Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Sep 30;10:16096. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72317-8

Functional parcellation of the default mode network: a large-scale meta-analysis

Shaoming Wang 1,2,#, Lindsey J Tepfer 1,#, Adrienne A Taren 3,#, David V Smith 1,
PMCID: PMC7528067  PMID: 32999307

Abstract

The default mode network (DMN) consists of several regions that selectively interact to support distinct domains of cognition. Of the various sites that partake in DMN function, the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), temporal parietal junction (TPJ), and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) are frequently identified as key contributors. Yet, it remains unclear whether these subcomponents of the DMN make unique contributions to specific cognitive processes and health conditions. To address this issue, we applied a meta-analytic parcellation approach used in prior work. This approach used the Neurosynth database and classification methods to quantify the association between PCC, TPJ, and MPFC activation and specific topics related to cognition and health (e.g., decision making and smoking). Our analyses replicated prior observations that the PCC, TPJ, and MPFC collectively support multiple cognitive functions such as decision making, memory, and awareness. To gain insight into the functional organization of each region, we parceled each region based on its coactivation pattern with the rest of the brain. This analysis indicated that each region could be further subdivided into functionally distinct subcomponents. Taken together, we further delineate DMN function by demonstrating the relative strengths of association among subcomponents across a range of cognitive processes and health conditions. A continued attentiveness to the specialization within the DMN allows future work to consider the nuances in sub-regional contributions necessary for healthy cognition, as well as create the potential for more targeted treatment protocols in various health conditions.

Subject terms: Decision, Cognitive neuroscience, Diseases of the nervous system, Reward, Social neuroscience

Introduction

The default mode network (DMN) underlies a striking array of divergent processes, from interpreting a social exchange to contemplating future plans. As a result of this broad functional spectrum, the DMN is a focal point of active interrogation: decades of research detail its involvement in a wide range of healthy cognitive functions and psychiatric illnesses13. For instance, early in its discovery, the DMN earned a reputation for self-referential processing4 and mind-wandering5 after evidence emerged that the network played a role in spontaneous thought processes when study participants were not actively engaged in a directed task. Later, studies involving autobiographical memory retrieval6, self-judgements7,8, prospective thinking6, decision-making9,10, and social cognition11 were also found to evoke activation across the DMN, indicating that the same networked areas may play an indirect role across these psychological processes10. Ongoing research revealed that the DMN tended to engage many of the same subregions, particularly the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and left and right temporal-parietal junction (left- and right-TPJ)12,13. These four regions have been shown to preferentially activate when the brain is at rest, and decrease when engaged in a goal-directed task14.

In addition to studies linking the DMN to various cognitive processes, recent efforts have explored the DMN’s role in health problems, including psychopathology and neurological disorders. A growing body of literature suggests that DMN dysfunction may underlie disease states including Alzheimer’s disease, schizophrenia, ADHD, Parkinson’s disease, depression, and anxiety15. Decreased activity of the DMN at rest and decreased task-induced deactivation of the DMN has been observed in individuals with autism16,17, particularly in the MPFC18,19. Patients with anxiety disorders show reduced deactivation of MPFC and increased deactivation of PCC15, while the component regions of DMN appear to change during major depressive episodes, with activity of the thalamus and subgenual cingulate increasingly seen at rest20. Alzheimer’s patients not only show altered DMN responses at rest, but different task-induced deactivation patterns during a working memory task21 and regional activation differences within the PCC22. In schizophrenia, both resting state and task-based DMN response changes have been associated with positive disease symptoms3,15,23.

While the aforementioned disease states share the commonality of generally altered DMN function, the findings from these studies also suggest that altered activity in different DMN nodes may be specific to different health conditions; for instance, with the PCC specifically implicated in Alzheimer’s and ADHD, and the MPFC in schizophrenia and anxiety15. However, despite the growing body of knowledge regarding the roles of these four DMN subregions in both the cognitive and psychiatric domains, it remains unclear whether further subdividing these regions would reveal distinct patterns of activity that show relative differences among one another in their contributions to various cognitive process and mental disorders alike.

Evidence for subregional specialization for a range of cognitive functions has been heavily documented in the MPFC in particular, exemplifying how a region may play different roles in cognitive function dependent upon subregional activation2426. For instance, the MPFC as a whole appears to play an important role in self-referential processing, but shows a divergence in activity among its subregions: the ventral MPFC deactivates more during self-referential judgments while the dorsal MPFC activity increases8. This ventral-dorsal subspecialization within the MPFC appears to extend to other cognitive domains, with ventral MPFC responsible for emotional processing and dorsal for cognitive function8. A recent meta-analysis further delineated regional differences in MPFC’s function, identifying anterior, middle, and posterior zones, responsible for episodic memory and social processing, cognitive control, and motor function, respectively27. This previous work suggests there may be overlap in function and sub-specialization within MPFC regions27, opening the door for further research into the more fine-grained aspects of MPFC’s function.

Although these previous lines of investigation into DMN functional specialization have largely focused on the MPFC, the PCC and the bilateral TPJ may also serve a wide range of functions. For example, the PCC is thought to play a key role in focused attention during task-based activities28,29 and continuous monitoring of internal and external stimuli at rest2. It has also been implicated in episodic memory retrieval30,31, emotional processing32, and self-referential processing33. Similarly, the TPJ34 has been shown to play a role in self-referential processing4, and is important for social cognition along with other DMN regions (PCC and MPFC11,35). Less is known about potential functional subspecialization within the PCC and TPJ, and it is possible that these DMN regions may also show intra-regional differences in cognitive processing similar to MPFC36,37.

To build a better understanding of whether cognitive processes and health conditions are linked to activation within distinct subunits of the DMN, we used the Neurosynth database and identified core DMN regions based on prior work and anatomical landmarks. We also used tools and methods developed in prior work38,39 that have successfully characterized the functional substructure of the medial prefrontal cortex27 and the lateral prefrontal cortex40. Specifically, we employed a naïve Bayesian classifier to quantify the association between activation in four particular DMN nodes—the PCC, TPJ, and MPFC activation—and topics pertaining to cognition and health. Our application of these analytical methods to four key regions of the DMN is important because it allowed us to quantify how those DMN regions are functionally associated with cognitive processes and disease states—thus extending and complementing prior efforts to fractionate the DMN12,36,37,39,4146. Our primary analyses focused on two key questions. First, are different psychological functions and disease states preferentially associated with distinct nodes of the DMN? Second, are there functionally distinct subregions within individual DMN nodes?

Results

To determine how the PCC, MPFC, and TPJ coactivated with the rest of the brain, we begun by searching for the term “DMN” in Neurosynth to create a functional mask, which we then constrained to the 4 anatomical regions of interest to the present study (PCC, MPFC, left and right TPJ) using the Harvard–Oxford probabilistic atlas in FSL (Fig. 1A). Note that our TPJ region corresponded to what was labeled as “posterior-TPJ” in a previous study47. After generating the DMN mask through functional mapping, we applied k-means clustering to determine whether the four subregions of the DMN showed functional segregation. When evaluating coactivation and functional preference profiles for the whole DMN, our contrasts reflect comparisons across DMN nodes; when investigating functional segregation of each DMN node, contrasts within a single DMN region represent comparisons among the clusters within an individual region. We note that our instantiation of our naïve Bayesian classifier, which was used to determine whether topics predicted activity in a region or cluster, used a fourfold cross-validation scheme, and classification accuracy was assessed by calculating the mean score across all four folds, scored using the area under the curve of the receiver operating characteristic (AUC-ROC). Permutation-based analyses accounted for the variability in the standard deviation values across folds.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Overview of methods. (A) We searched for the topic “DMN” in Neurosynth to create a functional mask, and then constrained the mask to 4 anatomical regions that belong to the DMN by using the Harvard–Oxford probabilistic atlas. (B) We applied k-means clustering to determine functionally different subregions within each of the 4 regions. (C) We generated whole-brain coactivation profiles to reveal functional networks for different regions. (D) Functional profiles were generated to identify which cognitive processes or health conditions best predicted each region’s (or subregion’s) activity. Figure was created using Python and Adobe Illustrator.

Coactivation and functional preference profiles for the DMN regions

Next, we sought to characterize functional similarities and differences across DMN regions. To determine how these four regions within the DMN coactivate with voxels across the brain, we adopted an approach used by previous work27,40, and identified voxels with a greater probability of coactivating with each ROI relative to the other regions within the DMN. First, we found that the four ROIs within the DMN coactivate with each other: the PCC and MPFC strongly coactivated, and also showed coactivation with both the left and right TPJ (Fig. 1B). This pattern corresponds with long-standing evidence that coactivation among the various components within the DMN represents a typical characteristic of the network. In addition to the “collective” coactivation pattern among the four ROIs, we also found that the four regions showed coactivation overlap in ways that were not shared among all four ROIs. For instance, relative to the MPFC, the PCC and bilateral TPJ showed stronger coactivation with the hippocampus. Additionally, we found that relative to the PCC and bilateral TPJ, the MPFC showed stronger coactivation with both the amygdala and the ventral striatum, regions known for emotion processing and decision-making (Fig. 1B). Putting our coactivation results in perspective, these patterns demonstrate that there are both functional similarities and differences that characterize how these four regions within the DMN may operate relative to one another.

To further explore functional properties among DMN regions, we performed a meta-analysis to select studies that activated a given ROI, and then trained a naive Bayesian classifier to predict which studies activated the region27. From our classifier, we extracted the log odds ratio to determine whether a topic predicts activation in a particular cluster, with positive values indicating that a topic is indeed predictive of cluster activation. We first described functional preference profiles based on cognitive processes, followed by those related to health conditions. Cognitive predictors were limited to 20 psychological topics—words that represent co-occurrence among fMRI study abstracts within the Neurosynth database, e.g., cognitive processes and health conditions—previously shown to be relevant to DMN function. We found that all four regions of interest within the DMN were primarily associated with the topics “social”, “decision making”, “awareness”, and “memory” (Fig. 1C), consistent with previous evidence suggesting DMN involvement across these domains. We also found that activity among the four DMN regions were differentially associated with certain topics: fear, emotion and reward topics were associated with activity in the MPFC; emotion topics were associated with PCC activity; and math, semantics, and reading topics were associated with activity in the left TPJ. Next, we assessed the 8 disorder-related topics to examine whether regions within the DMN were differentially associated with various health conditions as well. We found that some disorders were associated with the MPFC or PCC: topics relating to smoking, eating disorder and depression were associated with activity in the MPFC, and topics pertaining to smoking and Alzheimer’s/Parkinson’s disease were associated with activity in the PCC (Fig. 1D). These results are consistent with observed coactivation patterns among regions within the DMN, again supporting the notion that there are functional similarities as well as differences among these four regions of the DMN, including function relating to psychiatric and health conditions.

Next, we evaluated whether the strengths of associations between a topic and a particular region were significantly distinct compared to other regions within the DMN. Our post hoc analysis suggested that despite their associations, none of the topics showed a statistically greater preference for one region over another within the DMN (or any of the subregions in the results that follow). Note that we caution interpretation of these results because these comparisons were post hoc and exploratory27 (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Meta-analytic coactivation and functional profiles for the DMN. (A) Functionally defined regions within DMN: medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), left and right temporal parietal junction (left- and right-TPJ). (B) Coactivation profiles for 4 regions. Colored voxels indicate significantly greater coactivation with the region of same color (A) than control regions. Some regions were involved in overlapping functional networks whereas some did not show the same degree of overlap across functional networks. Related subcortical structures are labeled as: Hipp hippocampus, Amyg amygdala, VS ventral striatum. Strength of association with cognitive and disorder topics measured by LOR. (C) Functional profiles were generated by determining which cognitive topics best predicted each DMN cluster’s activity. All regions within DMN were primarily involved with social, decision making, awareness, and memory. Distinct functions were also observed across MPFC, PCC and left-TPJ. Log-odds ratio (LOR) is used to measure strength of association, and color-coded dots corresponding to each region are used to indicate significance (p < 0.05, corrected) based on permutation testing. (D) DMN and health conditions. Functional profiles related to health conditions were generated to determine whether regions within the DMN were differentially recruited by psychological diseases.

Functional distinctions within DMN subregions

We clustered individual voxels inside the MPFC, bilateral-TPJ and PCC based on their meta-analytic coactivation with voxels in the rest of the brain to elucidate more fine-grained functional differences among subregions27,48,49. We used silhouette scoring to select optimal solutions for each region, and generated coactivation and functional preferences profiles for each subregion. We describe the results for four regions separately.

Within the MPFC, we identified two clusters based on the silhouette score (Fig. 3A, left panel): a dorsal cluster (Fig. 3A right panel, orange) and a ventral cluster (Fig. 3A right panel, green). Our analysis did not reveal that either cluster coactivated more strongly with the rest of the brain. When exploring which topics predicted activation in these clusters, we found that both the dorsal and ventral MPFC clusters were associated with social, emotion, reward, decision-making, awareness, and memory; whereas the ventral cluster was associated with fear (Fig. 3B). Additionally, both clusters in MPFC were associated with depression and smoking, and the ventral cluster was associated with eating disorders (Fig. 3C; again, with no significant difference between the strength of association among cluster and topic associations).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Coactivation-based clustering and functional preference profiles of MPFC. (A) We identified two clusters within MPFC based on silhouette scoring: a dorsal cluster (orange) and a ventral cluster (green). Strength of association with cognitive and disorder topics measured by LOR. (B) Functional preference profiles of the MPFC for cognitive topics. Both clusters in MPFC were predicted by social, emotion, reward, decision-making, awareness and memory, whereas the ventral cluster was predicted by fear. (C) MPFC and health conditions. Both clusters in the MPFC were recruited by depression and smoking, but only the ventral cluster was associated with eating disorders.

Our silhouette score analysis revealed that a three-cluster solution was optimal for the right-TPJ (Fig. 4A, left panel): a dorsal cluster (Fig. 4A, green), a ventral one (Fig. 4A, yellow) and a posterior cluster (Fig. 4A, purple). All three clusters were strongly coactivated with the left-TPJ (Fig. 4B). The ventral cluster showed more coactivation with the PCC. Although topic-cluster associations did not differ amongst each other, activity in all three clusters were associated with social, memory, and awareness topics, with decision making associated with the dorsal cluster (Fig. 4C). No subregions showed any association with disorder-related topics (Fig. 4D).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Coactivation-based clustering and functional preference profiles of the right-TPJ. (A) We identified three clusters within the right-TPJ using silhouette scoring: a dorsal cluster (green), a ventral cluster (yellow) and a posterior cluster (purple). Strength of association with cognitive and disorder topics measured by LOR. (B) Coactivation contrasts for the right-TPJ. All clusters strongly coactivated with the left-TPJ. The ventral cluster showed more coactivation with PCC. (C) Functional preference profiles of the right-TPJ. All clusters in the right-TPJ were predicted by social, memory, and awareness, while only the dorsal cluster was predicted by decision making. (D) Right-TPJ and health conditions. No subregions were significantly associated with disease-related topics. Figure was created using Python and Adobe Illustrator.

We identified three clusters within the left-TPJ with silhouette scoring (Fig. 5A, left panel): an anterior cluster (Fig. 5A right panel, red), a posterior cluster (Fig. 5A right panel, yellow) and a ventral cluster (Fig. 5A right panel, green). We directly contrasted the coactivation patterns of each subregion. We found all three regions strongly coactivate with the right-TPJ. In addition, the posterior and ventral clusters showed stronger coactivation with the PCC (Fig. 5B). While both clusters also strongly coactivated with the MPFC, the posterior cluster showed strong coactivation with the ventral MPFC, whereas the ventral cluster coactivated with the dorsal MPFC more (Fig. 5B). Consistent with functional patterns typically associated with the DMN, all clusters in the left-TPJ were primarily predicted by social, decision-making, memory, and awareness (Fig. 5C). We found that the ventral cluster demonstrated an association with reading, semantics and emotion; the anterior cluster showed an association with reading and working memory; and the posterior cluster was associated with priming (Fig. 5C). No subregions within the left-TPJ showed any association with disorder-related topics (Fig. 5D). Note that the parcellations within the left- and right- TPJ partially mirror each other (Table 1), which increases confidence in the clustering of coactivation patterns27,40.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Coactivation-based clustering, meta-analytic coactivation contrasts and functional preference profiles of the left-TPJ. (A) We identified three clusters within the left-TPJ based on silhouette scoring: an anterior cluster (red), a posterior cluster (yellow) and a ventral cluster (green). (B) Coactivation contrasts of the left-TPJ. All three regions strongly coactivate with the right-TPJ. The posterior and ventral clusters showed stronger coactivation with the right-TPJ. The posterior cluster showed more coactivation with the ventral MPFC whereas the ventral cluster more strongly coactivated with the dorsal MPFC. Strength of association with cognitive and disorder topics, of the left-TPJ measured by LOR (Functional preference profiles). (C) All clusters in the left-TPJ were primarily predicted by social, decision-making, memory, and awareness. The ventral cluster was strongly associated with reading, semantics and emotion whereas the anterior cluster showed stronger association with reading and working memory. The posterior cluster was more strongly predicted by priming. (D) Left-TPJ and health conditions. No subregions showed strong associations with disease-related topics. Figure was created using Python and Adobe Illustrator.

Table 1.

Percent overlapping voxels within subregions of left- and right-TPJ.

% Overlap Left-TPJ
Right-TPJ Anterior Ventral Posterior
Dorsal 0.50 0.28 0.01
Ventral 0.01 0.63 0.00
Posterior 0.04 0.33 0.51

We identified three clusters within the PCC with silhouette scoring (Fig. 6A, left panel): a dorsal cluster (Fig. 6A right panel, blue), a medial cluster (Fig. 6A right panel, yellow) and a ventral cluster (Fig. 6A right panel, red). The medial cluster showed stronger coactivation with the bilateral-TPJ and MPFC, whereas the ventral cluster more strongly coactivated with the hippocampus (Fig. 6B). Similar to this coactivation pattern, all clusters in the PCC were predicted by memory, awareness, and decision-making, with the medial region showing an association with social cognition and emotion topics (Fig. 6C). When looking at the disorder topics, we found that all PCC clusters were associated with Alzheimer’s/Parkinson’s disease; the dorsal cluster was associated with smoking; and the ventral cluster was associated with PTSD (Fig. 6D), although these cluster-topic associations between the dorsal and ventral clusters were not significantly different in strength compared to each other.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Coactivation-based clustering, meta-analytic coactivation contrasts and functional preference profiles of the PCC. (A) We identified three clusters within the PCC on the basis of silhouette scoring: a dorsal cluster (blue), a medial cluster (yellow) and a ventral cluster (red). (B) Coactivation contrasts of the PCC. The medial cluster showed stronger coactivation with the bilateral-TPJ and MPFC whereas the ventral cluster more strongly coactivated with the hippocampus. (C) Functional preference profiles of PCC. All clusters in the PCC were predicted by memory, awareness, and decision making, while only the medial region was associated with social and emotion topics. (D) PCC and health conditions. All PCC clusters were associated with Alzheimer’s/Parkinson’s disease. The dorsal cluster was associated with smoking whereas the ventral cluster was associated with PTSD. Figure was created using Python and Adobe Illustrator.

Discussion

Previous research has linked the default mode network to a wide range of cognitive functions and disease states. Numerous studies have elucidated the functional architecture of the network through coactivation- or connectivity-based analysis3639,42,45,50,51. Yet, a comprehensive understanding of the mappings between psychological functions and different nodes within the default mode network remains in its early stages. To address this question, the present study builds upon previous work that used a high-powered meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data to parcellate the MPFC and LFC by topic modeling of psychological functions27,40. Our findings help further characterize the functional specialization and subspecialization in two other key DMN regions, the PCC and TPJ. Here, we report coactivation among DMN regions and between the DMN and hippocampus, amygdala, and striatum. Overlap in function among DMN regions was observed with all four ROIs for social cognition, decision-making, awareness, and memory. DMN regions were also differentiated by function: the MPFC was associated with emotion, reward, and fear; the PCC was associated with emotion; and the left-TPJ was associated with math, semantics, working memory, and reading. Moreover, we found that two of the DMN regions were associated with psychopathology and neurological diseases: we found that the PCC was associated with Alzheimer’s, Parkinson’s disease, and smoking and the MPFC was associated with smoking, eating disorders and depression. Further examination of these DMN ROIs revealed that they could be divided into subregions based on cognitive and disease-related functional subspecialization.

Across all four DMN nodes, we observed consistent patterns of involvement with social cognition, memory, decision making and awareness. Our analyses also showed strong coactivation patterns between PCC and TPJ, suggesting their interactive roles in support of functions, such as social cognition and memory processes 36,37,47. These findings recapitulate the notion that nodes within DMN jointly contribute to multiple psychological processes7,14,45. In addition, our results also revealed that the MPFC, as compared to other nodes, has more heterogeneous functional characteristics. We show that the MPFC coactivates with subcortical regions, such as the amygdala and ventral striatum. Consistent with this pattern, our analysis further shows that the MPFC is strongly associated with fear, emotion and reward, consolidating the vast literature on MPFC’s interactions with regions related to affect and reward learning11,52.

Within the MPFC, our finding of a strong association between the ventral cluster and fear is consistent with prior work implicating vmPFC in extinction of conditioned fear53,54. Our analyses further delineate psychological disorders associated with the MPFC. In particular, depression, which has been previously associated with altered DMN patterns55, loaded most strongly onto the MPFC. In patients with major depressive disorder, increased functional connectivity of the MPFC has been observed56, as well as increased MPFC activation during self-referential processing57. The fact that both dorsal and ventral MPFC clusters were associated with depression lends further support to previous work suggesting involvement of both MPFC subregions in major depressive disorder, with dorsal MPFC activation during depressive self-comparisons and ventral MPFC activation during the attentional component of depressive self-focus58.

Consistent with previous literature on the PCC, the present study shows associations between the PCC and social cognition. It has been shown that the PCC is a central node of the DMN for social cognition – e.g., ascribing mental states to others11,12,59 and social feedback60—and has functional interactions at rest with inferior parietal and superior temporal regions11. While previously the role of the PCC in social cognition has not been well-delineated, our results suggest that a medial cluster of the PCC may be most strongly associated with social cognition61. The same medial PCC cluster was shown to be specific to emotion; while there is relatively less evidence for the role of PCC in emotion, previous work has shown that the PCC activates in response to emotional words and may mediate interactions between emotional and memory processes32, consistent with our parallel finding of an association between the PCC and memory62. In addition to a primary association with memory, all PCC clusters loaded on Alzheimer’s/Parkinson’s disease, consistent with previous reports of PCC-DMN functional connectivity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment63. This association may also be linked to our finding of PCC-hippocampal coactivation, as previous research has suggested disrupted resting state functional connectivity between PCC and hippocampus as a mechanism underling Alzheimer’s disease63.

Our analyses also highlight the association of both the dorsal and ventral PCC with smoking. Although most prior neuroimaging studies of smoking behavior focus on anterior cingulate and subcortical circuitry, nicotine has also been shown to consistently enhance functional connectivity between the PCC and medial frontal/anterior cingulate cortex, as well as local connectivity between the dorsal and ventral PCC64. Additionally, PCC activity has been associated with craving65, viewing smoking cessation messages66,67, and suppressing cue-induced craving68. Taken together, these functional distinctions indicate that subregions within the PCC were differentially recruited by different cognitive processes, a pattern consistent with previous literature suggesting the multifaceted role of the PCC in cognition43,69,70.

We also report involvement with social cognition within the TPJ, with the left TPJ more strongly associated with math, semantics, reading, emotion, and working memory. Within the domain of social cognition, previous research has established the TPJ as most associated with mentalizing/heory of mind functions71. In addition, previous work has suggested functional heterogeneity within the TPJ on the basis of its functional and structural connectivity11,41,72. One study in particular attempted to map social cognition in the human brain, including parcellating the TPJ using diffusion-weighted imaging with comparison to non-human primates47. This work suggested that the posterior TPJ was most strongly associated with social cognition. Our analysis similarly shows strong loading of the ‘social’ term across all TPJ clusters; the previously reported social association with the posterior but not anterior TPJ may be a result of the TPJ mask definition, as Mars et al. (2011) included a broader anterior area of the TPJ that overlaps further with the inferior parietal lobe.

Although our study provides a comprehensive characterization of the functional roles of the DMN, we note that our findings should be viewed with three caveats. First, the classifier used in our analysis did not distinguish activations from deactivations. However, it is well established that the DMN might be activated for some processes (e.g., social cognition11,50,73; and deactivated for others (e.g., executive control7476). It is thus conceivable that a dataset capable of detecting deactivations would potentially extend our current findings and provide a full account of the functional architecture of the DMN. Second, the coactivation maps may not be directly related to connectivity between brain regions because they are based on correlations. Indeed, correlation between brain regions can be driven by a number of factors that are not related to connectivity or coupling, including changes in signal-to-noise ratio in either region or a change in another brain region77,78. A thorough examination of connectivity would necessitate integrating behavioral tasks with effective connectivity measures, such as psychophysiological interactions analysis7882 (PPI), an avenue for future investigations. This alternative approach would provide insight into how specific tasks and processes drive connectivity with the DMN. Finally, the nature of the topic modeling oversimplifies the mapping between psychological ontology to complex, dynamic brain activity40,83. For example, each topic used in our analysis represents a combination of many cognitive processes operating at different levels. As a result, mappings between specific psychological concepts and brain activity may require further parcellation of complex cognitive processes.

In addition to these limitations, we also note that our approach for defining and fractionating the DMN merits additional consideration. For example, we defined the DMN based on a combination of anatomy and function and then parcellated individual nodes of DMN. Although an analogous approach has been used in prior studies12, we note that other studies have parcellated networks using responses from all nodes61. Both approaches assume a given network is composed of distinct nodes, which depends critically on the definition of those nodes84,85. To address this issue, some papers have defined networks using continuous maps (e.g., those derived from independent component analysis) and have examined connectivity with those maps using dual-regression analysis8688. We believe that integrating this approach with our current analytical framework27,40 and unthresholded whole-brain maps89 will help future studies refine functional parcellations of the DMN. We also note that in our process of parcellating four regions within the DMN, our clusters had become relatively small, which may have limited our ability to find differences between cluster-topic associations among each DMN node. Future work may be able to address this limitation by using image-based meta analyses90,91, which may be possible as more researchers heed calls to share unthresholded statistical maps82,89,92.

To conclude, the present study applied a meta-analytic approach to characterize functional mappings among cognitive processes, health conditions, and the DMN. Although the DMN as a whole contributes to multiple cognitive processes, we identified distinct functional properties for each region, and extended these results by identifying further parcellation of functions for specific subregions. These results help clarify the functional roles of the DMN across a large corpus of neuroimaging studies and add to the body of literature that has focused on refining the theoretical and computational framework associated with the DMN93.

Methods

Our analysis was based on version 0.6 of the Neurosynth dataset (i.e., articles up to July 2015), which contains activation data from more than 12,000 fMRI studies94.

DMN mask

First, to find regions that are functionally related but restricted to the four anatomical regions within the DMN that we are interested in, we created a DMN mask that constrained wider functional activation within an anatomical atlas95 (Fig. 1A). Specifically, we performed a meta-analytical association test by searching the term “DMN" in Neurosynth to create a functional mask that specifically mapped the term “DMN” to brain regions. We chose an association test (formerly named a “reverse inference”) because it can help estimate the relative specificity between brain activity and the DMN94,96. The resulting mask included 9,650 activations from 366 studies, and thus identified voxels in studies where the term “DMN” was mentioned in their abstract given brain activation. All of the images are thresholded with a false discovery rate of 0.01. Next, we constrained the mask to the four DMN regions of interest by using the Harvard–Oxford probabilistic atlas97 at P > 0.25, which included the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and the left and right temporal parietal junction (left- and right-TPJ). This constraint was applied in the interest of specifying the output of our functional mapping and subsequent parcellation to a precise set of standardized coordinates within the four regions of interest.

Coactivation-based clustering

To determine more fine-grained functional differences within the four primary DMN regions of interest, we applied a clustering method used by previous work27 to cluster individual voxels inside each of the four regions based on their meta-analytic coactivation with voxels in the rest of the brain38 (Fig. 1B). To assess coactivation patterns for each of the four regions, we correlated the activation pattern of each voxel with the rest of the brain across all 366 studies. The resulting coactivation matrix is large and would require intensive computational power; to mitigate this, the coactivation matrix was passed through a principal component analysis (PCA), where the dimensionality of the matrix was reduced to 100 components27,40. Once the coactivation was reduced, we calculated the Pearson correlation distance between every voxel with each whole-brain PCA component in each of the four DMN regions. We then grouped the voxels within each separate region into 2–9 clusters by applying a k-means clustering algorithm to the correlation distance coefficients98. Finally, to determine the number of clusters for each region, we computed silhouette coefficients, allowing us to measure cohesion within each cluster99. Previous reports caution that issues may arise when identifying the appropriate number of clusters due to variations in goals and levels of analysis used across investigations83,100; however, recent work has confirmed that the silhouette score can successfully assess cluster solutions27,40,101,102.

Coactivation profiles

Next, we analyzed how the whole-brain coactivation patterns differed between regions to reveal their functional networks27 (Fig. 1C). To accomplish this, we began by setting up contrasts among each of the four regions’ coactivation patterns (e.g., the MPFC against the PCC, left TPJ and right TPJ) to detect the coactivation differences between each region. Specifically, we performed a meta-analytic contrast between studies that activated the region of interest (ROI; “active studies”) and studies that activated the other three regions of interest (“inactive studies”), to identify the voxels in the rest of the brain that had a greater probability of coactivating with a given ROI than other regions within the DMN. For instance, the purple voxels in Fig. 2B indicates that these voxels in particular (in this case, within the amygdala and striatum) tend to co-activate alongside the MPFC rather than the PCC, the left TPJ or the right TPJ. To generate coactivation images that highlight coactivation profiles that emerged as a result of contrasting the four DMN ROIs, we conducted a two-way chi-square test between two sets of studies—the active and inactive studies—and calculated p values to threshold the coactivation images using False Discovery Rate (q < 0.01). The resulting images were binarized and visualized using the NiLearn Library in Python103.

Meta-analytic functional preference profiles

To map between functional states (e.g., cognitive processes and health conditions) and the MPFC, PCC, and TPJ regions of the DMN, we re-purposed a set of 60 topics—generated as a part of a previous study27—which represent words that co-occur among fMRI study abstracts in the Neurosynth database. The set of 60 topics, which were produced in the exact same manner as seen in Poldrack et al.105), were derived from Dirichlet allocation topic modeling104, which is beneficial in reducing the level of redundancy and ambiguity in term-based meta-analysis maps in Neurosynth27. Per Poldrack et al.105 and De La Vega et al.27, topics that were irrelevant to either cognitive processes or health conditions (for instance, topics pertaining to experimental methods and design; N = 23 topics) were excluded from the original set of 60 generated topics. After the exclusion step, we were left with 37 topics: 29 cognitive topics (e.g., mentalizing, attention, imagery) and 8 disorder-related topics (e.g., PTSD, ADHD, Autism).”

We generated functional preference profiles by identifying which cognitive processes or health conditions best predicted activity within a given cluster across studies (Fig. 1D). Considering the redundancy and ambiguity in term-based meta-analytic maps (e.g., “memory” could refer to working memory or episodic memory), we followed the lead of Yarkoni et al. (2011), and trained a naive Bayesian classifier27 to evaluate two sets of studies. One set of studies included those that activated a cluster and the other set did not; a study was defined as activating a given cluster if at least 5% of voxels in the cluster were activated in that study. The classifier was then trained to predict whether a study activated a particular cluster, given the semantic representation of the latent conceptual structure underlying descriptive terms featured in the study27,40. Our instantiation of the classifier employed a fourfold cross-validation scheme, the default variance smoothing in scikit-learn (i.e., 1e−09), and did not utilize any priors. Input variables included the 60 psychological and health topics to predict whether activation is likely given the latent concepts within a study. As a final measure of our model’s performance, we calculated the mean score across all four folds, and scored them using a classification summary metric that considers the sensitivity and specificity: the area under the curve of the receiver operating characteristic (AUC-ROC).

Next, we extracted the log-odds ratio (LOR) of a topic, defined as the log of the ratio between the probability of a given topic in studies that activated a given cluster ROI and the probability of the topic in studies that did not activate the same cluster ROI, for each cluster separately. A positive LOR value indicates that a topic is predictive of activation in a given cluster. Based on LOR values, we identified 20 cognitive topics that loaded most strongly to the entire DMN mask for further analysis and then applied a procedure used in a previous study to determine the statistical significance of these associations27. To do so, we performed a permutation test for each region-topic log odds ratio 1,000 times. This resulted in a null distribution of LOR for each topic and each region. We calculated p values for each pairwise relationship between topics and regions and then adjusted the p-values using a False Discovery Rate at 0.01 to account for multiple comparisons within 20 selected cognitive topics and 8 disease-related topics (separately). We reported associations significant at the corrected p < 0.05 threshold. Finally, to determine whether certain topics showed greater preference for one region or cluster versus another, we conducted exploratory, post hoc comparisons by determining whether the 99.9% confidence intervals (CI) of the LOR of a specific topic for one region or cluster overlapped with the 99.9% CI of the same topic for another region. We use a more stringent threshold than that of previous work27 due to the increased number of comparisons we make across DMN regions. We used bootstrapping to generate CIs, sampled with replacement, and recalculated log-odds ratios for each cluster 1,000 times.

Acknowledgements

We thank Alejandro de la Vega for sharing his analysis code and answering technical questions. We also thank Victoria Kelly and Christian Reice for their comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. This work was supported in part by NIH Grant R21-MH113917 (DVS). Publication of this article was funded in part by the Temple University Libraries Open Access Publishing Fund. LJT is now at Dartmouth College.

Author contributions

S.W. participated in: formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing, visualization. A.A.T. participated in: writing—original draft, writing—review & editing. L.J.T participated in: writing—review & editing, validation, visualization. D.V.S. participated in conceptualization, supervision, formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing.

Data availability

All data are publicly available on Neurosynth database (https://neurosynth.org).

Code availability

In addition, all analysis code can be found on our lab GitHub (https://github.com/DVS-Lab/dmn-parcellation). All images can be found on NeuroVault (https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:6262).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Shaoming Wang, Lindsey J. Tepfer and Adrienne A. Taren.

References

  • 1.Shulman GL, et al. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: I. Increases in subcortical structures and cerebellum but not in nonvisual cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997;9:624–647. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Raichle ME, et al. A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Garrity AG, et al. Aberrant “default mode” functional connectivity in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2007;164:450–457. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Davey CG, Pujol J, Harrison BJ. Mapping the self in the brain’s default mode network. Neuroimage. 2016;132:390–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brewer JA, et al. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2011;108:20254–20259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Spreng RN, Grady CL. Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:1112–1123. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2001;98:4259–4264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071043098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293:2105–2108. doi: 10.1126/science.1062872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Harrison BJ, et al. Consistency and functional specialization in the default mode brain network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2008;105:9781–9786. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711791105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mars RB, et al. On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012;6:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Leech R, Kamourieh S, Beckmann CF, Sharp DJ. Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:3217–3224. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Braga RM, Sharp DJ, Leeson C, Wise RJS, Leech R. Echoes of the brain within default mode, association, and heteromodal cortices. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:14031–14039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0570-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;38:433–447. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Broyd SJ, et al. Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009;33:279–296. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Assaf M, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of default mode sub-networks in autism spectrum disorder patients. Neuroimage. 2010;53:247–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Padmanabhan A, Lynch CJ, Schaer M, Menon V. The default mode network in autism. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging. 2017;2:476–486. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kennedy DP, Redcay E, Courchesne E. Failing to deactivate: Resting functional abnormalities in autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2006;103:8275–8280. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0600674103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kennedy DP, Courchesne E. The intrinsic functional organization of the brain is altered in autism. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1877–1885. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Greicius MD, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: Abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:429–437. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Goekoop R, Stam CJ, Scheltens P. Altered resting state networks in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease: An fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005;26:231–239. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.He Y, et al. Regional coherence changes in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease: A combined structural and resting-state functional MRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;35:488–500. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bluhm RL, et al. Spontaneous low-frequency fluctuations in the BOLD signal in schizophrenic patients: Anomalies in the default network. Schizophr. Bull. 2007;33:1004–1012. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Mitchell JP, Macrae CN, Banaji MR. Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron. 2006;50:655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bzdok D, et al. Segregation of the human medial prefrontal cortex in social cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:232. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bado P, et al. Functional dissociation of ventral frontal and dorsomedial default mode network components during resting state and emotional autobiographical recall. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014;35:3302–3313. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.de la Vega A, Chang LJ, Banich MT, Wager TD, Yarkoni T. Large-scale meta-analysis of human medial frontal cortex reveals tripartite functional organization. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:6553–6562. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4402-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Small DM, et al. The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention. Neuroimage. 2003 doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(02)00012-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Castellanos FX, et al. Cingulate—precuneous interactions: A new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NIH Public Access. 2008;63:332–337. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cabeza R, Dolcos F, Graham R, Nyberg L. Similarities and differences in the neural correlates of episodic memory retrieval and working memory. Neuroimage. 2002;16:317–330. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:253–258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Maddock RJ. The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:310–316. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01374-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Northoff G, et al. Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage. 2006;31:440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Schurz M, Radua J, Aichhorn M, Richlan F, Perner J. Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;42:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Laird AR, et al. Behavioral interpretations of intrinsic connectivity networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23:4022–4037. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bzdok D, et al. Subspecialization in the human posterior medial cortex. Neuroimage. 2015;106:55–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bzdok D, et al. Characterization of the temporo-parietal junction by combining data-driven parcellation, complementary connectivity analyses, and functional decoding. Neuroimage. 2013;81:381–392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Eickhoff SB, et al. Co-activation patterns distinguish cortical modules, their connectivity and functional differentiation. Neuroimage. 2011;57:938–949. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ray KL, et al. Co-activation based parcellation of the human frontal pole. Neuroimage. 2015;123:200–211. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.de la Vega A, Yarkoni T, Wager TD, Banich MT. Large-scale meta-analysis suggests low regional modularity in lateral frontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2017 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bzdok D, et al. Left inferior parietal lobe engagement in social cognition and language. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016;68:319–334. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox PT, Bzdok D, Hensel L. Functional segregation of the human dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:304–321. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Margulies DS, et al. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:20069–20074. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kernbach JM, et al. Subspecialization within default mode nodes characterized in 10,000 UK Biobank participants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:12295–12300. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1804876115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Laird AR, et al. Investigating the functional heterogeneity of the default mode network using coordinate-based meta-analytic modeling. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:14496–14505. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4004-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: Component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2014;1316:29–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mars RB, et al. Diffusion-weighted imaging tractography-based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting-state functional connectivity. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:4087–4100. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5102-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Smith SM, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Chang LJ, Yarkoni T, Khaw MW, Sanfey AG. Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: Functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:739–749. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Amft M, et al. Definition and characterization of an extended social-affective default network. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015;220:1031–1049. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0698-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Smith DV, Sip KE, Delgado MR. Functional connectivity with distinct neural networks tracks fluctuations in gain/loss framing susceptibility. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:2743–2755. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Milad MR, et al. Recall of fear extinction in humans activates the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in concert. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:446–454. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Morgan MA, Ledoux JE. Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Behav. Neurosci. 1995;109:681–688. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.4.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Belzung C, Willner P, Philippot P. Depression: From psychopathology to pathophysiology. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015;30:24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Zhu X, et al. Evidence of a dissociation pattern in resting-state default mode network connectivity in first-episode, treatment-naive major depression patients. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;71:611–617. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lemogne C, et al. Self-referential processing and the prefrontal cortex over the course of depression: A pilot study. J. Affect. Disord. 2010;124:196–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lemogne C, Delaveau P, Freton M, Guionnet S, Fossati P. Medial prefrontal cortex and the self in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2012;136:e1–e11. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Leech R, Braga R, Sharp DJ. Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:215–222. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3689-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Sip KE, Smith DV, Porcelli AJ, Kar K, Delgado MR. Social closeness and feedback modulate susceptibility to the framing effect. Soc. Neurosci. 2015;10:35–45. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.944316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Alcalá-López D, et al. Computing the social brain connectome across systems and states. Cereb. Cortex. 2017 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Vatansever D, Manktelow AE, Sahakian BJ, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA. Angular default mode network connectivity across working memory load. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:41–52. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hafkemeijer A, van der Grond J, Rombouts SARB. Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Biochim. Biophys. Acta Mol. Basis Dis. 2012;1822:431–441. doi: 10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hong LE, et al. Association of nicotine addiction and nicotine’s actions with separate cingulate cortex functional circuits. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2009;66:431–441. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Garavan H, et al. Cue-induced cocaine craving: Neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2000;157:1789–1798. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Chua HF, et al. Self-related neural response to tailored smoking-cessation messages predicts quitting. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:426–427. doi: 10.1038/nn.2761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Chua HF, Liberzon I, Welsh RC, Strecher VJ. Neural correlates of message tailoring and self-relatedness in smoking cessation programming. Biol. Psychiatry. 2009;65:165–168. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Brody AL, et al. Neural substrates of resisting craving during cigarette cue exposure. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:642–651. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Utevsky AV, Smith DV, Huettel SA. Precuneus is a functional core of the default-mode network. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:932–940. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4227-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Acikalin MY, Gorgolewski KJ, Poldrack RA. A coordinate-based meta-analysis of overlaps in regional specialization and functional connectivity across subjective value and default mode networks. Front. Neurosci. 2017;11:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Saxe R. Uniquely human social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2006;16:235–239. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Schilbach L, et al. Introspective minds: Using ALE meta-analyses to study commonalities in the neural correlates of emotional processing, social & unconstrained cognition. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:2. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Schilbach L, Eickhoff SB, Rotarska-Jagiela A, Fink GR, Vogeley K. Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the ‘default system’ of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 2008;17:457–467. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Anticevic A, Repovs G, Shulman GL, Barch DM. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2638–2648. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Binder JR. Task-induced deactivation and the ‘resting’ state. Neuroimage. 2012;62:1086–1091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Koshino H, Minamoto T, Yaoi K, Osaka M, Osaka N. Coactivation of the default mode network regions and working memory network regions during task preparation. Sci. Rep. 2015;4:5954. doi: 10.1038/srep05954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Gerstein GL, Perkel DH. Simultaneously recorded trains of action potentials: Analysis and functional interpretation. Science (80-). 1969 doi: 10.1126/science.164.3881.828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Friston KJ. Functional and effective connectivity: A review. Brain Connect. 2011;1:13–36. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Friston KJ, et al. Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. Neuroimage. 1997 doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.O’Reilly JX, Woolrich MW, Behrens TEJ, Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H. Tools of the trade: Psychophysiological interactions and functional connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012;7:604–609. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Smith DV, Gseir M, Speer ME, Delgado MR. Toward a cumulative science of functional integration: A meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:2904–2917. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Smith DV, Delgado MR. Meta-analysis of psychophysiological interactions: Revisiting cluster-level thresholding and sample sizes. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:588–591. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Poldrack RA, Yarkoni T. From brain maps to cognitive ontologies: Informatics and the search for mental structure. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016;67:587–612. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cole DM, Smith SM, Beckmann CF. Advances and pitfalls in the analysis and interpretation of resting-state FMRI data. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2010 doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Smith SM, et al. Network modelling methods for FMRI. Neuroimage. 2011;54:875–891. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Filippini N, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-epsilon4 allele. PNAS. 2009;106:7209–7214. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Smith SM, Hyvärinen A, Varoquaux G, Miller KL, Beckmann CF. Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. Neuroimage. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Yu Q, et al. Comparing brain graphs in which nodes are regions of interest or independent components: A simulation study. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2017;291:61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2017.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Gorgolewski KJ, et al. NeuroVault.org: A web-based repository for collecting and sharing unthresholded statistical maps of the human brain. Front. Neuroinform. 2015;9:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2015.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Clithero JA, Rangel A. Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective value. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013 doi: 10.1093/scan/nst106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Luijten M, Schellekens AF, Kühn S, Machielse MWJ, Sescousse G. Disruption of reward processing in addiction. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Botvinik-Nezer R, et al. Variability in the analysis of a single neuroimaging dataset by many teams. Nature. 2020;2020:1–31. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2314-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Dohmatob E, Dumas G, Bzdok D. Dark control : A unified account of default mode function by control theory and reinforcement learning. BioRxiv. 2017 doi: 10.1101/148890. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat. Methods. 2011;8:665–670. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Poldrack RA, et al. Scanning the horizon: Towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:115–126. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Poldrack RA. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006;10:59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Thirion B, Varoquaux G, Dohmatob E, Poline JB. Which fMRI clustering gives good brain parcellations? Front. Neurosci. 2014;8:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Rousseeuw PJ. Silhouettes: A graphical aid to the interpretation and validation of cluster analysis. J. Comput. Appl. Math. 1987;20:53–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Varoquaux G, Thirion B. How machine learning is shaping cognitive neuroimaging. Gigascience. 2014;3:28. doi: 10.1186/2047-217X-3-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Pauli WM, O’Reilly RC, Yarkoni T, Wager TD. Regional specialization within the human striatum for diverse psychological functions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2016;113:1907–1912. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507610113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Eickhoff SB, Thirion B, Varoquaux G, Bzdok D. Connectivity-based parcellation: Critique and implications. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:4771–4792. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Abraham A, et al. Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front. Neuroinform. 2014;8:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Blei DM, et al. Latent dirichlet allocation. J. Mach. Learn. Res. 2003;3:993–1022. [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Poldrack RA, et al. Discovering relations between mind, brain, and mental disorders using topic mapping. PLOS Comput. Biol. 2012 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data are publicly available on Neurosynth database (https://neurosynth.org).

In addition, all analysis code can be found on our lab GitHub (https://github.com/DVS-Lab/dmn-parcellation). All images can be found on NeuroVault (https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:6262).


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES