Abstract
Background
Existing research evidence indicates that breast cancer patients have different degrees of cognitive dysfunction after chemotherapy, and polymorphisms in 3 genes (catechol-O-methyltransferase, COMT; apolipoprotein E, APOE; and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, BDNF) have been associated with cognitive impairment. However, the role of these 3 gene polymorphisms in modulating cognitive impairment in breast cancer survivors with varying hormonal receptor expression is not clear at present.
To explore the effects of genetic polymorphisms in BDNF, APOE, and COMT on the regulation of prospective memory impairments induced by chemotherapy in breast cancer patients with various expression levels of estrogen receptor (ER) and progesterone receptor (PR).
Material/Methods
A total of 232 patients with breast cancer (113 with ER−/PR− and 119 with ER+/PR+) were evaluated before and after chemotherapy for cognitive function, including prospective memory. Following previously published sequencing procedures, we assessed 6 single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), including BDNF (rs6265), APOE (rs429358, rs7412), and COMT (rs165599, rs4680, rs737865).
Results
The patients showed poorer prospective memory scores after chemotherapy than before chemotherapy. Furthermore, the ER−/PR− group showed poorer event-based prospective memory (EBPM) scores than the ER+/PR+ group (z=−7.831, p<0.01) after chemotherapy. The patients with the COMT rs737865G/G genotype, compared with those with the A/A and A/G genotypes, showed a linear EBPM performance (β=1.499, 95% confidence interval (CI)=1.017~2.211) and were less likely to have memory impairment. In contrast, APOE and BDNF polymorphisms did not influence cognitive performance.
Conclusions
The patterns of hormonal receptor expression may be related to prospective memory impairments induced by chemotherapy in breast cancer patients. Furthermore, the COMT polymorphism (rs737865) was linearly related to the extent of deficits in EBPM and may represent a potential genetic marker of risk for cognitive deficits triggered by chemotherapy in patients with breast cancer.
MeSH Keywords: Breast Neoplasms, Male; Chemotherapy, Adjuvant; Cognitive Therapy; Polymorphism, Genetic
Background
Breast cancer is a frequently occurring malignancy among women, with an estimated 252 710 new cases in 2017 in the U.S. [1]. Chemotherapy can effectively treat patients with breast cancer. However, side effects in the long run, such as cognitive impairment, have been widely associated with chemotherapy [2]. Numerous studies have documented impairments in short- and long-term memory, cognitive processing speed, attention, executive function, and language function after chemotherapy [3,4]. Lasting for many years, chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment (CICI) has been critically related to functional decline and impaired quality of life in patients with breast cancer [5–8].
One cognitive deficit is prospective memory (PM), which is the capability of memorizing things to be done at a certain time or place [9]. Prospective memory based on time (TBPM) and prospective memory based on event (EBPM) are distinguishable aspects of PM. In our previous study, we found PM impairments in breast cancer patients following chemotherapy. Also, patients with estrogen/progesterone receptor negative tumors (ER−/PR−) demonstrated significant deficits in EBPM but not TBPM [10,11].
Breast cancer is highly heterogeneous, with significant variations in histomorphology, immunophenotype, biological behavior, and therapeutic response. Molecular typing has distinguished breast cancer into luminal, HER-2 overexpression, basal-like, and normal breast-like subtypes [12]. A meta-analysis evaluated the variability of CICI in breast cancer patients [13]. For instance, our previous work showed heterogeneity among CICI in patients with breast cancer with various tumor hormone receptor expression profiles, with ER−/PR− but not ER+/PR+ showing deficits in EBPM after treatment with chemotherapy [10,14].
Previous studies have found that genetic polymorphisms in apolipoprotein E (APOE; rs429358 and rs7412), brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; rs6265), and catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT; rs4680, rs165599, and rs737865) are associated with cognitive function [15–17]. COMT is an enzyme that catalyzes the O-methylation of catecholamines, a group of neurotransmitters central to cognitive functioning [18]. The COMT gene is an important gene for dopamine transport. Some research has found that the COMT gene is associated with cognitive function, including executive function of the prefrontal lobe, working memory, and attentional cognitive control [19]. An earlier study suggested a role of the COMT genotype in influencing cognitive function in survivors with breast cancer undergoing chemotherapy [20]. APOE is a polymorphic protein, and one study found that apolipoprotein gene polymorphisms are a significant risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease [21]. Another study suggested that the APOE genotype, by itself and combined with treatment, influenced memory, attention, verbal learning, and executive function in postmenopausal breast cancer patients [22]. BDNF is a neurotrophin, and BDNF plays an important role in nerve repair and survival, dendrite and axon growth, and long-term potentiation, which is closely related to cognitive function [23]. Breast cancer patients who were carriers of the BDNF Met allele were less likely to experience impaired verbal fluency [24].
The present study investigated PM impairments induced by chemotherapy in breast cancer patients with various expression levels of hormonal receptors, and characterized the influence of COMT, APOE, and BDNF polymorphisms on CICI.
Material and Methods
Participants
A total of 232 breast cancer patients, admitted to the Cancer Treatment Center of the Affiliated Second Hospital of Anhui Medical University from 2013 to 2015, were enrolled. They were separated into 2 groups according to tumor progesterone receptor (PR) and estrogen receptor (ER) expression, including 113 ER−/PR− patients and 119 ER+/PR+ patients. The Research Ethics Committee of the Second Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, China, issued the approval for this study. All participants provided written informed consent prior to the study.
Most of the subjects were right handed, and all subjects had more than 5 years of education and were eligible if they met the following criteria: 1) they had breast cancer, as diagnosed by immunohistochemistry and pathological diagnosis; 2) they had received standard-dose chemotherapy treatment with fluorouracil, cyclophosphamide, paclitaxel, and doxorubicin, rather than hormonal therapy; 3) they were of any age and had any pathological subtype; 4) they had a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score ≥24; 5) they had a Karnofsky performance scale (KPS) score ≥80 regarding general activities in daily life; and 6) they had unimpaired vision, hearing, and language. The exclusion criteria were as follows: 1) a history of treatment with a variety of combinations of chemotherapy, radiotherapy, and hormonal therapy; 2) distant metastasis or advanced cachexia; 3) psychiatric diagnoses (e.g., paranoid disorders, depression, or anxiety); 4) an alcohol or drug dependence history; 5) clinically diagnosed dementia; and 6) abnormalities including intracranial metastases in accordance with brain CT or MRI.
Neuropsychological tests
Several neuropsychological tests were performed for the assessment of normal memory and cognitive functions at baseline, prior to chemotherapy, and following 6 cycles of postoperative adjuvant chemotherapy. For the assessment of visual spatial skills, language, calculation, short-term memory, and time and spatial orientation, the MMSE was administered. In the verbal fluency test (VFT), subjects were required to produce the names of as many animals as possible in 1 minute. For the measurement of short-term memory in the subjects, we employed a digit span test (DST) in which the subjects were required to recall the numbers read to them. The number of digits correctly recalled in serial order determined the total score.
Event-based prospective memory (EBPM) task
In the EBPM task, there were 32 Chinese cards with 12 Chinese words printed on each card; 2 of the words belonged to one category (small category), and the remaining 10 words belonged to another category (large category). The task of the subjects was to identify the 2 words belonging to the small category (target event). When seeing 2 specific animal words, the subjects were asked to knock on the table. The experimenter showed each card to the subject and then guided the subject to verbally answer the question at their speed. For each correct response to a target event (a total of 6 target events), we would give 1 point, for a total of 6 points. Two points were given for remembering to provide their telephone numbers when the test was done. By using a method similar to that developed by McDaniel et al., we recorded the subject’s performance on the word-selection task. The EBPM task has a maximum score of 8 points.
Time-based prospective memory (TBPM) task
The TBPM task utilized 100 cards, with 12 two-digit numbers printed on each card. The subjects were asked to select the minimum and maximum numbers on the cards. We asked the subjects to tap the desk at 5-min intervals from the start of the session for 15 min. The subjects could monitor the time with a digital clock placed on their right shoulder. We recorded the exact time at which the subjects tapped the desk. When the clock showed 17 min, the task ended. If the subjects tapped on the desk between 10 s before and 10s after the target time, 2 points were awarded. If the subjects tapped on the desk between 30 s before and 30 s after the target time,1 point was awarded. The TBPM task had a maximum score of 6 points.
Genotyping
Peripheral blood was sampled intravenously, collected in sterile anticoagulation tubes, and incubated at −80°C until use. With the use of a blood genomic QIAGEN kit (Shanghai Genesky Bio-Tech Co, Ltd; Shanghai, China), we extracted genomic DNA from the peripheral blood. DNA samples were kept at −20°C. With the use of the improved multiplex ligase detection reaction (iMLDR) technique, we performed genotyping, supported by Shanghai Genesky Biotechnologies Inc. (Shanghai, China). For each SNP, we distinguished the alleles with the use of different fluorescently labeled allele-specific oligonucleotide probe pairs. The prominent SNPs were distinguished by various extended lengths at the 3′-end. We established 2 negative controls: one in which the template was double-distilled water, and the other in which no primers were included with the DNA sample; the other conditions were all kept the same, on one plate. We designed duplicate tests, and the results were consistent. For verification of the results of the iMLDR technique, along with the use of Big Dye-terminator version 3.1 and an ABI3730XL automated sequencer (Applied Biosystems, Waltham, Massachusetts, USA), a random sample of up to 5% of the total sample DNA was directly sequenced.
Statistical analysis
The data here are reported as the mean±standard deviation (SD). Based on a one-way ANOVA and t test using SPSS (version 22.0, Chicago, IL, USA), we conducted statistical analysis. We calculated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and odds ratios (ORs) by logistic regression. For non-normally distributed data, we conducted a Mann-Whitney U test. In addition, using chi-square (χ2) tests, the frequencies of categorical variables (e.g., genotypes and alleles) were assessed. To analyze susceptibility to cognitive impairment, logistic regression was employed, and ORs and 95% CIs were calculated for the assessment of genetic effects. For individual SNP analyses, a general genetic model (dominant, recessive, additive models) was assumed, and the age, KPS score, years of education, and tumor pathology were adjusted. To characterize the associations between CICI and the COMT (rs737865) polymorphisms, we conducted linear regression. For all statistical tests, statistical significance was set at p<0.05.
Results
Clinical data
Table 1 lists the 232 patients who satisfied the inclusion criteria, including 113 with ER−/PR− and 119 with ER+/PR+ breast cancer. Age (48.5±10.7 vs. 48.9±10.1 years, respectively) and education (10.0 ±3.7 vs. 9.7±4.1 years, respectively) were not significantly different between the 2 groups. In the ER−/PR− group, 104 patients had normal invasive carcinoma of the breast, 3 had special invasive carcinoma of the breast, and 6 had carcinoma in situ. In the ER+/PR+ group, 113 patients had nonspecial-type invasive breast cancer, 5 had carcinoma in situ, and 1 had microinvasive carcinoma.
Table 1.
Parameters | Groups (Mean±SD) | ||
---|---|---|---|
A (n=113) | B (n=119) | ||
Age (years) | 48.50±10.70 | 48.92±10.14 | |
Education (years) | 9.98±3.66 | 9.74±4.07 | |
Pathological patterns | Non-special type invasive carcinoma of breast | 104 | 113 |
Special type invasive carcinoma of breast | 3 | 0 | |
Carcinoma in situ | 6 | 5 | |
Microinvasive carcinoma | 0 | 1 |
Neuropsychological test, EBPM, and TBPM performance
Table 2 suggests that the MMSE scores decreased significantly from before to after chemotherapy (27.27±1.57 vs. 26.65±1.64, p<0.05). Similarly, both DST and VFT scores decreased significantly from before (6.19±0.7 and 11.48±1.52, respectively) to after (5.95±0.97 and 9.96±2.13, respectively) chemotherapy. The EBPM and TBPM scores were also significantly decreased after chemotherapy: 2.72±1.01 vs. 2.00±1.19 (p<0.01) and 4.98±0.97 vs. 4.75±0.91 (p<0.05), respectively.
Table 2.
Task | Mean±SD | |
---|---|---|
Before chemotherapy (n=232) | After chemotherapy (n=232) | |
MMSE | 27.27±1.57 | 26.65±1.64* |
DST | 6.19±0.73 | 5.95±0.97** |
VFT | 11.48±1.52 | 9.96±2.13** |
EBPM | 2.72±1.01 | 2.00±1.19** |
TBPM | 4.98 ± 0.97 | 4.75 ±0.91* |
p<0.05,
p<0.01.
MMSE – Mini-Mental State Examination; DST – digit span test; VFT – verbal fluency test; EBPM – event-based prospective memory task; TBPM – time-based prospective memory task.
Neuropsychological test, EBPM, and TBPM performance: after vs. before chemotherapy
Table 3 expresses the cognitive impairment after chemotherapy in breast cancer patients with different hormone receptors. As shown in Table 3, the MMSE and TBPM scores increased slightly from before to after chemotherapy (ER−/PR−: 26.57±1.69 vs. 26.93±1.58; ER+/PR+: 4.73± 0.89 vs. 4.77 ±0.93), showing little distinction (p>0.05). In contrast, the DST, VFT, and EBPM scores increased remarkably from before to after chemotherapy; DST: (5.49±1.05) vs. (5.99±0.83), VFT: (8.89±1.84) vs. (10.97±1.90), and EBPM: (1.37±1.14) vs. (2.59±0.92). These differences were significant (p<0.01).
Table 3.
Task | Mean±SD | |
---|---|---|
ER−/PR− (n=113) | ER+/PR+ (n=119) | |
MMSE | 26.57±1.69 | 26.93±1.58# |
DST | 5.49±1.05 | 5.99±0.83** |
VFT | 8.89±1.84 | 10.97±1.90** |
EBPM | 1.37±1.14 | 2.59±0.92** |
TBPM | 4.73± 0.89 | 4.77 ±0.93# |
p>0.05,
p<0.01.
MMSE – Mini-Mental State Examination; DST – digit span test; VFT – verbal fluency test; EBPM – event-based prospective memory task; TBPM – time-based prospective memory task.
The unit SNP loci analysis
The 6 polymorphisms of the COMT, APOE, and BDNF genes were all determined to be consistent with Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium for the 2 groups (p>0.05), suggesting no interference of inbreeding and population migration or other population genetic influences.
According to sequencing analysis (Table 4), the allelic distributions of COMT, APOE, and BDNF were not significantly different between the 2 groups (p>0.05). In Table 5, the COMT rs165599 (dominant model: χ2=4.876, p=0.027) and rs737865 (recessive model: χ2=4.380, p=0.036) genotypic frequency distributions showed obvious differences. In addition, according to the results of the logistic regression analysis, the patients with the G/G (regulated, OR=2.019, 95% CI=1.097–3.717, p=0.024) genotype of COMT rs165599 had a notable increase in the likelihood of developing cognitive decline compared with the patients with the G/A and A/A genotypes. Compared with the patients with the G/A and A/A genotypes, the G/G genotype of COMT rs737865 exhibited a remarkably lower probability of developing cognitive decline (OR=0.519, 95% CI=0.272–0.991, p=0.047). The rs737865 polymorphism significantly increased the risk of CICI in recessive models (OR=2.888, 95% CI=1.096–7.612, p=0.032).When comparing the cognitive outcomes with the dominant models (OR=1.056, 95% CI=0.632–1.767, p=0.834) and additive models (OR=1.259, 95% CI=0.852–1.862,p=0.248), no significant associations were established for COMT (rs737865). Neither APOE (rs429358 and rs7412) nor BDNF (rs6265) showed any statistically notable distinctions between the 2 groups.
Table 4.
SNP | COMT | APOE | BDNF | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
rs4680 | rs165599 | rs737865 | rs429358 | rs7412 | rs6265 | |
CHR | 22 | 22 | 22 | 19 | 19 | 11 |
Allele position | 19951271 | 19956781 | 19930121 | 45411941 | 45412079 | 27679916 |
Ref allele | G | G | A | T | C | C |
Alt allele | A | A | G | C | T | T |
MAF | 0.256 | 0.479 | 0.315 | 0.080 | 0.088 | 0.492 |
P for HWE | 0.480 | 0.183 | 0.469 | 0.586 | 0.472 | 0.977 |
P* | 0.586 | 0.091 | 0.264 | 0.492 | 0.860 | 0.663 |
SNP – single-nucleotide polymorphism; CHR – chromosome; Ref allele – alleles in the loci on the reference sequence; Alt allele – the other (alternative) allele on the locus of the Ref allele; MAF – minor allele frequency (data from 1000 Genomes); HWE – Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium; P for HWE – p-value for HWE in 2 groups; P* – p-value for allele frequency differences between the 2 groups.
Table 5.
SNP | Model | Genotype | ER−/PR− | ER+/PR+ | P* (χ2) | Logistic regression | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
OR (95% CI) | P** | ||||||
rs4680 | Codominant | G/G | 70 | 64 | – | – | |
G/A | 33 | 49 | 0.120 | 0.868 (0.521–1.448) | 0.588 | ||
A/A | 10 | 6 | 1.842 (0.937–3.62) | 0.076 | |||
Dominant | G/G | 70 | 64 | 0.208 | 1.403 (0.833–2.362) | 0.204 | |
G/A+A/A | 43 | 55 | |||||
Recessive | G/G+G/A | 103 | 113 | 0.253 | 0.633 (0.232–1.723) | 0.370 | |
A/A | 10 | 6 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 1.14 (0.757–1.716) | 0.531 | |
rs6265 | Codominant | T/T | 23 | 30 | |||
T/C | 59 | 57 | 0.664 | 1.10 (0.767–1.596) | 0.586 | ||
C/C | 31 | 32 | 0.834 (0.499–1.396) | 0.490 | |||
Dominant | T/T | 23 | 30 | 0.379 | 1.008 (0.567–1.792) | 0.978 | |
T/C+C/C | 90 | 89 | |||||
Recessive | T/T+T/C | 82 | 87 | 0.926 | 1.29 (0.697–2.389) | 0.418 | |
C/C | 31 | 32 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 1.097 (0.762–1.578) | 0.618 | |
rs165599 | Codominant | G/G | 35 | 22 | – | – | |
G/A | 56 | 70 | 0.087 | 1.423 (0.967–2.093) | 0.073 | ||
A/A | 22 | 27 | 1.418 (0.842–2.387) | 0.190 | |||
Dominant | G/G | 35 | 22 | 0.027 | 2.019 (1.097–3.717) | 0.024 | |
G/A+A/A | 78 | 97 | |||||
Recessive | G/G+G/A | 91 | 92 | 0.548 | 1.245 (0.664–2.335) | 0.494 | |
A/A | 22 | 27 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 1.44 (0.979–2.118) | 0.064 | |
rs737865 | Codominant | A/A | 58 | 60 | – | – | |
A/G | 48 | 43 | 0.098 | 1.641 (0.997–2.703) | 0.052 | ||
G/G | 6 | 16 | 0.519 (0.272–0.991) | 0.047 | |||
Dominant | A/A | 58 | 60 | 0.836 | 1.056 (0.632–1.767) | 0.834 | |
A/G+G/G | 54 | 59 | |||||
Recessive | A/A+A/G | 106 | 103 | 0.036 | 2.888 (1.096–7.612) | 0.032 | |
G/G | 6 | 16 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 1.259 (0.852–1.862) | 0.248 | |
rs429358 | Codominant | T/T | 93 | 101 | – | – | |
T/C | 20 | 17 | 0.492 | 3.819e+004 (0-inf) | 0.999 | ||
C/C | 0 | 1 | 2.01e–005 (0-inf) | 0.999 | |||
Dominant | T/T | 93 | 101 | 0.597 | 0.813 (0.405–1.63) | 0.559 | |
T/C+C/C | 20 | 18 | |||||
Recessive | T/T+T/C | 113 | 118 | 0.329 | 1.521e+009 (0-inf) | 0.999 | |
C/C | 0 | 1 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 0.873 (0.448–1.703) | 0.691 | |
rs7412 | Codominant | C/C | 93 | 98 | – | – | |
C/T | 19 | 21 | 0.548 | 2.411e–005 (0-inf) | 0.999 | ||
T/T | 1 | 0 | 4.511e+004 (0-inf) | 0.999 | |||
Dominant | C/C | 93 | 98 | 0.992 | 1.033 (0.530–2.016) | 0.923 | |
C/T+T/T | 20 | 21 | |||||
Recessive | C/C+C/T | 112 | 119 | 0.304 | 5.73e–010 (0-inf) | 0.999 | |
T/T | 1 | 0 | |||||
Additive | – | – | – | – | 0.975 (0.521–1.856) | 0.938 |
χ2 – test of P values for SNP polymorphism distribution differences between 2 groups;
P-value for logistic regression analysis; OR – odds ratio (OR); 95% CI – 95% confidence interval; Models – various genetic models that were defined as 1 (MM+Mm) versus 0 (mm) for dominant; 1 (mm) versus 0 (MM+Mm) for recessive; and 0 (mm) versus 1 (Mm) versus 2 (MM) for additive and codominant (M and m represent major and minor alleles, respectively).
The analysis of correlations between BDNF, APOE, and COMT gene polymorphisms and CICI
The analysis of the correlations between CICI (MMSE, DST, VFT, EBPM, and TBPM) and COMT (rs737865) indicated a marked association between the A/A genotype (β=1.536; 95% CI=1.02~2.313; p=0.040) and EBPM scores (Table 6). Specifically, the recessive model (β=1.499, 95% CI=1.017~2.211, p=0.041) was found to be slightly associated with EBPM.
Table 6.
Model | Genotype | B (95% CI) | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
MMSE | Dominant | A/A | 1.078 (0.92~1.264) | 0.351 |
G/A+G/G | ||||
Recessive | A/A+G/A | 0.971 (0.746~1.256) | 0.830 | |
G/G | ||||
HOM | A/A | 1.007 (0.767~1.319) | 0.958 | |
HET | G/A | 1.090 (0.920~1.292) | 0.318 | |
DST | Dominant | A/A | 0.956 (0.73~1.25) | 0.746 |
G/A+G/G | ||||
Recessive | A/A+G/A | 0.841 (0.532~1.329) | 0.458 | |
G/G | ||||
HOM | – | 0.843 (0.525~−1.353) | 0.478 | |
HET | G/A | 1.004 (0.754~−1.336) | 0.979 | |
VFT | Dominant | A/A | 0.949 (0.84~1.072) | 0.400 |
G/A+G/G | ||||
Recessive | A/A+G/A | 1.209 (0.977~1.496) | 0.081 | |
G/G | ||||
HOM | A/A | 1.168 (0.93~1.466) | 0.181 | |
HET | G/A | 0.910 (0.799~1.036) | 0.153 | |
EBPM | Dominant | A/A | 1.117 (0.898~1.388) | 0.321 |
G/A+G/G | ||||
Recessive | A/A+G/A | 1.499 (1.017~2.211) | 0.041 | |
G/G | ||||
HOM | A/A | 1.536 (1.02~2.313) | 0.040 | |
HET | G/A | 1.039 (0.826~1.306) | 0.745 | |
TBPM | Dominant | A/A | 0.941 (0.707~1.253) | 0.941 |
G/A+G/G | ||||
Recessive | A/A+G/A | 1.021 (0.627~1.663) | 0.934 | |
G/G | ||||
HOM | A/A | 1.002 (0.589~1.705) | 0.994 | |
HET | G/A | 0.959 (0.710~1.295) | 0.783 |
HOM – homozygote; HET – heterozygote; beta – regression coefficient; 95% CI – 95% confidence interval
Discussion
The present results showed (1) that ER−/PR− patients performed more poorly on the DST, VFT, and EBPM tests following chemotherapy than ER+/PR+ patients; and (2) that the COMT rs737865 polymorphism was a candidate genetic marker of risk for CICI in patients with ER−/PR− breast cancer, with a linear effect (β=1.499, 95% CI=1.017~2.211) on EBPM performance. To the best of our knowledge, these results are the first to demonstrate a link between COMT genotypes and CICI in breast cancer patients with a differential expression of hormonal receptors.
Memory impairment is a frequent cognitive adverse effect of chemotherapy [25], and may result in difficulties in everyday functioning and reduced quality of life in breast cancer patients [26,27]. Following our previous study documenting PM deficits [11], we replicated the finding here that patients with ER−/PR− breast cancer experience worse chemotherapy-associated EBPM impairments than those with ER+/PR+ breast cancer [10].
Many studies have indicated important roles for estrogen and progesterone in cognitive functioning. Estrogen can promote synaptogenesis, regulate neurotransmission, prevent oxidative stress, and induce growth factor production [28,29]. Estrogen regulates the release of choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) and acetylcholine acetylase (AChE); thus, estrogen regulates cholinergic signaling central to learning and memory [30]. Studies have shown that the ERa estrogen receptor affects depression and cognitive dysfunction as well, and that ERa affects neuronal growth, neuroprotection, and cell signaling transcription [31,32]. ERa can effectively maintain hippocampal function. When E2 levels are low or high-affinity ERa is reduced, neuroprotection and synaptic function-related transcription are impaired, resulting in memory impairment [33]. Progesterone is not only synthesized in the nervous system (brain, spinal cord, and peripheral nerves) but also affects the nervous system’s function and structure. Progesterone is a typical neuroactive steroid, exerting its neurotrophic and protective effects by binding to receptors, and activating signal transduction pathways, which in turn influence the nervous system’s function and structure [34,35]. Studies have shown that progesterone replacement therapy can delay apoptosis of nerve cells, reduce the volume of cerebral infarction, reduce experimental cerebral ischemic injury, promote the synthesis of myelin, and improve neurocognitive functions such as learning and memory [36–38]. A study on the relationship between physiological hormone cycles in animals and learning and memory abilities found that improvements in learning and memory were related to increases in estrogen and progesterone levels. When combined with estrogen, progesterone enhances estrogen’s ability to improve learning and memory [39]. Progesterone receptors are widely expressed in the human brain and vital for cognitive function. In our study, patients with ER−/PR− breast cancer had more severe deficits in EBPM performance than patients with ER+/PR+ breast cancer.
Although the mechanisms underlying CICI remain unknown, according to preliminary evidence, genetic variations are likely to increase the extent of cognitive impairment. COMT polymorphisms are denoted as a valine (Val or G) and methionine (Met or A) substitution at codon 108/158. The activity of the COMT enzyme with the A/A genotype was 3- to 4-fold lower than that with the G/G genotype, resulting in reduced dopamine degradation [40]. The COMT Val158Met polymorphism has been shown to modulate the cognitive and symptom profiles in patients with schizophrenia, with more negative outcomes related to adverse childhood experiences in Met carriers [41]. Small and colleagues reported that COMT-Val+ carriers undergoing chemotherapy had worse performance on tests that required frontal cortical dopamine neurotransmission relative to COMT Met/A allele carriers after chemotherapy in breast cancer patients [20]. Our previous study showed that the COMT (rs165599) polymorphism influenced CICI in triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC) patients [14]. In this study, we observed that patients with ER−/PR− breast cancer had worse performance than patients with ER+/PR+ breast cancer on neuropsychological tests of cognitive function, including EBPM. This revealed that COMT (rs737865) genotypes (A/A), to a certain extent, can alter the changes in cognitive function that result following breast cancer treatment, especially on the EBPM task.
APOE ɛ4 is associated with a decline in cognitive function, including Alzheimer disease [42–44]. Koleck et al. found that breast cancer patients with the APOE-ɛ4 genotype showed deficits in executive function, attention, verbal learning, and memory after chemotherapy [22]. Here, we found that APOE (rs429358 and rs7412) showed no statistically significant differences between the 2 groups, which revealed that the expression of APOE may have nothing to do with the extent of CICI in breast cancer survivors with different expression levels of tumor hormonal receptors. The current study included 232 breast cancer survivors treated with standard chemotherapy regimens, among whom only 19 (8.19%) were over 65 years of age. Thus, the APOE polymorphisms may not have been related to cognitive decline in these breast cancer patients. Similarly, BDNF polymorphisms did not show a significant relationship with cognitive impairment in patients with breast cancer undergoing chemotherapy.
Limitations of the study
Finally, some limitations should be acknowledged. First, this study classified breast cancer into only 2 groups: an ER+/PR+ group and an ER−/PR− group, and failed to make comparisons with healthy controls. The expression of COMT, APOE, and BDNF gene polymorphisms in healthy women still needs further research. The second limitation was that the experiment used the results of a subjective memory scale; objective cognitive tests could be further examined in future research. Third, the sample size of this experiment was small, making it a small-sample cross-sectional study that lacked a sufficient number of breast cancer patients for broad generalization. Fourth, this study focused only on the molecular genetic changes and their relationship to memory impairments after chemotherapy in breast cancer patients. Specific brain regions related to memory impairment were not examined; imaging of brain function would be a good area for further study in the future.
Conclusions
In summary, we reported differences in chemotherapy-induced prospective memory impairments and genetic polymorphisms in patients with breast cancer with various expression patterns of hormonal receptors. The results suggest that the heterogeneity in CICI may be modified by the COMT (rs737865) polymorphism, which may alter the risk of cognitive impairment in patients with breast cancer with various expression patterns of tumor hormonal receptors.
Acknowledgments
We would like to express our sincere gratitude to all participants, researchers, and others who provided support during the process of this study.
Footnotes
Ethics declarations
The Research Ethics Committee of the Second Affiliated Hospital of Anhui Medical University, China, issued the approval for this study.
Conflicts of interest
None.
Source of support: This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (No. 81872504; 81372487). The funding body/bodies covering the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data were funded by grant number 81872504. The writing of the manuscript was covered in part with funds from grant number 81372487
References
- 1.DeSantis CE, Ma J, Goding Sauer A, et al. Breast cancer statistics, 2017, racial disparity in mortality by state. Cancer J Clin. 2017;67(6):439–48. doi: 10.3322/caac.21412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gokal K, Munir F, Ahmed S, et al. Does walking protect against decline in cognitive functioning among breast cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy? Results from a small randomised controlled trial. PLoS One. 2018;13(11):e0206874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cerulla N, Arcusa A, Navarro JB, et al. Cognitive impairment following chemotherapy for breast cancer: The impact of practice effect on results. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2019;41(3):290–99. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1546381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Winocur G, Berman H, Nguyen M, et al. Neurobiological mechanisms of chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment in a transgenic model of breast cancer. Neuroscience. 2018;369:51–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.10.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vitali M, Ripamonti CI, Roila F, et al. Cognitive impairment and chemotherapy: A brief overview. Crit Rev Oncol Hematol. 2017;118:7–14. doi: 10.1016/j.critrevonc.2017.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bernstein LJ, McCreath GA, Komeylian Z, Rich JB. Cognitive impairment in breast cancer survivors treated with chemotherapy depends on control group type and cognitive domains assessed: a multilevel meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;83:417–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Oberste M, Schaffrath N, Schmidt K, et al. Protocol for the “Chemobrain in Motion – study” (CIM – study): A randomized placebo-controlled trial of the impact of a high-intensity interval endurance training on cancer related cognitive impairments in women with breast cancer receiving first-line chemotherapy. BMC Cancer. 2018;18(1):1071. doi: 10.1186/s12885-018-4992-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Myers JS. Chemotherapy-related cognitive impairment: The breast cancer experience. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2012;39(1):E31–40. doi: 10.1188/12.ONF.E31-E40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Duivon M, Perrier J, Joly F, et al. Impact of breast cancer on prospective memory functioning assessed by virtual reality and influence of sleep quality and hormonal therapy: PROSOM-K study. BMC Cancer. 2018;18(1):866. doi: 10.1186/s12885-018-4762-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Li W, Gan C, Lv Y, et al. Chemotherapy-induced prospective memory impairment in breast cancer patients with different hormone receptor expression. Medicine. 2017;96(13):e6514. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000006514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cheng H, Yang Z, Dong B, et al. Chemotherapy-induced prospective memory impairment in patients with breast cancer. Psychooncology. 2013;22(10):2391–95. doi: 10.1002/pon.3291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Perou CM, Sorlie T, Eisen MB, et al. Molecular portraits of human breast tumours. Nature. 2000;406(6797):747–52. doi: 10.1038/35021093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jim HS, Phillips KM, Chait S, et al. Meta-analysis of cognitive functioning in breast cancer survivors previously treated with standard-dose chemotherapy. J Clin Oncol. 2012;30(29):3578–87. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2011.39.5640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cheng H, Li W, Gan C, et al. The COMT (rs165599) gene polymorphism contributes to chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment in breast cancer patients. Am J Transl Res. 2016;8(11):5087–97. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sampedro F, Marin-Lahoz J, Martinez-Horta S, et al. Reduced gray matter volume in cognitively preserved COMT (158)Val/Val Parkinson’s disease patients and its association with cognitive decline. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020;14(2):321–28. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-0022-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Shahid M, Kim J, Leaver K, et al. An increased rate of longitudinal cognitive decline is observed in Parkinson’s disease patients with low CSF Aβ42 and an APOE ɛ4 allele. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;127:278–86. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2019.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Aas M, Djurovic S, Ueland T, et al. The relationship between physical activity, clinical and cognitive characteristics and BDNF mRNA levels in patients with severe mental disorders. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2019;20(7):567–76. doi: 10.1080/15622975.2018.1557345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Gonzalez-Castro TB, Hernandez-Diaz Y, Juarez-Rojop IE, et al. The role of COMT gene Val108/158Met polymorphism in suicidal behavior: Systematic review and updated meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2018;14:2485–96. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S172243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Khanthiyong B, Thanoi S, Reynolds GP, Nudmamud-Thanoi S. Association study of the functional Catechol-O-Methyltranferase (COMT) Val(158)Met polymorphism on executive cognitive function in a Thai sample. Int J Med Sci. 2019;16(11):1461–65. doi: 10.7150/ijms.35789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Small BJ, Rawson KS, Walsh E, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase genotype modulates cancer treatment-related cognitive deficits in breast cancer survivors. Cancer. 2011;117(7):1369–76. doi: 10.1002/cncr.25685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shinohara M, Tashiro Y, Suzuki K, et al. Interaction between APOE genotype and diabetes in cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement (Amst) 2020;12(1):e12006. doi: 10.1002/dad2.12006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Koleck TA, Bender CM, Sereika SM, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype and cognitive function in postmenopausal women with early-stage breast cancer. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2014;41(6):E313–25. doi: 10.1188/14.ONF.E313-E325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Caffino L, Mottarlini F, Fumagalli F. Born to protect: Leveraging BDNF against cognitive deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. CNS Drugs. 2020;34(3):281–97. doi: 10.1007/s40263-020-00705-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ng T, Teo SM, Yeo HL, et al. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor genetic polymorphism (rs6265) is protective against chemotherapy-associated cognitive impairment in patients with early-stage breast cancer. Neuro Oncol. 2016;18(2):244–51. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/nov162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rendeiro C, Sheriff A, Bhattacharya TK, et al. Long-lasting impairments in adult neurogenesis, spatial learning and memory from a standard chemotherapy regimen used to treat breast cancer. Behav Brain Res. 2016;315:10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Andryszak P, Wiłkość M, Żurawski B, Izdebski P. Verbal memory in breast cancer patients treated with chemotherapy with doxorubicin and cyclophosphamide. Eur J Cancer Care (Engl) 2018;27(1):ecc.12749. doi: 10.1111/ecc.12749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Bedard M, Verma S, Collins B, et al. Prospective memory impairment in chemotherapy-exposed early breast cancer survivors: Preliminary evidence from a clinical test. J Psychosoc Oncol. 2016;34(4):291–304. doi: 10.1080/07347332.2016.1181133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Osterlund MK. Underlying mechanisms mediating the antidepressant effects of estrogens. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2010;1800(10):1136–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbagen.2009.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Walf AA, Frye CA. A review and update of mechanisms of estrogen in the hippocampus and amygdala for anxiety and depression behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(6):1097–111. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Gibbs RB. Estrogen therapy and cognition: A review of the cholinergic hypothesis. Endocrine Rev. 2010;31(2):224–53. doi: 10.1210/er.2009-0036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ishunina TA, Swaab DF. Hippocampal estrogen receptor-alpha splice variant TADDI in the human brain in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroendocrinology. 2009;89(2):187–99. doi: 10.1159/000158573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gingerich S, Kim GL, Chalmers JA, et al. Estrogen receptor alpha and G-protein coupled receptor 30 mediate the neuroprotective effects of 17beta-estradiol in novel murine hippocampal cell models. Neuroscience. 2010;170(1):54–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Foster TC, Rani A, Kumar A, et al. Viral vector-mediated delivery of estrogen receptor-alpha to the hippocampus improves spatial learning in estrogen receptor-alpha knockout mice. Mol Ther. 2008;16(9):1587–93. doi: 10.1038/mt.2008.140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.O’Connor CA, Cernak I, Johnson F, Vink R. Effects of progesterone on neurologic and morphologic outcome following diffuse traumatic brain injury in rats. Exp Neurol. 2007;205(1):145–53. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.01.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Jiang C, Zuo F, Wang Y, et al. Progesterone exerts neuroprotective effects and improves long-term neurologic outcome after intracerebral hemorrhage in middle-aged mice. Neurobiol Aging. 2016;42:13–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wang KK, Larner SF, Robinson G, Hayes RL. Neuroprotection targets after traumatic brain injury. Curr Opin Neurol. 2006;19(6):514–19. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e3280102b10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Dickert NW, Scicluna VM, Baren JM, et al. Patients’ perspectives of enrollment in research without consent: the patients’ experiences in emergency research-progesterone for the treatment of traumatic brain injury study. Crit Care Med. 2015;43(3):603–12. doi: 10.1097/CCM.0000000000000747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schumacher M, Guennoun R, Ghoumari A, et al. Novel perspectives for progesterone in hormone replacement therapy, with special reference to the nervous system. Endocr Rev. 2007;28(4):387–439. doi: 10.1210/er.2006-0050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Markham JA, Pych JC, Juraska JM. Ovarian hormone replacement to aged ovariectomized female rats benefits acquisition of the morris water maze. Horm Behav. 2002;42(3):284–93. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2002.1819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Lin CH, Fan JY, Lin HI, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genetic variants are associated with cognitive decline in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2018;50:48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2018.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Green MJ, Chia TY, Cairns MJ, et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) genotype moderates the effects of childhood trauma on cognition and symptoms in schizophrenia. J Psych Res. 2014;49:43–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Small BJ, Rosnick CB, Fratiglioni L, Backman L. Apolipoprotein E and cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. 2004;19(4):592–600. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.4.592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Luck T, Then FS, Luppa M, et al. Association of the apolipoprotein E genotype with memory performance and executive functioning in cognitively intact elderly. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(3):382–87. doi: 10.1037/neu0000147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Greenwood PM, Lambert C, Sunderland T, Parasuraman R. Effects of apolipoprotein E genotype on spatial attention, working memory, and their interaction in healthy, middle-aged adults: Results From the National Institute of Mental Health’s BIOCARD study. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(2):199–211. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.2.199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]