Abstract
Cortical inhibition plays an important role in information processing in the brain. However, the mechanisms by which inhibition and excitation are coordinated to generate functions in the six layers of the cortex remain unclear. Here, we measured laminar-specific responses to stimulus orientations in primary visual cortex (V1) of awake monkeys (male, Macaca mulatta). We distinguished inhibitory effects (suppression) from excitation, by taking advantage of the separability of excitation and inhibition in the orientation and time domains. We found two distinct types of suppression governing different layers. Fast suppression (FS) was strongest in input layers (4C and 6), and slow suppression (SS) was 3 times stronger in output layers (2/3 and 5). Interestingly, the two types of suppression were correlated with different functional properties measured with drifting gratings. FS was primarily correlated with orientation selectivity in input layers (r = −0.65, p < 10−9), whereas SS was primarily correlated with surround suppression in output layers (r = 0.61, p < 10−4). The earliest SS in layer 1 indicates the origin of cortical feedback for SS, in contrast to the feedforward/recurrent origin of FS. Our results reveal two V1 laminar subnetworks with different response suppression that may provide a general framework for laminar processing in other sensory cortices.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT This study sought to understand inhibitory effects (suppression) and their relationships with functional properties in the six different layers of the cortex. We found that the diversity of neural responses across layers in primary visual cortex (V1) could be fully explained by one excitatory and two suppressive components (fast and slow suppression). The distinct laminar distributions, origins, and functional roles of the two types of suppression provided a simplified representation of the differences between two V1 subnetworks (input network and output network). These results not only help to elucidate computational principles in macaque V1, but also provide a framework for general computation of cortical laminae in other sensory cortices.
Keywords: cortical layers, Macaque monkey, neural dynamics, primary visual cortex, suppression
Introduction
The laminar structure of the cerebral cortex is a common anatomic feature in the brain (Schroeder et al., 1998). The cortex has six cell layers with distinct intralaminar and interlaminar connectivity patterns (Lund, 1988; Callaway, 1998; Sincich and Horton, 2005). Consequently, cortical layers have different functional properties (Leventhal et al., 1995; Martinez et al., 2002; Buffalo et al., 2011; Goense et al., 2012; Self et al., 2013; Smith et al., 2013; van Kerkoerle et al., 2017; Bijanzadeh et al., 2018). However, the mechanisms by which different layers gain distinct functions by dynamically combining excitation and inhibition remain unclear (Hirsch and Martinez, 2006; Adesnik and Naka, 2018).
The goal of the current study was to reveal inhibitory effects and their relationships with functional properties throughout the depth of the macaque primary visual cortex (V1) and across V1 laminae. V1 has well-studied local laminar connections (Lund, 1988; Callaway, 1998; Sincich and Horton, 2005), which are assumed to be similar to laminar cortical circuitry in other cortical regions (Schroeder et al., 1998; Schroeder and Foxe, 2002; Linden and Schreiner, 2003). V1 layers 4C and 6, as input layers, receive excitatory drives from LGN and send excitatory signals to V1 output layers, layers 2/3 and 4B, after local intracortical processing. V1 output layers have strong horizontal connections and feedback connections (Rockland and Pandya, 1979; Stettler et al., 2002; Lund et al., 2003). The functional properties of cells in different V1 layers are markedly different (Hawken et al., 1988; Sato et al., 1996; Ringach et al., 2002; Gur et al., 2005; Yeh et al., 2009), reflecting different combinations of layer-specific inhibition and excitation (Xing et al., 2012; Bijanzadeh et al., 2018). Cortical inhibition plays important roles in the functional properties of V1, such as selectivity for stimulus orientation, size, luminance, and spatial frequency (Bredfeldt and Ringach, 2002; Tucker and Fitzpatrick, 2006; Liu et al., 2011; Xing et al., 2011; Adesnik et al., 2012). It has been reported that multiple forms of inhibitory effects exist in V1 (Ringach et al., 2003; Silberberg and Markram, 2007; Lee et al., 2012; Wilson et al., 2012), potentially because of unique neural circuitries (Adesnik et al., 2012; Bijanzadeh et al., 2018). However, the functional roles of inhibitory effects in macaque V1 are still largely unknown, or under debate (Mazer et al., 2002; Shapley et al., 2003; Goris et al., 2015). Unlike previous studies in rodents (Isaacson and Scanziani, 2011; Liu et al., 2011) and cats (Anderson et al., 2000; Martinez et al., 2002; Priebe and Ferster, 2006), directly measuring inhibition in V1 layers in monkey cortex is technically difficult. This difficulty leads to a lack of information about the laminar distribution of inhibition and their relationships with the functional properties of Monkey V1 layers.
To understand the different types of suppression across the layers of macaque V1, we activated V1 by rapidly flashing grating patches at different orientations and simultaneously recorded local field potentials (LFPs) and spiking activity in all layers of awake macaque V1. We then reconstructed the temporal development of orientation selectivity across layers. Benefitting from differences in time course and orientation selectivity between excitation and two types of suppression (Ringach et al., 2003; Xing et al., 2005), we distinguished spatiotemporal responses across V1 layers and found distinct laminar patterns for two types of suppression. To further investigate the functional roles of the two types of suppression, two important functional properties, orientation selectivity (Ringach et al., 2002; Gur et al., 2005) and surround suppression (Shushruth et al., 2009; Henry et al., 2013), were measured using a drifting grating stimulus. Interestingly, the two types of suppression were significantly correlated with orientation selectivity and surround suppression in a laminar-specific manner.
Materials and Methods
Preparation of awake monkeys
All procedures were conducted in compliance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals, and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of Beijing Normal University. Four male adult rhesus monkeys (DD, DY, DQ, and DK, Macaca mulatta, 5-7 years old, 6–8 kg) were used. Under general anesthesia induced with ketamine (10 mg·kg−1) and maintained with isoflurane (1.5%–2.0%), a titanium post was attached to the skull with bone screws for immobilizing the animal's head during behavioral training. After the animal had been trained in a simple fixation task, a circular titanium chamber (20 mm in diameter) with a removable lid was fixed over the craniotomy (15 mm anterior to the occipital ridge and 14 mm lateral from the midline), with dental cement for chronic recordings from V1. Antibiotics and analgesics were used after the surgery.
Behavioral task
A trial began when a monkey began fixating on a 0.1° fixation point (FP) presented on a CRT screen. In each trial, the FP was displayed in the center of the screen. The animal's eye positions were sampled at 120 Hz using an infrared tracking system (ISCAN). Within 300 ms after FP presentation, the animal was required to fixate within an invisible circular window (between 0.6° and 1° in radius) around the FP. After the animal maintained fixation for 100–400 ms (∼200 ms in most cases), the stimulus was displayed for 2–4 s (dependent on stimulus type), followed by a blank interval of 300 ms. The FP then disappeared, and the animal received a drop of water as reward. A trial was aborted if the animal's fixation moved outside the fixation window.
Electrophysiological recording
We simultaneously recorded neuronal activity from different layers in V1 using a linear array (U-probe, Plexon; 24 recording channels spaced 100 μm apart, each 15 μm in diameter). The linear array was controlled by a microelectrode drive (NAN Instruments), and the depth of each probe placement was adjusted to extend through all V1 layers. Raw data were acquired with a 128-channel system (Blackrock Microsystems). The raw data were high-pass filtered (seventh-order Butterworth with 1000 Hz corner frequency), and multiunit spiking activities (MUAs) were detected by applying a voltage threshold with a signal-to-noise ratio of 5.5. Single-unit activities (SUAs) were detected by offline spike sorting. Spike waveforms were carefully verified using custom spike sorting software (Yeh et al., 2009; Xing et al., 2010). Criteria for single units included a fixed shape of the action potential and the absence of spikes during the absolute refractory period. The raw data were also low-pass filtered (seventh-order Butterworth with 300 Hz corner frequency) to obtain LFPs. SUAs, MUAs, and LFPs were all downsampled to 500 Hz.
Visual stimulation
Visual stimuli were generated with a stimulus generator (ViSaGe; Cambridge Research Systems) under the control of a PC running a custom C++ program developed in our laboratory. The stimuli were displayed on a 22-inch CRT monitor (Dell, P1230, 1200 × 900 pixels, mean luminance 45.8 cd/m2, 100 Hz refresh rate). The typical viewing distance was 114 cm (with seven exceptions: the viewing distance was 57 cm for six recording session and 80 cm for one session). Three types of stimuli were used. Sparse noise was used to simultaneously map receptive fields (RFs). Random orientation presentation was used to measure orientation dynamics, align laminar positions, and check the verticality of the probe. Drifting grating stimuli were used to measure surround suppression and orientation selectivity.
RF mapping
After manually mapping the RFs of recording channels, we used sparse noise (Jones and Palmer, 1987) to identify the precise RF center. The sparse noise consisted of a sequence of randomly positioned (usually on a 13 × 13 or 11 × 11 sample grid) dark and bright squares (0.1°-0.3°, contrast 0.9°) against a gray background (luminance 45.8 cd/m2). Each sparse noise image appeared for 20 ms and with at least 50 repetitions. The sequence was cut into small segments based on trial length. We obtained a two-dimensional map of each channel. Responses averaged from x and y axes of each map were fitted with a one-dimensional Gaussian function to estimate the center position and radius of each RF (σ of Gaussian function). RFs were located within 5° of the fovea.
Orientation dynamics
After the RF mapping experiment, a sequence of random flashed gratings with different orientations (random orientation experiment) was used to measure dynamic responses to orientations. Sinusoidal gratings of 18 different orientations equally spaced from 0° to 180°, plus “blanks” (defined as uniform frames with the same luminance as the mean luminance of the grating images; 10% or 20% of all stimuli) were used. For each orientation, the spatial phase was also varied: each orientation in the set was presented at eight different spatial phases, equally spaced from 0° to 360°. The size of the grating was 0.5°–2.5° in radius (at least 4 times larger than the RF of layer 4Cα for most probe placements, in 9 probe placements, the stimulus size was set at 2.5–4 times larger than the RF size of 4Cα), fixed within each session. We set the stimulus sizes to be at least 2.5 times larger than RFs of recorded sites to activate both local and global neural mechanisms in V1. Other parameters (2 cycles/deg for spatial frequency and 90% for contrast) of the gratings were fixed in all sessions. All of the gratings and blanks were randomly chosen and consisted of a sequence. Each stimulus in a sequence was randomly chosen and flashed for 20 ms with at least 50 repetitions (repetition varies from 50 to 300 between recording sessions). The sequence was cut into small segments based on trial length (2.2–4 s, with 110–200 stimuli). Each trial displayed one segment until all segments were used. Figure 1A illustrates the reverse correlation method in the orientation domain (Ringach et al., 1997; Dragoi et al., 2002; Xing et al., 2005, 2011). The dynamic response of each site was smoothed with a rectangular window filter with a width of 20 ms (10 time points). We were then able to calculate the orientation tuning of each channel at different times relative to stimulus onset (see Fig. 1B). We used the stimulus-driven energy ratio (SER) to select visually driven sites. To define the SER, we calculated the energy of all orientations at different time delays as Energy(θ,t) = Resp(θ,t)2. We then averaged all orientations and defined the peak time as the time delay at which the energy reached its maximum. The SER was then calculated as the maximum energy divided by the mean energy before stimulus onset (−20 to 0 ms). SUAs and MUAs with SER >30 were used for further analysis.
Orientation tuning curves measured with drifting gratings
Orientation tuning curves were measured with drifting sinusoidal gratings in 24 probe placements (MUA; DD, with 15 probe placements and 148 sites; DY, with 6 probe placements and 66 sites; DQ, with 3 probe placements and 30 sites). Gratings were presented for 2 s, and response was the mean firing rate during this period. Orientation was varied over a range of 360° in steps of 15° or 20°. Each randomized orientation was presented at least 5 times (∼10 repeats in most cases). The size of the grating was 0.5°–2.5° in radius (at least 2 times larger than the RF of layer 4Cα), fixed within each recording session. The temporal frequencies of the drifting grating were 4.17 or 5. Other parameters (2 cycles/deg for spatial frequency and 90% for contrast) of the gratings were fixed in all sessions. Spontaneous firing rates were measured with a uniform screen of the same mean luminance as that of the grating stimuli.
Surround suppression measured with drifting gratings
After measuring the optimal orientation for each probe placement, we measured the size tuning by varying the radius of the stimulus patch from 0.015° to 6° for a sinusoidal grating (2 cycles/deg for spatial frequency and 90% for contrast) in 15 probe placements (MUA; DD, with 9 probe placements and 74 sites; DQ, with 4 probe placements and 43 sites; DK, with 2 probe placements and 24 sites). The temporal frequencies of the drifting grating were 4.17 or 5. The center of the stimulus was placed at the center of the RF. Each stimulus was presented for 2 s, with 10 repeats. The size tuning of each site was fitted with the difference of two Naka–Rushton functions (Naka and Rushton, 1966). Surround suppression was computed according to the following formula: surround suppression = [1 − (Rlarg/Ropt)], where Ropt and Rlarg denote the responses elicited by the optimal and largest radius stimuli, respectively. Thus, sites showing no suppression to large radius stimuli would have a surround suppression of 0, whereas those showing total response suppression would have a surround suppression of 1.
Laminar alignment
To align different probe placements in depth, we used the laminar pattern of MUA responses combined with current source density (CSD) analysis (Mitzdorf and Singer, 1979; Schroeder et al., 1998) of LFP signals. The MUAs and CSDs across laminar channels were measured during the presentation of random orientations. We averaged the responses in all stimulus conditions and calculated the MUA and CSD laminar patterns of every probe placement. We then summarized common signatures to guide laminar alignment. Because the thickness of the cortex and verticality of the probe differed between probe placements, we assigned the recording site of each channel to a relative depth (ReD) (Hawken et al., 1988). The ReD is the normalized cortical depth, ranging from 0 to 1. The boundaries between layers as a function of ReD were estimated based on previous anatomic (Lund, 1988; Callaway, 1998) and electrophysiological studies (Ringach et al., 2002; Yeh et al., 2009; Xing et al., 2012). Three signatures were used to calculate ReD. First, the CSD was smoothed in the cortical space. The location of the earliest current sink of CSDs (Cha1) was then referred to as the middle of layer 4Cα (Mitzdorf and Singer, 1979; Maier et al., 2011). We defined the ReD value of this signature (ReDs1) as 0.49. The response location with the earliest MUA responses was also calculated to define layer 4Cα for some probe placements (Maunsell and Gibson, 1992) that exhibited a blurry CSD pattern. For most probe placements, the location detected from MUA was the same as that for CSD. Second, half a channel above the uppermost channel (Cha2) exhibiting visually driven spiking responses (SER > 3.5, and its lower three continuous channels also met the condition of SER > 3.5) referred to as the boundary of cortex and pia mater was set as ReDs2 = 0). Third, the polarity inversion accompanied by the sink-source configuration (Cha3) was referred to as the boundary of layer 5 and 6 (ReDs3 = 0.81). This signature can be found in previous studies (Mitzdorf and Singer, 1979; Self et al., 2013), and was easy to detect in our data. After the three signatures were detected, we used the three signature pairs (Cha1 and Cha2; Cha1 and Cha3; Cha2 and Cha3) to calculate ReD between adjacent channels (ReDinter) as follows:
The ReD of every channel can be calculated from ReDinter and one of the ReD values of the signatures as follows:
We then compared laminar patterns of MUA using the three ReDs calculated from different signature pairs, and selected the ReD with minimum difference from two standards. One is the difference between ReD of earliest response channel with the center of layer 4Cα (ReD = 0.49). The other was the difference between ReD of the latest response channel with center of layer 5 (ReD = 0.74). The criterion of selection was based on previous studies of the laminar pattern in macaque V1 (Maunsell and Gibson, 1992; Xing et al., 2012; van Kerkoerle et al., 2017).
Measure orientation selectivity
The orientation tuning curves were shifted so that their preferred orientation was 0° (see Fig. 1B). We then fitted tuning curves with the von Mises function (Khatri and Mardia, 1977) and used the fitted tuning curves (spaced from −90° to 90°, at 1° intervals) to estimate two aspects of orientation selectivity, as follows. We found the peak response (Rpref) in the fitted curve; its orientation was defined as the preferred orientation. The responses to the orientation of 90° on either side of its preferred orientation were defined as the orthogonal response (Rorth). We computed the ratio Rorth/Rpref, which was defined as the ratio of orthogonal responses and preferred responses (O/P ratio) (Gegenfurtner et al., 1996; Ringach et al., 2002). We also subtracted Rorth and found the points on both sides of the peak at which the responses were half of the peak response. Half of the distance between the two points was defined as the bandwidth (Campbell et al., 1968; Rose and Blakemore, 1974). The fitting goodness was quantified to select reliable sites for subsequent analysis. We defined fitting reliability (FitR) as 1 minus the ratio of fitting residual and total variation of the data as follows:
Only sites with FitR >0.6 were used to analyze bandwidth.
Model fitting and evaluation
To dissect the excitation and two types of suppression that underlie orientation selectivity, we fitted a three-component model to the dynamic responses of each recorded channel; the experimental data contained 2718 data points (18 orientations × 151 time points). Parameters (αE, αSS, KE, KSS, θE, θSS, ET(τ), FST(τ) and SST(τ); τ from −50 to 250 ms, 2 ms interval) were searched to minimize L under constraints P by the MATLAB function “fmincon” as follows:
To evaluate the goodness of fit of the model, we defined the goodness of fit G described by the following equation:
The fitting error in Figure 3 is described by the following:
Statistics
All error bars and measures of dispersion represent mean ± SEM. All p values were two-tailed.
Results
With a linear array (U-probe, 24 channels, 100 μm between adjacent channels), we simultaneously recorded the spiking activity and LFP evoked by grating patches presented at different orientations throughout the depth of V1 (Fig. 1A). SUA was isolated using offline spike sorting. We cross-correlated (also called reverse correlation, or spike-triggered average; see Materials and Methods) neural activity (SUA, MUA, and LFP) with stimulus orientations and calculated the dynamics of orientation tuning (Fig. 1A,B; see Materials and Methods). Based on the stimulus-driven MUA patterns (Fig. 1C) and the CSD patterns of visually evoked LFP (Fig. 1D), we defined the borders of adjacent cortical layers and aligned relative cortical depth for these channels (Materials and Methods). The MUA and CSD patterns for different probe placements are very similar, and they are similar to averaged MUA and CSD patterns, supporting the precise alignment of cortical depth and the assignment for cortical layers (Fig. 1C,D). Based on the aligned cortical depth of channels in each probe placement, we constructed the temporal development of orientation tuning.
Temporal development of orientation selectivity and its laminar variation
For a given probe placement, we shifted the preferred orientation of MUA responses from each channel to 0°, producing a spatiotemporal pattern for temporal development of orientation tuning across V1 layers (Fig. 2A). Such a spatiotemporal pattern shows a clear temporal order of response onset from input layers (L4C and L6) to output layers (L2/3, L4B, and L5) and distinct dynamics of orientation tuning clustered within each cortical layer. The temporal order of neural responses and laminar clustering of tuning dynamics for all probe placements were similar to each other, similar to the averaged laminar pattern of the orientation tuning, and consistent between 2 macaque monkeys (Fig. 2B,C for 2 Monkeys DD and DY), which again confirmed the depth alignment and laminar assignment. The dynamic response patterns for SUA (Fig. 2D) and MUA (Fig. 2B,C) were also similar to each other (Fig. 2E-J for individual SUA sites). To investigate the laminar pattern of orientation dynamics at a finer scale and with a better signal-to-noise ratio, we mainly analyzed MUA responses. Because the laminar patterns of MUA response dynamics were consistent between the 2 monkeys, all of the subsequent results are presented by combining data from the 2 monkeys (Monkey 1, DD, with 27 probe placements and 293 sites; Monkey 2, DY, with 9 probe placements and 114 sites).
Two important dynamic features can be observed in the neural responses from single recorded sites (Figs. 1, 2) as well as in population-averaged responses (Figs. 1, 2, and the second column of Fig. 3). The first feature is that the response to the nonpreferred orientation (Rorth) decays faster than the response of the preferred orientation (Rpref). Moreover, the decay of Rorth can go below the baseline at ∼50-60 ms in input layers. The suppressive feature is also clear for individual Sites 12 and 14 shown in Figure 1B (also see blue regions at nonpreferred orientations in layer 4C and 6 at ∼50-60 ms in Fig. 2A,D). We can see that Rorth (blue dots) in the two example sites decays below the baseline from 54 to 64 ms. The other important feature for response dynamics is that Rpref peak negatively at ∼100 ms after stimulus onset (see the dark blue regions around preferred orientation in population responses of L2/3 and L5 in Fig. 2C and the second column of Fig. 3). The late suppression of Rpref is also clear for individual Site 6 in Figure 1B (red dots around 94 and 114 ms) which is in L2/3. The two features, early negative Rorth in L4C/6 and late negative Rpref in L2/3 and L5, strongly indicate that two suppressive mechanisms with different time course are involved in the neural responses in V1.
Excitation and two types of suppression fully explain the V1 laminar response pattern
To distinguish excitation and the two suppressive mechanisms in different layers of Monkey V1, we modified a three-component model (Eqs. 1–6) (Xing et al., 2005, 2011) with one excitation and two types of suppression to fit the data from awake monkey.
In the three-component model (Eqs. 1–6), we assumed that the dynamic responses to stimulus orientations (R(θ,τ) in the second column of Fig. 3) were a linear combination of three components (Eq. 1): one excitatory component (E(θ,τ) in the fourth column of Fig. 3) and two suppressive components (a fast suppression component, FS(θ,τ), in the fifth column of Fig. 3 and a slow suppression component, SS(θ,τ), in the sixth column of Fig. 3). R, E, FS, and SS are all functions of stimulus orientations and time; and E, FS, and SS are assumed to be orientation and time separable, meaning that the function of orientation and time for E, FS, or SS can be simplified as the product of a function of time, XT(τ), and a function of orientation, VX(θ) (X = E, FS, or SS, in Eqs. 2–4). The orientation tuning of FS was flat, and the orientation tuning functions for E and SS were independent von Mises functions (Khatri and Mardia, 1977) plus an orientation-independent term α (Eqs. 5, 6). The orientation tuning functions VE(θ) and VSS(θ) are independent of each other.
The three-component model did a very good job to explain the dynamic responses to different stimulus orientations in different cortical layers both at the population-averaged level (Fig. 3, explained variance > 99%, fitting error < 1% for all layers) and at the level of individual sites (Fig. 4; for most MUA recording sites, 395 of 407, explained >86% of variance, with a rate of fitting error of <13%; mean ± SEM; goodness of fit, 0.947 ± 0.002, N = 407). These findings support the idea that V1 responses can be fully explained by one excitatory and two suppressive mechanisms.
To check whether the two suppressive mechanisms are necessary, we also tested an alternative hypothesis: V1 responses can be simply explained by an orientation-tuned excitation without any suppression, which is against the idea that V1 responses are mainly governed by an excitation and two types of suppression. We fitted the dynamic response with a feedforward model with only one excitation tuned to stimulus orientations. The goodness of fit for the feedforward model was significantly lower than that for the three-component model (mean ± SEM for goodness of fit; feedforward model, 0.778 ± 0.006; three-component model, 0.947 ± 0.002; N = 407, paired t test, p < 0.001). The proportion of sites with high goodness of fit (higher than 0.86) for the three-component model was much larger than the feedforward model (395 of 407, 97.1% for three-component model; 109 of 407, 26.8% for feedforward model). The good performance of our three-component model suggests that dynamic responses in all V1 layers were mainly governed by three neural mechanisms with distinct laminar distribution and neural dynamics, and probably different neural bases (Fig. 4).
FS and SS have distinct laminar variations
As shown in Figures 3 and 4, FS and SS exhibited marked differences in laminar distribution, dynamic properties, and orientation selectivity. FS was strongest in the input layers (L4Cα, L4Cβ, and L6; Fig. 5A,B; Table 1), whereas SS was strongest in the output layers (L2/3 and L5; Fig. 5C,D; Table 1). As their definitions suggest, FS was only slightly slower than excitation (Fig. 5E,F; Table 1), whereas SS was much slower than FS (Fig. 5E,F; Table 1). Interestingly, SS, on average, exhibited the shortest latency in the superficial layer close to layer 1 (Fig. 5C,E,F). SS in layer 2/3 and 5 was tuned to stimulus orientation but was weaker than the tuning of excitation in the same layers (Fig. 5G,H; mean ± SEM; L2/3, O/P ratio of excitation: 0.18 ± 0.02; SS: 0.40 ± 0.02; N = 99, paired t test, p < 0.0001; bandwidth of excitation: 23.45 ± 1.07 deg; SS: 32.86 ± 1.14 deg; N = 47, paired t test, p < 0.0001; L5, O/P ratio of excitation: 0.39 ± 0.03; SS: 0.52 ± 0.03; N = 49; paired t test, p < 0.0001; bandwidth of excitation: 30.77 ± 2.20 deg; SS: 30.14 ± 2.82 deg; N = 11; paired t test, p = 0.86; for details of the measurement of selectivity, see Materials and Methods).
Table 1.
FS index |
SS index |
Relative latency of FS |
Relative latency of SS |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean ± SEM | No. of sites | Mean ± SEM | No. of sites | Mean ± SEM (ms) | No. of sites | Mean ± SEM (ms) | No. of sites | |
L2/3 | 0.23±0.02 | 115 | 0.28±0.01 | 115 | 20.51±1.66 | 82 | 44.70±1.48 | 112 |
L4B | 0.46±0.04 | 29 | 0.17±0.02 | 29 | 18.07±3.00 | 27 | 46.32±3.62 | 25 |
L4Cα | 0.72±0.02 | 68 | 0.07±0.01 | 68 | 11.35±0.44 | 68 | 60.32±6.05 | 31 |
L4Cβ | 0.47±0.02 | 57 | 0.10±0.01 | 57 | 21.31±1.30 | 55 | 61.03±5.10 | 31 |
L5 | 0.39±0.03 | 63 | 0.23±0.02 | 63 | 18.25±1.64 | 55 | 46.81±2.18 | 54 |
L6 | 0.59±0.03 | 52 | 0.12±0.01 | 52 | 14.94±1.25 | 51 | 58.82±5.73 | 34 |
aThe FS and SS index was defined as the maximum value of mean strength. The latency was defined as the time at which each component first reached 2 × SDs of baseline fluctuations (−20 ms to 10 ms of raw dynamic response). Relative latency was defined as the latency difference between FS and excitation (latency of FS minus latency of excitation) and SS and excitation (latency of SS minus latency of excitation).
The results described in this section show that laminar processing in V1 can be simplified into two stages, which included an input layers (L4C) stage with strong FS and an output layers (L2/3) stage with strong SS (Fig. 6A). The segregated distribution of two types of suppression raise a key question: what is the function of the two distinct types of suppression for laminar processing?
SS is related to surround suppression in V1 output layers
One of the canonical cortical functions in V1 is surround suppression, indicating the ability of V1 to integrate spatial context (Allman et al., 1985). V1 neurons respond best to stimuli of optimal size falling on the cell's RF, and are suppressed by stimuli larger than the optimal stimulus size. To elucidate the relationship between the two suppression types and surround suppression, patches of drifting gratings of increasing size centered over the RF (Fig. 6A) were used in 15 probe placements to estimate surround suppression (for details, see Materials and Methods). For most recording sites in V1, there was surround suppression when stimulus size changed from the optimal size to a larger size (for an example site, see Fig. 6B).
Interestingly, only SS in the output layer was strongly positively correlated with surround suppression (L2/3; Fig. 6F; r = 0.61, p < 10−4, N = 40). The correlation between FS and surround suppression in the output layer was weak and not significant (Fig. 6E; r = −0.23, p = 0.16, N = 40). Neither FS nor SS correlated with surround suppression in input layer (L4C; N = 41; Fig. 6C for FS and surround suppression, r = −0.14, p = 0.39; Fig. 6D for SS and surround suppression, r = −0.11, p = 0.48). The significant positive correlation between surround suppression (measured using a drifting grating) and SS (estimated using a flashed grating) suggests that SS, but not FS, may participate in spatial context processing and caused the enhancement of surround suppression in laminar processing in the output layer of V1.
To be noticed, there is also moderate surround suppression in input layers measured with drifting gratings (Fig. 6C,D) (Solomon et al., 2002; Alitto and Usrey, 2008; Henry et al., 2013). However, we do not see significant correlation between the weak SS and the moderate surround suppression in input layers. It is possible that the weak SS in the input layers also represents surround mechanism participating in spatial context processing, but the surround activated by rapidly presented stimuli was weaker than the surround activated by drifting gratings; therefore, we do not see any correlation between surround suppression and the SS in V1 input layer.
FS improves orientation selectivity in V1 input layers
We next determined how the two suppression types participate in orientation processing. Several previous studies reported that broadly tuned suppression plays a major role in the enhancement of orientation selectivity (Sillito, 1975; Ringach et al., 2003; Liu et al., 2011; Xing et al., 2011). However, other studies found that inhibitory tuning is as narrow as excitatory tuning, and concluded that inhibition cannot sharpen orientation tuning (Ferster, 1986; Anderson et al., 2000; Tan et al., 2011). The current results revealed that FS and SS in macaque V1 were both broadly tuned, indicating a possible contribution to orientation selectivity. The functional differences between the two types of suppression in orientation processing and the ways in which they modulated orientation information in different layers are considered in more depth below.
To determine how orientation selectivity relates to inhibitory and excitatory mechanisms, we also measured orientation tuning curves with drifting sinusoidal gratings as stimuli, with 24 probe placements (for details, see Materials and Methods; Fig. 7A), as shown for two example MUA sites in Figure 7B, C. The orientation selectivity was defined as the O/P ratio (see Materials and Methods; for all individual sites and laminar distribution, see Fig. 7D,E). The O/P ratio of excitation (measured using a flashed grating stimulus) was positively correlated with the O/P ratio (measured by drifting grating, mean ± SEM of O/P ratio; for L2/3, 0.255 ± 0.037, N = 76; for L4B, 0.228 ± 0.033, N = 22; for L4Cα, 0.356 ± 0.032, N = 35; for L4Cβ, 0.689 ± 0.032, N = 40; for L5, 0.464 ± 0.035, N = 40; for L6, 0.288 ± 0.057, N = 29) in both input layers (Fig. 7F; L4C; r = 0.60, p < 10−7, N = 75) and the output layer (Fig. 7I; L2/3; r = 0.66, p < 10−9, N = 76). However, in the input layer, the O/P ratio was also significantly negatively correlated with the strength of FS (Fig. 7G; L4C; r = −0.65, p < 10−9, N = 75). In the output layer, FS and the O/P ratio were not significantly correlated (Fig. 7J; L2/3; r = 0.12, p = 0.30, N = 76). In contrast to FS, the correlation between SS and the O/P ratio was weak in the input layers (Fig. 7H for L4C, r = 0.38, p < 10−3, N = 75) and not significant in the output layers (Fig. 7K for L2/3, r = −0.13, p = 0.26, N = 76). Overall, we found that FS made a laminar-specific contribution to orientation selectivity. Our results are consistent with the notion that FS enhances orientation selectivity at the input layers of V1 by reducing neural responses to orthogonal orientation.
The output layer inherits the effects of FS from the input layer
Different from results in the input layer (L4C), orientation selectivity in the output layer (L2/3) was only significantly correlated with the selectivity of excitation (Fig. 7I-K), indicating that excitation but not suppression played a major role in L2/3. Although it is currently unclear how excitation in L2/3 is generated, several sources could be involved, including feedforward excitation from L4C, recurrent excitation within L2/3, and recurrent excitation between L2/3 and L5 (Lund, 1988; Callaway, 1998). Because of the diversity of the sources of excitation, whether the selectivity of L2/3 can directly benefit from FS generated in L4C remains an open question. If feedforward excitation plays an important role, FS in L4C can indirectly enhance orientation selectivity in L2/3, and there will be a correlation between the strength of FS in L4C and selectivity in L2/3. In contrast, if recurrent excitation is the dominant determinant of selectivity in L2/3, FS in L4C and orientation selectivity of L2/3 would be expected to be uncorrelated.
To further investigate the functional implications of our findings, we investigated the relationship between FS of L4C and orientation selectivity of L2/3. For each site of L2/3 (N = 76), we averaged FS of all simultaneously recorded L4C sites. The O/P ratio of L2/3 exhibited a significant negative correlation with the averaged FS of L4C (Fig. 8A; r = −0.37, p = 0.001, N = 76). To further examine this effect, we separated L2/3 sites into weak and strong populations based on the simultaneously measured strength of FS in L4C (N = 30 for each population). A strong FS population exhibited sharpened orientation tuning with reduced orthogonal response compared with a weak FS population (Fig. 8B). These results indicate that the effects of FS in the input layer can be directly inherited by the output layer through feedforward circuitry, and that these inherited suppressive effects can shape selectivity of output layer.
Laminar variation of orientation preference
In addition to the neural mechanisms for variations of orientation selectivity across V1 layers, several studies (Bauer et al., 1980, 1983; Bauer and Dow, 1989) have reported that preferred orientations at different layers within a column perpendicular to V1 surface can be substantially different, especially between upper/middle layers (L2/3/4C) and lower layers (L5/6). We also checked whether the shift of preferred orientation between layer 4C and layer 5/6 are different from the orientation shift between layer 4C and layer 2/3 in our dataset.
For each probe placement, we estimated both the preferred orientation and the center position of the RF for each recorded site (see examples in Fig. 9). Most of our probe placements are perpendicular to V1 surface, according to the mean distance of all pairs of RF centers in each probe placement (MCD). The mean value of MCDs, 0.06 degree in visual angle (Fig. 10A,D for individual sites), is much smaller than the mean size of the V1 RFs (0.3° in diameter). There is no significant difference (Fig. 10E,F; paired t test, N = 36, p = 0.98) between mean shift of RF centers in layer 2/3 and those in layer 5/6 (both relative to RF centers in layer 4C).
We analyzed orientation preferences in the way similar for the analysis on RF centers. The mean difference of orientation preference (MPD) is 20.48° (Fig. 10B). Orientation preference shifts is significant correlated with RF center shifts (Fig. 10C; r = 0.511, N = 47, p = 0.0002). We used RF center shifts (Fig. 10A) as a criterion to select perpendicular probe placements. There are shifts of orientation preferences for layer 2/3 and layer 5/6 relative to those in layer 4C (Fig. 10G). However, for probe placements perpendicular to V1 surface, judged by RF center shift (Fig. 10A; center shifts are <0.08°), the shift of orientation preferences in layer 2/3 and layer 5/6 is not significantly different (Fig. 10H,I; mean ± SEM; for L2/3 relative to L4C, 21.53 ± 4.22 deg; for L5/6 relative to L4C, 23.36 ± 3.18 deg; paired t test, N = 30, p = 0.72). We further compared the shift of orientation preferences in L2/3 and those in L5/6 with stricter criteria for selecting perfectly perpendicular probe placements (MCD is <0.04°, 0.05°, 0.06°, and 0.07°), and we did not find significant differences in any condition (Fig. 11). Our results are consistent with the notion that orientation preferences change in a smooth fashion within V1 column.
Discussion
Our experimental results provide a complete picture of the temporal dynamics of orientation selectivity across macaque V1 layers (Fig. 2). Based on the temporal dynamics of orientation selectivity, we distinguished one excitatory and two suppressive components that collaboratively process visual information across V1 layers (Figs. 3, 4). The two suppressive components, FS and SS, exhibited distinct laminar distributions and caused diversity of neural dynamic responses to stimulus orientations (Figs. 5, 12A). We further investigated how suppressive mechanisms contribute to orientation and spatial context processing by laminar circuitry (summarized in Fig. 12B). Laminar processing in V1 can be simplified into two stages (input layers and output layers). FS largely modulated tuned excitation by reducing the excitation for nonpreferred orientations in the input layer, whereas SS contributed to integrating spatial context and was correlated with surround suppression in the output layer. Together, these results suggest that two suppressive mechanisms have distinct laminar distributions and play different functional roles in macaque V1.
FS and SS in V1 layers
Based on the laminar distributions and time courses of fast and SS, we believe that these two types of suppression arise from different neural circuitries. FS is likely to be a local process because it is only slightly slower than excitation, with a 10 ms time delay (Figs. 4, 5E,F) and is located in layers with more local connections. SS is likely to be because of long-range connections or feedback because it is substantially slower than FS (∼40 ms time delay compared with excitation and FS; Fig. 5E,F) and is mainly located in the output layers, which contain large numbers of unique horizontal (Stettler et al., 2002; Lund et al., 2003) and feedback connections (Rockland and Pandya, 1979; Stettler et al., 2002). Interestingly, the earliest SS appeared in the superficial layer close to layer 1 (Fig. 5C,E,F). Our results regarding SS, based on spike activity, were highly similar to those reported in a recent study (Bijanzadeh et al., 2018), based on LFP and CSD, which suggested that SS could be because of feedback connections. In addition, the two types of suppression may be governed by different neurotransmitter receptors. The earlier latency and shorter duration of FS indicate that the suppression is related to GABAA receptors with fast and transient synaptic function (Marienhagen et al., 1997). GABAB, with slow and sustained synaptic properties (Marienhagen et al., 1997), may contribute to SS. Coincidentally, the laminar distribution of SS in our results is similar to the laminar distribution of GABAB density in V1 (Eickhoff et al., 2007).
Neural mechanisms for orientation selectivity
Orientation selectivity is an important function in V1 for understanding cortical computational principles (Hubel and Wiesel, 1968; Priebe and Ferster, 2012). Several neural mechanisms, including the organization of excitatory LGN input (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Reid and Alonso, 1995), cortical inhibition, nonlinear transduction of membrane potential to spiking activity (Gardner et al., 1999; Tan et al., 2011), and recurrent excitation (Somers et al., 1995; Hansel and Sompolinsky, 1996) have been proposed to enhance orientation selectivity. However, previous theories related to these neural mechanisms have been controversial and mutually exclusive. A recent study used a computational model to distinguish three mechanisms (excitation, inhibition, and nonlinearity), supporting the notion that excitatory input is the most important mechanism contributing to selectivity and its variation in V1 (Goris et al., 2015). Our results revealed that, in addition to excitation, inhibition also plays important roles and neural mechanisms for orientation selectivity might be layer-specific. The relationships between inhibitory effects (FS) and orientation selectivity differ between input and output layers (Fig. 7), suggesting that there are layer-specific mechanisms for orientation selectivity in V1. In input layers (L4C), both FS and excitation relate to selectivity (Fig. 7F,G); but in output layers (L2/3), only excitation relates to selectivity (Fig. 7I,J). More importantly, although orientation selectivity of L2/3 is seemingly only governed by excitation, we found that FS in L4C was also significantly correlated with selectivity of L2/3. The effects of FS in L4C can be inherited by L2/3 and contribute to selectivity of L2/3.
Laminar variation of orientation selectivity
In addition to the overall variation of orientation selectivity for SUA and MUA in V1, differences of mean orientation selectivity across V1 layers (laminar variation of orientation selectivity) were reported by studies in various species, including macaque monkey (Livingstone and Hubel, 1984; Ringach et al., 2002; Gur et al., 2005), cat (Martinez et al., 2002), mouse (Niell and Stryker, 2008), rat (Girman et al., 1999), and tree shrew (Chisum et al., 2003). The mechanisms underlying this laminar variation remain unclear (Hirsch and Martinez, 2006). A previous study by Martinez et al. (2002) found that the relative tuning of excitation and inhibition changed with laminar variation, providing a mechanistic view of laminar variation in cat V1. Given the fundamental differences in the neuronal mechanisms underlying orientation selectivity between species (Bosking et al., 1997; Ohki et al., 2005; Hirsch and Martinez, 2006; Scholl et al., 2013), it is still important to understand the mechanisms underlying laminar variation of orientation selectivity in macaque V1.
We also found that mean value of O/P ratio in each V1 layer varies (Fig. 7E). Interestingly, the significant differences of O/P ratio among V1 layers are mostly from comparisons between L4Cβ, a sublayer of L4C, and other V1 layers. The O/P ratio in L4Cβ is significantly larger than all other layers (one-way ANOVA test, p < 0.01 for difference between L4Cβ and all other layers). Surprisingly, the O/P ratio in L4Cα, the upper half of L4C next to L4Cβ, is only significantly lower than that in L4Cβ, but not different from that in any other layer (one-way ANOVA test, p < 0.01 for the difference between L4Cα and L4Cβ; p > 0.05 for difference between L4Cα and all other layers except L4Cβ).
Our finding is consistent with previous studies in macaque monkey (Ringach et al., 2002; Gur et al., 2005), but it is very contrary to the general impression that orientation selectivity of input layer (L4C) is low or nonexistent in macaque V1 and orientation selectivity in L2/3 is much better than that in L4C. We think the results can be explained by the laminar variation of excitation and suppression in V1 layers revealed by the current study. First, the L4Cα and L4Cβ substantially differ for their O/P ratio, because FS in L4Cα is stronger than that in L4Cβ (Fig. 5A,B; mean ± SEM; FS for L4Cα, 0.72 ± 0.02, N = 68; FS for L4Cβ, 0.47 ± 0.02, N = 57; two-sided t test, p < 10−8), and the O/P ratio of excitation in L4Cα is lower than that in L4Cβ (Fig. 5G,H, black curve; two-sided t test, p < 10−7), although excitation in both L4Cα and L4Cβ is poorly tuned to orientation (Fig. 5G, black curve; mean ± SEM; for L4Cα, 0.474 ± 0.025, N = 65; for L4Cβ, 0.656 ± 0.010, N = 46). It is the combination of excitation and FS that leads to a large difference for O/P ratio between L4Cα and L4Cβ. Second, the comparable O/P ratio in L2/3 and L4Cα is because of the following two factors. (1) FS in L2/3 is generally weak (mean ± SEM; for L2/3, 0.23 ± 0.02, N = 115; one-way ANOVA test, p < 10−5 for comparison between L2/3 and all other layers; see Table 1). (2) Under our experimental conditions, excitatory inputs to L2/3 are related to neural activity in L4Cα more than L4Cβ because achromatic (black/white) stimuli used in our experiment activated L4Cα more than L4Cβ (mean ± SEM; firing rates for L4Cα, 70.3 ± 4.5 spikes/s, N = 68; for firing rates for L4Cβ, 39.9 ± 2.6 spikes/s, N = 59; two-sided t test, p < 10−6). The weak FS and L4Cα-dominant excitation in L2/3 lead to a comparable O/P ratio in L2/3 and L4Cα. In summary, we think that the laminar variation of the O/P ratio is largely because of the laminar-specific excitation and FS in V1, which starts in V1 input layer.
Our explanation for the laminar variation of orientation selectivity measured by O/P ratio does not indicate that no other mechanism is involved in processing orientation information in L2/3. There is a significant difference between L2/3 and L4Cα for their tuning bandwidth, another way to measure orientation selectivity (mean ± SEM for bandwidth; 31.17 ± 0.92 deg, N = 69, for L2/3; 30.61 ± 1.15 deg, N = 22, for L4B; 35.91 ± 1.03 deg, N = 35, for L4Cα; 41.74 ± 0.84 deg, N = 25, for L4Cβ; 37.86 ± 1.13 deg, N = 36, for L5; 37.28 ± 1.23 deg, N = 25, for L6; one-way ANOVA test, p < 0.01 for comparison between L2/3 and all other layers except L4B). The significant change of orientation bandwidth between L2/3 and L4Cα indicates that recurrent connections within L2/3, interlaminar connections between L2/3 and L5, or static nonlinearity in L2/3 may also contribute to enhance orientation selectivity in layers 2/3.
General computation across V1 layers
The laminar-specific excitation and two types of suppression found in our study may represent a general computation for information processing of other visual features, including spatial frequency, brightness, and color. Strong and FS in input layers can increase global feature selectivity (Xing et al., 2011), by suppressing thalamocortical inputs responding to nonpreferred features. The output layers receive multiple sources of forward and recurrent excitatory connections. SS in output layers can play important roles in information integration, feedback control, and feature binding.
From the laminar distributions of the excitation and two types of suppression in the current study, we can summarize laminar processing into two distinct subnetworks. The first subnetwork operates at the thalamocortical stage within input layers, which transforms thalamic input and generates tuning properties via strong FS. The second subnetwork is intracortical levels of processing within output layers, which further modulates information from input layers by strong SS and sends computational results from this cortical area to other areas. The two subnetworks across layers may provide general computation for all primary sensory cortices because the circuitry within V1 is comparable with other primary sensory cortices (Lund, 1988; Linden and Schreiner, 2003).
Differences between the current study and earlier work
In an earlier study, we demonstrated dynamic responses to stimulus orientations (similar to Fig. 1B) and proposed a three-component model with two types of suppression (Xing et al., 2005). However, the present study is the first to report a complete picture of dynamic laminar processing of orientation information and laminar distribution of suppressive components. Furthermore, the current study also demonstrated a laminar-specific relationship between V1 suppression and two important functional properties, orientation selectivity and surround suppression. The results described above represent new findings that distinguish the current study from our previous studies (Xing et al., 2005, 2011). Because of the limited number of single neurons (i.e., SUA), earlier studies were not able to demonstrate such laminar patterns. By using simultaneously recorded MUAs, the current study substantially increased the number of sites for each V1 depth and the statistical power for detecting laminar patterns. Another unique feature of the current study is that we drove all V1 layers using gratings with the same spatial frequency, which was optimized for input layer 4Cα (2 cycles/degree). In contrast, in our earlier work, we drove each recorded site using its own optimal spatial frequency, which varied between 0.1 and 10 cycles/deg (Xing et al., 2005). Differences in stimulus parameters are crucial for studying laminar processing. Our earlier work demonstrated that some single neurons exhibited orientation tuning with a “Mexican hat” shape (Xing et al., 2005). In contrast, in the current dataset from awake monkeys, we seldom observed this tuning pattern, possibly because of the differences between optimizing spatial frequency for each single neuron versus fixing spatial frequency for all recorded sites. Cells or sites with “Mexican hat” tuning typically exhibit an optimal spatial frequency higher than the spatial frequency we used in this study. It may be valuable for future studies to investigate the laminar processing of spatial frequency in V1 layers. Another potentially important difference is that our earlier studies were based on data from anesthetized monkeys, whereas the current results were based on awake monkey data.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China Grant 31371110, National Key Basic Research Program of China 2014CB846101, Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, 111 Project Grant BP0719032, Thousand (Young) Talents Program of China, New Century Excellent Researcher Award Program, and Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning. We thank Robert Shapley for comments and advice; and Wu Li, Feng Wang, and Xibin Xu for help with behavioral setup of awake monkey at the early stage.
References
- Adesnik H, Bruns W, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Scanziani M (2012) A neural circuit for spatial summation in visual cortex. Nature 490:226–231. 10.1038/nature11526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adesnik H, Naka A (2018) Cracking the function of layers in the sensory cortex. Neuron 100:1028–1043. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alitto HJ, Usrey WM (2008) Origin and dynamics of extraclassical suppression in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the macaque monkey. Neuron 57:135–146. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allman J, Miezin F, McGuinness E (1985) Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. Annu Rev Neurosci 8:407–430. 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson JS, Carandini M, Ferster D (2000) Orientation tuning of input conductance, excitation, and inhibition in cat primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 84:909–926. 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.909 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer R, Dow BM (1989) Complementary global maps for the orientation coding in upper and lower layers of the monkey's foveal striate cortex. Exp Brain Res 76:503–509. 10.1007/BF00248906 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer R, Dow BM, Vautin RG (1980) Laminar distribution of preferred orientations in foveal striate cortex of the monkey. Exp Brain Res 41:54–60. 10.1007/BF00236679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer R, Dow BM, Snyder AZ, Vautin R (1983) Orientation shift between upper and lower layers in monkey visual-cortex. Exp Brain Res 50:133–145. 10.1007/BF00238240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bijanzadeh M, Nurminen L, Merlin S, Clark AM, Angelucci A (2018) Distinct laminar processing of local and global context in primate primary visual cortex. Neuron 100:259–274.e254. 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosking WH, Zhang Y, Schofield B, Fitzpatrick D (1997) Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. J Neurosci 17:2112–2127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bredfeldt CE, Ringach DL (2002) Dynamics of spatial frequency tuning in macaque V1. J Neurosci 22:1976–1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buffalo EA, Fries P, Landman R, Buschman TJ, Desimone R (2011) Laminar differences in gamma and alpha coherence in the ventral stream. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:11262–11267. 10.1073/pnas.1011284108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaway EM. (1998) Local circuits in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Annu Rev Neurosci 21:47–74. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.47 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell FW, Cleland BG, Cooper GF, Enroth-Cugell C (1968) Angular selectivity of visual cortical cells to moving gratings. J Physiol 198:237–250. 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chisum HJ, Mooser F, Fitzpatrick D (2003) Emergent properties of layer 2/3 neurons reflect the collinear arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew visual cortex. J Neurosci 23:2947–2960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragoi V, Sharma J, Miller EK, Sur M (2002) Dynamics of neuronal sensitivity in visual cortex and local feature discrimination. Nat Neurosci 5:883–891. 10.1038/nn900 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Rottschy C, Zilles K (2007) Laminar distribution and co-distribution of neurotransmitter receptors in early human visual cortex. Brain Struct Funct 212:255–267. 10.1007/s00429-007-0156-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferster D. (1986) Orientation selectivity of synaptic potentials in neurons of cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 6:1284–1301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner JL, Anzai A, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD (1999) Linear and nonlinear contributions to orientation tuning of simple cells in the cat's striate cortex. Vis Neurosci 16:1115–1121. 10.1017/s0952523899166112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gegenfurtner KR, Kiper DC, Fenstemaker SB (1996) Processing of color, form, and motion in macaque area V2. Vis Neurosci 13:161–172. 10.1017/s0952523800007203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girman SV, Sauve Y, Lund RD (1999) Receptive field properties of single neurons in rat primary visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 82:301–311. 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goense J, Merkle H, Logothetis NK (2012) High-resolution fMRI reveals laminar differences in neurovascular coupling between positive and negative BOLD responses. Neuron 76:629–639. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goris RL, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA (2015) Origin and function of tuning diversity in macaque visual cortex. Neuron 88:819–831. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur M, Kagan I, Snodderly DM (2005) Orientation and direction selectivity of neurons in V1 of alert monkeys: functional relationships and laminar distributions. Cereb Cortex 15:1207–1221. 10.1093/cercor/bhi003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansel D, Sompolinsky H (1996) Chaos and synchrony in a model of a hypercolumn in visual cortex. J Comput Neurosci 3:7–34. 10.1007/BF00158335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawken MJ, Parker AJ, Lund JS (1988) Laminar organization and contrast sensitivity of direction-selective cells in the striate cortex of the Old World monkey. J Neurosci 8:3541–3548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry CA, Joshi S, Xing D, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ (2013) Functional characterization of the extraclassical receptive field in macaque V1: contrast, orientation, and temporal dynamics. J Neurosci 33:6230–6242. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4155-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch JA, Martinez LM (2006) Laminar processing in the visual cortical column. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:377–384. 10.1016/j.conb.2006.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160:106–154. 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1968) Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 195:215–243. 10.1113/jphysiol.1968.sp008455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacson JS, Scanziani M (2011) How inhibition shapes cortical activity. Neuron 72:231–243. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones JP, Palmer LA (1987) The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol 58:1187–1211. 10.1152/jn.1987.58.6.1187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khatri CG, Mardia KV (1977) von Mises Fisher matrix distribution in orientation statistics. J R Stat Soc B Met 39:95–106. 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1977.tb01610.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SH, Kwan AC, Zhang SY, Phoumthipphavong V, Flannery JG, Masmanidis SC, Taniguchi H, Huang ZJ, Zhang F, Boyden ES, Deisseroth K, Dan Y (2012) Activation of specific interneurons improves V1 feature selectivity and visual perception. Nature 488:379–383. 10.1038/nature11312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leventhal AG, Thompson KG, Liu D, Zhou YF, Ault SJ (1995) Concomitant sensitivity to orientation, direction, and color of cells in layer-2, layer-3, and layer-4 of monkey striate cortex. J Neurosci 15:1808–1818. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-01808.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linden JF, Schreiner CE (2003) Columnar transformations in auditory cortex? A comparison to visual and somatosensory cortices. Cereb Cortex 13:83–89. 10.1093/cercor/13.1.83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu BH, Li YT, Ma WP, Pan CJ, Zhang LI, Tao HW (2011) Broad inhibition sharpens orientation selectivity by expanding input dynamic range in mouse simple cells. Neuron 71:542–554. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Livingstone MS, Hubel DH (1984) Anatomy and physiology of a color system in the primate visual-cortex. J Neurosci 4:309–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lund JS. (1988) Anatomical organization of macaque monkey striate visual cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 11:253–288. 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001345 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lund JS, Angelucci A, Bressloff PC (2003) Anatomical substrates for functional columns in macaque monkey primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 13:15–24. 10.1093/cercor/13.1.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maier A, Aura CJ, Leopold DA (2011) Infragranular sources of sustained local field potential responses in macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 31:1971–1980. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5300-09.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marienhagen J, Keller BU, Zippelius A (1997) Kinetic model of excitatory synaptic transmission to cerebellar Purkinje cells. J Theor Biol 188:227–240. 10.1006/jtbi.1997.0464 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez LM, Alonso JM, Reid RC, Hirsch JA (2002) Laminar processing of stimulus orientation in cat visual cortex. J Physiol 540:321–333. 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.012776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maunsell JH, Gibson JR (1992) Visual response latencies in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 68:1332–1344. 10.1152/jn.1992.68.4.1332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazer JA, Vinje WE, McDermott J, Schiller PH, Gallant JL (2002) Spatial frequency and orientation tuning dynamics in area V1. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:1645–1650. 10.1073/pnas.022638499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitzdorf U, Singer W (1979) Excitatory synaptic ensemble properties in the visual cortex of the macaque monkey: a current source density analysis of electrically evoked potentials. J Comp Neurol 187:71–83. 10.1002/cne.901870105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naka KI, Rushton WA (1966) S-potentials from luminosity units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae). J Physiol 185:587–599. 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp008003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niell CM, Stryker MP (2008) Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci 28:7520–7536. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0623-08.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohki K, Chung S, Ch'ng YH, Kara P, Reid RC (2005) Functional imaging with cellular resolution reveals precise micro-architecture in visual cortex. Nature 433:597–603. 10.1038/nature03274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priebe NJ, Ferster D (2006) Mechanisms underlying cross-orientation suppression in cat visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 9:552–561. 10.1038/nn1660 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Priebe NJ, Ferster D (2012) Mechanisms of neuronal computation in mammalian visual cortex. Neuron 75:194–208. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid RC, Alonso JM (1995) Specificity of monosynaptic connections from thalamus to visual cortex. Nature 378:281–284. 10.1038/378281a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley R (1997) Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque primary visual cortex. Nature 387:281–284. 10.1038/387281a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ (2002) Orientation selectivity in macaque V1: diversity and laminar dependence. J Neurosci 22:5639–5651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley R (2003) Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque V1: the role of global and tuned suppression. J Neurophysiol 90:342–352. 10.1152/jn.01018.2002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rockland KS, Pandya DN (1979) Laminar origins and terminations of cortical connections of the occipital lobe in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res 179:3–20. 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90485-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose D, Blakemore C (1974) An analysis of orientation selectivity in the cat's visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 20:1–17. 10.1007/BF00239014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sato H, Katsuyama N, Tamura H, Hata Y, Tsumoto T (1996) Mechanisms underlying orientation selectivity of neurons in the primary visual cortex of the macaque. J Physiol Lond 494:757–771. 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholl B, Tan AY, Corey J, Priebe NJ (2013) Emergence of orientation selectivity in the mammalian visual pathway. J Neurosci 33:10616–10624. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0404-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, Foxe JJ (2002) The timing and laminar profile of converging inputs to multisensory areas of the macaque neocortex. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14:187–198. 10.1016/s0926-6410(02)00073-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schroeder CE, Mehta AD, Givre SJ (1998) A spatiotemporal profile of visual system activation revealed by current source density analysis in the awake macaque. Cereb Cortex 8:575–592. 10.1093/cercor/8.7.575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Self MW, van Kerkoerle T, Super H, Roelfsema PR (2013) Distinct roles of the cortical layers of area V1 in figure-ground segregation. Curr Biol 23:2121–2129. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapley R, Hawken M, Ringach DL (2003) Dynamics of orientation selectivity in the primary visual cortex and the importance of cortical inhibition. Neuron 38:689–699. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00332-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shushruth S, Ichida JM, Levitt JB, Angelucci A (2009) Comparison of spatial summation properties of neurons in macaque V1 and V2. J Neurophysiol 102:2069–2083. 10.1152/jn.00512.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silberberg G, Markram H (2007) Disynaptic inhibition between neocortical pyramidal cells mediated by Martinotti cells. Neuron 53:735–746. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sillito AM. (1975) Contribution of inhibitory mechanisms to receptive-field properties of neurons in striate cortex of cat. J Physiol 250:305–329. 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sincich LC, Horton JC (2005) The circuitry of V1 and V2: integration of color, form, and motion. Annu Rev Neurosci 28:303–326. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith MA, Jia X, Zandvakili A, Kohn A (2013) Laminar dependence of neuronal correlations in visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 109:940–947. 10.1152/jn.00846.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon SG, White AJ, Martin PR (2002) Extraclassical receptive field properties of parvocellular, magnocellular, and koniocellular cells in the primate lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurosci 22:338–349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Somers DC, Nelson SB, Sur M (1995) An emergent model of orientation selectivity in cat visual cortical simple cells. J Neurosci 15:5448–5465. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-08-05448.1995 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stettler DD, Das A, Bennett J, Gilbert CD (2002) Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque primary visual cortex. Neuron 36:739–750. 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)01029-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan AY, Brown BD, Scholl B, Mohanty D, Priebe NJ (2011) Orientation selectivity of synaptic input to neurons in mouse and cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 31:12339–12350. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2039-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker TR, Fitzpatrick D (2006) Luminance-evoked inhibition in primary visual cortex: a transient veto of simultaneous and ongoing response. J Neurosci 26:13537–13547. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3723-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Roelfsema PR (2017) Layer-specificity in the effects of attention and working memory on activity in primary visual cortex. Nat Commun 8:15555. 10.1038/ncomms15555 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson NR, Runyan CA, Wang FL, Sur M (2012) Division and subtraction by distinct cortical inhibitory networks in vivo. Nature 488:343–348. 10.1038/nature11347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Shapley RM, Hawken MJ, Ringach DL (2005) Effect of stimulus size on the dynamics of orientation selectivity in macaque V1. J Neurophysiol 94:799–812. 10.1152/jn.01139.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Yeh CI, Shapley RM (2010) Generation of black-dominant responses in V1 cortex. J Neurosci 30:13504–13512. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2473-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley RM (2011) Untuned suppression makes a major contribution to the enhancement of orientation selectivity in macaque V1. J Neurosci 31:15972–15982. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2245-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xing D, Yeh CI, Burns S, Shapley RM (2012) Laminar analysis of visually evoked activity in the primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:13871–13876. 10.1073/pnas.1201478109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh CI, Xing D, Williams PE, Shapley RM (2009) Stimulus ensemble and cortical layer determine V1 spatial receptive fields. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:14652–14657. 10.1073/pnas.0907406106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]