Skip to main content
BMC Geriatrics logoLink to BMC Geriatrics
. 2020 Sep 21;20:360. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01772-9

Association between rural-to-urban migration and the cognitive aging trajectories of older Chinese adults: results from a prospective cohort analysis

Jinzhao Xie 1,#, Jing Liao 1,2,#, Jing Zhang 1, Jing Gu 1,2,
PMCID: PMC7507287  PMID: 32957920

Abstract

Background

Increasingly, older Chinese adults from rural areas are moving to urban areas to live with their children who have already migrated to these areas. However, few studies have examined this pattern of migration and its effects on cognitive function. We aimed to investigate the association between domestic rural-to-urban migration and the trajectories of cognitive function in older Chinese adults, as well as the factors contributing to these association.

Methods

Data for this study were drawn from three waves of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study. Migrants were defined as participants who had rural hukou status (under China’s household registration system) but resided in an urban area. Cognitive functions were measured using an adapted Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination. We used multilevel linear regression models to examine the association between internal migration and cognitive function trajectories.

Results

The study included 3876 Chinese adults aged ≥60 years at baseline. Compared with their rural non-migrant counterparts, migrants (n = 850) had higher levels of education and reported more interactions with family. Additionally, female migrants were more likely to participate in leisure activities. All cognitive function scores declined over time, but no significant differences were observed in rates of cognitive decline between migrants and non-migrants, regardless of sex. Female migrants exhibited significantly better performance in terms of total cognition (β = 0.77, P < .001) and mental status (β = 0.68, P < .001) than female non-migrants, whereas no inter-group difference was observed regarding memory (β = 0.09, P > .05). Among the male subjects, no significant differences in cognitive function levels were observed between migrants and non-migrants. A series of adjusted models revealed that psychosocial factors such as residing with children, caring for grandchildren, depression and participation in leisure activities partly explained the association between migration and cognition in women.

Conclusions

Rural-to-urban migration was positively associated with cognitive functions only in women. However, this pattern did not affect the rate of cognitive decline in either sex. Our findings provide directions for tailored interventions improving cognitive functions of older adults and rural non-migrating older adults, especially female non-migrants.

Keywords: Internal migration, Older adults, Cognitive aging, Cohort analysis

Background

Estimates indicate that more than 10.4 million individuals with cognitive impairment resided in China in 2016 [1]. Cognitive impairment is associated with a poor quality of life among older adults and imposes heavy burdens on families and societies [2]. In 2018, the number of Chinese adults aged ≥60 years was 249 million, and this group accounted for 17.9% of the total population [3]. Potentially, Chinese health and social care systems may be unprepared to meet the increasing needs of older patients with cognitive impairment. Moreover, the unstable ‘4–2-1’ family structure (four grandparents, two parents, and one child) and the substantial number of working-age adults who migrate from rural to urban areas for employment have undermined traditional practices of family care [4]. Increasingly, older Chinese adults are moving to live near their migrant children, and many provide care for their grandchildren [5]. This unique trend in migration is associated with significant changes in the living environments and social networks of older Chinese adults, which may have important but not fully understood effects on their health and quality of life [6, 7].

The extent to which migration may affect cognitive aging remains unclear [8]. Various aspects may contribute to the effects of migration on the cognition of older adults, including socioeconomic status, physical and mental health and behavioural and environmental factors [9]. Older adults who migrate to cities alongside their adult children may benefit from increased family interactions, support from family members and the opportunity to provide intergenerational care to their grandchildren [9]. Moreover, urban environments provide more opportunities for various leisure activities, which may also contribute to the maintenance of cognitive function [10]. However, some empirical studies have also found that rural-to-urban elderly migrants adapted poorly to aspects of their daily lives [7] and were likely to lose their original social networks, which increased their risk of depression [11]. The association between migration and cognitive function change may show sex-related differences, as women tend to be more involved in family and social activities than men [12, 13]. Consequently, women may be more likely to experience the cognitive benefits associated with these forms of engagement [1416].

Most previous studies have explored the relationship between international migration and changes in cognitive function; very few have focused on internal migration [8]. In the US, a retrospective cohort study of 1789 Hispanic-American participants indicated no association between migration and cognitive function [17]. Another study of 1085 participants in Europe observed poorer cognitive function in non-European migrants than in local citizens [18]. In China, a 12-year longitudinal study indicated that rural-to-urban and rural residents exhibited more rapid declines in cognitive function than urban residents [19]. In that study, rural-to-urban migrants were compared with native urban citizens rather than with their rural non-migrant counterparts, although the latter group might be more comparable. Moreover, the effects of psychosocial factors on the association between migration and cognitive function were not explored.

Despite the increasing internal rural-to-urban migration of older adults in China, evidence regarding the potential association between migration and cognition remains lacking. Additionally, longitudinal studies are needed to expand the body of evidence regarding the long-term effects of migration on cognitive aging trajectories. This study aimed to investigate the sex-specific association between internal rural-to-urban migration and cognitive aging trajectories and to explore the potential contributing psychosocial factors in a nationally representative longitudinal sample of community-dwelling older Chinese adults, which refer to those aged ≥60 years. We hypothesised that rural-to-urban elderly migrants would exhibit better cognitive function and a slower rate of cognitive decline than their non-migrant rural counterparts. Furthermore, we hypothesised that these associations would be more evident in women than in men and that this difference would be partly attributable to increased family interactions and social engagements.

Methods

Study sample

This study used data from three waves of the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS 2011–2015), whose design was based on the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) in the US. The CHARLS comprised a nationally representative sample of adults in China aged ≥45 years. The CHARLS sample was obtained using four-stage stratified sampling with the probability-proportional-to-size (PPS) technique [20]. The baseline survey covered 28 provinces, 150 counties/districts and 17,708 respondents from 10,257 households and was conducted between June 2011 and March 2012. Two follow-up interviews were conducted in 2013 and 2015. We restricted our sample to 3876 respondents who met the following criteria: (1) aged ≥60 years at baseline, (2) completion of all three study waves (2011–2015), (3) no history of diseases with potentially strong effects on cognitive function (e.g., cancer, stroke, memory-related disease) at baseline and (4) rural hukou status at baseline.

Measures

Migrants and non-migrants

We divided our sample into two groups, migrants and non-migrants, based on the hukou system, which was used to classify rural and urban residents in previous studies [21, 22]. Older adults (≥60 years) with rural hukou status who resided in urban areas during all three study waves (n = 850) were defined as rural-to-urban elderly migrants. Non-migrants were defined as respondents with rural hukou status who resided in rural areas during the three study waves (n = 3026).

Cognitive function

Cognitive function was measured using an adapted Chinese version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE), which included similar concepts to those used to measure cognitive function in the US Health and Retirement Study (HRS) [23]. According to previous publications [2426], we divided cognitive function into two dimensions: episodic memory and mental status. We generated an episodic memory score (range: 0–10) as the average of the immediate and delayed recall scores. The mental status score (range: 0–11) was based on the following three items: figure drawing, serial subtraction of 7 from 100 (up to five times) and the ability to identify the date (month, day, year), day of the week and season of the year. The total cognition score, which incorporated both dimensions, ranged from 0 to 21. A higher score indicated better cognitive function.

Psychosocial factors

Data on the psychosocial factors were obtained from the baseline survey. The psychosocial factors comprised the family connections, social attachment and depression. Family connections was determined by whether the respondent was coupled, lived with his/her adult children and had provided any care to his/her grandchildren. According to the CHARLS code book, if the respondent reported that he/she co-resided with any adult child, regardless of whether he/she took care of grandchildren, the respondent was classified as ‘lives with children’. Caring for grandchildren was defined as the provision of care to any grandchildren younger than 16 years during the past year, regardless whether the respondent lived with grandchildren.

The measure of social attachment was adapted from the definition provided in the English Longitudinal Study of Aging (ELSA). Specifically, that study divided social attachment into four domains: civic participation, leisure activities, cultural engagement and social networks. To accommodate the Chinese social background of our subjects, we excluded cultural engagement, which was assessed by the frequency with which the participants reported visiting art galleries, museums or exhibitions and attending theatres, concerts, operas and cinemas, from our analysis. Civic participation was defined as the participation in activities associated with a community-related organisation or in volunteer or charity activities. Subjects who reported that they had participated in one of the above-mentioned activities within 1 month before the interview were classified as having civic participation. Participation in leisure activities was defined as playing mah-jong, cards or chess; visiting a community; attending an athletic, social or other type of club; or attending an educational or training course within 1 month before the interview. The domain of social network was restricted to friendships and was defined as interactions with friends within 1 month before the interview. Other core social networks experienced by elders were measured under the domain of family connections.

Depression was measured using the 10-item Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D-10). The total scores ranged from 0 to 30, and a higher score indicated more severe depression.

Covariates

The subjects’ demographic characteristics, socioeconomic and health statuses and health behaviours were considered as covariates in our study. In accordance with prior CHARLS studies and the distribution of educational attainment among older Chinese adults, the subjects were classified into four educational levels: illiterate; some primary school (not completed); finished primary school; and higher than primary school [27, 28]. Household income was defined as the sum of all annual income at the household level and was stratified into three levels (low, medium and high) according to the lower and upper quartiles. Retirement status was dichotomised as retired or not retired. Retirement was defined as a history of employment (including agricultural and non-agricultural work) and a current status of no longer working, while non-retirement was defined as participation in current employment (agricultural and non-agricultural) or no history of such work throughout one’s lifetime.

Health status was assessed according to the number of activities of daily living (ADL) in which the subject experienced disability and chronic disease status. ADLs were determined as the number of activities during which the subject experienced difficulties (range: 0–6). Chronic disease status was determined from self-reported diagnoses. According to previous studies, smoking, alcohol consumption and afternoon napping may affect cognitive function in the elderly [9, 23, 29]. Therefore, we considered these three items as health behaviours. The subjects were categorised as non-smokers, light/moderate smokers (< 20 cigarettes per day currently or a history of smoking) or heavy smokers (≥20 cigarettes per day currently). They were further categorised into three alcohol consumption categories: non-drinkers, ≤1 drink per month or > 1 drink per month. The subjects were further categorised as non-nappers, short nappers (< 30 min), moderate nappers (30–90 min) or extended nappers (> 90 min) [29].

Statistical methods

The characteristics of the sample were described according to sex and migrant status. Continuous variables are reported as means and standard deviations, while categorical variables are reported as percentages. The t-test was used to compare normally distributed continuous variables. The chi-square test was used to compare the nominal variables, namely retirement, family connections, social attachment and chronic disease, while the rank-sum test was used to compare the ordinal variables of age group, education level, household annual income, smoking, alcohol consumption and afternoon napping.

We examined differences in the subjects’ cognitive function trajectories using multilevel linear regression analyses in which the follow-up wave was set as the first level (low level) and coded as 0, 1 or 2 to represent the longitudinal term, and individuals were set as the second level (high level). We assumed that individuals would have different baseline levels of cognitive function and different rates of cognitive decline. Therefore, we estimated the random coefficient models. First, we detected the difference in cognitive trajectories between migrants and non-migrants and evaluated the presence of a sex-specific difference by establishing a model that included the interacting terms of time, migration status and sex. Because we identified a significant interaction between sex and the migration status with respect to the total cognition and mental status scores (see Additional file 1: Table S1), we stratified all of the analyses by sex.

Given the observed difference in cognitive function between female migrants and non-migrants, we constructed a series of adjustment models to explore the possible underlying factors. In these adjustment models, the psychosocial factors and covariates were entered at level 2 (interpersonal level). Model 1 was adjusted for the age group and time of follow-up. Model 2 comprised model 1 plus the socioeconomic status, while models 3 and 4 added psychosocial factors. Finally, model 5 included the health status and health behaviours. We used multiple imputation by chained equations (MICE) to impute any missing values. This process was performed using R version 3.4.5 with the ‘mice’ package. We also conducted a sensitivity analysis by running models in which the missing values had not been imputed, and achieved similar results. In all of the analyses, statistical significance was based on a two-tailed P value < 0.05. All of the analyses were performed using R software version 3.4.5.

Results

Baseline characteristics of the overall sample and subgroups stratified by migration status and sex

The total sample comprised 3876 participants (52.2% female, 21.9% migrants), with an average baseline age of 67.5 ± 6.5 years. Of the participants, 43.7% were illiterate, 30.3% had retired, 78.1% lived in a coupled household, 48.7% lived with children and 39.9% had cared for grandchildren in the past year. Few reported civic participation (0.9%), 14.7% had participated in leisure activities during the past month and 32.7% reported interaction with friends during the previous month. Nearly three quarters of the participants had chronic diseases, and the average depression score was 9.1 ± 6.5. More than half of the participants were non-smokers and non-drinkers, and nearly half did not take naps. The average total cognition score was 9.2 ± 3.8.

Among the female subjects, migrants accounted for 23.6% (n = 478) of the participants. Female migrants were more likely to live with children (58.6% vs 47.5%, P < 0.001), care for grandchildren (48.1% vs 34.5%, P < 0.001) and participate in leisure activities (16.3% vs 8.6%, P < 0.001) than were female non-migrants. Among the male subjects, migrants accounted for 20.1% (n = 372) of the participants and were more likely than male non-migrants to live with children (57.8% vs 44.5%, P < 0.001) and care for grandchildren (52.2% vs 39.7%, P < 0.001). However, there was no significant difference in the frequency of participation in leisure activities between male migrants and non-migrants (20.4% vs 19.1%, P = 0.598).

Although female migrants had better baseline total cognition (8.8 ± 3.8 vs 8.1 ± 3.8, P = 0.001) and mental status scores (5.8 ± 3.0 vs 5.2 ± 3.0, P < 0.001) than female non-migrants, similar differences were not observed among the male participants. Moreover, no significant inter-group differences in episodic memory were observed. The detailed baseline characteristics are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Baseline Characteristics of the Overall Sample and According to Migration Status by Sex

Variables Total (n = 3876) Female (n = 2024) P Male (n = 1852) P
Non-migrant (n = 1546) Migrant (n = 478) Non-migrant (n = 1480) Migrant (n = 372)
Demographic
 Age, mean ± SD 67.5 ± 6.5 67.6 ± 6.6 68.2 ± 7.2 0.061 67.3 ± 6.2 67.1 ± 6.0 0.556
 Age group, n (%) 0.193 0.693
   < 65 1622 (41.8) 635 (41.1) 191 (40.0) 629 (42.5) 167 (44.9)
  65 ~ 70 1190 (30.7) 492 (31.8) 138 (28.9) 450 (30.4) 110 (29.6)
   > 70 1064 (27.5) 419 (27.1) 149 (31.2) 401 (27.1) 95 (25.5)
 Education level, n (%) 0.032 0.001
  Illiterate 1694 (43.7) 986 (63.8) 270 (56.5) 376 (25.4) 62 (16.7)
  Some primary school 860 (22.2) 280 (18.1) 100 (20.9) 362 (24.5) 118 (31.7)
  Finished primary school 943 (24.3) 222 (14.4) 83 (17.4) 504 (34.1) 134 (36.0)
  Higher than primary school 379 (9.8) 58 (3.8) 25 (5.2) 238 (16.1) 58 (15.6)
 Retired, n (%) 1173 (30.3) 544 (35.2) 213 (44.6) <.001 288 (19.5) 128 (34.4) <.001
 Household annual income, n (%) <.001 <.001
  Low 1098 (28.3) 473 (30.6) 125 (26.2) 429 (29.0) 71 (19.1)
  Medium 2074 (53.5) 821 (53.1) 241 (50.4) 810 (54.7) 202 (54.3)
  High 704 (18.2) 252 (16.3) 112 (23.4) 241 (16.3) 99 (26.6)
Family connections, n (%)
 Coupled household 3026 (78.1) 1128 (73.0) 327 (68.4) 0.061 1238 (83.6) 333 (89.5) 0.006
 Living with children 1889 (48.7) 735 (47.5) 280 (58.6) <.001 659 (44.5) 215 (57.8) <.001
 Caring for grandchildren 1545 (39.9) 534 (34.5) 230 (48.1) <.001 587 (39.7) 194 (52.2) <.001
Social attachment, n (%)
 Civic participation 33 (0.9) 11 (0.7) 4 (0.8) 0.999 14 (0.9) 4 (1.1) 0.999
 Leisure activities 569 (14.7) 133 (8.6) 78 (16.3) <.001 282 (19.1) 76 (20.4) 0.598
 Friendships 1269 (32.7) 554 (35.8) 161 (33.7) 0.420 452 (30.5) 102 (27.4) 0.266
Depression, (mean ± SD, 0–30) 9.1 ± 6.5 10.3 ± 6.8 9.0 ± 6.7 <.001 8.2 ± 6.0 7.5 ± 5.7 0.039
Health
 ADLs, (mean ± SD, 0–6) 0.5 ± 1.1 0.6 ± 1.3 0.5 ± 1.1 0.035 0.4 ± 1.0 0.3 ± 0.7 0.069
 Chronic diseases 2757 (71.1) 1144 (74.0) 348 (72.8) 0.646 1018 (68.8) 247 (66.4) 0.411
Health behaviours
 Smoking, n (%) 0.115 0.477
  Non-smokers 2582 (66.6) 1457 (94.2) 438 (91.6) 553 (37.4) 134 (36.0)
  Light/moderate smokers 601 (15.5) 64 (4.1) 30 (6.3) 411 (27.8) 96 (25.8)
  Heavy smokers 693 (17.9) 25 (1.6) 10 (2.1) 516 (34.9) 142 (38.2)
 Alcohol consumption, n (%) 0.954 0.183
  Non-drinker 2665 (68.8) 1349 (87.3) 418 (87.4) 713 (48.2) 185 (49.7)
   ≤ 1 drink per month 239 (6.2) 65 (4.2) 21 (4.4) 115 (7.8) 38 (10.2)
   > 1 drink per month 972 (25.1) 132 (8.5) 39 (8.2) 652 (44.1) 149 (40.1)
 Afternoon napping, n (%) 0.066 0.507
  Non-napper 1878 (48.5) 876 (56.7) 240 (50.2) 602 (40.7) 160 (43.0)
  Short napper 327 (8.4) 114 (7.4) 35 (7.3) 149 (10.1) 29 (7.8)
  Moderate napper 1111 (28.7) 386 (25.0) 145 (30.3) 467 (31.6) 113 (30.4)
  Extended napper 560 (14.4) 170 (11.0) 58 (12.1) 262 (17.7) 70 (18.8)
Cognition, mean ± SD
 Total cognition (range 0–21) 9.2 ± 3.8 8.1 ± 3.8 8.8 ± 3.8 0.001 10.3 ± 3.7 10.5 ± 3.4 0.274
 Mental status (range 0–11) 6.2 ± 3.1 5.2 ± 3.0 5.8 ± 3.0 <.001 7.2 ± 2.9 7.5 ± 2.7 0.086
 Episodic memory (range 0–10) 3.0 ± 1.7 2.9 ± 1.7 3.0 ± 1.7 0.651 3.1 ± 1.6 3.0 ± 1.6 0.556

Abbreviations: SD standard deviation, SES socioeconomic status, ADL activity of daily living disability

Sex-specific differences in cognitive trajectories between migrants and non-migrants

Table 2 presents the results of the multilevel model analyses. After the analyses were adjusted for age groups, the time terms in all models were significantly negative, indicating that all cognitive functions declined with time. Among women, migrants achieved better scores for total cognition (β = 0.77, P < 0.001) and mental status domains (β = 0.68, P < 0.001) except episodic memory (β = 0.09, P > 0.05) when compared with non-migrants. Among men, however, there were no significant differences in any of the cognitive function levels between migrants and non-migrants. The interacting terms of migration and time were not significant in all models, indicating that the differences in the rates of cognitive function decline between migrants and non-migrants were not significant. The sex-specific differences in total cognitive function according to migrant status are illustrated in Fig. 1.

Table 2.

Sex-specific Differences in Cognitive Trajectories Between Migrants and Non-migrants

Mixed Effect β (SE)
Total cognition Mental status Episode Memory
Female
 Fixed effect
  Constant 9.40 (0.12)*** 6.09 (0.10)*** 3.34 (0.05)***
  Time −0.99 (0.05)*** − 0.59 (0.05)*** − 0.40 (0.03)***
  Migrant 0.77 (0.18)*** 0.68 (0.15)*** 0.09 (0.08)
  Age65 ~ 70 (Ref. Age60~) −0.83 (0.16)*** − 0.47 (0.12)*** − 0.39 (0.06)***
  Age > 70 (Ref. Age60~) −2.68 (0.17)*** −1.69 (0.13)*** −1.10 (0.07)***
  Migrant x Time −0.01 (0.11) − 0.03 (0.09) 0.03 (0.05)
 Random effect
  ID 2.27 1.72 0.97
  Time 0.26 0.12 0.35
Male
 Fixed effect
  Constant 11.39 (0.12)*** 7.88 (0.09)*** 3.54 (0.05)***
  Time −0.85 (0.05)*** −0.49 (0.04)*** −0.36 (0.03)***
  Migrant 0.16 (0.19) 0.25 (0.15) −0.09 (0.09)
  Age65 ~ 70 (Ref. Age60~) −0.68 (0.16)*** −0.36 (0.12)** − 0.35 (0.07)***
  Age > 70 (Ref. Age60~) −2.52 (0.17)*** −1.61 (0.13)*** −1.00 (0.07)***
  Migrant x Time 0.16 (0.12) 0.06 (0.10) 0.11 (0.06)
 Random effect
  ID 2.03 1.53 0.83
  Time 0.45 0.31 0.27

* P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001;

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The sex-specific differences in total cognitive function trajectories function by migrant status. a Relationship between time (horizontal axis) and total cognitive function (range:0–21; vertical axis) of female according to migrant status. The dotted line and solid line represent migrant and stayer respectively, the dots represent the mean total cognitive function of each fellow-up point. b Association between time and total cognitive function of male according to migrant status. Over all the total cognitive function of male was higher than that of female. The cognitive performance of migrants was better than stayers, which was more pronounced in female

Sex-specific contributions of psychosocial factors to the association between migration and cognitive function

A series of adjustment models revealed the contributions of psychosocial factors to differences in the cognitive function levels between migrants and non-migrants. Regarding total cognition in the female participants, after controlling for the factors included in models 1–5 in a stepwise manner, the estimated effect of migration remained positive (model 1: β = 0.78, P < 0.001; model 2: β = 0.47, P < 0.001; model 3: β = 0.48, P < 0.001; model 4: β = 0.35, P < 0.01; model 5: β = 0.32, P < 0.05). The results of model 4 revealed that living with children (β = − 0.28, P < 0.05) and depression (β = − 0.06, P < 0.001) were associated with a lower total cognition level, while participation in leisure activities (β = 0.87, P < 0.001) was positively associated with the cognitive function level. In model 4, the estimated effect of migration remained positive, although the decrease in this value from 0.47 in model 2 to 0.35 in model 4 indicated that psychosocial factors explained 25.5% of the difference in the total cognitive function level between migrants and non-migrants. After all of the covariates were adjusted in model 5, the estimated effects of living with children, participation in leisure activities and depression remained significant (Table 3). Among the male participants, migration status remained a non-significant factor after controlling for psychosocial factors included in models 2–5. Greater participation in leisure activities and more interactions with friends were associated with a higher total cognitive level (see Additional file 1: Table S3).

Table 3.

Association Between the Total Cognitive Trajectory and Migration Status in Female Subjects

Mixed Effect β (SE)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Fixed effect
 constant 9.39 (0.12)*** 8.08 (0.16)*** 8.00 (0.20)*** 8.55 (0.23)*** 8.49 (0.25)***
 Time −0.99 (0.05)*** −0.99 (0.05)*** − 0.99 (0.05)*** −0.99 (0.05)*** − 0.99 (0.05)***
 Migrant (Ref. Non-migrants) 0.78 (0.16)*** 0.47 (0.14)*** 0.48 (0.14)*** 0.35 (0.14)** 0.32 (0.14)*
 Age65 ~ 70 (Ref. Age60~) −0.83 (0.16)*** −0.85 (0.14)*** − 0.85 (0.14)*** −0.79 (0.14)*** − 0.78 (0.14)***
 Age > 70 (Ref. Age60~) −2.68 (0.17)*** −1.72 (0.16)*** −1.66 (0.16)*** −1.65 (0.16)*** − 1.61 (0.16)***
SES
 Education level (Ref. illiterate)
  Some primary school 2.09 (0.15)*** 2.09 (0.15)*** 2.06 (0.15)*** 2.03 (0.15)***
  Finished primary school 3.88 (0.17)*** 3.88 (0.17)*** 3.73 (0.17)*** 3.69 (0.17)***
  Higher than primary school 5.17 (0.30)*** 5.14 (0.30)*** 4.92 (0.29)*** 4.88 (0.29)***
 Retired (Ref. no) −0.09 (0.13) −0.05 (0.13) − 0.06 (0.12) −0.03 (0.12)
 Household annual income (Ref. low)
  Medium −0.15 (0.13) −0.10 (0.14) − 0.21 (0.14) −0.21 (0.14)
  High 0.26 (0.17) 0.41 (0.19)* 0.15 (0.19) 0.14 (0.19)
Family connections
 Coupled household 0.15 (0.14) 0.16 (0.14) 0.15 (0.14)
 Living with children −0.36 (0.13)** −0.28 (0.12)* −0.29 (0.12)*
 Caring for grandchildren 0.16 (0.12) 0.19 (0.12) 0.21 (0.12)
Social attachment
 Civic participation (Ref.no) 0.59 (0.65) 0.52 (0.65)
 Leisure activities (Ref.no) 0.87 (0.19)*** 0.83 (0.19)***
 Friendships (Ref.no) 0.25 (0.12)* 0.21 (0.12)
Depression −0.06 (0.01)*** −0.06 (0.01)***
Health
 ADLs −0.12 (0.05)*
 Chronic diseases (Ref. Non-disease) −0.05 (0.13)
Health behaviours
 Smoking (Ref. Non-smoker)
  Light/moderate smokers 0.30 (0.27)
  Heavy smokers 0.44 (0.43)
 Alcohol consumption (Ref. Non-drinker)
   ≤ 1 drink per month −0.01 (0.28)
   > 1 drink per month −0.05 (0.21)
 Afternoon napping (Ref. Non-napper)
  Short napper 0.65 (0.22)**
  Moderate napper 0.31 (0.13)*
  Extended napper −0.19 (0.18)
 Random effect
  ID 2.27 1.76 1.76 1.65 1.64
  Time 0.25 0.17 0.16 0.20 0.20
  AIC 32,734 32,075 32,069 31,986 31,980

Abbreviations: AIC Akaike Information Criterion, SES socioeconomic status, ADL activity of daily living disability

Model 1: Adjusted for age group and time of follow-up; Model 2: Model 1 + socioeconomic status; Model 3: Model 2 + family connections; Model 4: Model3 + social attachment + depression; Model 5: Model 4 + health and health behaviours

* P < 0.05; ** P < 0.01; *** P < 0.001;

Regarding mental status, female migrants had a better mental status than female non-migrants (model 1: β = 0.66 P < 0.001; model 2: β = 0.41, P < 0.001; model 3: β = 0.41, P < 0.001; model 4: β = 0.32, P < 0.01; model 5: β = 0.29, P < 0.05; see Additional file 1: Table S2). Participation in leisure activities (model 4: β = 0.55, P < 0.001) remained significantly associated with a better mental status. Caring for grandchildren (model 4: β = 0.21, P < 0.05) was also associated with a better mental status in the female participants, whereas living with children (model 4: β = − 0.27, P < 0.01) and depression (model 4: β = − 0.04, P < 0.001) were associated with worse mental health. Among the male participants, migration status remained a non-significant factor after controlling for the factors included in models 2–5 in a stepwise manner. Greater participation in leisure activities (model 4: β = 0.58, P < 0.001) was associated with a higher cognitive level, whereas living with children (model 4: β = − 0.25, P < 0.05) was associated with a worse mental status (see Additional file 1: Table S4).

Discussion

We observed a better cognition level among rural-to-urban elderly female (but not male) migrants, compared with their non-migrant counterparts. These differences were evident with respect to the overall level of cognitive function but not the rate of cognitive decline, and were partly explained by variations in the socio-economic status, behavioural and psychosocial factors and other health-related factors. This work indicates the potential directions of specific interventions for internal elderly migrants and identifies the population that requires most attention.

In our study, we did not observe a significant difference in the rate of cognitive decline between migrants and non-migrants, which might be explained by the following reasons. First, as both rural-to-urban migrants and rural non-migrants had rural life experiences, the differences in the rates of cognitive decline between these populations may be less significant than those observed between rural and urban residents [19]. Second, according to Cattell’s categorisation of cognitive abilities and previous studies, fluid abilities such as memory tend to decline linearly from early adulthood and are more difficult to improve or otherwise change in older adults [3032]. This was consistent with our observations.

We observed a higher cognitive function level in female migrants than in female non-migrants. This might be explained partially by the finding that female migrants were more likely to take care of grandchildren and participate in leisure activities than were their non-migrant counterparts. Moreover, female migrants were less likely to present with depression. According to previous studies, caring for grandchildren and leisure activities were positively associated with cognitive function [10, 14, 15]. However, no significant difference in cognitive function was observed between male migrants and non-migrants, indicating that men and women may not achieve the same benefits from caring for grandchildren. Moreover, our findings show that male migrants were not more likely than non-migrants to participate in leisure activities. Studies of additional reasons for sex-related difference observed in our findings are warranted.

Although both female and male migrants were more likely to provide intergenerational care than non-migrants, our findings suggest that this activity only provided cognitive benefits in terms of the mental status among female migrants. Previous studies have argued that voluntarily providing care to grandchildren might have a positive effect on an elderly caregiver’s cognitive function by enhancing the caregiver’s senses of self-esteem and self-worth and providing a new purpose in later life [3335]. However, this effect may be somewhat sex-specific and could therefore provide more benefits to female caregivers [14, 15]. Our study yielded similar results. We think that this outcome may be partly attributable to traditional social norms in China, where women are typically expected to be responsible for domestic affairs and family life and to play a nurturing role and serve as kin-keepers, whereas men are expected to fulfil the role of breadwinners. Accordingly, grandfathers who migrate to urban areas specifically to provide care for grandchildren would deviate from this traditional social norm. Therefore, female rural-to-urban elderly migrants would more easily benefit from caring for grandchildren.

Our findings also suggested other explanations regarding the sex-specific difference in cognitive function between migrants and non-migrants. We observed that female elderly migrants performed better in terms of social attachment than female non-migrants, particularly in leisure activities. This finding was inconsistent with those of previous studies, which reported that migrants experienced poor social adaptation and integration [7, 21, 36]. This inconsistency may be related to differences in the selection of the control group, which comprised non-migrant rural elderly people in our study but local citizens in other studies. Compared with rural areas, urban areas feature a wealth of community activities and facilities for leisure activities [37, 38], which provide migrants with more opportunities for participation. Consequently, rural-to-urban migrants have more opportunities for leisure activities. However, increased participation in leisure activities was only observed in female migrants. Compared with men, women tend to have more larger and more varied social networks and to exchange support with a greater number of members in their networks [39]. In contrast, men often depend solely on their spouses and may be less likely to participate in social activities in the community [40]. Our findings showed that living with children was negatively associated with cognitive function. From our data, older adults living with children at baseline reported lower cognitive function (mental status) at both baseline and follow up. Findings that living with children was associated with physical disabilities were reported in other studies [4143]. This pattern may be attributed to the fact that older adults with physical or cognitive impairment tend to live with their children for support.

Our study had some limitations that should be acknowledged. First, although we used hukou status to classify the participants into rural and urban populations in accordance with the Chinese context and previous studies [38, 44, 45], we were unable to obtain detailed information on aspects of migration such as the migration time, process and reasons. Further studies should be conducted to explore the effects of migration based on different reasons on cognitive function. Second, it was difficult to match rural-to-urban migrants with rural non-migrants who shared the same residential location information in the hukou system. We believe that given the significant development gap between different regions of China, it would be more reasonable to compare populations that originated in the same region. Third, although this analysis covered a 4-year period, it might not have been long enough to enable the development of differences in the rates of cognitive decline. Fourth, our study explored the associations between baseline psychosocial factors and the subjects’ cognitive trajectory. However, the psychosocial statuses of the subjects may have changed over time. Further studies that explore the effects of time-variant psychosocial factors on cognitive function are warranted. Lastly, our study might have been subject to the ‘healthy migrant effect’. To minimise this effect, however, we adjusted for several health conditions as covariates in our regression models. Still, other potential contributors to the healthy migrant effect may have been overlooked. Further longitudinal studies are warranted to explore the cognitive trajectory of migrants before and after migration and thus control the influence of the healthy migrant effect.

Conclusions

In conclusion, our research has advanced the body of knowledge regarding the associations between rural-to-urban migration and the cognitive function trajectories of elderly Chinese residents. Moreover, we have revealed a sex-related difference in this association and explored the potential underlying psychosocial factors. Our results indicate that both elderly male migrants and rural non-migrants require more attention and that interventions targeting the preservation of cognitive function in elderly internal migrants should be developed according to these sex-related differences. Finally, we hope that our study will serve as a basis for further studies of the mechanism underlying the relationship between migration and cognitive function in older adults.

Supplementary information

12877_2020_1772_MOESM1_ESM.docx (40.1KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table S1 Differences in Cognitive Trajectories Between Migrants and Non-migrants. Table S2 Longitudinal Association Between Mental Status and Migrant for Female. Table S3 Longitudinal Association Between the Total Cognitive Trajectory and Migrant Status in Male Subjects. Table S4 Longitudinal Association Between the Mental Status and Migrant Status in Male Subjects.

Acknowledgements

The data used in this study are from China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). The authors are grateful for the permission to use the data here.

Abbreviations

ADL

Activities of daily living

CES-D-10

the 10-item Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale

CHARLS

China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study

ELSA

English Longitudinal Study of Aging

HRS

Health and Retirement Study

MMSE

Mini-Mental Status Examination

PPS

Probability-proportional-to-size

Authors’ contributions

JX and JL contributed equally to this study. JG, JL, and JX designed the study. JX, JL, and JZ conducted statistical analyses of the data. JX and JL drafted the paper. JG and JZ provided comments to the draft paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (71804201), the Natural Science Foundation of Guangdong Province (2018A0303130046, 2017A030310346) and Sun Yat-Sen University Scientific Research Foundation (17ykpy15).

Availability of data and materials

The CHARLS datasets, which analysed during the current study, are publicly available at the National School of Development, Peking University (http://charls.pku.edu.cn/en) and can be obtained after submitting a data use agreement to the CHARLS team.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The analysis is based on CHARLS data open to researchers. The original CHARLS was a survey approved by the Ethics Review Committee of Peking University, and all participants gave written informed consent at the time of participation.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Jinzhao Xie and Jing Liao contributed equally to this work.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s12877-020-01772-9.

References

  • 1.Nichols E, Szoeke CEI, Vollset SE, Abbasi N, Abd-Allah F, Abdela J, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:88–106. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30403-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bolandzadeh N, Kording K, Salowitz N, Davis JC, Hsu L, Chan A, et al. Predicting cognitive function from clinical measures of physical function and health status in older adults. PLoS One. 2015. 10.1371/journal.pone.0119075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 3.The State Council of the People’s Republic of China. China’s total population at the end of 2018 is close to 1.4 billion. 2019. http://www.gov.cn/xinwen/2019-01/21/content_5359797.htm. Accessed 3 Apr 2020.
  • 4.Fang EF, Scheibye-Knudsen M, Jahn HJ, Li J, Ling L, Guo H, et al. A research agenda for aging in China in the 21st century. Ageing Res Rev. 2015;24:197–205. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Liu J. Ageing, migration and familial support in rural China. Geoforum. 2014;51:305–312. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2013.04.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wang Q. Health of the elderly migration population in China: benefit from individual and local socioeconomic status? Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017. 10.3390/ijerph14040370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 7.Ruan Y, Zhu D, Lu J. Social adaptation and adaptation pressure among the "drifting elderly" in China: a qualitative study in Shanghai. Int J Health Plann Manag. 2019. 10.1002/hpm.2750. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 8.Xu H, Zhang Y, Wu B. Association between migration and cognitive status among middle-aged and older adults: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2017. 10.1186/s12877-017-0585-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 9.Xu H, Vorderstrasse AA, McConnell ES, Dupre ME, Ostbye T, Wu B. Migration and cognitive function: a conceptual framework for Global Health research. Glob Health Res Policy. 2018;3:34. doi: 10.1186/s41256-018-0088-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wang HX, Xu W, Pei JJ. Leisure activities, cognition and dementia. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1822;2012:482–491. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Li Q, Zhou X, Ma S, Jiang M, Li L. The effect of migration on social capital and depression among older adults in China. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2017;52:1513–1522. doi: 10.1007/s00127-017-1439-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Liao J, McMunn A, Mejias ST, Brunner EJ. Gendered trajectories of support from close relationships from middle to late life. Ageing Soc. 2018;38:746–765. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X16001264. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zhang C, Fong VL, Yoshikawa H, Way N, Chen XY, Lu ZH. The rise of maternal grandmother child Care in Urban Chinese Families. J Marriage Fam. 2019;81:1174–1191. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12598. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Di Gessa G, Glaser K, Tinker A. The impact of caring for grandchildren on the health of grandparents in Europe: a lifecourse approach. Soc Sci Med. 2016;152:166–175. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hughes ME, Waite LJ, LaPierre TA, Luo Y. All in the family: the impact of caring for grandchildren on grandparents' health. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62:S108–S119. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.2.S108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Liao J, Scholes S. Association of Social Support and Cognitive Aging Modified by sex and relationship type: a prospective investigation in the English longitudinal study of ageing. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;186:787–795. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwx142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lawton DM, Gasquoine PG, Weimer AA. Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex. 2015;66:141–145. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Segers K, Benoit F, Colson C, Kovac V, Nury D, Vanderaspoilden V. Pioneers in migration, pioneering in dementia: first generation immigrants in a European metropolitan memory clinic. Acta Neurol Belg. 2013;113:435–440. doi: 10.1007/s13760-013-0245-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Xu H, Dupre ME, Gu D, Wu B. The impact of residential status on cognitive decline among older adults in China: results from a longitudinal study. BMC Geriatr. 2017;17:107. doi: 10.1186/s12877-017-0501-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zhao Y, Hu Y, Smith JP, Strauss J, Yang G. Cohort profile: the China health and retirement longitudinal study (CHARLS) Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:61–68. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Li J, Rose N. Urban social exclusion and mental health of China’s rural-urban migrants - a review and call for research. Health Place. 2017;48:20–30. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2017.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zheng L, Hu R, Dong Z, Hao Y. Comparing the needs and utilization of health services between urban residents and rural-to-urban migrants in China from 2012 to 2016. BMC Health Serv Res. 2018;18:717. doi: 10.1186/s12913-018-3522-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Xiang Y, Zare H, Guan C, Gaskin D. The impact of rural-urban community settings on cognitive decline: results from a nationally-representative sample of seniors in China. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:323. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-1003-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Zhang L, Yang J, Liao Z, Zhao X, Hu X, Zhu W, et al. Association between diabetes and cognitive function among people over 45 years old in China: a cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019. 10.3390/ijerph16071294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25.Lei X, Smith JP, Sun X, Zhao Y. Gender differences in cognition in China and reasons for change over time: evidence from CHARLS. J Econ Ageing. 2014;4:46–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jeoa.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sha T, Cheng W, Yan Y. Prospective associations between pulse pressure and cognitive performance in Chinese middle-aged and older population across a 5-year study period. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10:29. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0355-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Huang W, Zhou Y. Effects of education on cognition at older ages: evidence from China's great famine. Soc Sci Med. 2013;98:54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Huang W, Lei X, Ridder G, Strauss J, Zhao Y. Health, height, height shrinkage, and SES at older ages: evidence from China. Am Econ J Appl Econ. 2013;5:86–121. doi: 10.1257/app.5.2.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Li J, Cacchione PZ, Hodgson N, Riegel B, Keenan BT, Scharf MT, et al. Afternoon napping and cognition in Chinese older adults: findings from the China health and retirement longitudinal study baseline assessment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017;65:373–380. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cattell. The measurement of adult intelligence Psychol Bull. 1943;40:153–193. doi: 10.1037/h0059973. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16:754–760. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gold DP, Andres D, Etezadi J, Arbuckle T, Schwartzman A, Chaikelson J. Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. Psychol Aging. 1995;10:294–303. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.10.2.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ku LJ, Stearns SC, Van Houtven CH, Lee SY, Dilworth-Anderson P, Konrad TR. Impact of caring for grandchildren on the health of grandparents in Taiwan. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68:1009–1021. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Xu H. Physical and mental health of Chinese grandparents caring for grandchildren and great-grandparents. Soc Sci Med. 2019. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.05.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Luo Y, Pan X, Zhang Z. Productive activities and cognitive decline among older adults in China: evidence from the China health and retirement longitudinal study. Soc Sci Med. 2019. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.09.052. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 36.Lin Y, Zhang Q, Chen W, Shi J, Han S, Song X, et al. Association between social integration and health among internal migrants in ZhongShan, China. PLoS One. 2016. 10.1371/journal.pone.0148397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 37.Guo Q, Bai X, Feng N. Social participation and depressive symptoms among Chinese older adults: a study on rural-urban differences. J Affect Disord. 2018;239:124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.06.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Zhu Y, Osterle A. Rural-urban disparities in unmet long-term care needs in China: the role of the hukou status. Soc Sci Med. 2017;191:30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Antonucci TC, Akiyama H. An examination of sex differences in social support among older men and women. Sex Roles. 1987;17:737–749. doi: 10.1007/BF00287685. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fuhrer R, Stansfeld SA. How gender affects patterns of social relations and their impact on health: a comparison of one or multiple sources of support from “close persons”. Soc Sci Med. 2002;54:811–825. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00111-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gu D, Feng Q, Yeung WJ. Reciprocal dynamics of solo-living and health among older adults in contemporary China. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2019;74:1441–1452. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gby140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wang H, Chen K, Pan Y, Jing F, Liu H. Associations and impact factors between living arrangements and functional disability among older Chinese adults. PLoS One. 2013. 10.1371/journal.pone.0053879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 43.Chen W, Fang Y, Mao F, Hao S, Chen J, Yuan M, et al. Assessment of disability among the elderly in Xiamen of China: a representative sample survey of 14,292 older adults. PLoS One. 2015. 10.1371/journal.pone.0131014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 44.Guo J, Guan L, Fang L, Liu C, Fu M, He H, et al. Depression among Chinese older adults: a perspective from Hukou and health inequities. J Affect Disord. 2017;223:115–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Zhang X, Yu B, He T, Wang P. Status and determinants of health services utilization among elderly migrants in China. Glob Health Res Policy. 2018. 10.1186/s41256-018-0064-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

12877_2020_1772_MOESM1_ESM.docx (40.1KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Table S1 Differences in Cognitive Trajectories Between Migrants and Non-migrants. Table S2 Longitudinal Association Between Mental Status and Migrant for Female. Table S3 Longitudinal Association Between the Total Cognitive Trajectory and Migrant Status in Male Subjects. Table S4 Longitudinal Association Between the Mental Status and Migrant Status in Male Subjects.

Data Availability Statement

The CHARLS datasets, which analysed during the current study, are publicly available at the National School of Development, Peking University (http://charls.pku.edu.cn/en) and can be obtained after submitting a data use agreement to the CHARLS team.


Articles from BMC Geriatrics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES