Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Sep 24;15(9):e0239693. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239693

Political and personal reactions to COVID-19 during initial weeks of social distancing in the United States

Sarah R Christensen 1, Emily B Pilling 1, J B Eyring 1, Grace Dickerson 1, Chantel D Sloan 1, Brianna M Magnusson 1,*
Editor: Geilson Lima Santana2
PMCID: PMC7514047  PMID: 32970761

Abstract

Objective

To examine perceptions, behaviors, and impacts surrounding COVID-19 early in the pandemic response.

Materials and methods

A cross-sectional survey of 1,030 U.S. adults was administered on March 31st, 2020. This survey examined attitudes toward media, government, and community responses to COVID-19 by political ideology and sociodemographic factors. Knowledge, anxieties, and impacts of COVID-19 were also assessed.

Results

Conservatives were more likely to report that COVID-19 was receiving too much media coverage and people were generally overreacting; liberals were more likely to report the government had not done enough in response to the pandemic. Females and those with lower income experienced more COVID-19 related economic anxieties. Those working and with children at home reported higher social, home, and work disruption. Social distancing behaviors were more common among liberals and were associated with increases in depressive symptoms. General knowledge about COVID-19 was widely exhibited across the sample, however, Black and Hispanic respondents were less likely to correctly answer questions about the availability of a vaccine and modes of transmission.

Conclusions

Public health experts should consider the political climate in crafting messaging that appeals to the values of those across the political spectrum. Research on the COVID-19 pandemic should continue to monitor the effects of social distancing on mental health and among vulnerable populations.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic of 2019–2020 presents physical, social, emotional, and financial challenges. Effective communication of public health information is central to a successful pandemic response. The first confirmed case of community transmitted COVID-19 in the U.S. was reported on February 26th, 2020. By March 31st, cases rapidly increased to over 181,000 –the highest reported incidence of any country. By August 20th, 2020, confirmed cases in the US reached over 5.5 million and deaths due to COVID-19 exceeded 172,000. [1] National and state responses to the crisis quickly became the central messaging for all major news outlets. Local governments implemented varying restrictions and strategies to interrupt outbreaks and mitigate disease burden in their communities. Concern exists that variation in messaging across communities is resulting in inconsistent expectations, anxieties, and responses among the public.

Recent studies conducted in China and Italy have investigated the psychological and mental health impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on patients, health care workers, and the general population. [26] China’s general population experienced an increase in panic disorder, depression, and anxiety after strict quarantine measures were enforced. [3] Over one-half of China’s general population reported moderate-to-severe psychological impacts and one-third reported increased levels of anxiety. [6] Groups in China’s general population that were associated with worse mental health outcomes included females, students, those with underlying health conditions, and those with specific symptoms. Among health care workers in China, there was an increase in depression, anxiety, insomnia, and distress. [2] Women, nurses, those in Wuhan, and front-line health care workers were all groups that were associated with worse mental health outcomes. [2] Similar results were found in health care workers in Italy. [4] About half of the health care workers sampled showed signs of PTSD, about one-fourth showed signs of depression, about one-fifth showed signs of anxiety and stress, and 8% showed signs of insomnia. Groups that were associated with worse mental health outcomes included those who were younger, female, and front-line health care workers. [4] Finally, a systematic review of studies relating to the mental health impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic broadly confirmed many of these more specific results in showing that the most common adverse psychological impacts across the general, health care, and clinical populations were PTSD, depression, and anxiety. [5] Populations that were especially vulnerable to worse mental health outcomes included health care workers and patients with severe complications, and groups that were associated with worse mental health outcomes included females and young people.

Our study further analyzes this phenomenon in the general U.S. population and focuses on bringing further understanding to the differential effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on subsets of the general population.

Access to conflicting and misleading information in the media, as well as decreasing trust of government and scientific communities, impacts the ability to effectively communicate during crisis situations. [7] Media messaging holds immense power in swaying public trust in crisis response tactics. Research on media following Hurricane Katrina suggested that as media politicized coverage of disaster relief efforts, individuals faced extreme difficulty in forming independent opinions of crisis management processes. [8] As public opinion is swayed to align with polarized agendas, the ability of authority figures to promote coordinated responses with public support is severely undermined. [7]

Acknowledging underlying attitudes is crucial during the process of analyzing, predicting, and attempting to guide public behavior during a crisis. Consistent with Theory of Planned Behavior, [9] understanding the public’s attitudes toward COVID-19 and the response of the media, government, and public to the pandemic may provide insight into behavioral intention and adherence to preventative behaviors in the wake of this ongoing global pandemic. In light of differences in vulnerability to COVID-19, access to information, sources of information, and timing of national, state, and local responses to COVID-19, it is reasonable to assume that individual responses in the midst of the pandemic vary widely. This study examines individual attitudes, behaviors, anxieties, mental health impacts, and knowledge early in the pandemic response, as well as those outcomes by sociodemographic characteristics and political ideology. The purpose of this study was to determine how complex factors within society shaped early perceptions of and responses to COVID-19.

Materials and methods

An anonymous cross-sectional internet survey was administered to 1,030 adults residing in the U.S. on March 31st, 2020. The sample was recruited by Qualtrics (Provo, UT, USA). Quotas for sex, race, and income, derived from U.S. Census data, [10] were used to increase demographic representation. Implied consent was provided prior to the survey. Participants were presented with information about the survey as well as potential risks, benefits and compensation. Following this information participants were presented with a statement which read, “The completion of this survey implies your consent to participate. If you choose to participate, please proceed with the questions.” Compensation was valued at <$5 U.S. The study was approved by the Brigham Young University Institutional Review Board.

Questions assessed political ideology, scientific trust, and media consumption, as well as attitudes, anxieties, impacts, and knowledge related to COVID-19. Respondents also assessed mental health and demographic information.

Three author-constructed questions assessed attitudes toward the response to COVID-19. Respondents were asked about pandemic media coverage (too much, right amount, or too little), government action (not enough, responding correctly, or too much), and public response (overreacting, responding correctly, or too much). Respondents also answered two true/false questions: if they believed their state would experience a major outbreak of the virus and if they would isolate if they contracted the virus.

We used the Political Polarization in the American Public Survey, [11] which has been validated as a reliable measure of political partisanship, [12] to examine personal political ideologies through a series of 10 dichotomous statements on political issues. Responses were scored (liberal = -1 vs. conservative = +1), summed, and categorized as leans liberal, moderate, and leans conservative. Respondents also self-characterized their political views on a 7-point scale (extremely liberal to extremely conservative).

Two additional items asked about attitudes toward global warming (most scientists think global warming is happening vs. there is a lot of disagreement as to whether global warming is happening) [13] and trust in government to make vaccination decisions (I trust that the government makes the best decisions when it comes to vaccination requirements vs. I do not trust the government to make decisions about what vaccinations are required). [14] A subset of questions from the Reuters Institute Digital News Report [15] was used to assess media consumption. Respondents indicated their usual sources of media (ABC, FOX, NPR etc.) which were each given a bias score based on Ad Fontes Media’s source evaluations. [16]

Pandemic-related behavior change was assessed by eleven author-constructed items asking respondents to compare their behavior on March 31st with their behavior before the pandemic on a 5-point scale from “much less than usual” to “much more than usual”. Behaviors included virtual communication, face-to-face contact, visiting restaurants/bars, stores, work, and travel. Four items asked respondents to indicate agreement on a 7-point scale that “events related to COVID-19 had interrupted” their social life, home life, work or vocational life, and/or hurt their mental health.

Fourteen author constructed items assessed pandemic-related anxieties using a 7-point agreement scale. Four statements related to fear if they, an older family member, a young family member, or a healthy adult family member became ill with COVID-19. Two statements assessed anxieties related to healthcare equipment and personnel, three statements assessed economic concerns, and four items assessed concerns related to children at home (e.g. routines affected, children without care, etc.). Questions regarding work- and child-related anxieties were only asked of those working for pay before the pandemic and those who had children under 18 in the household respectively. The last question assessed concerns that mental health would suffer due to social distancing measures.

Respondents assessed change in their own mental health from before the COVID-19 pandemic on a 7-point scale from much worse to much better. Respondents completed the PHQ-9 (a valid measure of depressive severity) [17] retrospectively for the two-week period preceding social distancing and for the current two-week period. Higher PHQ-9 scores indicated more depressive symptoms. An increase in depressive symptoms is indicated by a positive change score.

True/false and multiple-choice items developed by the authors were used to assess respondent knowledge regarding common symptoms of COVID-19, recommended preventative measures, viral spread, and comparisons of COVID-19 and seasonal influenza.

Respondents reported age, biological sex, race, ethnicity, marital status, education level, whether they were currently in school, employment status prior to the pandemic, average hours worked currently and prior to the pandemic, household size, household income, whether they received government nutritional program assistance, children at home, state of residence, and flu vaccination history.

Frequencies, proportions, and means were calculated. Chi-square, t, and F tests were used to examine the influence of demographic characteristics, political ideology, and mental health on attitudes, knowledge, anxieties, behavior change, and impact variables. Logistic regression was used to assess the relationship between political ideology and attitudes towards media, government, and community responses to COVID-19 while controlling for sociodemographic characteristics, political ideology, media bias, global warming agreement, and trust in government vaccination requirements. Initial covariate selection included all variables that were significant (p <0.05) in bivariate tests, including: political ideology, bias score for consumed news media, attitudes toward global warming and vaccination, sex, race, poverty level, and education. The final model was achieved by sequentially removing non-significant predictors and assessing the impact on model fit using the Bayesian Information Criterion and Akaike Information Criterion. Non-significant predictors were retained if removing them worsened model fit.

Two attitude questions were dichotomized for logistic regression, due to small cell counts. Those who responded there was “too little media coverage” (n = 88) were merged with those who responded “the right amount” of media coverage. Similarly, those who responded the government had “done too much” (n = 51) were merged with those who responded the government had “done the right amount” in response to the pandemic. Logistic regression models were tested with the grouped categories as outlined above, and with the small categories coded as missing; results were similar.

Logistic regression was also used to assess the relationship between knowledge about COVID-19 while controlling for sociodemographic characteristics and media bias. Initial covariate selection included all variables that were significant (p <0.05) in bivariate tests, including: bias score for consumed news media, sex, race, poverty level, and education. The final model was achieved by sequentially removing non-significant predictors and assessing the impact on model fit using the Bayesian Information Criterion and Akaike Information Criterion. Non-significant predictors were retained if removing them worsened model fit. All analyses were completed in SAS 9.4.

Results

The sample included 1,030 U.S. adults from 48 U.S. states and D.C. No respondents resided in Vermont or Wyoming. The sample was 47.5% male, 49.0% white, 23.4% Black, 12.0% Hispanic and 12.3% Asian. About 29% of the sample had children under 18 living at home, about 23% were living under the Federal Poverty Line, and about 24% had received government benefits in the last six months. Sample demographics overall and stratified by political ideology are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Sample demographics: Overall and by political ideology.

Political Ideologya
Total Sample Leans Liberal Moderate Leans Conservative
n = 1,030 n = 523 n = 359 n = 148
n (%) n (%) p- valueb
Age in years (Mean, SD) 48.8 (18.7) 48.6 (18.7) 45.9 (18.3) 56.7 (17.8) <0.001
Sex
 Male 489 (47.5) 227 (43.4) 162 (45.1) 100 (67.6) <0.001
 Female 541 (52.5) 296 (56.6) 197 (54.9) 48 (32.4)
Race
 White 505 (49.0) 234 (44.7) 160 (44.6) 111 (75.0) <0.001
 Black 241 (23.4) 146 (27.9) 76 (21.2) 19 (12.8)
 Hispanic 123 (12.0) 63 (12.1) 50 (13.9) 10 (6.8)
 Asian 127 (12.3) 61 (11.7) 61 (17.0) 5 (3.4)
 Other 34 (3.3) 19 (3.6) 12 (3.3) 3 (2.0)
Marital Status
 Married 447 (43.4) 206 (39.4) 153 (42.6) 88 (59.5) 0.001
 Never Married 377 (36.6) 211 (40.3) 135 (37.6) 31 (21.0)
 Widowed/Divorced /Separated 206 (20.0) 106 (20.3) 71 (19.8) 29 (19.6)
Children Under 18 living at home
 Yes 298 (28.9) 145 (27.7) 123 (34.3) 30 (20.3) 0.005
Income as a percentage of the Federal Poverty Level (FPL)
 0–99% of the FPL 240 (23.3) 124 (23.7) 101 (28.1) 15 (10.1) 0.002
 100–199% of the FPL 166 (16.1) 83 (15.9) 59 (16.4) 24 (16.2)
 200–299% of the FPL 191 (18.5) 93 (17.8) 61 (17.0) 37 (25.0)
 300–399% of the FPL 121 (11.8) 62 (11.9) 33 (9.2) 26 (17.6)
 400+ % of the FPL 312 (30.3) 161 (30.8) 105 (29.3) 46 (31.1)
Received government benefits in the last 6 months
 Yes 249 (24.2) 122 (23.3) 107 (29.8) 20 (13.5) <0.001
Education
 High School Education or Less 208 (26.4) 96 (18.4) 85 (23.7) 27 (18.3) 0.396
 Some College or Associates Degree 135 (17.2) 189 (35.1) 135 (37.6) 54 (36.5)
 Bachelor's Degree 279 (35.5) 149 (28.5) 86 (24.0) 44 (29.7)
 Masters or Advanced Degree 165 (21.0) 89 (17.0) 53 (14.8) 23 (15.5)
Currently Enrolled in School
 Yes 113 (11.0) 58 (11.1) 48 (13.4) 7 (4.7) 0.018
Working for pay before COVID-19 Outbreak
vYes 566 (55.0) 283 (54.1) 213 (59.3) 70 (47.3) 0.040
Flu vaccine for current influenza season
 Yes 537 (52.1) 265 (50.7) 185 (51.5) 87 (58.8) 0.210
Frequency of Flu Vaccine over the last 5 years
 Annually 406 (39.4) 192 (36.7) 144 (40.1) 70 (47.3) 0.640
 At least once, but not every year 301 (29.2) 158 (30.2) 104 (29.0) 39 (26.4)
 Never 323 (31.4) 173 (33.1) 111 (30.9) 39 (26.4)

a. Political Ideology calculated using the 10-item Political Polarization in the American Public Survey [11]

b. p-value derived from a Chi-square test for the difference of proportions

The majority (63.1%) felt that COVID-19 was receiving the right amount of media coverage, with 28.4% responding that the pandemic was receiving too much media coverage. Conservatives were most likely to feel the pandemic was receiving too much media coverage (50%) compared to moderates (30.1%) and liberals (21.0%; p-value <0.001). Table 2 provides the adjusted logistic regression analysis for the attitude questions. Compared to conservatives, liberals had three times the odds (aOR 3.3; 95%CI: 2.1–5.2) and moderates had twice the odds (aOR 2.3; 95%CI: 1.5–3.6) of reporting that the media coverage was the right amount/too much.

Table 2. Political and sociodemographic influences on attitudes about media coverage, government action, and community responses to COVID-19.

Covid-19 is receiving… Too little/the right amount media coverage (n = 738)a The US government has … Not done enough in response to COVID-19 (n = 569)b People are generally … Responding correctly to COVID-19 (n = 420)c People are generally … Under-reacting to COVID-19 (n = 411)c
n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI)
Political Ideologyd
 Leans Liberal 413 (79.0) 3.3 (2.1–5.2) 370 (70.8) 5.7 (3.3–9.7) 224 (42.8) 2.9 (1.6–5.3) 233 (44.6) 3.0 (1.6–5.5)
 Moderate 251 (68.9) 2.3 (1.5–3.6) 170 (47.4) 2.5 (1.5–4.3) 141 (39.3) 1.7 (1.0–2.9) 139 (38.7) 1.8 (1.0–3.3)
 Leans Conservative 74 (50.0) 1.0 29 (19.6) 1.0 55 (37.2) 1.0 39 (26.4) 1.0
News Source Biase
 Leans Liberal 345 (75.9) 1.1 (0.7–1.8) 320 (70.3) 2.7 (1.7–4.2) 184 (40.4) 1.3 (0.7–2.1) 204 (44.8) 1.9 (1.1–3.4)
 Moderate 246 (72.6) 1.0 (0.7–1.6) 166 (49.0) 1.3 (0.8–2.0) 141 (41.6) 1.1 (0.7–1.8) 135 (39.8) 1.5 (0.9–2.5)
 Leans Conservative 104 (64.2) 1.0 49 (30.3) 1.0 67 (41.4) 1.0 47 (29.0) 1.0
Global Warming Question
 Most scientists think global warming is happening. 545 (74.4) -- 476 (65.0) 2.7 (2.0–3.9) 305 (41.6) 1.3 (0.9–2.0) 313 (42.7) 1.6 (1.0–2.4)
 There is a lot of disagreement as to whether global warming is happening. 192 (64.9) -- 92 (31.1) 1.0 115 (38.9) 1.0 97 (32.8) 1.0
Vaccine Question
 I trust that the government makes the best decisions when it comes to vaccination requirements. 449 (72.3) -- 304 (49.0) 0.4 (0.3–0.5) 289 (46.5) 1.7 (1.2–2.5) 224 (36.1) 1.0 (0.7–1.5)
 I do not trust the government to make decisions about what vaccinations are required. 288 (70.6) -- 264 (64.7) 1.0 131 (32.1) 1.0 186 (45.6) 1.0
Sex
 Female 410 (75.8) 1.5 (1.1–2.0) 324 (60.0) 1.3 (1.0–1.7) 202 (37.3) 1.3 (0.9–2.0) 252(46.6) 2.2 (1.5–3.2)
 Male 328 (67.0) 1.0 245 (50.1) 1.0 218 (44.6) 1.0 159(32.5) 1.0
Race
 White 361 (71.5) 1.0 243 (48.1) -- 224 (44.4) 1.0 196 (38.8) 1.0
 Black 189 (78.4) 1.1 (0.8–1.7) 154 (63.9) -- 91 (37.8) 0.7 (0.4–1.2) 109 (45.2) 0.8 (0.5–1.3)
 Hispanic 84 (68.3) 0.7 (0.5–1.2) 68 (55.3) -- 45 (36.6) 0.4 (0.3–0.8) 47 (38.2) 0.5 (0.3–0.8)
 Asian 81 (63.8) 0.5 (0.3–0.8) 84 (66.1) -- 47 (37.0) 0.4 (0.2–0.6) 48 (37.8) 0.4 (0.2–0.6)
 Other 23 (67.7) 0.7 (0.3–1.6) 20 (58.8) -- 13 (38.2) 0.4 (0.2–1.2) 11 (32.4) 0.4 (0.2–1.1)

a. Reference group: …Too much media coverage

b. Reference group: …Done the right amount/done too much in response to COVID-19

c. Reference group: …Overreactive to COVID-19

d. Political Ideology calculated using the 10-item Political Polarization in the American Public Survey [11]

e. News Score Bias were calculated using the Ad Fontes Media’s source evaluation and was averaged for each respondent [16]

The majority (55.2%) responded that the U.S. government had not done enough in response to COVID-19, while 40% felt the government responded correctly. Just 5% felt the government had done too much. Liberals (70.8%) were most likely to respond that the government had not done enough in response to COVID-19, compared to 19.6% of conservatives. Nearly 10% of conservatives reported that the government had done too much in response to the pandemic (p-value <0.001). In the adjusted logistic regression model (Table 2) liberals had 5.7 (95%CI: 3.3–9.7) and moderates had 2.5 (95%CI 1.5–4.3) times the odds of responding that the government had not done enough in response to COVID-19 compared to conservatives. Those who consumed liberal leaning news media and who indicated there was scientific agreement about global warming also had higher odds of feeling the government had not done enough.

Approximately 19% felt that people were generally overreacting to COVID-19 and 40% felt that people were generally under-reacting. Conservatives (36.5%) were most likely to feel that people were overreacting while liberals (44.6%) were most likely to feel that people were under-reacting (p-value <0.001). In the multinomial logistic regression model (Table 2), compared to conservatives, liberals had approximately three times the odds of reporting people were responding correctly (aOR 2.9; 95%CI 1.6–5.3) or under-reacting (aOR: 3.0; 95%CI: 1.6–5.5). Females and those who consumed liberal news had significantly higher odds of feeling people were under-reacting to COVID-19.

Despite variation in opinions regarding the response to COVID-19, 80% felt that their state would experience a major outbreak of the disease. A similar percentage of liberals and moderates felt a major outbreak would occur (82.4%, 79.3%), while a smaller percentage of conservatives (67.6%; p-value = <0.001) agreed. Regardless of political ideology nearly all respondents (95.8%) reported they would self-isolate in the event that they became ill with COVID-19.

Respondents reported moderate agreement with all four statements evaluating fear related to becoming sick or having a family member become sick with COVID-19. Respondents agreed most strongly that they would be scared if an elderly family member contracted COVID-19 (mean: 5.28; SD: 1.21), followed by a young family member (mean: 5.07; SD: 1.34), a healthy adult family member (mean: 4.93; SD: 1.39) and themselves (mean 4.86: SD: 1.48). Respondents also reported moderate agreement regarding concerns that the country would not have sufficient healthcare providers (mean: 4.84; SD: 1.35) or supplies (mean: 4.78; SD: 1.40) to meet the needs of those infected with COVID-19.

Regarding events surrounding COVID-19, a majority (64.9%) agreed they were afraid they may not be able to purchase supplies, food, and/or medication they needed. Similarly 64.4% of those who were working before the pandemic agreed they were afraid they may not be able to financially provide for themselves or their families if asked not to work due to social distancing, and 66.8% agreed that they were afraid they would not be able to provide for themselves or their families if they became sick with COVID-19. Table 3 shows the distribution of economic anxieties by sociodemographic factors. After adjusting for the other factors in the table, females and those with lower income had higher mean agreement with all three economic anxieties statements as compared to males and those with higher income.

Table 3. Economic anxieties related to COVID-19 by demographic variables.

I am afraid that I will not be able to purchase the supplies, food, medication etc. needed to provide for my family due to events related to COVID-19 I am afraid that I will not be able to financially provide for myself and/or my family if I am asked not to come to work due to social distancing policies/practices I am afraid that I will not be able to financially provide for myself and/or my family if I become infected with COVID-19 and am unable to work
Means (SD) Means (SD) Means (SD)
Total Sample—Mean (SD) 4.03 (1.68) 3.96 (1.88) 4.12 (1.84)
Sex
 Male 3.82 (1.66) 3.75 (1.90) 3.85 (1.91)
 Female 4.21 (1.67) 4.20 (1.83) 4.42 (1.72)
p-value (single predictor model)a <0.001 0.005 <0.001
p-value (full model/lsmean)b 0.006 0.172 0.017
Federal Poverty Level (FPL) -
 <100% FPL 4.28 (1.7) 4.54 (1.59) 4.63 (1.6)
 100–199% FPL 4.34 (1.56) 4.39 (1.69) 4.66 (1.66)
 200–299% FPL 3.96 (1.64) 4.04 (1.77) 4.36 (1.63)
 300–399% FPL 3.72 (1.55) 3.41 (1.99) 3.62 (1.91)
 400+%FPL 3.82 (1.75) 3.56 (2.01) 3.59 (1.97)
p-value (single predictor model)a <0.001 <0.001 <0.001
p-value (full model/lsmean)b 0.018 0.006 <0.001
Education Level
 High School degree or less 4.16 (1.66) 4.46 (1.61) 4.40 (1.73)
 Some college or associates degree 4.10 (1.62) 4.18 (1.75) 4.52 (1.63)
 Bachelor's degree 3.95 (1.68) 3.71 (2.02) 3.81 (1.97)
 Masters or more advanced degree 3.82 (1.81) 3.54 (1.96) 3.64 (1.92)
p-value (single predictor model)a 0.168 <0.001 <0.001
p-value (full model/lsmean)b 0.919 0.111 0.076
Race
 White, non-Hispanic 3.87 (1.7) 3.54 (1.97) 3.71 (1.98)
 Black, non-Hispanic 4.12 (1.67) 4.11 (1.84) 4.3 (1.74)
 Hispanic 4.29 (1.76) 4.48 (1.59) 4.72 (1.51)
 Asian 4.19 (1.46) 4.15 (1.74) 4.1 (1.74)
 Other race, non-Hispanic 4.12 (1.72) 3.57 (2.4) 3.76 (2.39)
p-value (single predictor model)a 0.042 <0.001 <0.001
p-value (full model/lsmean)b 0.393 0.042 0.156

a. p-value derived from single predictor F test comparing group means

b. p-value derived from full model F test comparing group means after accounting for all other parameter in the table

In general, respondents reported changing their behaviors consistent with public health guidelines for social distancing. Table 4 shows the distribution of changes in behavior. The degree to which people reported their behavior changed differed by political ideology. Liberals were more likely to report a change in their behavior in the desired direction compared to conservatives.

Table 4. Influence of political ideology on behavior changes and the impact on behavior changes on mental health.

Political Ideologya
Total Sample Change in PHQ9 Scoreb,c Leans Liberal Moderate Leans Conservative p-valuee
n (%) Mean (SD) p-valued n (%)
Please indicate the extent to which you have changed your social contact routines/behaviors in response to COVID-19
Have in-person contact with family members who live near me
Much less than usual 366 (40.00) 0.69 (3.89) 0.001 208 (39.77) 112 (31.28) 46 (31.08) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 217 (23.72) 0.19 (3.19) 111 (21.22) 78 (21.79) 28 (18.92)
About the same as usual 236 (25.79) -0.33 (2.82) 118 (22.56) 80 (22.35) 38 (25.68)
Somewhat more than usual 54 (5.90) -0.06 (3) 9 (1.72) 34 (9.5) 11 (7.43)
Much more than usual 42 (4.59) -1.17 (4.49) 18 (3.44) 20 (5.59) 4 (2.7)
Have in-person contact with close friends
Much less than usual 561 (57.66) 0.6 (3.7) 0.001 313 (59.85) 179 (50.14) 69 (46.62) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 195 (20.04) -0.09 (2.75) 101 (19.31) 57 (15.97) 37 (25)
About the same as usual 143 (14.70) -0.36 (2.62) 60 (11.47) 59 (16.53) 24 (16.22)
Somewhat more than usual 44 (4.52) -0.39 (4.51) 12 (2.29) 25 (7) 7 (4.73)
Much more than usual 30 (3.08) -1.2 (4.42) 11 (2.1) 15 (4.2) 4 (2.7)
Have in-person contact with colleagues and work friends
Much less than usual 486 (60.90) 0.48 (3.82) <0.001 268 (51.34) 163 (45.53) 55 (37.16) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 134 (16.79) -0.09 (2.99) 64 (12.26) 47 (13.13) 23 (15.54)
About the same as usual 101 (12.66) -0.13 (3.17) 45 (8.62) 39 (10.89) 17 (11.49)
Somewhat more than usual 39 (4.89) -1.72 (4.36) 10 (1.92) 24 (6.7) 5 (3.38)
Much more than usual 38 (4.76) -0.87 (4.17) 11 (2.11) 21 (5.87) 6 (4.05)
Gone to restaurants and bars
Much less than usual 731 (77.60) 0.31 (3.52) 0.020 399 (76.29) 227 (63.23) 105 (70.95) 0.001
Somewhat less than usual 90 (9.55) -0.01 (3) 39 (7.46) 41 (11.42) 10 (6.76)
About the same as usual 61 (6.48) -0.61 (3.01) 26 (4.97) 26 (7.24) 9 (6.08)
Somewhat more than usual 27 (2.87) 1.7 (3.94) 7 (1.34) 15 (4.18) 5 (3.38)
Much more than usual 33 (3.50) -0.88 (3.57) 10 (1.91) 20 (5.57) 3 (2.03)
Gone to stores (grocery, retail, etc.)
Much less than usual 419 (41.12) 0.56 (4.06) 0.010 225 (43.02) 142 (39.55) 52 (35.14) 0.003
Somewhat less than usual 302 (29.64) 0.02 (2.83) 160 (30.59) 99 (27.58) 43 (29.05)
About the same as usual 198 (19.43) -0.22 (2.4) 89 (17.02) 66 (18.38) 43 (29.05)
Somewhat more than usual 53 (5.20) 0.32 (3.8) 25 (4.78) 21 (5.85) 7 (4.73)
Much more than usual 47 (4.61) -0.77 (3.58) 22 (4.21) 22 (6.13) 3 (2.03)
Gone to my place of work
Much less than usual 395 (60.68) 0.36 (4.16) 0.011 214 (40.92) 137 (38.16) 44 (29.73) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 88 (13.52) 0.07 (3.63) 42 (8.03) 35 (9.75) 11 (7.43)
About the same as usual 109 (16.74) -0.42 (3.09) 52 (9.94) 38 (10.58) 19 (12.84)
Somewhat more than usual 26 (3.99) -1.15 (3.16) 7 (1.34) 18 (5.01) 1 (0.68)
Much more than usual 33 (5.07) -1.09 (3.95) 10 (1.91) 17 (4.74) 6 (4.05)
Virtually communicated with others (email, phone, videoconference, etc.)
Much less than usual 50 (5.20) -0.16 (5.15) 0.287 21 (4.02) 25 (6.98) 4 (2.7) 0.009
Somewhat less than usual 34 (3.54) -0.26 (2.53) 16 (3.06) 10 (2.79) 8 (5.41)
About the same as usual 253 (26.33) -0.15 (2.91) 113 (21.61) 90 (25.14) 50 (33.78)
Somewhat more than usual 188 (19.56) 0.18 (3.11) 104 (19.89) 59 (16.48) 25 (16.89)
Much more than usual 436 (45.37) 0.45 (3.83) 237 (45.32) 152 (42.46) 47 (31.76)
Have face-to-face contact with others
Much less than usual 600 (59.46) 0.41 (3.58) 0.068 325 (62.14) 195 (54.47) 80 (54.05) 0.224
Somewhat less than usual 215 (21.31) 0.14 (2.89) 108 (20.65) 76 (21.23) 31 (20.95)
About the same as usual 110 (10.90) -0.29 (2.8) 48 (9.18) 40 (11.17) 22 (14.86)
Somewhat more than usual 43 (4.26) -0.77 (3.32) 15 (2.87) 22 (6.15) 6 (4.05)
Much more than usual 41 (4.06) -0.61 (4.92) 18 (3.44) 17 (4.75) 6 (4.05)
Have in-person contact with those who live in my home
Much less than usual 117 (13.03) -0.2 (4.19) 0.620 58 (11.09) 53 (14.8) 6 (4.05) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 93 (10.36) 0.25 (3.37) 43 (8.22) 36 (10.06) 14 (9.46)
About the same as usual 475 (52.90) 0.1 (2.99) 242 (46.27) 141 (39.39) 92 (62.16)
Somewhat more than usual 85 (9.47) 0.51 (4.71) 42 (8.03) 32 (8.94) 11 (7.43)
Much more than usual 128 (14.25) 0.37 (3.5) 63 (12.05) 55 (15.36) 10 (6.76)
Have in-person contact with strangers
Much less than usual 660 (68.75) 0.35 (3.58) 0.179 365 (69.79) 211 (58.77) 84 (56.76) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 124 (12.92) 0.2 (2.6) 63 (12.05) 36 (10.03) 25 (16.89)
About the same as usual 105 (10.94) -0.13 (3.11) 40 (7.65) 42 (11.7) 23 (15.54)
Somewhat more than usual 34 (3.54) -0.12 (4.28) 6 (1.15) 23 (6.41) 5 (3.38)
Much more than usual 37 (3.85) -1 (4.45) 17 (3.25) 16 (4.46) 4 (2.7)
Traveled to another city/state/country
Much less than usual 557 (74.07) 0.43 (3.85) 0.116 312 (59.66) 172 (47.91) 73 (49.32) <0.001
Somewhat less than usual 72 (9.57) 0.15 (3.34) 26 (4.97) 37 (10.31) 9 (6.08)
About the same as usual 69 (9.18) -0.32 (2.79) 30 (5.74) 31 (8.64) 8 (5.41)
Somewhat more than usual 32 (4.26) -0.19 (3.79) 9 (1.72) 17 (4.74) 6 (4.05)
Much more than usual 22 (2.93) -1.14 (5.05) 9 (1.72) 11 (3.06) 2 (1.35)

a. Political Ideology calculated using the 10-item Political Polarization in the American Public Survey [11]

b. Not applicable coded as missing

c. Change in PHQ-9 score calculated as after-before such that a positive number indicates an increase in depressive symptoms [17]

d. p-value derived from F test

e. p-value derived from Chi-square test

In answer to a direct question, 33.3% reported that their perceived mental health was worse than before the pandemic, while 15.8% reported their mental health was better. We examined the change in depressive symptoms using the change in PHQ-9 scores. For behavior changes related to in-person contact with family, close friends, and colleagues, as contact decreased, there was a slight, but statistically significant increase in depressive symptoms (Table 4). A similar pattern was seen for frequenting your usual place of work, restaurants and bars, and stores. There was no statistically significant association between reduction in travel or contact with strangers and depressive symptoms.

Overall, respondents indicated highest mean agreement that COVID-19 had interrupted their social life (mean: 4.44; SD: 1.61). Table 5 provides the mean agreement scores and t-test analyses for differences in social life, work life, home life, and mental health interruptions due to COVID-19 overall and across child and work status. Those with children at home indicated higher agreement that the pandemic had interrupted social, work, and home lives and hurt their mental health compared to those who did not have children at home. Those who were working before the pandemic similarly reported higher levels of interruption for social, work, and home life and worse mental health as opposed to those who were not working.

Table 5. Life disruption due to COVID-19: Overall, for those with/without children in the home, and for those working/not working for pay.

Total Sample Children under 18 living in the home Working for pay before COVID-19
Yes No Yes No
Mean (SD)a Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p-valueb Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p-valueb
Events related to COVID-19 have…
 interrupted my social life 4.44 (1.61) 4.77 (1.44) 4.3 (1.66) <0.001 4.66 (1.48) 4.17 (1.73) <0.001
 interrupted my work life or vocation 3.70 (2.13) 4.59 (1.72) 3.33 (2.18) <0.001 4.65 (1.65) 2.53 (2.06) <0.001
 interrupted my home life 3.58 (1.95) 4.27 (1.74) 3.29 (1.96) <0.001 3.83 (1.89) 3.26 (1.98) <0.001
 have hurt my mental health 2.91 (2.05) 3.5 (2) 2.67 (2.02) <0.001 3.31 (2.03) 2.43 (1.96) <0.001

a. Anxiety scores were coded from 0–6 and averaged such that higher scores indicate higher levels of anxiety

b. p-value derived from Chi-square test

The sample was generally knowledgeable about COVID-19. The vast majority (93.6%) correctly identified that the World Health Organization had declared COVID-19 a global pandemic. Nearly all correctly identified that COVID-19 was spread by respiratory droplets from coughs and sneezes (90.0%), and by touching infected surfaces followed by touching your face (91.9%). A smaller, but still large percentage, (77.7%) correctly identified that at the time the survey was distributed (March 31st, 2020) there was no vaccination for COVID-19. A bivariate analysis showed that general knowledge largely differed by media bias and sociodemographic characteristics (sex, race, poverty level, and education). However, after adjusting for all parameters using logistic regression, education was no longer significant, sex was only significant on two of the four questions, while race and income were significant on three of the four questions (Table 6). While media bias was not significant for most questions, removing it from the model worsened model fit.

Table 6. Sociodemographic and media influences on general knowledge of COVID-19.

The World Health Organization has declared COVID-19 a global pandemic (true) There is a currently available vaccine for COVID-19 (false) COVID-19 is spread person-to-person through inhalation of respiratory droplets when and infected person coughs or sneezes (true) One can contract COVID-19 by touching infected surfaces and then touching your nose or mouth (true)
Frequency Fully Adjusted Frequency Fully Adjusted Frequency Fully Adjusted Frequency Fully Adjusted
n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI) n (%) aOR (95% CI)
News Bias Scorea
 Leans Liberal 430 (94.5) 1.1 (0.5–2.4) 367 (80.8) 1.4 (0.9–2.2) 418 (91.9) 2.2 (1.3–3.8) 423 (93.0) 1.5 (0.8–2.8)
 Moderate 315 (92.9) 0.9 (0.5–2.0) 260 (76.7) 1.0 (0.7–1.6) 312 (92.0) 2.2 (1.2–3.9) 312 (92.0) 1.3 (0.6–2.5)
 Leans Conservative 151 (93.2) 1.0 124 (76.5) 1.0 137 (84.6) 1.0 147 (90.7) 1.0
Sex
 Female 503 (93.0) -- 413 (76.5) -- 496 (91.7) 1.5 (1.0–2.4) 507 (93.7) 2.3 (1.4–3.9)
 Male 461 (94.3) -- 387 (79.1) -- 431 (88.1) 1.0 440 (90.0) 1.0
Race
 White 479 (94.9) -- 426 (84.4) 1.0 462 (91.5) 1.0 482 (95.5) 1.0
 Black 221 (91.7) -- 157 (65.2) 0.4 (0.3–0.5) 207 (85.9) 0.4 (0.3–0.7) 211 (87.6) 0.3 (0.1–0.5)
 Hispanic 110 (89.4) -- 82 (66.7) 0.3 (0.2–0.5) 106 (86.2) 0.5 (0.3–1.0) 108 (87.8) 0.3 (0.1–0.7)
 Asian 122 (96.1) -- 107 (84.3) 0.8 (0.5–1.4) 120 (94.5) 1.4 (0.6–3.3) 115 (90.6) 0.3 (0.1–0.7)
 Other 32 (94.1) -- 28 (84.9) 1.3 (0.4–3.9) 32 (94.1) 2.3 (0.3–17.3) 31 (91.2) 0.6 (0.1–2.9)
Income
 <100%FPL 215 (89.6) 1.0 160 (66.7) 1.00 211 (87.9) -- 209 (87.1) 1.0
 100–199% FPL 158 (95.2) 2.1 (0.9–5.2) 135 (81.3) 2.0 (1.2–3.4) 148 (89.2) -- 159 (95.8) 3.3 (1.3–8.3)
 200–299% FPL 174 (91.1) 1.0 (0.5–1.9) 159 (78.0) 1.4 (0.9–2.3) 166 (86.9) -- 173 (90.6) 1.2 (0.6–2.3)
 300–399% FPL 114 (94.2) 1.6 (0.6–3.8) 97 (80.2) 1.6 (0.9–2.9) 109 (90.1) -- 113 (93.4) 1.8 (0.7–4.3)
 400+%FPL 303 (97.1) 3.4 (1.5–7.6) 259 (83.3) 1.8 (1.2–2.9) 293 (93.9) -- 293 (93.9) 1.7 (0.9–3.4)
Education
 1 (Less than HS) 11 (84.6) -- 6 (46.2) -- 12 (92.3) -- 12 (92.3) --
 2 (HS or GED) 174 (89.2) -- 139 (71.3) -- 170 (87.2) -- 172 (88.2) --
 3 (Some college) 226 (93.0) -- 186 (76.5) -- 218 (89.7) -- 222 (91.4) --
 4 (Assoc Deg) 130 (96.3) -- 101 (74.8) -- 116 (85.9) -- 124 (91.9) --
 5 (Bach Deg) 262 (93.9) -- 235 (84.2) -- 253 (90.7) -- 258 (92.5) --
 6 (Mast Deg) 125 (98.4) -- 101 (80.2) -- 122 (96.1) -- 121 (95.3) --
 7 (PhD or equiv) 36 (94.7) -- 32 (84.2) -- 36 (94.7) -- 38 (100.0) --

a. News Score Bias were calculated using the Ad Fontes Media’s source evaluation and was averaged for each respondent [16]

NOTE: The correct answer to these questions reflect the knowledge about COVID-19 at the time the survey was distributed (March 31st 2020).

Compared to white respondents, Black and Hispanic respondents had 0.4 (95%CI: 0.3–0.5) and 0.3 (95%CI: 0.2–0.5) times the odds of correctly reporting that there was not a vaccine for COVID-19 at the time of the survey (March 31st, 2020); Black respondents had 0.4 (95%CI: 0.3–0.7) times the odds of correctly reporting that COVID-19 is primarily transferred through respiratory droplets; and Black, Hispanic, and Asian respondents had 0.3 (95%CI: 0.1–0.5), 0.3 (95%CI: 0.1–0.7), and 0.3 (95%CI: 0.1–0.7) times the odds of correctly reporting that one can contract COVID-19 by touching infected surfaces and then touching one’s nose or mouth.

Compared to men, women had 2.3 times the odds (95%CI: 1.4–3.9) of correctly reporting that one can contract COVID-19 by touching infected surfaces and then touching one’s nose or mouth. Compared to those whose news bias score leaned conservative, those whose news bias score was moderate or leaned liberal had 2.2 (95%CI: 1.2–3.9) and 2.2 (95%CI: 1.3–3.8) times the odds of correctly reporting that COVID-19 is primarily transferred through respiratory droplets.

Greater than 90% of respondents correctly identified fever, cough, and shortness of breath as symptoms for COVID-19. However, a majority also said that nausea (69.5%), aches (53.0%), and nasal congestion (66.7%) were common symptoms of COVID-19. Likewise, more than 85% of respondents correctly identified hand washing (94.1%), not touching your face (90.4%), avoiding contact with sick persons (88.0%), avoiding large groups (89.8%) and sanitizing surfaces (88.6%) as recommendations from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) to prevent COVID-19. A smaller, but still large percentage identified avoiding eating in restaurants and bars (70.6%) as recommended. A little over half (50.4%) correctly identified wearing a facemask in public as not being an official recommendation of the CDC. This recommendation was released on April 3rd, 2020 (four days after the survey was administered).

In comparing COVID-19 to seasonal influenza, the majority (65.5%) correctly identified COVID-19 as having a higher case-fatality rate. However, 22% felt that seasonal influenza and COVID-19 had similar risk of death and 12% reported that seasonal influenza was more deadly than COVID-19. Those who were politically conservative were more likely (p-value <0.001) to say that the seasonal influenza was more deadly than COVID-19 (25.7%) compared to moderates (10.3%) and liberals (9.9%).

Discussion

Political ideology was the strongest factor associated with attitudes toward the COVID-19 response. This finding is consistent with research suggesting that as new politicized issues emerge, ideology is predictive of adopting beliefs which are suggested to be consistent with an ideology. [18] Suggestions of beliefs that correspond with ideology may be implied by the deliverer of information (e.g. a conservative or liberal lawmaker) or through language cues in information sources, such as media.

Political ideology was further associated with behavior change surrounding COVID-19. This finding is consistent with the Theory of Planned Behavior [9]. As political ideology was associated with attitudes toward the COVID-19 response, it is reasonable to assume that those with attitudes suggesting government or community over-response to the pandemic would be associated with beliefs that recommended behavior changes were unnecessary.

The U.S. political climate continues to affect individual, organizational, and governmental responses as the pandemic evolves. However, our results suggest that, even early in the pandemic, political ideology played a large role in the attitudes and behaviors adopted by U.S. adults. The ability of political ideology (and related measurements such as news source bias) to predict an individual’s attitudes about and adherence to recommended behaviors in response to a public health crisis raises concerns about the efficacy of existing strategies to manage such crises in this era of extreme politicization. [8] This suggests the necessity of developing politically neutral strategies that facilitate effective communication surrounding public health crises.

Sociodemographic characteristics were associated with pandemic-related economic anxieties (sex, income, and race), attitudes toward the community response (sex and race), and knowledge (race) about COVID-19. Many of these discrepancies point to persistent gender, income, and racial inequality in the U.S. These phenomena are particularly well illustrated when analyzing the disproportionate burden of economic anxieties felt by minority races, lower income individuals, and females. Higher economic anxiety would be expected among those in lower income brackets, as they have a reduced ability to weather income loss or unexpected expenses. It is also unsurprising that racial minorities in the U.S. are experiencing higher economic anxieties, given the conflation of poverty and race in the U.S.

Increased economic anxiety in females is consistent with other research. This may be at least partially explained by poorer perceived economic stability relative to males. [19] Disparities in care-giving responsibilities may also help explain sex differences in economic anxieties. Females generally have more care-giving responsibilities for home, children, and family, as dictated by societal tradition. [20] Responsibility for maintaining family schedules and routines during this pandemic would likely add disproportionately to the physical and emotional strain on females in the U.S.

While general knowledge about COVID-19 was high, most respondents also identified symptoms including nausea, aches, and nasal congestion which were not part of the initial symptom list. This finding may reflect the emerging nature of information about COVID-19 or inaccurate information spreading by word-of-mouth rather than official sources. While general knowledge about COVID-19 was widely exhibited across most sociodemographic and political characteristics (a promising demonstration of the wide reception of public health messages and recommendations), Black and Hispanic respondents were generally less likely to respond correctly to knowledge questions, which may be a result of a larger proportion of Black and Hispanic respondents lacking access to adequate resources or receiving misinformation. This is particularly concerning given racial differences in the rate of severe complications and deaths from COVID-19. [21] At the time of writing, 48% of deaths due to COVID-19 in Chicago had occurred in African Americans, despite the fact that their percent of confirmed cases (33%) mirrored their proportion of the Chicago population (30%). [21] Unfortunately, these racial and economic disparities mirror well-documented disparities for many other respiratory infectious diseases, including severe outcomes from influenza. These disparities, rooted in historic, racially-motivated policies, limit African Americans’ access to care and information and exacerbate factors that place them at higher risk for pre-existing conditions.

After only two weeks of social distancing in most areas of the country, one-third of respondents reported worse mental health than before COVID-19. This finding is consistent with research which identifies social isolation as a significant factor in mental health. As social distancing fundamentally requires separation from most sources of community (i.e. work, religious communities, friends, family, etc.), increases in loneliness as the pandemic progresses may be expected. [22] Social isolation and loneliness are linked to significant increases in morbidity and mortality, which raises concerns about population well-being in the event of protracted social distancing and supports the need to find means of social connection that are consistent with social distancing recommendations. [23]

Disruption to social, work, and home life and worsened mental health due to COVID-19 were higher for those with children at home and for those who were working for pay before the pandemic. Due to school closures, many with children at home are managing new roles as full-time caregivers and managing educational activities, often while maintaining their own employment responsibilities. Higher levels of disruption and worsened mental health among the employed likely results from disruption of daily routines, job insecurity, or an absence of valued social interaction.

Strengths & limitations

We acknowledge that these results were based off a cross-sectional study regarding an emerging infection. At the time of data collection, information about COVID-19 was nascent. Knowledge, best practices, attitudes, and impacts have rapidly changed since the collection of these data. Therefore, the generalizability of our results are limited to adult populations in the U.S. during the early weeks of the pandemic’s influence in the U.S. Nevertheless, early stage information regarding this pandemic, may prove useful for future outbreaks of emerging infections.

This study was conducted approximately two weeks after implementation of initial social distancing guidelines. As such, it provides the opportunity to examine the early impacts of COVID-19 and associated social distancing in the U.S. population. Although this provides useful information, it is unlikely to represent the attitudes, anxieties, and behaviors of the population throughout the pandemic. Quota sampling for sex, race, and income provided a sample that is statistically similar to the overall population of U.S. adults; however, samples derived from internet panels may differ in unmeasurable ways from the U.S. population. Our sample under-represents households with children at home (28.9% of sample vs. 45.0% U.S. households). [10]

Conclusion

These findings underscore the need to develop public health messaging that considers the influence of the political climate. Strict fact-based messaging may simply be insufficient to engage the community in desired public health actions, particularly for highly politicized events such as COVID-19. Public health experts should consider differential messaging that appeals to the values of those across the political spectrum.

Acknowledgments

We thank William F. Christensen for his comments and feedback.

Data Availability

We have published our data as well as the accompanying codebook and survey on OpenICPSR. https://doi.org/10.3886/E119629V1.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): Cases and deaths in the U.S. August 20, 2020. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/cases-updates/us-cases-deaths.html
  • 2.Lai J, Ma S, Wang Y, et al. Factors associated with mental health outcomes among health care workers exposed to coronavirus disease 2019. JAMA Network Open. 2020;3(3):e203976 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, Xu Y. A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: implications and policy recommendations. General Psychiatry. 2020;33(2). 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rossi R, Socci V, Pacitti F, et al. Mental health outcomes among frontline and second-line health care workers during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic in Italy. JAMA Network Open. 2020;3(5):e2010185 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10185 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Talevi D, Socci V, Carai M, et al. Mental health outcomes of the COVID-19 pandemic. Rivista di Psichiatria. 2020;55(3):137–144. 10.1708/3382.33569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020;17(5):1729 10.3390/ijerph17051729 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Reynolds B, Quinn SC. Effective communication during an influenza pandemic: The value of using a crisis and emergency risk communication framework. Health Promotion Practice. 2008;9(4):13S–17S. The remote server returned an error: (404) Not Found. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Littlefield RS, Quenette AM. Crisis leadership and Hurricane Katrina: The portrayal of authority by the media in natural disasters. Journal of Applied Communication Research. 2007;35(1):26–47. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 1991;50(2):179–211. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.U.S. Census Bureau. Families by Presence of Own Children Under 18. 2019.
  • 11.Doherty C. Political polarization in the American public: How increasing ideological uniformity and partisan antipathy affect politics, compromise and everyday life. Pew Research Center. 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fiorina MP, Abrams SJ. Political polarization in the American public. Annual Review of Political Science. 2008;11:563–588. 10.2139/ssrn.1010098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Leiserowitz A, Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Smith N. Global warming’s six Americas, May 2011. Yale University and George Mason University. 2011.
  • 14.SAGE Working Group on Vaccine Hesitancy. Vaccine hesitancy survey questions related to SAGE vaccine hesitancy matrix: Examples of survey questions designed to assess determinants of vaccine hesitancy. WHO;2018.
  • 15.Newman N, Fletcher R, Kalogeropoulos A, Nielsen R. Reuters institute digital news report 2019. Reuters Institute for the Study of Journalism;2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ad Fontes Media. The media bias chart version 5.1. 2020 [https://www.adfontesmedia.com/static-mbc/?v=402f03a963ba].
  • 17.Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of general internal medicine. 2001;16(9):606–613. 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Malka A, Lelkes Y. More than ideology: conservative–liberal identity and receptivity to political cues. Social Justice Research. 2010;23(2):156–188. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Edison Research. Women and economic anxiety. Marketplace-Edison Research Poll. 2019.
  • 20.Bianchi SM, Sayer LC, Milkie MA, Robinson JP. Housework: Who did, does or will do it, and how much does it matter? Social Forces. 2012;91(1):55–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Reyes C, Husain N, Gutowski C, Clair SS, Pratt G. Chicago’s coronavirus disparity: Black Chicagoans are dying at nearly six times the rate of white residents, data show. Chicago Tribune. 2020.
  • 22.Leigh-Hunt N, Bagguley D, Bash K, et al. An overview of systematic reviews on the public health consequences of social isolation and loneliness. Public Health. 2017;152:157–171. 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.07.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Holt-Lunstad J, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D, Smith TB. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: A meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2015;10(2):227–237. 10.1177/1745691614568352 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Geilson Lima Santana

29 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-15888

Political and personal reactions to COVID-19 during initial weeks of social distancing in the United States

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Magnusson,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 12 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Geilson Lima Santana, M.D., Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

Dear author, thank you for your submission about such an important issue.

However, I believe some work needs to be done in order to improve it.

I suggest structuring your paper according to the Strobe Guideline for cross-sectional studies: https://www.strobe-statement.org/index.php?id=available-checklists

I've also noticied you haven't numbered all pages and lines throughout the paper. PlosOne guidelines asks to include page numbers and line numbers in the manuscript file. Use continuous line numbers (do not restart the numbering on each page). https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines

Please, pay especial attention to reviewers comments and suggestions.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have indicated that proceeding to complete the survey was taken as implied consent in your Ethics statement. However, the manuscript indicates that informed consent was provided prior to the survey. Please correct and clarify. If implied consent please indicate whether this consent method was reviewed and approved by your Institutional Review Board.

3. Please note that PLOS ONE uses a single-blind peer review procedure. We would therefore be grateful if you could include in the information that has been redacted for peer review in the manuscript.

4. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

5. We note that you have included the phrase “data not shown” in your manuscript. Unfortunately, this does not meet our data sharing requirements. PLOS does not permit references to inaccessible data. We require that authors provide all relevant data within the paper, Supporting Information files, or in an acceptable, public repository. Please add a citation to support this phrase or upload the data that corresponds with these findings to a stable repository (such as Figshare or Dryad) and provide and URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers that may be used to access these data. Or, if the data are not a core part of the research being presented in your study, we ask that you remove the phrase that refers to these data.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Comments to authors

In this article, authors investigated perceptions, behaviors and impacts surrounding COVID-19 early in the pandemic response in the U.S. general population. A web-based survey was administered to 1030 U.S. adults. The survey assessed political ideology, scientific trust, and media consumption, as well as demographic information, mental health, attitudes, anxieties, impacts, and knowledge related to COVID-19. Results showed that factors such as political ideology played a large role in the attitudes and behaviors adopted by U.S. adults early in the pandemic. Further, one-third of respondents reported worse mental health than before COVID-19.

The study is well designed. The methods are adequately described and the results are clearly presented.

The manuscript has the potential to provide a beneficial addition to the current research concerning the impact of the COVID-19 global emergency. However, there is one main suggestion below.

- There are some key citations left out. Since the study also evaluated mental health outcomes such as depressive symptoms, recent literature concerning the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health should be considered. In the Introduction section, it would be good to provide a brief overview of the few available studies investigating COVID-19-related mental health outcomes.

In this regard, some recent articles:

Qiu, J., Shen, B., Zhao, M., Wang, Z., Xie, B., and Xu, Y. (2020). A nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: Implications and policy recommendations. Gen. Psychiatry 33, 19–21. doi:10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213.

Rossi R, Socci V, Pacitti F, et al. Mental Health Outcomes Among Frontline and Second-Line Health Care Workers During the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19) Pandemic in Italy. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(5):e2010185. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10185

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., et al. (2020). Immediate Psychological Responses and Associated Factors during the Initial Stage of the 2019 Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) Epidemic among the General Population in China. Int J Env. Res Public Heal. 17. doi:10.3390/ijerph17051729.

Talevi D, Socci V, Carai M, Carnaghi G, Faleri S, Pacitti F (2020) Mental health outcomes of the COVID-19 pandemics. Gli esiti della salute mentale della pandemia di CoViD-19. Rivista Psichiatria 55(3):137-144 doi: 10.1708/3382.33569.

Lai, J., Ma, S., Wang, Y., Cai, Z., Hu, J., Wei, N., et al. (2020). Factors Associated With Mental Health Outcomes Among Health Care Workers Exposed to Coronavirus Disease 2019. JAMA Netw. open 3, e203976. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3976.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript lacks a solid theoretical foundation and the exposition of the background is limited, the introductory section is insufficient to justify the need to carry out the investigation.

The methodology section has not been described in detail, nor the characteristics of the sample, nor statistical aspects for the estimation of the sample size, nor are data provided on the validity and reliability of the instruments used for data collection ...

The results are based on descriptive statistics, so the scope of the derived statements is limited.

Finally, the discussion and conclusions do not meet the minimum standards, one of the reasons is the poor theoretical foundation in the introduction, and this does not allow a good discussion of the data.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Geilson Lima Santana

14 Sep 2020

Political and personal reactions to COVID-19 during initial weeks of social distancing in the United States

PONE-D-20-15888R1

Dear Dr. Magnusson,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Geilson Lima Santana, M.D., Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Geilson Lima Santana

17 Sep 2020

PONE-D-20-15888R1

Political and personal reactions to COVID-19 during initial weeks of social distancing in the United States

Dear Dr. Magnusson:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Geilson Lima Santana

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    We have published our data as well as the accompanying codebook and survey on OpenICPSR. https://doi.org/10.3886/E119629V1.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES