Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 Jun 30;50(5):921–942. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2694

Subjective status and perceived legitimacy across countries

Mark J Brandt 1,, Toon Kuppens 2, Russell Spears 2, Luca Andrighetto 3, Frederique Autin 4, Peter Babincak 5, Constantina Badea 6, Jaechang Bae 7, Anatolia Batruch 8, Julia C Becker 9, Konrad Bocian 10, Bojana Bodroža 11, David Bourguignon 12, Marcin Bukowski 13, Fabrizio Butera 8, Sarah E Butler 14, Xenia Chryssochoou 15, Paul Conway 16, Jarret T Crawford 17, Jean‐Claude Croizet 18, Soledad de Lemus 19, Juliane Degner 20, Piotr Dragon 13, Federica Durante 21, Matthew J Easterbrook 22, Iniobong Essien 23, Joseph P Forgas 24, Roberto González 25, Sylvie Graf 26, Peter Halama 27, Gyuseog Han 28, Ryan Y Hong 29, Petr Houdek 30, Eric R Igou 31, Yoel Inbar 32, Jolanda Jetten 33, William Jimenez Leal 34, Gloria Jiménez‐Moya 25, Jaya Kumar Karunagharan 35, Anna Kende 36, Maria Korzh 37, Simon M Laham 38, Joris Lammers 39, Li Lim 33, Antony S R Manstead 40, Janko Međedović 41, Zachary J Melton 42, Matt Motyl 43, Spyridoula Ntani 15, Chuma Kevin Owuamalam 35, Müjde Peker 44, Michael J Platow 45, J P Prims 42, Christine Reyna 46, Mark Rubin 47, Rim Saab 48, Sindhuja Sankaran 49, Lee Shepherd 50, Chris G Sibley 51, Agata Sobkow 52, Bram Spruyt 53, Pernille Stroebaek 54, Nebi Sümer 55, Joseph Sweetman 56, Catia P Teixeira 2, Claudia Toma 57, Adrienn Ujhelyi 36, Jojanneke van der Toorn 58, Alain van Hiel 59,60, Alejandro Vásquez‐Echeverría 61, Alexandra Vazquez 62, Michelangelo Vianello 63, Marek Vranka 64, Vincent Yzerbyt 65, Jennifer L Zimmerman 46
PMCID: PMC7507836  PMID: 32999511

Abstract

The relationships between subjective status and perceived legitimacy are important for understanding the extent to which people with low status are complicit in their oppression. We use novel data from 66 samples and 30 countries (N = 12,788) and find that people with higher status see the social system as more legitimate than those with lower status, but there is variation across people and countries. The association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy was never negative at any levels of eight moderator variables, although the positive association was sometimes reduced. Although not always consistent with hypotheses, group identification, self‐esteem, and beliefs in social mobility were all associated with perceived legitimacy among people who have low subjective status. These findings enrich our understanding of the relationship between social status and legitimacy.

Keywords: legitimacy, social identity, status, system justification

1. INTRODUCTION

Scholars across the social sciences have debated whether or not people with low social status are complicit in their oppression. On the one hand, unequal social systems tend to be relatively stable across time, suggesting some degree of passivity, if not complicity among the populace; however, on the other hand, social movements aimed at disrupting or altering social systems are often spearheaded by the very people disaffected by existing status arrangements. The status‐legitimacy hypothesis (as coined by Brandt, 2013; Jost, Banaji, & Nosek, 2004; Jost & Hunyady, 2003; Jost, Pelham, Sheldon, & Ni Sullivan, 2003) predicts that people with lower status will, at least in some conditions, be more likely to see the social system as legitimate than people with higher status. In this article, we examine the conditions under which this hypothesis finds support. We used a novel, 30‐country study on the association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy. We had two aims. First, we aimed to understand whether and when people with lower subjective status perceive higher levels of legitimacy than people with higher subjective status. Second, we aimed to understand what types of factors are associated with perceived legitimacy for people with low subjective status.

1.1. Status‐legitimacy hypothesis

The logic behind the status‐legitimacy hypothesis is that people have motivations to see themselves, their groups, and their larger social systems in a positive light (for full details see Jost et al., 2003). For people with high social status, all of these motivations are generally consistent with one another; however, for people with low social status who are disadvantaged by the social system, seeing the self and one's group as positive conflicts with the motivation to see the social system as positive. To resolve this psychological conflict people with low social status may, under some conditions, legitimize the social system more than people with high social status.

Scholars have debated the status‐legitimacy hypothesis, testing the hypothesis several times with mixed results (find support: e.g., Henry & Saul, 2006; Li, Yang, Wu, & Kou, 2020; Sengupta, Osborne, & Sibley, 2015; see Jost, 2017 for a recent summary of relevant work; do not find support: e.g., Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi‐Cioldi, 2012; Vargas‐Salfate, Paez, Liu, Pratto, & Gil de Zúñiga, 2018; Zimmerman & Reyna, 2013). This work suggests that there is still broad scholarly interest in how status and legitimacy are related. We aim to add two things to this literature. With Approach 1, we test potential individual‐ and societal‐level moderators of the associations between different levels of subjective status and perceived legitimacy (i.e., a relative focus). Under what conditions and among people with what kinds of perceptions and psychological characteristics is there evidence for the status‐legitimacy hypothesis (cf. McGuire, 2013)? With Approach 2, we focus on people with low levels of social status and tested potential predictors of perceived legitimacy among this group. Unlike the first approach, Approach 2 does not focus on whether the predictors differ from people with high levels of status.

1.2. Approach 1: Testing moderators of the association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy

The status‐legitimacy hypothesis follows from the idea that people are addressing the threatening feelings of aversive anxiety and arousal resulting from psychological conflict. People alter their behaviors, beliefs, and perceptions in order to address this threat, and when the feelings of threat are assuaged they are less likely to alter their behaviors, beliefs, and perceptions (for reviews see Jonas et al., 2014; Proulx, Inzlicht, & Harmon‐Jones, 2012). Factors that make social hierarchy appear less consequential in material or psychological terms (we discuss several factors below) should reduce the effects of low subjective status on the anxious arousal that results from status‐based dissonance. Therefore, this reasoning suggests that when people's feelings of threat are addressed, the status‐legitimacy link will be positive (i.e., low status people will be less inclined to legitimize the system as a way to assuage feelings of threat). Conversely, when people's anxious arousal is exacerbated, the status‐legitimacy link will be negative (i.e., low status people will see the system as more legitimate).

1.2.1. Moderators that reduce threat

There are potentially many factors that can mitigate anxious arousal. We focus on factors that have appeared in the system justification and social identity literatures because these literatures have been the focus of debates on this issue (for a recent example see Jost, 2019; Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2019). The most obvious factor is the identification with and salience of a valued group, which is one way social identity theorists suggest people address anxious arousal (Hogg, 2014) or cope with the threat of having a low status (McMahon & Watts, 2002; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Consistent with this, the original paper proposing the status‐legitimacy hypothesis argued that if group interests are salient and accessible (something that is likely to be correlated with identification), then support for the status‐legitimacy hypothesis is unlikely to emerge (Jost et al., 2003, 2004). Similarly, people with high self‐esteem and positive self‐views make plans, have high levels of personal agency, and high levels of self‐certainty (e.g., Campbell, 1990; Harter, 1978), suggesting that they are less likely to be psychologically affected by uncontrollability and similar types of threats associated with low status (see Laurin, Kay, & Landau, 2018 or Schoel, Bluemke, Mueller, & Stahlberg, 2011 for a similar argument in different domains). This is consistent with the idea that self‐esteem should push against system justification motivations for low status people (cf. Jost, Gaucher, & Stern, 2015, p. 330).

1.2.2. Structural moderators that increase threat through dissonance

Other factors can exacerbate feelings of anxious arousal by increasing the amount of dissonance people experience. The most direct prediction comes from assessing the contradictory cognitions that could increase dissonance. The dissonance that people experience from being in a low status position might emerge “from the contradictory cognitions that (a) the system is putting me (and my group) at a disadvantage, and (b) through our acquiescence, my group and I are contributing to the stability of the system” (Jost et al., 2003, p. 16). Therefore, people who recognize that they are not doing enough to mitigate inequality should experience more dissonance. This recognition should increase perceived legitimacy for people who are low status and is part of the contradictory cognitions originally predicted to cause lower status people to see the system as more legitimate than higher status people do.

The amount of civil liberties, meritocratic culture, and inequality within a country could also affect feelings of dissonance. Countries and contexts with more civil liberties and more meritocratic cultures increase the amount of dissonance people low in status experience by implying they have choice and control over their outcomes (Jost et al., 2003, p. 17). Inequality may increase the conflict between self/group motivations and acceptance of the system for low status group members (e.g., Henry & Saul, p. 376). It may also create the impression that people have less control over their place within society. Both factors should increase the amount of anxious uncertainty and increase the necessity of rationalizing the system. No support was found for these predictions in some prior studies (Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi‐Cioldi, 2012; Trump & White, 2018; for one exception on one measure see Vargas‐Salfate et al., 2018).

1.2.3. Structural moderators that affect threat

Structural factors, including levels of inequality or the stability of the hierarchy, influence what types of options people perceive that they have and their place within the society. These perceptions may affect people's perceptions of the whole system. For example, to the extent that inequality exacerbates feelings of hierarchy and the threatening feelings of low status, as well as making status differences more salient, this should further motivate challenges to the inequality by the low status group (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). This is the opposite of the inequality prediction above. When looking at the stability of the hierarchy, one of the traditional predictions of social identity theory is that low status group members are less likely to seek social change when the status hierarchy is perceived as stable, compared to when the hierarchy is perceived as unstable (Ellemers, van Knippenberg, & Wilke, 1990; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). That is, when the status hierarchy is stable, there is little scope and hope for social change (i.e., no cognitive alternative to the status quo), and people are more likely to accept the system as legitimate. This hypothesis is also consistent with work finding that system stability increases a system justification motivation (Laurin, Gaucher, & Kay, 2013).

1.2.4. Summary

We have identified several potential moderators that might help us predict when we are more or less likely to find support for the status‐legitimacy hypothesis. These moderators are expressed as individual hypotheses in Table 1. In the cross‐national study that follows, we test these eight hypotheses.

TABLE 1.

Summary of the moderator hypotheses tested in this investigation (Approach 1)

Variable Moderator hypothesis
Reduce threat
Group Identification Identification‐Moderation Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative for people low in group identification and positive for people high in group identification.
Self‐Esteem Self‐Esteem‐Moderation Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative for people low in self‐esteem and positive for people high in self‐esteem.
Increase threat through dissonance
Inequality contribution Inequality Contribution‐Moderation Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative for people who see themselves as contributing to inequality and positive for people who do not see themselves as contributing to inequality.
Civil Liberties Civil Liberties Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with high levels of civil liberties and positive for people in countries with low levels of civil liberties.
Meritocracy Meritocracy Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with meritocratic cultures and positive for people in countries with less meritocratic cultures.
Inequality System Justification Theory (SJT) Inequality Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with high levels of inequality and positive for people in countries with low levels of inequality.
Structural Factors That Affect Threat
Inequality Social Identity Theory (SIT) Inequality Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative in countries with low levels of inequality and positive for people in countries with high levels of inequality.
Stability Stability‐Moderator Hypothesis: The link between status and legitimacy will be negative when people see the status hierarchy as stable and positive when people see the status hierarchy as unstable.

1.3. Approach 2: Finding the predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of subjective status

In addition to searching for moderators, we also ask what predicts perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of social status. Put another way, assuming that people with low levels of subjective status vary in the extent to which they see the system as legitimate, what predicts this variation? In contrast to the prior section, this line of questioning does not necessarily imply moderation effects, as the predictors of perceived legitimacy for lower status groups could be the same as the predictors of perceived legitimacy for higher status groups (moderation is possible, but not necessary). This approach moves away from the explicit status comparisons of the status‐legitimacy hypothesis and is consistent with the broader questions that inspired this hypothesis: Do people see systems that oppress them as just and legitimate, and if so under what conditions? We focus on four different variables that might predict perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of social status (see Table 2).

TABLE 2.

Summary of the predictor hypotheses tested in this investigation (Approach 2)

Variable Predictor hypothesis
Social mobility Social Mobility‐Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low status who see the system as having high social mobility will be more likely to see the system as legitimate.
Stability Stability‐Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low status who see the status hierarchy as stable will be more likely to see the system as legitimate.
Identification Identification‐Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low status who have high group identification will be less likely to see the system as legitimate.
Self‐esteem Self‐Esteem Legitimacy Hypothesis: People with low status who have high self‐esteem will be less likely to see the system as legitimate.

Some work suggests that people desire to live in social systems in which there is upward mobility and the system is relatively stable (Laurin et al., 2013; Martorana, Galinsky, & Rao, 2005). Consistent with this, people who perceived no possibilities to move up to a higher status group are less likely to justify the system and more likely to engage in collective action (Day & Fiske, 2017; Ellemers et al., 1990; Mandisodza, Jost, & Unzueta, 2006; Tajfel, 1981; Wright, Taylor, & Moghaddam, 1990). Similarly, systems with more stable social hierarchies are seen as more legitimate (Laurin et al., 2013) and are less likely to trigger efforts to change the system (Bettencourt, Charlton, Dorr, & Hume, 2001; Ellemers et al., 1990; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). This suggests that when people see the system as providing social mobility and stability, they are more likely to see the system as legitimate.

When people's group‐ and self‐interests are prioritized, it may be less likely that people perceive the system as legitimate if they or their group are not benefitting from that system. That is, higher levels of group identification and self‐esteem may both be associated with lower levels of perceived system legitimacy among people with low social status. This follows from the idea that group‐ and self‐interest motivations are negatively related to system‐related motivations among low status groups (Jost et al., 2004). It is also consistent with the idea that the effects of group‐ and self‐interests—when sufficiently strong—may be more prominent than the effects of system‐interests (Jost et al., 2003, 2004, 2011) and with the finding that people with low levels of group identification may be more likely to accept the current situation (Rubin & Hewstone, 2004; Spears, Jetten, & Doosje, 2001). According to these ideas, people with lower subjective status who have high group identification or who have high self‐esteem will be less likely to see the system as legitimate.

From our reading of the literature, the predictions in the prior paragraph seem most consistent with a straightforward extension of social identity theory's work on group identification to research on perceived legitimacy. However, it is important to note that recent work by some scholars (and co‐authors of this article) has predicted the opposite, at least as a function of an additional qualifier. A series of papers by Owuamalam and colleagues have argued that high group identifiers and people under conditions of high group salience may be likely to see the system as legitimate to the extent that it is seen as one that affords the group collective social mobility in the long term and allows them to eventually achieve social change as a group (e.g., due to longer‐term status instability; Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2016, 2019; Owuamalam, Rubin, Spears, & Weerabangsa, 2017; for a countervailing view see Jost, 2019). In short, using the system rather than rejecting it, can be seen as a viable vehicle for group interests, especially for high identifiers. This idea can also be tested with our data and essentially predicts the opposite of the predictions in the prior paragraph.

1.3.1. Summary

We identified several potential predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with lower social status that might help us understand the reasons some people with low social status perceive the social system as legitimate. Some predictors indicate that the system is fulfilling the person's goals and other predictors indicate that personal and group goals are prioritized. These hypotheses are specified individually in Table 2. In the cross‐national study that follows, we test these four hypotheses.

1.4. An international, multi‐lab approach

We conducted a cross‐country study on the association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy that allowed us to test for moderators of the status‐legitimacy relationships, as well as to understand what predicts perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of subjective status. We consider three types of effects:

To assess moderators (Approach 1):

1. We test the interactions between status and the proposed moderator variables. If the interaction is significant, we test whether the effect of subjective status is negative (consistent with the status‐legitimacy hypothesis) at the predicted levels of the moderator variables (e.g., when identification is low).

The assessment of predictors of perceived legitimacy among people with low status will take two steps (Approach 2):

2a. We examine the main effects of the predictors on perceived legitimacy. Because the hypotheses about predictors of perceived legitimacy are not specific to people with lower levels of subjective status (e.g., social mobility could predict perceived legitimacy for people with both high and low status), this main effect analysis tells us if there is an average effect across the sample.

2b. When there is a significant interaction between the predictor and status, we examine whether the predictor still has the predicted significant effect (e.g., a positive effect of social mobility) for people with lower levels of status. This will tell us whether the effect of the predictor is specific to people with low subjective status.

2. METHOD

2.1. Participants and procedure

Sixty‐six distinct samples were collected by researchers from 30 countries. We aimed for 150 participants per sample, so that we had at least 150 participants per country to give us approximately 80% power to detect a small to medium effect (i.e., r = .22) within each sample. Data collection was not continued after analysis. To ensure that respondents were part of the social and political system, we only included participants who indicated they were either born in the country or had lived in the country for six or more years and so not every sample resulted in 150 participants. All exclusions are reported. Countries, samples, type of sample, proportion of men, mean age, and sample size are presented in Table 3. Samples included a mix of student samples, community samples, and samples with both students and members of the community (the latter two were both considered non‐student samples for the sake of analyses). Samples are primarily samples of university students, although some include community samples from Mechanical Turk (USA2), community email lists (e.g., USA6), or a representative sample (NLD3). Multiple samples per country helps guard against the possibility that results for any particular country are dependent on one particular sample. Research was conducted in accordance with APA and national guidelines. Data, code, and materials are available at the following link: https://osf.io/5uxc7/.

TABLE 3.

Sample demographic information. Sorted in alphabetical order by country

Sample Country Type Proportion Male M age N
AUS1 Australia Student 0.27 21.4 209
AUS2 Australia Student 0.34 19.5 114
AUS3 Australia Student 0.34 19.2 80
AUS4 Australia Student 0.29 20.1 163
AUS5 Australia Student 0.32 23.2 222
BEL1 Belgium Student 0.35 20.0 623
BEL2 Belgium Student 0.54 21.7 137
BEL3 Belgium Student 0.28 19.3 194
BEL4 Belgium Student 0.24 21.9 91
CAN1 Canada Student 0.33 18.7 180
CHL1 Chile Student 0.28 20.9 156
COL1 Colombia Student 0.27 19.9 139
CZE1 Czech Republic Non‐Student 0.23 27.5 154
CZE2 Czech Republic Non‐Student 0.29 23.7 241
CZE3 Czech Republic Non‐Student 0.30 24.8 311
DNK1 Denmark Student 0.16 24.0 162
FRA1 France Student 0.31 21.2 303
FRA2 France Student 0.11 20.8 163
FRA3 France Student 0.46 18.6 180
DEU1 Germany Student 0.36 25.3 50
DEU2 Germany Student 0.18 22.6 133
DEU3 Germany Student 0.22 22.7 151
DEU4 Germany Non‐Student 0.81 37.2 89
GBR1 Great Britain Student 0.14 19.8 213
GBR2 Great Britain Student 0.07 19.1 138
GBR3 Great Britain Student 0.18 19.6 169
GBR4 Great Britain Student 0.08 19.1 118
GRC1 Greece Non‐Student 0.49 35.9 444
HUN1 Hungary Student 0.22 20.2 144
IND1 India Non‐Student 0.74 31.2 449
IRL1 Ireland Student 0.46 24.6 145
ITA1 Italy Student 0.47 43.0 103
ITA2 Italy Student 0.47 44.0 103
ITA3 Italy Student 0.05 26.9 109
LBN1 Lebanon Student 0.51 18.9 204
MYS1 Malaysia Student 0.43 20.8 146
MYS2 Malaysia Non‐Student 0.44 24.6 63
NLD1 Netherlands Student 0.26 19.8 184
NLD2 Netherlands Student 0.19 20.0 232
NLD3 Netherlands Non‐Student 0.49 40.4 766
NLD4 Netherlands Student 0.20 21.2 176
NZL1 New Zealand Student 0.18 21.0 180
POL1 Poland Student 0.16 28.0 214
POL2 Poland Non‐Student 0.20 23.2 160
POL3 Poland Non‐Student 0.65 26.4 166
RUS1 Russia Student 0.34 19.4 117
SRB1 Serbia Student 0.24 20.3 159
SRB2 Serbia Non‐Student 0.39 30.7 173
SGP1 Singapore Student 0.27 19.9 196
SVK1 Slovakia Student 0.19 22.6 268
SVK2 Slovakia Non‐Student 0.49 46.7 166
KOR1 South Korea Student 0.30 20.6 119
ESP1 Spain Student 0.24 23.1 148
ESP2 Spain Student 0.30 32.7 252
CHE1 Switzerland Student 0.51 20.3 131
TUR1 Turkey Student 0.30 20.5 122
TUR2 Turkey Student 0.23 20.0 99
USA1 United States Student 0.20 20.6 224
USA2 United States Non‐Student 0.57 35.1 214
USA3 United States Student 0.42 19.8 316
USA4 United States Student 0.27 19.9 195
USA5 United States Student 0.17 19.4 181
USA6 United States Non‐Student 0.23 36.1 368
USA7 United States Non‐Student 0.48 41.5 116
URY1 Uruguay Student 0.28 22.2 169
URY2 Uruguay Student 0.21 20.6 184

Participating labs used materials designed by the first three authors. These labs translated the materials and adjusted them when necessary for their language and cultural context (e.g., replacing “United States” in the system justification measure). Participants first completed measures of demographics, including the measure of subjective social status. They then completed measures about either their perceptions of themselves and their group's position in society (the moderators and predictors) or their perceptions of system legitimacy (the outcomes). The supplemental materials include a list of all measures and manipulations. Analyses include participants who are over 18 and have completed at least the subjective status and the system justification, trust in government, confidence in societal institutions, and legitimacy of the status hierarchy measures. The final sample included 12,788 participants (4,252 men, 8,478 women, 58 with missing responses, M age = 25.3, SD age = 10.7).

2.2. Key predictor variable: subjective status

To measure subjective social status, we used the MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status (Adler, Epel, Castellazzo, & Ickovics, 2000), modified to capture people's sense of status within their country. We chose this measure to allow easier comparisons across countries. Participants were asked to rate themselves on a ladder that ranged from 1 to 10, where 10 was high status and 1 was low status. The instructions for the measure read as follows:

Think of this ladder as representing where people stand in [country]. At the top of the ladder are people who are the best off—those who have the most money, the most education, and the most respected jobs. At the bottom are the people who are the worst off—who have the least money, least education, and the least respected jobs or no job. The higher up you are on this ladder, the closer you are to the people at the very top; the lower you are, the closer you are to the people at the very bottom. Please choose the number of the rung of the ladder where you think you stand at this time in your life, relatively to other people in [country].

This one‐item measure had adequate test‐retest reliability in prior samples (e.g., Operario, Adler, & Williams, 2004) and is correlated with objective measures of status (e.g., income; Goodman et al., 2003; Sakurai, Kawakami, Yamaoka, Ishikawa, & Hashimoto, 2010). Responses in our sample were above the midpoint (M = 6.03, SD = 1.53), but spanned the entire range of the measure. See supplemental materials for means across countries on this and all other individual‐level variables.

2.3. Key outcome variable: perceived legitimacy

We included four measures of perceived legitimacy and system justification. Multiple research groups studying the status‐legitimacy hypothesis have used all these measures (e.g., Brandt, 2013; Henry & Saul, 2006; Jost et al., 2003; Li et al., 2020). The 8‐item general system justification scale (Kay & Jost, 2003) includes items like “In general, I find society to be fair” and “Society is set up so that people usually get what they deserve” (M α = .78, SD α = .06, α range [.66, .85]; Scale M = −0.64; Scale SD = 1.14; −3 = Disagree strongly to +3 = Agree strongly).

The 4‐item trust in government scale often included in the American National Election Studies (2015; cf. Brandt, 2013) includes items like “How much of the time do you think you can trust the government to do what is right?” (1 = None of the time, 2 = Some of the time, 3 = Most of the time, 4 = Just about always) and “Would you say the government is pretty much run by a few big interests looking out for themselves or that it is run for the benefit of all the people?” (1 = Few big interests, 2 = Benefit of all). Items were first standardized using z‐scores and then averaged to form a scale (M α = .61, SD α = .17, α range [−.10, .78,]; Scale M = −0.02; Scale SD = 0.75).

The 7‐item measure assessing confidence in societal institutions adopted from the General Social Survey (2017), and used in Brandt (2013), was used to tap into perceptions of both governmental and economic systems. For each of seven institutions, participants are asked how much confidence they have in them (0 = None at all, 3 = A great deal). Institutions include the armed forces, the police, the courts, the governments of the country, congress, major companies, and banks and financial institutions (M α = .77, SD α = .04, α range [.63, .83,]; Scale M = 1.35; Scale SD = 0.55).

Finally, a 2‐item measure of the perceived legitimacy of the status hierarchy was used to capture perceived legitimacy in this specific domain (based on Mummendey, Kessler, Klink, & Mielke, 1999). The items were prefaced with “Differences in power and status between groups in [country] are...” and then participants rated the stems “…illegitimate” and “…unfair” on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly (M r = .52, SD r = .14, r range [.33, .80]; Scale M = −0.83; Scale SD = 1.30). These items were measured in the same block of questions as the stability of the status hierarchy measure (see below).

2.4. Individual level measures of moderators and predictors

2.4.1. Group identification

We measured group identification with a three‐item scale about participants’ identification with their social class. The scale was prefaced with, “The following questions are about people with a similar background and social class as yourself. Social class refers to people with similar opportunities in terms of income, education, and social standing, as in the ladder measure that you completed earlier.” The items were “I identify with people from my social class”, “I feel solidarity with my social class”, and “My social class is an important part of how I see myself”. Participants responded to these items on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly (M α = .67, SD α = .10, α range [.38, .78]; Scale M = 0.51; Scale SD = 1.17).

2.4.2. Self‐esteem

We measured self‐esteem with the validated single‐item measure (Robins, Hendin, & Trzesniewski, 2001). This measure reads “I have high self‐esteem” with answers that ranged from 0 = Not at all to 6 = Very true of me (Item M = 3.50; Item SD = 1.57).

2.4.3. Social mobility

We measured perceptions of social mobility with six items. They included, “In general, people can easily get ahead in society”, “In general, people can climb the social ladder and be successful”, “People with a similar background and social class to my own can easily get ahead in society”, “It is easy for people with a similar background and social class to my own to climb the social ladder and be successful”, “I am motivated to climb up the social ladder”, and “I am able to climb up the social ladder”. All items were measure on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly and had good reliability (M α = .72, SD α = .06, α range [.52, .81]; Scale M = 0.82; Scale SD = 0.93).

2.4.4. Contribute to inequality

We created five items to measure whether people felt that they contributed to inequality and the stability of the system. One item, “I could do more to change differences in power and status between groups in society”, did not correlate as expected with the other items and substantially reduced the reliability of the scale (α = .58), so we omitted it. The remaining four items read “I contribute to keeping society the way it is”, “I contribute to maintaining the current social hierarchy”, “I don't do anything to change the current differences in power and status in society”, and “I am not trying to change the current differences in power and status in society”. All items were measured on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly and the four remaining items created a reliable scale (M α = .71, SD α = .09, α range [.46, .84]; Scale M = −0.14; Scale SD = 1.12).

2.4.5. Stability of the status hierarchy

We used a 2‐item measure of the perceived stability of the status hierarchy. This was based on measures used in studies from a social identity perspective (Mummendey et al., 1999). The items were prefaced with “Differences in power and status between groups in [country] are...” and then participants rated the stems “…difficult to change” and “…will remain stable over time” on a scale ranging from −3 = Disagree strongly to 3 = Agree strongly (M r = .31, SD r = .14, r range [−.06, .76]; Scale M = 0.82; Scale SD = 1.14).

2.5. Societal level measures of moderators

We included three measures to assess societal level conditions. To capture the amount of civil liberties, we used the 2015 Civil Liberties subscale of the Democracy index created by the Economist Intelligence Unit (2016). This index uses a combination of survey data and expert ratings to estimate how democratically free individual countries are. It has been used in prior work testing similar questions (Brandt, 2013).

To assess the extent to which the culture holds meritocratic values, we combined four items from international surveys. The first two are the importance of ambition and hard work for getting ahead in life in the ISSP (2009). The third and fourth come from the sixth wave of the World Values Survey (2016). The third asks participants to respond to an item ranging from 1 = “Competition is good. It stimulates people to work hard and develop new ideas” to 10 = “Competition is harmful. It brings out the worst in people”. The fourth asks participants to respond to an item ranging from 1 = “In the long run, hard work usually brings better life” to 10 = “Hard work doesn't generally bring success—it's more a matter of luck and connections”. All four items were standardized, the third and fourth items were reverse scored, and all four items were combined to form a scale (α = .82).

To measure the objective inequality, we used the Gini index from 2013 and obtained from the World Bank (World Bank, 2016). For some countries, the 2013 data were not available, and in these cases the most recent data were included instead. The Gini index assesses the amount of income inequality within a region, with higher scores indicating greater inequality. It is a common measure of inequality within a society (e.g., Oishi, Kesebir, & Diener, 2011).

2.6. Covariates

We included three covariates for analyses that focused on the individual level: self‐reported age, gender (0 = women, 1 = men), and type of sample (0 = student, 1 = non‐student) to adjust for potential background influences. For the models that include predictors at the societal level of analysis, we included two country‐level covariates. We included the GDP per capita for each of the countries to control for overall wealth (World Bank, 2016). To control for broader regional trends (Kuppens & Pollet, 2014), we also included contrast codes for each of the continents represented in our data.

2.7. A note on coding

We coded all of the variables to range from 0 to 1. Multilevel regression coefficients are then the proportion difference in the outcome variable as one goes from 0 (minimum) to 1 (maximum) on the predictor variable. For example, a coefficient of .05 is a 5% difference in the outcome between people scoring the lowest and the highest on the predictor variable.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Preliminary analyses: the effects of subjective status on perceived legitimacy

We use multilevel models to account for participants’ nesting within countries and within samples/labs. Models were estimated using lme4 (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, 2016) packages in R (R Core Team, 2016). Because we are interested in the individual‐level association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy, we centered subjective status at the country‐mean (Enders & Tofighi, 2007). The correlations between the predictor variables and the measures of perceived legitimacy are in Figure 1 (created with GGally, Schloerke et al., 2016). The measures of system justification, trust, confidence, and legitimacy of the status hierarchy were moderately inter‐correlated (M α = .70, SD α = .06, α range [.56, .82]). To combine items, reduce the overall number of models, and facilitate generalization across measures, we nested these four measures within participants, resulting in a four‐level multilevel model: legitimacy measures nested in persons, nested in samples/labs, nested in countries. In short, this estimates the average effect across multiple measures of system legitimacy that have been used in the literature, while increasing measurement precision (see for similar suggestions, Gelman, 2018; McShane, Tackett, Böckenholt, & Gelman, 2019). It is analogous to conducting four studies and estimating the meta‐analytic estimate across the four studies.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Correlations (above the diagonal) and density plots (below the diagonal) between subjective status, sense of power, and the outcome variables. All variables were country‐mean‐centered before calculating the correlations. Density plots for each individual variable are on the diagonal. Density plots are a variant of a histogram. The area below the diagonal uses density plots between two variables. SJ = System Justification, Trust = Trust in Government, Confid = Confidence in Societal Institutions, Legit = Legitimacy of the status hierarchy [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

First, we tested the main effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy. This test conceptually replicates many prior tests of these hypotheses (e.g., Brandt, 2013; Henry & Saul, 2006; Jost et al., 2003). We regressed perceived legitimacy on country‐mean‐centered subjective status. We also included country‐mean‐centered sense of power in this analysis and all other analyses because it was part of our original analysis plan (see Appendix S1). People with higher subjective status are more likely to see the social system as legitimate compared to people with lower subjective status (b = .152, SE = 0.005, 95% CI [0.142, 0.162]). There is a 15% difference in perceived legitimacy between people with the highest and people with the lowest levels of subjective status. This is in contrast to the prediction of the status‐legitimacy hypothesis.

To test whether these results are impacted by covariates, we included age (country‐mean‐centered), gender (country‐mean‐centered), type of sample (grand mean‐centered), and the type of perceived legitimacy measure (contrast coded) as covariates, including the interactions between these contrast codes and status and sense of power. This gives us the average effect of the primary predictors across the four measures of perceived legitimacy and controls for mean differences between the measures. The estimate for subjective status is nearly identical to the estimate without covariates (b = .151, SE = 0.005, 95% CI [0.141, 0.161]).

The effect of subjective status is not the same across all people in all situations. We re‐estimated the model with covariates and included random slopes at the country‐level for subjective status and sense of power. The estimated slope of subjective status on perceived legitimacy for each of the countries is in Figure 2. Although the effect of subjective status is always estimated to be positive when predicting perceived legitimacy, it varies in size. In the next section of the article, we consider moderators of the effects of subjective status.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Estimated slopes of subjective status for each country from the multilevel model. Dots are randomly jittered horizontally to help show the data.

3.2. Approach 1: Moderators of subjective status on perceived legitimacy

We tested individual‐level moderators and country‐level moderators. Individual level moderators were country‐mean‐centered and included in separate models as fixed effects in the four‐level multilevel model used above (see Figure 3 for correlations between these measures). We built the country‐level models using the same four‐level multilevel models, with the addition of the country‐level moderators and country‐level covariates. All country‐level variables were grand‐mean‐centered, with the exception of the contrast codes for continents. In the country‐level models, we included random slopes for subjective status and sense of power.

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3

Correlations (above the diagonal) and density plots (below the diagonal) between primary predictor variables and the individual level variables. All variables were country‐mean centered before calculating the correlations. Density plots for each individual variable are on the diagonal. ID = Group identification; SelfEst = Self‐esteem; SocMob = Social mobility; ConInqu = Contribution to inequality; Stable = Stability of the status hierarchy [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

To interpret the interactions, we used marginal effects plots created with interplot (Solt & Hu, 2015). This plots the marginal effect (sometimes called the simple effect) of the predictor variable for the entire range of the moderator variable. This helps us to understand the effect across the range of the moderator variables, rather than only focusing on points ±1 SD of the mean of the moderator variable (cf. Aiken & West, 1991). We also included a histogram of the moderator variable in each plot to illustrate how much of the sample is located at different levels of the moderator variable (cf. Hainmueller, Mummolo, & Xu, 2019).

3.2.1. Moderators that reduce threat

We tested the hypotheses that are based on the assumption that group identification and self‐esteem reduce the experience of threat. If the hypotheses are supported, we should find positive interactions between the moderators and status, such that at high levels of the moderator variables the link between status and perceived legitimacy is positive and at low levels of the moderator variables the link is negative.

The results for group identification and self‐esteem are in Figures 4 and 5, respectively. One significant positive interaction emerged between identification and subjective status (Figure 4a). The positive marginal effect of subjective status is weaker at low levels of identification and non‐significant at the lowest levels of identification (Figure 4b). It is stronger and significant at high levels. This interaction is in the direction predicted by the hypothesis; however, the effect of subjective status is never significantly negative. Because this effect is not negative, it is only partially consistent with the full prediction that perceived legitimacy will be higher for low status than for high status groups among lower identifiers.

FIGURE 4.

FIGURE 4

Panel A: Effects of subjective status and group identification on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of group identification (x‐axis). Panel C: Marginal effect of group identification on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of subjective status (x‐axis). For both Panels B and C, the grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

FIGURE 5.

FIGURE 5

Effects of subjective status and self‐esteem on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

Self‐esteem, however, did not significantly interact with subjective status (Figure 5). In short, although group identification showed partial support for the hypotheses in the status domain, there was no support for the hypothesis for self‐esteem.

3.2.2. Moderators that increase threat through dissonance

Inequality contribution, civil liberties, meritocracy, and inequality were all expected to increase the experience of threat through dissonance. If the hypotheses are supported, we should find negative interactions between the moderators and status, such that at high levels of the moderator variables the link between status and perceived legitimacy is negative and at low levels of the moderator variables the link is positive.

There were no significant negative interactions between any of the four moderator variables expected to increase the experience of threat (Figures 6, 7, 8, 9). In the case of civil liberties, there was a positive interaction between civil liberties and subjective status. When probing the interaction, we found that the effect of subjective status (Figure 7b) was positive and stronger at high levels of civil liberties. At lower levels of civil liberties, the effect of subjective status was still positive, but weaker and non‐significant. This interaction is in the opposite direction predicted by the hypothesis. In short, in no cases did we find support for the hypotheses about moderators that could increase the experience of threat.

FIGURE 6.

FIGURE 6

Effects of subjective status and contributing to inequality on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

FIGURE 7.

FIGURE 7

Panel A: Effects of subjective status and civil liberties on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of civil liberties (x‐axis). The grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is highlighted with the dashed line. See footnote 13 for outlier explanation [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

FIGURE 8.

FIGURE 8

Effects of subjective status and meritocracy on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

FIGURE 9.

FIGURE 9

Effects of subjective status and inequality on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. A null effect is highlighted with the dashed line. See footnote 13 for outlier explanation [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

3.2.3. Structural factors as moderators

The hypotheses suggested that inequality and perceived stability of the social system were structural factors that might moderate the association between status and perceived legitimacy. In contrast to the hypothesis tested in the prior paragraph, one hypothesis is that countries with high levels of inequality will have stronger, positive associations between status and perceived legitimacy because it makes status differences more salient. We did not find evidence for this (Figure 9).

For stability, the hypothesis predicted that the link between status and perceived legitimacy would be negative when perceived stability was high compared to low. This hypothesis would be supported with a negative interaction effect. This effect did not emerge (Figure 10a). Instead, we found the opposite interaction effect (Figure 10b). Contrary to the hypothesis, the link between status and perceived legitimacy is strong and positive when stability is high and it is weaker or non‐significant when stability is low.

FIGURE 10.

FIGURE 10

Panel A: Effects of subjective status and stability of the subjective status hierarchy on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of subjective status on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of stability (x‐axis). Panel C: Marginal effect of stability on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of subjective status (x‐axis). For Panels B and C the grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

In short, we do not find evidence that inequality may exacerbate status‐based conflict. We also find that stability is a significant moderator, but in the opposite direction from that expected. It indicates that stability is interpreted differently in the context of real‐life social inequality (see also Verkuyten & Reijerse, 2008), compared to stability that is manipulated in groups created in the lab (e.g., Ellemers et al., 1990). A possible interpretation of the pattern found in this study could be that the perception that social inequality will not change might be an additional reason for judging that inequality is unacceptable (assuming people perceive there to be inequality). At the same time, we note that the reliability of our stability measure could be improved.

3.3. Approach 2: Predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of subjective status

We tested the hypotheses about the predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of subjective status using the same models described and presented above. To assess whether there was support for the hypotheses, we first examined the main effects of the predictors on perceived legitimacy. This main effect analysis tells us whether there is an average effect across the sample. When there is a significant interaction between the predictor and status, we examine whether the predictor still has the expected effect (e.g., a positive effect of social mobility) for people with lower levels of status (regardless of the pattern for those higher in status). This will tell us whether the effect of the predictor is specific to people with low social status or sense of power.

The hypotheses predicted that people would see the system as more legitimate when it fulfilled a social mobility or stability related goal. For social mobility, there was clear support for the hypothesis. There was a large positive effect of social mobility, such that higher levels of perceived social mobility were associated with higher levels of perceived legitimacy (Figure 11a). This positive effect was moderated by subjective status (Figure 11a), suggesting that the positive effect is variable across levels of subjective status. At low levels of subjective status (Figure 11b), perceived social mobility remained a significant positive predictor of perceived legitimacy. This is consistent with the hypothesis.

FIGURE 11.

FIGURE 11

Panel A: Effects of subjective status and social mobility on perceived legitimacy. Covariates are included in the model. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. Panel B: Marginal effect of social mobility on perceived legitimacy (y‐axis) across the range of subjective status (x‐axis). For Panel B, the grey band around the slope is the 95% confidence interval. In all panels a null effect is highlighted with the dashed line

For stability, results were not consistent with the hypothesis. There was a negative effect of stability on perceived legitimacy (Figure 10a). This negative effect was moderated by subjective status (Figure 10a); however, at low levels of subjective status (Figure 10c) the effect of stability remained negative and significant.

In short, when people see social mobility as a possibility, people with low subjective status see the system as more legitimate; however, perceiving the system as more stable is associated with less legitimacy at lower levels of status, which is inconsistent with the hypothesis.

We also made the prediction that when personal or group interests are fulfilled, perceived legitimacy of the system is less necessary. The hypotheses predicted that identification and self‐esteem would be negatively associated with levels of perceived legitimacy for people with low levels of subjective status.

Inconsistent with hypotheses, there are significant positive effects of identification (Figure 4a) and self‐esteem (Figure 5). To see whether this was consistent across people with low subjective status, we examined the interaction effects. The positive effect of self‐esteem was not moderated by subjective status (Figure 5). The positive effect of group identification was moderated by subjective status (Figure 4a). Here, we find that the effect of identification is not different from zero for people who are very low status (Figure 4c). Although this does not confirm the hypothesis, it is also not contrary to it.

4. GENERAL DISCUSSION

Debates around status and perceptions of legitimacy in psychology are characterized by mixed findings and different theoretical foci from different research groups. Consistent with some past work (e.g., Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi‐Cioldi, 2012; Kraus & Callaghan, 2014; Vargas‐Salfate et al., 2018; Zimmerman & Reyna, 2013), but inconsistent with the status‐legitimacy hypothesis (e.g., Henry & Saul, 2006; Jost et al., 2003), we found that subjective status is positively associated with perceived legitimacy. However, this is not our primary contribution. We sought to advance this debate beyond straightforward main effects by taking two approaches.

4.1. Approach 1: Moderator of the association between subjective status and perceived legitimacy

Approach 1 examined how constructs that reduce threat, constructs that increase threat, and perceived structural factors may be moderators of the association between status and perceived legitimacy. We tested eight specific moderator hypotheses and found partial support for one: the identification‐moderation hypothesis (see Table 4). That is, the most common result was no clear support for the hypotheses. Sometimes the lack of support was due to non‐significant interactions. The sensitivity analyses reported in footnotes 9, 11, and 12 indicate that we have substantial power to detect main effects and interactions at the individual level. For country‐level interaction effects, we only had sufficient power to detect large effects. In the cases of null results, studies with even greater statistical power may find evidence in support of these hypotheses, in opposition to these hypotheses, or for a null effect (time will tell). In many other cases, the lack of support was due to a significant interaction that was the opposite of the prediction (e.g., stability‐moderator hypothesis). In these cases (highlighted with ✗op in Table 4), it seems less plausible that the issue was a lack of statistical power. The predictive power of the hypotheses for moderators was essentially nil.

TABLE 4.

Summary of moderation hypotheses (Approach 1) and whether they were supported

Moderator hypothesis
Reduce threat
Identification‐moderation hypothesis
Self‐esteem‐moderation hypothesis
Increase threat through dissonance
Inequality contribution‐moderation hypothesis
Civil liberties hypothesis op
Meritocracy hypothesis
SJT inequality hypothesis
Structural factors that affect threat
SIT inequality hypothesis
Stability‐moderator hypothesis op

✓ = indicates support and partial support for the hypothesis, ✗ = indicates no support for the hypothesis. ✗op = indicates a significant interaction effect in the opposite direction.

Abbreviations: SIT, Social identity theory; SJT, System justification theory.

4.2. Approach 2: Predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with lower levels of subjective status

Approach 2 examined the predictors of perceived legitimacy for people with low subjective status. We tested four specific hypotheses about these predictors and found support for one: the social mobility‐legitimacy hypothesis (see Table 5). Most of the predictions were not confirmed and, in most cases, results were opposite to the predicted direction. As one example, the identification‐legitimacy hypothesis predicted a negative association for members of low status/power groups. Yet, we found the opposite. Some members of our team have started to think through this type of effect (and have published some findings consistent with the data in this manuscript). As explained earlier, they have proposed that among people with low subjective status, identification will be positively associated with system legitimacy, especially when the system can be used as a vehicle for improvements to group status in the long term (Owuamalam, Rubin, & Spears, 2018). Our data lendsome credence to this hypothesis.

TABLE 5.

Summary of predictor hypotheses (Approach 2) and whether they were supported

Predictor hypothesis
Social Mobility‐Legitimacy Hypothesis
Stability‐Legitimacy Hypothesis op
Identification‐legitimacy hypothesis
Self‐esteem legitimacy hypothesis op

✓ = indicates support and partial support for the hypothesis, ✗ = indicates no support for the hypothesis. ✗op = indications a significant interaction effect in the opposite direction.

Although almost never in a direction supportive of the hypotheses (see Table 5), we find that group identification, self‐esteem, and beliefs in social mobility are all associated with greater perceived legitimacy among people with low subjective status. In this way, we helped fulfill Jost’s (2017) call to better understand what leads people who are oppressed to uphold the system.

4.3. Strengths, limitations, and future directions

We used a design that leverages larger samples from multiple countries to understand how subjective status is associated with perceived legitimacy and related constructs. This helps us avoid low statistical power (Sedlmeier & Gigerenzer, 1989) and uses less‐WEIRD sampling (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). That said, hardly any of the samples were representative, and we were not able to recruit participants from all the regions of the globe. Most notably, we were unable to secure data from the continent of Africa, and Asia is underrepresented. However, given the consistency of our results with those of researchers who have used data from even more diverse and representative samples (Brandt, 2013; Caricati, 2017; Caricati & Lorenzi‐Cioldi, 2012; Vargas‐Salfate et al., 2018), we believe that our data provide a reasonable approximation at this time for the regions that we do cover. A related limitation is that although we report the range of Cronbach's α and r for all scales across countries, we have not formally tested measurement invariance (He & van de Vijver, 2012). It is encouraging that some of the scales we use have been shown to be invariant across at least some countries (e.g., Davidov & Coromina, 2013). Clearly, more work is needed on the social psychology of perceived legitimacy and its development in understudied regions; this future work will surely have both practical and theoretical benefits.

Many of our samples are student samples. This allowed us to use our resources efficiently, but necessarily implies that the samples are younger and better educated than the general population. Although studies on similar topics (and social psychology more broadly) have relied on student and non‐representative samples, it is always possible that samples with more objectively and subjectively low status people, or that deliberately recruit people from disadvantaged groups (e.g., homeless shelters, soup kitchens) would reveal different results. That being said, 34% of our sample self‐reported a subjective social status below the midpoint of the scale, indicating that our sample cannot easily be dismissed as a sample consisting only of people who seem themselves as high status. Moreover, our samples included representative (sample NLD3) and community samples similar to those which have been used to study low social status in other work (e.g., MTurk in sample USA1 was used in Plantinga, Krijnen, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2018; Shah, Shafir, & Mullainathan, 2015). Additionally, our student samples are from a diverse array of university types, including public and private universities and community colleges serving a diverse range of students. Lastly, the main effects of status are similar to studies using representative samples (e.g., Brandt, 2013), suggesting that our findings are consistent with data using other sampling techniques.

4.4. Where to now?

Our project provides scholars with additional evidence that they can use to inform the direction of research on status and legitimacy. This additional evidence comes in two forms. First, these hypotheses were inspired by both social identity and system justification theories and primarily made inaccurate predictions in our study. This may be because the prediction was wrong, or because some auxiliary assumptions did not hold in our particular samples and measures. For example, nearly every moderator predicted to reduce feelings of threat or increase feelings of threat through dissonance were not supported. This may mean that these moderators are not associated with threat as expected (auxiliary assumption did not hold), or that threat is not a key mechanism linking status to legitimacy (theoretical prediction was wrong). Learning from theoretical failures and inaccurate predictions like these can often be more informative than learning from theoretical successes (Ferguson & Heene, 2012; Popper, 1959).

It is also plausible that some of the inaccurate predictions (and perhaps most of them) can be addressed by increasing the specificity of the theories and how theoretical concepts are translated into concrete operationalizations. For example, social identity theory has traditionally treated legitimacy as a moderator and used outcome measures such as group evaluations, allocation, and social change strategies. Here, we tried to extend the theory's predictions to legitimacy, but did not find consistent support. This may suggest that social identity theory's predictions about social change strategies do not extend to legitimacy in a straightforward way, which may also help to explain the limited support for predictions derived from social identity theory. Recent work has therefore investigated how a social identity perspective can be better applied to predict system legitimacy (Owuamalam et al., 2019).

Second, our data provide descriptive information about the precisely estimated associations between a large number of socially and politically relevant variables. That is, not only did our study fail to confirm a number of hypotheses about status and legitimacy, but it also provides the relevant associations and non‐associations that relevant theories need to explain. For example, despite expectations of both perspectives, a negative association between status and legitimacy was not found at low levels of identification. Theories need to account for this pattern of results. Similarly, perceived stability was associated with lower levels of legitimacy, despite expectations. Updates will need to account for this pattern of results. Although post‐hoc explanations are possible, we hope that theorists can build clearly specified models that can be used to explain the current data and to make new, falsifiable predictions (e.g., Muthukrishna & Henrich, 2019).

4.5. Conclusion

We started this project because we noted that there is evidence both consistent and inconsistent with the status‐legitimacy hypothesis. Our study found very little support for moderator hypotheses across 30 countries and several moderator variables. To the extent that the phenomenon predicted by the status‐legitimacy hypothesis exists, this appears to be quite rare. That does not mean that people with low subjective status never see the system as relatively legitimate. Our data also shed light on when this is most likely to be the case. We find that group identification, self‐esteem, and beliefs in social mobility are all associated with perceived legitimacy among people who are low status (as well as those with high status). We hope that our findings inspire scholars interested in this domain to pursue questions related to status and legitimacy to enrich our understanding of these constructs around the world.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST

There are no conflicts of interest to report.

TRANSPARENCY STATEMENT

Data, code, and materials are available at the following link: https://osf.io/5uxc7/.

Supporting information

Appendix S1

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

All authors provided feedback on the drafts of the manuscript and helped collect data. Mark J. Brandt, Toon Kuppens, and Russell Spears organized the project, designed the study, and wrote the initial drafts of the manuscript. Julia C. Becker, Matthew J. Easterbrook, Simon M. Laham, Jolanda Jetten, Chuma Kevin Owuamalam, Mark Rubin, Joseph Sweetman, and Jojanneke van der Toorn provided additional feedback in the drafting of the manuscript. This research was supported by Fondecyt program (1161371), the Center for Social Conflict and Cohesion Studies (15130009), the Center for Intercultural and Indigenous Research (15110006), the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement No 759320), and the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (PSI2016‐79971‐P). The contribution of Sylvie Graf was supported by the grant 20‐01214S from the Czech Science Foundation and by RVO: 68081740 of the Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences.

Brandt MJ, Kuppens T, Spears R, et al. Subjective status and perceived legitimacy across countries. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2020;50:921–942. 10.1002/ejsp.2694

Footnotes

1

A related hypothesis predicts that lower power will be associated with higher levels of perceived legitimacy (van der Toorn et al., 2015). Our data also test this hypothesis; however, to sharpen the focus of the manuscript at the request of reviewers we have moved the power related analyses and discussion to supplemental materials.

2

Social identity theory and system justification theory have also both used self‐esteem as an outcome variable. Although this is interesting, it is not the focus of our investigation.

3

Recently, system justification theorists have proposed that a low sense of power, rather than status, is associated with greater perceived legitimacy. Whereas status indicates the amount of prestige and respect a person or group is accorded in the system, power indicates the amount of control a person has over valued resources (Magee & Galinsky, 2008). Trusting and legitimizing outside sources of control (e.g., governments) can help restore people's sense of control, something that is lacking with low feelings of power (van der Toorn et al., 2015; cf. Friesen et al., 2014; Kay et al., 2008). By perceiving the system as legitimate, people with low feelings of power can regain some feelings of control. We therefore simultaneously tested all hypotheses for interpersonal sense of power. Complete results are in the supplemental materials.

4

Six or more years was chosen to ensure that participants who were international or exchange students were not included in the sample.

5

For each scale, alpha or the correlation coefficient (for 2‐item scales) was calculated in each country separately. Mean, standard deviation, and range of alphas and correlation coefficients across countries are reported in the text.

6

The negative α (−.06) for the trust in government scale is found in Russia. The next lowest α is .20 in Lebanon.

7

The negative correlation (−.06) between the stability items is found in Malaysia. The next lowest correlation is .18 in Canada.

8

We also conducted the analyses with only the system justification scale because this scale is perhaps the most well‐developed of the four scales and it was reliable in all of the countries. Conclusions in the main text are essentially unchanged. There are two exceptions. The negative interaction between stability and status was negative and non‐significant and the positive interaction between civil liberties and status was positive and non‐significant when only looking at the system justification scale. Importantly, the sizes of these coefficients were very similar to the coefficients in the models including all legitimacy measures, suggesting that the reduction in error variance when using additional data is the reason for the different conclusions when using all legitimacy measures or only the system justification scale.

9

We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our model and sample size had to detect the effects of subjective status when b = .01, .02, .05, and .10. These analyses showed that we had adequate power to detect effects of at least .02 (i.e. a 2% difference in the outcome variable between the minimum and maximum of the predictor variable; power ≈ 49%, 97%, 100%, 100% respectively).

10

The variance for the intercept at each level of the multilevel models that included covariates was also calculated (Participants σ = .049, Labs σ = .006, Countries σ = .098, Residual σ = .18). Descriptively, there is more variation between countries than there is between labs.

11

We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our model and sample size had to detect individual‐level interaction effects when b = .02, .05, .10, and .20. We used the model with the identification moderator as our base model for these analyses. These analyses showed that we had adequate power to detect moderation effects of at least .10 (i.e., a .10 difference in the unstandardized slope of status/power at the minimum and maximum of the moderator variable; power ≈12%, 46%, 98%, 100% respectively).

12

We used the simr package in R (Green & MacLeod, 2016) to see how much power our model and sample size had to detect country‐level interaction effects for both subjective status and sense of power when b = .01, .02, .05, .10, and .20 We used the model with the meritocracy moderator as our base model for these analyses. These analyses showed that we had adequate power to detect country‐level moderation effects of at least .20 (power ≈6%, 10%, 45%, 87% respectively).

13

To check the robustness of our results to outliers at the country‐level (cf. Ullrich, & Schlüter, 2012), we visually inspected histograms of the country‐level predictors. There were clear outliers for both the Gini index and the measure of civil liberties. These models were run both with and without outliers. All models are in the figures summarizing results and the primary models discussed in the text include all data.

14

See Appendix S1 for data that can inform scholars interested in people's sense of power.

REFERENCES

  1. Adler, N. E. , Epel, E. S. , Castellazzo, G. , & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychology, 19, 586–592. 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiken, L. S. , & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  3. American National Election Studies (2015). Codebook for the time series cumulative data file 1948–2012. Retrieved from http://www.electionstudies.org/studypages/anes_timeseries_cdf/anes_timeseries_cdf_codebook_int.pdf [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates, D. , Mächler, M. , Bolker, B. , & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bettencourt, B. , Charlton, K. , Dorr, N. , & Hume, D. L. (2001). Status differences and in‐group bias: A meta‐analytic examination of the effects of status stability, status legitimacy, and group permeability. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 520–542. 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brandt, M. J. (2013). Do the disadvantaged legitimize the social system? A large‐scale test of the status–legitimacy hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 765–785. 10.1037/a0031751 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self‐esteem and clarity of the self‐concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 538–549. 10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caricati, L. (2017). Testing the status‐legitimacy hypothesis: A multilevel modeling approach to the perception of legitimacy in income distribution in 36 nations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 157, 532–540. 10.1080/00224545.2016.1242472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Caricati, L. , & Lorenzi‐Cioldi, F. (2012). Does status matter? Testing hypotheses from strong form of system justification theory. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 25, 67–95. [Google Scholar]
  10. Davidov, E. , & Coromina, L. (2013). Evaluating measurement invariance for social and political trust in Western Europe over four measurement time points (2002–2008). Ask: Research & Methods, 22, 37–54. [Google Scholar]
  11. Day, M. V. , & Fiske, S. T. (2017). Movin’ on up? How perceptions of social mobility affect our willingness to defend the system. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 267–274. 10.1177/1948550616678454 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Economist Intelligence Unit (2016). Democracy Index 2015. Democracy in an age of anxiety. London, UK: The Economist; Retrieved from https://www.eiu.com/democracy2015 [Google Scholar]
  13. Ellemers, N. , Knippenberg, A. , & Wilke, H. (1990). The influence of permeability of group boundaries and stability of group status on strategies of individual mobility and social change. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 233–246. 10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00902.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Enders, C. K. , & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross‐sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12, 121–138. 10.1037/1082-989X.12.2.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ferguson, C. J. , & Heene, M. (2012). A vast graveyard of undead theories: Publication bias and psychological science’s aversion to the null. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 555–561. 10.1177/1745691612459059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Friesen, J. P. , Kay, A. C. , Eibach, R. P. , & Galinsky, A. D. (2014). Seeking structure in social organization: Compensatory control and the psychological advantages of hierarchy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 590–609. 10.1037/a0035620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gelman, A. (2018). The failure of null hypothesis significance testing when studying incremental changes, and what to do about it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 16–23. 10.1177/0146167217729162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. General Social Survey (2017). General social surveys, 1972–2016: Cumulative Codebook. Retrieved from http://gss.norc.org/documents/codebook/GSS_Codebook.pdf [Google Scholar]
  19. Goodman, E. , Adler, N. E. , Daniels, S. R. , Morrison, J. A. , Slap, G. B. , & Dolan, L. M. (2003). Impact of objective and subjective social status on obesity in a biracial cohort of adolescents. Obesity Research, 11, 1018–1026. 10.1038/oby.2003.140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Green, P. , & MacLeod, C. J. (2016). SIMR: An R package for power analysis of generalized linear mixed models by simulation. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 493–498. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hainmueller, J. , Mummolo, J. , & Xu, Y. (2019). How much should we trust estimates from multiplicative interaction models? Simple tools to improve empirical practice. Political Analysis, 27, 163–192. 10.1017/pan.2018.46 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34–64. [Google Scholar]
  23. He, J. , & van de Vijver, F. (2012). Bias and equivalence in cross‐cultural research. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2, 2307‐919 10.9707/2307-0919.1111 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Henrich, J. , Heine, S. J. , & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Henry, P. J. , & Saul, A. (2006). The development of system justification in the developing world. Social Justice Research, 19, 365–378. 10.1007/s11211-006-0012-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hogg, M. A. (2014). From uncertainty to extremism: Social categorization and identity processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 338–342. 10.1177/0963721414540168 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. International Social Survey Programme (2009). ISSP 2009 Social Inequality IV. Retrieved from https://www.gesis.org/issp/modules/issp‐modules‐by‐topic/social‐inequality/2009/ [Google Scholar]
  28. Jonas, E. , McGregor, I. , Klackl, J. , Agroskin, D. , Fritsche, I. , Holbrook, C. , … Quirin, M. (2014). Threat and defense: From anxiety to approach In Olson J. M. & Zanna M. P. (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 49, pp. 219–286). Cambridge, MA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  29. Jost, J. T. (2017). Working class conservatism: A system justification perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 18, 73–78. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Jost, J. (2019). A quarter century of system justification theory: Questions, answers, criticisms, and societal applications. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58, 263–314. 10.1111/bjso.12297 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jost, J. T. , Banaji, M. R. , & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Jost, J. T. , Gaucher, D. , & Stern, C. (2015). “The world isn’t fair”: A system justification perspective on social stratification and inequality In Dovidio J. F. & Simpson J. A. (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology (vol. 2, pp. 317–340). Washington, DC: APA. [Google Scholar]
  33. Jost, J. , & Hunyady, O. (2003). The psychology of system justification and the palliative function of ideology. European Review of Social Psychology, 13, 111–153. 10.1080/10463280240000046 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jost, J. T. , Liviatan, I. , van der Toorn, J. , Ledgerwood, A. , Mandisodza, A. , & Nosek, B. A. (2011). System justification: How do we know it’s motivated In Ramona Bobocel D., Kay A. C., Zanna M. P. & Olson J. M. (Eds.), The psychology of justice and legitimacy (pp. 173–204). New York, NY: Taylor and Francis. [Google Scholar]
  35. Jost, J. T. , Pelham, B. W. , Sheldon, O. , & Ni Sullivan, B. (2003). Social inequality and the reduction of ideological dissonance on behalf of the system: Evidence of enhanced system justification among the disadvantaged. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 13–36. 10.1002/ejsp.127 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kay, A. C. , Gaucher, D. , Napier, J. L. , Callan, M. J. , & Laurin, K. (2008). God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 18–35. 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.18 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kay, A. C. , & Jost, J. T. (2003). Complementary justice: Effects of “poor but happy” and “poor but honest” stereotype exemplars on system justification and implicit activation of the justice motive. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 823–837. 10.1037/0022-3514.85.5.823 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kraus, M. W. , & Callaghan, B. (2014). Noblesse oblige? Social status and economic inequality maintenance among politicians. PLoS One, 9, e85293 10.1371/journal.pone.0085293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kuppens, T. , & Pollet, T. V. (2014). Mind the level: Problems with two recent nation‐level analyses in psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1110 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kuznetsova, A. , Brockhoff, P. B. , & Christensen, R. H. B. (2016). lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0‐33. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R‐project.org/package=lmerTest [Google Scholar]
  41. Laurin, K. , Gaucher, D. , & Kay, A. (2013). Stability and the justification of social inequality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 246–254. [Google Scholar]
  42. Laurin, K. , Kay, A. C. , & Landau, M. J. (2018). Structure and Goal Pursuit: Individual and cultural differences. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 491–494. 10.1177/2515245918797130 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  43. Li, W. , Yang, Y. , Wu, J. , & Kou, Y. (2020). Testing the status‐legitimacy hypothesis in China: Objective and subjective socioeconomic status divergently predict system justification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 46, 1044–1058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Magee, J. C. , & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Social hierarchy: The self‐reinforcing nature of power and status. Academy of Management Annals, 2, 351–398. [Google Scholar]
  45. Mandisodza, A. N. , Jost, J. T. , & Unzueta, M. M. (2006). “Tall poppies” and “American dreams” reactions to rich and poor in Australia and the United States. Journal of Cross‐Cultural Psychology, 37, 659–668. 10.1177/0022022106292076 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Martorana, P. V. , Galinsky, A. D. , & Rao, H. (2005). From system justification to system condemnation: Antecedents of attempts to change power hierarchies In Thomas‐Hunt M. C. (Ed.), Status and groups (research on managing groups and teams, Vol. 7) (pp. 283–313). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited. [Google Scholar]
  47. McGuire, W. J. (2013). An additional future for psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 414–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McMahon, S. D. , & Watts, R. J. (2002). Ethnic identity in urban African American youth: Exploring links with self‐worth, aggression, and other psychosocial variables. Journal of Community Psychology, 30, 411–431. 10.1002/jcop.10013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. McShane, B. B. , Tackett, J. L. , Böckenholt, U. , & Gelman, A. (2019). Large‐scale replication projects in contemporary psychological research. The American Statistician, 73, 99–105. 10.1080/00031305.2018.1505655 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mummendey, A. , Kessler, T. , Klink, A. , & Mielke, R. (1999). Strategies to cope with negative social identity: Predictions by social identity theory and relative deprivation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 229–245. 10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Muthukrishna, M. , & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(3), 221–229. 10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Oishi, S. , Kesebir, S. , & Diener, E. (2011). Income inequality and happiness. Psychological Science, 22, 1095–1100. 10.1177/0956797611417262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Operario, D. , Adler, N. E. , & Williams, D. R. (2004). Subjective social status: Reliability and predictive utility for global health. Psychology & Health, 19, 237–246. 10.1080/08870440310001638098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Owuamalam, C. K. , Rubin, M. , & Spears, R. (2016). The system justification conundrum: Re‐examining the cognitive dissonance basis for system justification. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1889 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01889 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Owuamalam, C. K. , Rubin, M. , & Spears, R. (2018). Addressing evidential and theoretical inconsistencies in system‐justification theory with a social identity model of system attitudes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 91–96. 10.1177/0963721417737136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Owuamalam, C. K. , Rubin, M. , & Spears, R. (2019). Revisiting 25 years of system motivation explanation for system justification from the perspective of social identity model of system attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58, 362–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Owuamalam, C. K. , Rubin, M. , Spears, R. , & Weerabangsa, M. M. A. (2017). Why do people from low‐status groups support class systems that disadvantage them? A test of two mainstream explanations in Malaysia and Australia. Journal of Social Issues, 73, 80–98. [Google Scholar]
  58. Plantinga, A. , Krijnen, J. M. , Zeelenberg, M. , & Breugelmans, S. M. (2018). Evidence for opportunity cost neglect in the poor. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31, 65–73. 10.1002/bdm.2041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
  60. Proulx, T. , Inzlicht, M. , & Harmon‐Jones, E. (2012). Understanding all inconsistency compensation as a palliative response to violated expectations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 285–291. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; Available from https://www.R‐project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  62. Robins, R. W. , Hendin, H. M. , & Trzesniewski, K. H. (2001). Measuring global self‐esteem: Construct validation of a single‐item measure and the Rosenberg Self‐Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 151–161. 10.1177/0146167201272002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rubin, M. , & Hewstone, M. (2004). Social identity system justification and social dominance: Commentary on Reicher Jost et al., and Sidanius et al. Political Psychology, 25, 823–844. 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00400.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sakurai, K. , Kawakami, N. , Yamaoka, K. , Ishikawa, H. , & Hashimoto, H. (2010). The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Social Science & Medicine, 70, 1832–1839. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Schloerke, B. , Crowley, J. , Cook, D. , Briatte, F. , Marbach, M. , Thoen, E. , … Larmarange, J. (2016). GGally: Extension to ‘ggplot2’. R package version 1.3.0. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R‐project.org/package=GGally [Google Scholar]
  66. Schoel, C. , Bluemke, M. , Mueller, P. , & Stahlberg, D. (2011). When autocratic leaders become an option: Uncertainty and self‐esteem predict implicit leadership preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 521–540. 10.1037/a0023393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sedlmeier, P. , & Gigerenzer, G. (1989). Do studies of statistical power have an effect on the power of studies? Psychological Bulletin, 105, 309–316. 10.1037/0033-2909.105.2.309 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sengupta, N. K. , Osborne, D. , & Sibley, C. G. (2015). The status‐legitimacy hypothesis revisited: Ethnic‐group differences in general and dimension‐specific legitimacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54, 324–340. 10.1111/bjso.12080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Shah, A. K. , Shafir, E. , & Mullainathan, S. (2015). Scarcity frames value. Psychological Science, 26, 402–412. 10.1177/0956797614563958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Solt, F. , & Hu, Y. (2015). Interplot: Plot the effects of variables in interactions terms. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R‐project.org/package=interplot [Google Scholar]
  71. Spears, R. , Jetten, J. , & Doosje, B. (2001). The (il)legitimacy of ingroup bias: From social reality to social resistance In Jost J. & Major B. (Eds.), The psychology of legitimacy: Emerging perspectives on ideology, justice, and intergroup relations (pp. 332–362). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  72. Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social psychology. CUP Archive. [Google Scholar]
  73. Tajfel, H. , & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict In Austin W. G. & Worchel S. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. [Google Scholar]
  74. Trump, K. S. , & White, A. (2018). Does inequality beget inequality? Experimental tests of the prediction that inequality increases system justification motivation. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 5, 206–216. 10.1017/XPS.2018.2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ullrich, J. , & Schlüter, E. (2012). Detecting nasty data with simple plots of complex models: Comment on Brandt (2011). Psychological Science, 23(7), 824–825. 10.1177/0956797611434541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Van der Toorn, J. , Feinberg, M. , Jost, J. T. , Kay, A. C. , Tyler, T. R. , Willer, R. , & Wilmuth, C. (2015). A sense of powerlessness fosters system justification: Implications for the legitimation of authority, hierarchy, and government. Political Psychology, 36, 93–110. 10.1111/pops.12183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Vargas‐Salfate, S. , Paez, D. , Liu, J. H. , Pratto, F. , & Gil de Zúñiga, H. (2018). A comparison of social dominance theory and system justification: The role of social status in 19 nations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 1060–1076. 10.1177/0146167218757455 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Verkuyten, M. , & Reijerse, A. (2008). Intergroup structure and identity management among ethnic minority and majority groups: The interactive effects of perceived stability, legitimacy, and permeability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 106–127. 10.1002/ejsp.395 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  79. World Bank (2016). World Development Indicators. Retrieved from https://data.worldbank.org/data‐catalog/world‐development‐indicators [Google Scholar]
  80. World Values Survey Association (2016). World Values Survey Wave 6 2010‐2014. Retrieved from http://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/WVSDocumentationWV6.jsp [Google Scholar]
  81. Wright, S. C. , Taylor, D. M. , & Moghaddam, F. M. (1990). Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From acceptance to collective protest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 994–1003. 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.994 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zimmerman, J. L. , & Reyna, C. (2013). The meaning and role of ideology in system justification and resistance for high‐and low‐status people. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 1–23. 10.1037/a0032967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Appendix S1


Articles from European Journal of Social Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES