aim at providing the minimal sensory stimulation necessary to promote wake, but simultaneously avoiding highly novel experience or forcing high levels of loco-motor activity. Procedures for this form of gentle handling involve multiday habituation prior to sleep deprivation. Mice are housed in the same home cage and room that the deprivation will occur in and are handled daily by the experimenters [28]. The sleep deprivation method itself (also referred to as ‘mild stimulation’) includes keeping animals in their home cage and interrupting transitions to sleep by tapping on the cage wall, gently moving the cage itself, or, if necessary, disturbing the nesting material in the cage. Other interventions include using brushes or cotton tipped applicators to stroke or gently tap animals to prevent sleep transitions. This method of sleep deprivation has been validated in mice using EEG measurements and prevents all rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and approximately 95% of non-REM (NREM) sleep [8,29,30]. A major goal of this method is to keep animals awake over a period of hours without exposing them to novel or highly arousing stimuli.

For people in real-world conditions, sleep loss is often self-imposed and associated with novel experiences, social interactions, and high levels of activity. For this reason, and because novel experiences can be highly effective (from an experimental standpoint) at promoting wake in animals, some laboratories have turned towards using exposure to novelty as a strategy for sleep deprivation. This includes placing animals in novel cages, exposing animals to novel objects or bedding from other cages, providing novel treats, and/or giving first-time access to a running wheel during the light (rest) phase of the circadian cycle. As a strategy for keeping animals awake, this method is highly effective, as animals tend to be highly aroused and active in the context of novel experiences.

Critically, all of the studies described previously in the section ‘Evidence for Net Synaptic Strengthening in Wake and Net Weakening in Sleep’ have used novel sensory stimuli to enforce wake (Table 1); by contrast, much of the work highlighted in the section ‘Evidence for Net Synaptic Strengthening during Sleep and Net Weakening during Wake’ has used either gentle handling methods of sleep deprivation or techniques that minimize direct sensory input to the circuit under study (e.g., conducting sleep deprivation in darkness to study plastic changes to V1) (Table 2). We propose that these differences in methodology are a major cause for discrepancies between the two sets of studies with regard to changes in plasticity-related gene expression, physiology, and anatomy across periods of extended wake (Figure 1).

Table 1.

Overview of Methodological Aspects and Outcomes of Experimental Studies Showing Net Synaptic Weakening in Sleep

Species Sex Age Brain region studied Sleep deprivation method Result following sleep deprivation Refs
Rat Male and female 11–12 weeks Barrel and frontal cortex Novel objects Tapping on cage Increased firing rate, which reversed during recovery sleep [2]
Rat Male and female 11–12 weeks Whole neocortex Novel objects Increased phosphorylation and protein levels of AMPARs
Elevated phosphorylation levels of CaMKII, GSK3beta
[3]
Mouse Not defined 8–10 weeks Hippocampus Novel cage Tapping on cage Disturbing bedding Increased phosphorylation and protein levels of AMPAR subunits driven by homer1a [4]
Mouse Male and female ~4 weeks M1 and S1 Novel objects Running wheel Tapping on cage Increased axon-spine interface and elevated spine head volume in weaker, more plastic spines [5]
Mouse Male and female 4 weeks Hippocampus Novel objects Tapping on cage Increased axon-spine interface Increase in non-perforated spines [6]
Mouse Not defined 23–44 days Barrel cortex Novel objects Running wheel Net increase in cortical spines [7]
Mouse Male and female 2 weeks M1 Forced locomotion Increased axon-spine interface [43]

Table 2.

Overview of Methodological Aspects and Outcomes of Experimental Studies Showing Net Weakening during Wake

Species Sex Age Brain region studied Sleep deprivation method Result following sleep deprivation Refs
Mouse Male 2–6 months V1 Gentle handling (in darkness) Disruption of orientation-selective potentiation of firing responses after visual experience [8,9]
Cat Male/female 28–42 days V1 Gentle handling, novel objects, play, forced locomotion (in darkness) Reduced phosphorylation of GluR1, CaMK2a, and potentiation of visual responses [11]
Cat Male/female 28–42 days V1 As above Reduced Arc expression [15]
Mouse Male 8–12 weeks Hippocampus Gentle handling Impaired L-LTP Increased cofilin activity Spine loss in CA1 Recovery sleep reverses CA1 spine loss [18]
Mouse Male 8–12 weeks Hippocampus Gentle handling Impaired protein synthesis Attenuated mTORC1-mediated phosphorylation of 4EBP2 [19]
Mouse Male 8–12 weeks Hippocampus Gentle handling Impaired long-lasting forms of LTP that depend on cAMP Decreased pCREB levels Increased PDE4A5 protein levels [21]
Mouse Male 12–14 weeks Hippocampus Gentle handling Reduced Arc mRNA and protein expression in DG, no change in protein in CA1/3 [22]
Mouse Male/female 4 weeks M1 Gentle handling Reduced dendritic spine formation [24]
Mouse Male 8–12 weeks Hippocampus Gentle handling Spine loss in DG [26]

This discrepancy based on method raises the question: which method is optimal? One serious caveat of using novelty to enforce wake in studies relating to brain function is that novelty drives synaptic plasticity throughout the brain. Thus, in these experiments it is nearly impossible to discriminate between the impact of prolonged wake and the impact of novel experiences on synaptic plasticity. For example, exposure to novel objects alone increases the amplitude of evoked potential responses to perforant path input (an indicator of synaptic potentiation in the DG and elsewhere [13,31,32]), increases Fos protein expression in the rodent hippocampal DG (e.g., [33,34]), and elevates Fos expression in the occipital cortex, perirhinal cortex, and anterior cingulate gyrus [35]. Environmental novelty (e.g., simple placement in a novel arena) increases neuronal activity (i.e., Fos protein expression) throughout the rat hippocampus [36,37].

Another example of novel experience being an experimental confound is illustrated by a series of experiments showing dendritic spine morphological changes (i.e., increases in the size of axon–spine interfaces) following a few hours of enforced wake. Measures were made using serial EM to quantify dendritic structures in the primary somatosensory (S1) and primary motor (M1) cortex following ad lib sleep versus following wake enforced in mice through exploration of novel objects and use of a running wheel [5]. As mentioned, novel objects and running wheels produce widespread plasticity throughout the brain, but physical interaction over a prolonged interval of time with objects (and particularly with a running wheel) has long been known to selectively induce plasticity in S1 and M1 [3842]. In a more striking example, dendrites were measured in M1 of mouse pups, following ad lib ‘sleep’ (defined as immobility) versus enforced ‘wake’ (defined as locomotion) [43]. There is general agreement that motor activity can drive M1 plasticity, as evinced, for instance, by the use of locomotion (or other forms of motor practice) in rehabilitation for stroke or other brain injuries affecting motor function in humans [4448]. The effects of forced locomotion on motor cortex plasticity has been extensively studied in rodents, with clear increases in the expression of plasticity-related genes, functional plasticity, and anatomical remodeling [4952]. So, could the effects of enforced waking on M1 dendritic spines be entirely due to forced locomotion rather than brain state? This seems likely in view of other published observations. For example, extended wake across a day reversed (rather than enhanced) the effects of motor training in humans in augmenting motor cortex excitability [53]. In mice, sleep deprivation by gentle handling reverses, rather than enhances, the effects of single-trial motor learning on M1 dendritic spine formation [24]. Furthermore, as shown in Table 2, sleep deprivation using similar techniques (objects, play, locomotion) seems to have the opposite effect in cat V1 neurons during the critical period, provided visual input is prevented during this time [11,15]. These findings suggest that enforced motor activity, rather than enforced wake, gives rise to the plastic changes reported in cortical neurons in these regions.

Together, these examples underline the idea that the experimental methodology for promoting wake is varied, and these methods have widespread implications for synaptic plasticity. While the precise behavioral and physiological mechanisms (e.g., stress/arousal, hormonal changes, Hebbian mechanisms) by which exposure to novel stimuli drives plasticity may be diverse, the fact remains that increased exposure to these stimuli drives plasticity. Thus, for any studies attributing function regarding plasticity mechanisms to sleep versus wake brain states, it is vital to minimize plasticity caused by the sleep deprivation method – an effect orthogonal to (and possibly opposite to) the effects of sleep loss itself. In our view, novel environments, experiences, and objects should be avoided in experiments aiming to identify synaptic events occurring as a function of sleep versus wake.

Additional Caveats: Effects of Age, Sex, and Circadian Phase on Synaptic Structure and Plasticity

Additional methodological considerations are essential to fully clarify how sleep and wake affect synaptic function. For example, several studies (e.g., [4]) have used the collection of brain samples at circadian timepoints (e.g., a few hours into the light versus dark phase) as a proxy for sleep and wake. Depending on the species, this strategy may be essential for studies not relying on experimental sleep deprivation. For example, rats and mice (and other species such as cats) have very few periods of spontaneous sustained wake (lasting tens of minutes or longer) during the light phase in their standard home-cage environment [54]. While relative sleep amounts do vary (to an extent that varies by species) across the light–dark cycle, recent work in mice has demonstrated that the synaptic abundance of both mRNAs and proteins associated with synaptic plasticity is regulated primarily as a function of time of day (rather than as a function of sleep versus wake) [55]. Therefore, synaptic differences between samples taken at different circadian timepoints are likely to be due to clock-mediated factors, not sleep and wake amounts.

A second methodological issue is animal sex as a biological variable. Many studies have used a combination of male and female mice (sometimes in unknown ratios) in various treatment groups. This almost certainly is a confounding variable with regard to synaptic structure. In part, the estrus cycle is accompanied by about 30% change in dendritic spine numbers in structures like the hippocampus and neocortex, mediated by changes in estradiol [5659], as well as cyclic changes in sensory evoked plasticity [60]. Further, sleep architecture, sleeping EEG activity, and apparent sleep need all seem to vary as a function of the estrous cycle [61]. Besides the estrus cycle, there are likely to be additional sex differences that contribute to altered sleep and synaptic plasticity.

A final methodological issue of concern for studies of synaptic structure and function is the age of experimental animals used in these studies. Many studies aimed at assessing the role of sleep and wake in synaptic weakening versus strengthening (or elimination versus formation) have used what can be considered ‘adolescent’ animals (e.g., 3–4-week-old mice). Critically, across species (i.e., humans, monkeys, and mice) this stage of development is characterized by large-scale synaptic elimination and strengthening of inhibitory (relative to excitatory) connections [6264]. Thus, comparisons of sleep and wake effects on spine formation, elimination, or density in studies using mice in the adolescent age range with experiments using more mature animals may yield discrepant findings.

Concluding Remarks

The general discordance of findings between studies using gentle handling and studies using novelty regarding the synaptic effects of sleep loss suggests that these differences are driven in a large part by methodology. It is perhaps unsurprising that enforced wake via novel experiences and locomotion leads to evident synaptic strengthening in brain regions such as the hippocampus, and particularly in S1 and M1 where many of these results have been reported. It is also arguably unsurprising that, by contrast, enforced wake with minimal sensorimotor experience results in less, or no, apparent synaptic strengthening. Together, these discrepancies underscore the notion that the sleep deprivation method per se has a substantial impact on brain plasticity, which can obscure effects due purely to individual animals’ sleep versus wake amounts. Concordant with these discrepancies, two largely opposing theories have emerged regarding the impact of sleep and wake states on brain plasticity. One theory (the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis [65]) proposes that sleep decreases synaptic strength throughout the brain while wake increases synaptic strength. Of note, studies supporting this theory are typically ones where experimental sleep deprivation is achieved using novelty methods. Further, animal age and sex, as well as circadian time, are confounding factors in some of these studies. The second theory proposes that sleep can augment synaptic strength following learning and that extended wake blocks these effects [66]. Studies supporting this idea have generally focused on brain regions engaged by prior learning and used gentle handling techniques aimed at minimizing sensory stimulation as a method for experimental sleep deprivation. However, a full description of gentle handling effects under different conditions (i.e., following learning versus no learning) is still lacking. For example, while the effects of gentle-handling sleep deprivation on neocortical synapses are well described following learning [66], less is known about the effects under steady-state conditions. This is in contrast to the hippocampus, where the effects of gentle handling on sleep loss outside learning are well described [27,67]. To further test these hypotheses, a full characterization of different sleep deprivation methods’ effects on synapses is essential. It remains to be determined whether sleep and wake have consistent effects on synaptic strength throughout the brain. In addition, methodological caveats (e.g., age, sex differences, circadian time) must be taken into account.

The essential functions of sleep in regulating synapses can be further clarified by studying the effects of sleep and sleep deprivation across phylogeny. The studies described here were focused on the mammalian neocortex and hippocampus, where much of the seminal work on experience-dependent synaptic plasticity has been performed. Critically, however, alterations in synaptic structure have also been reported across period of sleeps or sleep deprivation in invertebrate species, including Drosophila [68,69] and Caenorhabditis elegans [70]. Addressing the question of how experience during wake interacts with sleep loss in these species (with simpler, more experimentally tractable nervous systems) may deepen our understanding of data from mammalian studies by providing an evolutionary context (see Outstanding Questions).

Outstanding Questions.

To what extent is synaptic strength affected by the presentation of novel stimuli or tasks during sleep deprivation? Resolving this question will require a direct comparison of the effects of different methods of sleep deprivation (i.e., gentle handling versus provision of novel stimuli) on synapses in the hippocampus and cortex.

Because memory consolidation is initiated by experience in wake, there must be circuit-specific changes occurring during sleep that are set up by prior learning. What is the nature of circuit-specific changes in sleep (e.g., at the molecular and network activity level) and what are the experience-dependent mechanisms required to initiate them?

How widespread and uniform are effects of sleep and wake states on synapses? Are sleep-dependent changes in synaptic strength circuit or cell-type specific? For example, are excitatory and inhibitory synapses similarly affected by sleep and wake?

How do interactions between sleep/wake states and the estrous and circadian cycles regulate brain plasticity? Do these variables act on the same molecular substrates or do they each affect synaptic structure and function through independent signaling mechanisms?

What features of sleep states critically impact plasticity? Sleep and wake affect numerous aspects of brain physiology, including (but not limited to) effects on neuronal activity patterns, neuromodulation, transcription, and translation. Recently developed genetic tools may further our understanding of specific features of sleep states that are essential for brain plasticity and cognitive benefits.

Finally, while comparisons between sleep and sleep deprivation may be enlightening, recent data suggest that different states and features of sleep (e.g., REM versus NREM sleep, specific sleep oscillations) have differential effects on synaptic plasticity. For example, two-photon imaging of dendrites in mice has shown that while spines are formed preferentially during post-learning NREM sleep [24], spines are pruned (and newly formed spines are selectively strengthened) during post-learning REM [25]. Future studies are needed to determine how specific sleep states and state-specific activity patterns [71] contribute to synaptic function. The use of opto- and pharmacogenetics to selectively manipulate, increase, decrease, or mimic sleep-associated activity patterns in specific brain circuits [10,30,7275] may provide new insight into these questions. In the context of sleep deprivation studies, these techniques may even bypass the need for externally driven sleep deprivation as an essential experimental strategy and could therefore reveal causal roles for specific features of sleep states in regulating synaptic plasticity.

Highlights.

Sleep deprivation has been reported to either strengthen or weaken neocortical and hippocampal synapses, with results varying between studies using distinct methods used to keep animals awake.

Novelty exposure as a method to promote wake in experimental animals can lead to a net increase in synaptic strength in regions including the hippocampus, M1, and S1. These effects of environmental novelty can be difficult to disentangled from effects of prolonged wake per se.

Sleep deprivation using the gentle handling method is aimed at avoiding novelty-induced synaptic plasticity. Gentle-handling sleep deprivation (in contrast to novelty-induced sleep deprivation) can cause a net decrease in synaptic strength in both the hippocampus and the neocortex.

Fluctuations in gonadal hormones during the estrus cycle (and biological differences between males and females) can cause large-scale changes in neocortical and hippocampal spine density and synaptic efficacy. These changes can mask the effects of learning or sleep–wake cycles on synapses.

Circadian rhythms may modulate synaptic structure and function independent of sleep and wake. Thus, in studies comparing timepoints across the 24-h light–dark cycle, it is nearly impossible to dissociate the impact of circadian time from other factors such as sleep and wake on synaptic plasticity.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Human Frontiers Science Program Organization (HFSP grant RGY0063/2017 to R.H. and S.J.A.), the National Institutes of Health (R01 NS104776 to S.J.A.), and the National Science Foundation (BCS-1749430 to S.J.A.).

References

  • 1.Tononi G and Cirelli C (2003) Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain Res. Bull 62, 143–150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Vyazovskiy VV et al. (2009) Cortical firing and sleep homeostasis. Neuron 63, 865–878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Vyazovskiy VV et al. (2008) Molecular and electrophysiological evidence for net synaptic potentiation in wake and depression in sleep. Nat. Neurosci 11, 200–208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Diering GH et al. (2017) Homer1a drives homeostatic scaling-down of excitatory synapses during sleep. Science 355, 511–515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Vivo L et al. (2017) Ultrastructural evidence for synaptic scaling across the wake/sleep cycle. Science 355, 507–510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Spano GM et al. (2019) Sleep deprivation by exposure to novel objects increases synapse density and axon-spine interface in the hippocampal CA1 region of adolescent mice. J. Neurosci 39, 6613–6625 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Maret S et al. (2011) Sleep and waking modulate spine turn-over in the adolescent mouse cortex. Nat. Neurosci 14, 1418–1420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Aton SJ et al. (2014) Sleep promotes cortical response potentiation following visual experience. Sleep 37, 1163–1170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Durkin J and Aton SJ (2016) Sleep-dependent potentiation in the visual system is at odds with the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis. Sleep 39, 155–159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Durkin J et al. (2017) Cortically coordinated NREM thalamocortical oscillations play an essential, instructive role in visual system plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 114, 10485–10490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Aton SJ et al. (2009) Mechanisms of sleep-dependent consolidation of cortical plasticity. Neuron 61, 454–466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Aton SJ et al. (2013) Visual experience and subsequent sleep induce sequential plastic changes in putative inhibitory and excitatory cortical neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 110, 3101–3106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cooke SF and Bear MF (2010) Visual experience induces long-term potentiation in the primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci 30, 16304–16313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Smith GB et al. (2009) Bidirectional synaptic mechanisms of ocular dominance plasticity in visual cortex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci 364, 357–367 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Seibt J et al. (2012) Protein synthesis during sleep consolidates cortical plasticity in vivo. Curr. Biol 22, 676–682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dumoulin MC et al. (2015) Extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) activity during sleep consolidates cortical plasticity in vivo. Cereb. Cortex 25, 507–515 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Chauvette S et al. (2012) Sleep oscillations in the thalamocortical system induce long-term neuronal plasticity. Neuron 75, 1105–1113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Havekes R et al. (2016) Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. eLife 5, e13424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tudor JC et al. (2016) Sleep deprivation impairs memory by attenuating mTORC1-dependent protein synthesis. Sci. Signal 9, ra41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wong LW et al. (2019) The p75 neurotrophin receptor is an essential mediator of impairments in hippocampal-dependent associative plasticity and memory induced by sleep deprivation. J. Neurosci 39, 5452–5465 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Vecsey CG et al. (2009) Sleep deprivation impairs cAMP signalling in the hippocampus. Nature 461, 1122–1125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Delorme JE et al. (2018) Sleep loss disrupts Arc expression in dentate gyrus neurons. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem 160, 73–82 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hagewoud R et al. (2010) Sleep deprivation impairs spatial working memory and reduces hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation. J. Sleep Res 19, 280–288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Yang G et al. (2014) Sleep promotes branch-specific formation of dendritic spines after learning. Science 344, 1173–1178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Li W et al. (2017) REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nat. Neurosci 20, 427–437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Raven F et al. (2018) A brief period of sleep deprivation causes spine loss in the dentate gyrus of mice. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem 160, 83–90 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Raven F et al. (2018) The role of sleep in regulating structural plasticity and synaptic strength: implications for memory and cognitive function. Sleep Med. Rev 39, 3–11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Vecsey CG et al. (2013) Daily acclimation handling does not affect hippocampal long-term potentiation or cause chronic sleep deprivation in mice. Sleep 36, 601–607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Meerlo P et al. (2001) A social conflict increases EEG slow-wave activity during subsequent sleep. Physiol. Behav 73, 331–335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ognjanovski N et al. (2018) Hippocampal network oscillations rescue memory consolidation deficits caused by sleep loss. Cereb. Cortex 28, 3711–3723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stanton PK and Sarvey JM (1985) The effect of high-frequency electrical stimulation and norepinephrine on cyclic AMP levels in normal versus norepinephrine-depleted rat hippocampal slices. Brain Res. 358, 343–348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lacaille JC and Harley CW (1985) The action of norepinephrine in the dentate gyrus: beta-mediated facilitation of evoked potentials in vitro. Brain Res. 358, 210–220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bernstein HL et al. (2019) Novelty and novel objects increase c-Fos immunoreactivity in mossy cells in the mouse dentate gyrus. Neural Plast. 2019, 1815371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kitchigina V et al. (1997) Novelty-elicited, noradrenaline-dependent enhancement of excitability in the dentate gyrus. Eur. J. Neurosci 9, 41–47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Zhu XO et al. (1995) Effects of the novelty or familiarity of visual stimuli on the expression of the immediate early gene c-fos in rat brain. Neuroscience 69, 821–829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.VanElzakker M et al. (2008) Environmental novelty is associated with a selective increase in Fos expression in the output elements of the hippocampal formation and the perirhinal cortex. Learn. Mem 15, 899–908 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zhu XO et al. (1997) Differential activation of the rat hippocampus and perirhinal cortex by novel visual stimuli and a novel environment. Neurosci. Lett 229, 141–143 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sumiyoshi A et al. (2014) Regional gray matter volume increases following 7 days of voluntary wheel running exercise: a longitudinal VBM study in rats. Neuroimage 98, 82–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Valles A et al. (2011) Genomewide analysis of rat barrel cortex reveals time- and layer-specific mRNA expression changes related to experience-dependent plasticity. J. Neurosci 31, 6140–6158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Staiger JF et al. (2000) Exploration of a novel environment leads to the expression of inducible transcription factors in barrel-related columns. Neuroscience 99, 7–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Staiger JF et al. (2002) Excitatory and inhibitory neurons express c-Fos in barrel-related columns after exploration of a novel environment. Neuroscience 109, 687–699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stell M and Riesen A (1987) Effects of early environments on monkey cortex neuroanatomical changes following somatomotor experience: effects on layer III pyramidal cells in monkey cortex. Behav. Neurosci 101, 341–346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.de Vivo L et al. (2019) Evidence for sleep-dependent synaptic renormalization in mouse pups. Sleep 42, zsz184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.de Bode S et al. (2007) Locomotor training remodels fMRI sensorimotor cortical activations in children after cerebral hemispherectomy. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 21, 497–508 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goldstein LB (2006) Neurotransmitters and motor activity: effects on functional recovery after brain injury. NeuroRx 3, 451–457 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kantak S et al. (2018) Behavioral and neurophysiological mechanisms underlying motor skill learning in patients with post-stroke hemiparesis. Clin. Neurophysiol 129, 1–12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Coxon JP et al. (2014) Primary motor cortex disinhibition during motor skill learning. J. Neurophysiol 112, 156–164 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Murdoch K et al. (2016) The effect of aerobic exercise on neuroplasticity within the motor cortex following stroke. PLoS One 11, e0152377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Takamatsu Y et al. (2016) Running exercise enhances motor functional recovery with inhibition of dendritic regression in the motor cortex after collagenase-induced intracerebral hemor-rhage in rats. Behav. Brain Res 300, 56–64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ferreira AF et al. (2010) Moderate exercise changes synaptic and cytoskeletal proteins in motor regions of the rat brain. Brain Res. 1361, 31–42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Real CC et al. (2010) Exercise-induced plasticity of AMPA-type glutamate receptor subunits in the rat brain. Brain Res. 1363, 63–71 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Adkins DL et al. (2006) Motor training induces experience-specific patterns of plasticity across motor cortex and spinal cord. J. Appl. Physiol (1985) 101, 1776–1782 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.de Beukelaar TT et al. (2016) A day awake attenuates motor learning-induced increases in corticomotor excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci 10, 138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Wimmer ME et al. (2013) Aging in mice reduces the ability to sustain sleep/wake states. PLoS One 8, e81880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Noya SB et al. (2019) The forebrain synaptic transcriptome is organized by clocks but its proteome is driven by sleep. Science 366, eaav2642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Woolley CS et al. (1990) Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci 10, 4035–4039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Woolley CS and McEwen BS (1992) Estradiol mediates fluctuation in hippocampal synapse density during the estrous cycle in the adult rat. J. Neurosci 12, 2549–2554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kato A et al. (2013) Female hippocampal estrogens have a significant correlation with cyclic fluctuation of hippocampal spines. Front. Neural Circuits 7, 149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Chen JR et al. (2009) Gonadal hormones modulate the dendritic spine densities of primary cortical pyramidal neurons in adult female rat. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2719–2727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Alexander BH et al. (2018) Stable density and dynamics of dendritic spines of cortical neurons across the estrous cycle while expressing differential levels of sensory-evoked plasticity. Front. Mol. Neurosci 11, 83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Swift KM et al. (2019) Sex differences within sleep in gonadally-intact rats. Sleep. Published online November 30, 2019. 10.1093/sleep/zsz289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Huttenlocher PR and Dabholkar AS (1997) Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol 387, 167–178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Bourgeois JP et al. (1994) Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 4, 78–96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.De Felipe J et al. (1997) Inhibitory synaptogenesis in mouse somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 7, 619–634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tononi G and Cirelli C (2014) Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Puentes-Mestril C and Aton SJ (2017) Linking network activity to synaptic plasticity during sleep: hypotheses and recent data. Front. Neural Circuits 11, 61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Havekes R et al. (2015) Animal studies on the role of sleep in memory: from behavioral performance to molecular mechanisms. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci 25, 183–206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bushey D et al. (2011) Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: structural evidence in Drosophila. Science 332, 1576–1581 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Donlea JM et al. (2009) Use-dependent plasticity in clock neurons regulates sleep need in Drosophila. Science 324, 105–108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dabbish NS and Raizen DM (2011) GABAergic synaptic plasticity during a developmentally regulated sleep-like state in C. elegans. J. Neurosci 31, 15932–15943 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Puentes-Mestril C et al. (2019) How rhythms of the sleeping brain tune memory and synaptic plasticity. Sleep 42, zsz095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Ognjanovski N et al. (2017) Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons coordinate hippocampal network dynamics required for memory consolidation. Nat. Commun 8, 15039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Boyce R et al. (2016) Causal evidence for the role of REM sleep theta rhythm in contextual memory consolidation. Science 352, 812–816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Xia F et al. (2017) Parvalbumin-positive interneurons mediate neocortical–hippocampal interactions that are necessary for memory consolidation. eLife 6, e27868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Rolls A et al. (2011) Optogenetic disruption of sleep continuity impairs memory consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 108, 13305–13310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES