Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2020 Oct 2;10:335. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-01019-6

Polygenic risk score, psychosocial environment and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Søren D Østergaard 1,2,3, Betina B Trabjerg 3,4, Thomas D Als 3,5, Clara Albiñana Climent 3,4, Florian Privé 3,4, Bjarni Jóhann Vilhjálmsson 3,4, Marie Bækvad-Hansen 3,6, Jonas Bybjerg-Grauholm 3,6, David M Hougaard 3,6, Merete Nordentoft 3,7, Thomas Werge 3,8, Ditte Demontis 3,5,9, Preben B Mortensen 3,4,10, Anders D Børglum 3,5,9, Ole Mors 2,3,11, Esben Agerbo 3,4,10,
PMCID: PMC7532146  PMID: 33009369

Abstract

The objective of the present study was to investigate whether the polygenic liability for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and the psychosocial environment impact the risk of ADHD in interaction or independently of each other. We conducted a register- and biobank-based cohort study of 13,725 individuals with ADHD and 20,147 randomly drawn population-based controls. These 33,872 cohort members were genotyped on the Infinium PsychChip v1.0 array (Illumina). Subsequently, we calculated the polygenic risk score (PRS) for ADHD and extracted register data regarding the following risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment for each cohort member at the time of birth: maternal/paternal history of mental disorders, maternal/paternal education, maternal/paternal work status, and maternal/paternal income. We used logistic regression analyses to assess the main effects of the PRS for ADHD and the psychosocial environment on the risk of ADHD. Subsequently, we evaluated whether the effect of the PRS and the psychosocial environment act independently or in interaction upon the risk of ADHD. We found that ADHD was strongly associated with the PRS (odds ratio: 6.03, 95%CI: 4.74–7.70 for highest vs. lowest 2% liability). All risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment were associated with an increased risk of ADHD. These associations were only slightly attenuated after mutual adjustments. We found no statistically significant interaction between the polygenic liability and the psychosocial environment upon the risk of ADHD. In conclusion, we found main effects of both polygenic liability and risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment on the risk of ADHD—in the expected direction.

Subject terms: Psychiatric disorders, Medical genetics, ADHD

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder with a prevalence of approximately 5–7% among children/adolescents1,2. ADHD is characterized by hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsiveness3—all to an extent, which causes significant burden on those affected, their families and society as a whole410.

The specific causes of ADHD remain largely unknown, but a number of twin studies have suggested that the disorder is among the most heritable (heritability recently estimated at 74% based on data from 37 twin studies11 in the entire spectrum of mental disorders1113. Accordingly, recent studies focusing on both common- and rare genetic variants have identified genes associated with ADHD1416. Also, a number of early adverse psychosocial factors have consistently been linked to ADHD1721. Furthermore, there is a growing body of literature supporting the hypothesis that genetic and psychosocial risk factors do not only have direct effects on the risk of ADHD, but also interact2224. However, most studies on the combined effect of genetic and psychosocial risk factors for ADHD are based on relatively small samples of patients and healthy controls volunteering for study participation, who have self-reported historical data on their psychosocial environment2529. The combination of the use of healthy controls and self-reported historical data on psychosocial environment introduces a substantial risk of selection and recall bias30,31. Relatedly, most prior gene–environment interaction studies in ADHD have focused on candidate genes in the dopaminergic and serotonergic system that were not replicated in the recent GWAS of ADHD15,2529. Therefore, studies of the effect of interaction between genetic risk and psychosocial environment upon the risk of ADHD, which avoid the abovementioned threats to internal validity are needed. Here, we aimed to conduct such a study using data from the Lundbeck Foundation Initiative for Integrative Psychiatric Research (iPSYCH) case-cohort32. Specifically, the aim of the study was twofold:

  • I.

    To investigate the main effects of the polygenic liability for ADHD and the psychosocial environment on the risk of ADHD: Here, we will investigate the association between a polygenic risk score (PRS) for ADHD, maternal/paternal education, maternal/paternal work status, maternal/paternal income and maternal/paternal history of mental disorder and the risk of developing ADHD in a population-based sample.

  • II.

    To investigate polygenic by psychosocial risk factor interaction on the risk of ADHD: Here, we will test whether there are interactions between the genetic liability (PRS) and the risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment (maternal/paternal education, maternal/paternal work status, maternal/paternal income and maternal/paternal history of mental disorder) with regard to the risk of developing ADHD

Methods

Setting

This study was based on data from the iPSYCH case-cohort, which consists of 86,189 singletons born in Denmark between May 1, 1981 and December 31, 2005, who resided in Denmark on their 1st birthday32. The case-cohort comprises 57,377 individuals registered with either schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, unipolar depression, autism or ADHD diagnosis in the Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register33,34 as well as 30,000 randomly drawn population controls32. These individuals were genotyped using the Infinium PsychChip v1.0 array (Illumina) and psychosocial information for each individual (parental education, work status, income, and psychiatric history) was extracted from nationwide Danish registers. Since the establishment of the iPSYCH case-cohort was not based on opt-in participation32, the risk of selection and attrition bias is minimal. Furthermore, as proxy information on the psychosocial environment is registered routinely for all individuals with an address in Denmark17, there is virtually no recall bias either. Therefore, this dataset allows us to investigate the combined contribution of genetic and psychosocial risk factors for ADHD in a population-based sample virtually without the impact of the typical biases affecting studies of this type.

Population

In this study, we focused on the individuals that were included in the iPSYCH case-cohort due to an ADHD diagnosis (ICD-10 code F90.0) as well as the iPSYCH population controls for whom there were valid genotypes and who had parents that were both born in Denmark32. To further mitigate confounding by population stratification, we computed the orthogonalized Gnanadesikan-Kettenrin robust Mahalanobis distance of the ten leading ancestral principal components, and excluded individuals who had a logarithm-distance larger than three (1120 with ADHD and 1730 population controls)35,36. This led to inclusion of a total of 33,872 individuals, namely 13,725 with ADHD and 20,147 population controls. There were 771 sibling constellations (2–4 cohort members with the same parents) within this population. For 454 (58.9%) of these constellations, all siblings were in the ADHD group, for 154 (20.0%), all siblings were among the population controls, and for 163 (21.1%) the sibling constellation represented a mix of individuals with ADHD and population controls. A total of 750 (97.3%) of the sibling constellations consisted of two siblings. The median age at diagnosis of ADHD was eleven years (interquartile range: 9 years) and total of 593 (4.3%) of the individuals with ADHD were also registered with a diagnosis of oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder (as defined by Wimberley et al.37) either prior to or at the time of the ADHD diagnosis.

Data sources

For the 33,872 individuals in the study cohort, we extracted data from six sources, namely the Danish Civil Registration System (link between children and parents—as well as vital status)38, the Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register (diagnoses for all individuals assessed or treated for mental disorders at a psychiatric hospital in Denmark)33,34, the Danish Education Register (education)39, the Danish Register on Personal Labour Market Affiliation (work status)40, the Danish Register on Personal Income and Transfer Payments (income)41, and the Danish Neonatal Screening Biobank42 (genotypes generated via the Infinium PsychChip v1.0 array (Illumina) for obtaining PRS for ADHD)32. Linkage of information from these six sources is made possible by the unique personal registration number, which is assigned to all individuals residing in Denmark38.

Definition of genetic variable (PRS for ADHD)

The PRS for ADHD for each individual was estimated as described in Demontis et al15. The 23 genotyping batches of the iPSYCH sample were split into five groups of approximately equal size, aiming for an equal number of ADHD cases within each group. Five leave-one-out analyses were then conducted, using four of five groups as discovery datasets for estimation of SNP effect sizes, while estimating PRS for the remaining target group. The GWAS meta-analyses of the discovery samples were conducted using an SNP list filtered for minor allele frequency >0.01 and an imputation threshold score above 0.8 intersecting across batches. INDELs and variants in the extended MHC region (chromosome 6: 25–34 Mb) were also removed. Meta-analysis and “LD-clumping” of significant SNPs were conducted using the ricopili pipeline43. PRS was then estimated for each target sample using a range of meta-analysis p-value thresholds (5 × 10−8, 1 × 10−6, 1 × 10−4, 1 × 10−3, 0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.2, 0.5, 1.0), multiplying the natural log of the odds ratio of each variant by the allele-dosage of each variant. Whole-genome PRS were obtained by summing values over variants for each individual. The PRS was standardized within target groups. The significance of the case-control score difference was tested by standard logistic regression including principal component and dummy variables indicating genotyping batch as covariates and the proportion of variance explained was estimated for each p-value threshold by comparing the full model with a reduced model without PRS and thus including covariates only (i.e. Nagelkerke’s R2). The standardized score for the p-value threshold with the highest Nagelkerke’s R2 (p < 0.2) was used in the subsequent analyses. The standardized PRS for ADHD was divided into 50 groups each representing 2% of the distribution among the randomly drawn population controls.

Definition of variables pertaining to the psychosocial environment

While we have previously used a register-based operationalization of Rutter’s indicators of adversity in relation to ADHD17, this was not possible in the present study due to restrictions enforced by the Danish Neonatal Screening Biobank. Therefore, in this study, we focused on the psychosocial environment in the family of the cohort members instead, as there is a well-known association between this construct and concrete adverse incidents among children4450. Specifically, we focused on maternal/paternal history of mental disorders, maternal/paternal education, maternal/paternal work status, and maternal/paternal income.

Maternal/paternal history of mental disorders

At the date of birth of the cohort members, we assessed whether their parents were registered with a diagnosis of a mental disorder in the Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register33. The following categories were defined based on diagnoses from the International Classification of Diseases, 8th revision (ICD-8), which was used as diagnostic reference from 1969 to 1993 and from the International Classification of Diseases, 10th revision (ICD-10), which was used from 1994 and onwards: Schizophrenia and related disorders (ICD-8: 295.x9, 296.89, 297.x9, 298.29–298.99, 299.04, 299.05, 299.09, 301.83. ICD-10: F20-F29), Mood disorders (ICD-8: 296.x9 (excl. 296.89), 298.09, 298.19, 300.49, 301.19. ICD-10: F30-F39), Neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders (ICD-8: 300.x9 (excl. 300.49), 305.x9 305.68, 307.99. ICD-10: F40-F48), and other mental disorders not listed under the categories above (ICD-8: 290–315. ICD-10: F00-F99)51. The categories were hierarchical and mutually exclusive such that schizophrenia and related disorders trumped mood disorders, which trumped neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders, which trumped other mental disorders.

Maternal/paternal education

Information regarding the parents’ highest completed level of education in the year of the birth of the cohort members was extracted from the Danish Education Register39 using the following levels: primary school, high school or vocational education (e.g. carpenter or bricklayer), short- or medium-length higher education (e.g. nurse, schoolteacher and bachelor level university degrees), long academic education (master level university degrees or PhD).

Maternal/paternal work status

Information regarding the parents’ primary work status in the year prior to the birth of the cohort member was extracted from the Integrated Database for Labor Market Research40 using the following levels: unemployed or otherwise outside the labor marked, student in education, blue collar worker, self-employed, clerical worker or leading wage-earner.

Maternal/paternal income

The parents’ income in the year prior to the cohort members’ birth was extracted from the Registers on Personal Income and Transfer Payments41. The income was scaled to 2004 level using the price index from the World Bank. The income of the mothers and fathers of the randomly drawn population controls was used to create income quintiles. Individuals with missing values were placed in the lowest quintile.

Statistics

The associations between the PRS for ADHD, the risk factors related to the psychosocial environment and ADHD were assessed using logistic regression. Odds ratios and 95% likelihood ratio-based confidence intervals were computed. For the main effects of the PRS for ADHD and the risk factors related to the psychosocial environment, crude odds ratios for ADHD adjusted for sex and year of birth were computed. To assess whether the impact of the psychosocial factors was mediated through the genetic liability for ADHD, we calculated odds ratios that were also adjusted for the PRS. Furthermore, to investigate whether the PRS effect was partly explained or confounded by the psychosocial environment, we calculated odds ratios that were fully adjusted for all psychosocial factors. To explore the polygenic liability distribution across case-control status and the factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment, we calculated population marginal means52. Lastly, we evaluated whether the PRS (continuous) interacted with the risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment upon the risk of ADHD. As a sensitivity analysis, we challenged the necessity for excluding ancestral principal components outliers, by repeating the analyses outlined above, without exclusion of individuals (1120 with ADHD and 1730 population controls) based on ancestral principal components outliers53,54. Analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 and R version 3.5.1.

Ethics

The study was approved by the Danish Health Data Authority, the Danish data protection agency, The Danish Neonatal Screening Biobank Steering Committee and the Danish Scientific Ethics Committee.

Results

The characteristics of the cohort members—including information regarding the PRS for ADHD and the risk factors related to the psychosocial environment—are shown in Table 1. The distribution of the PRS for ADHD for the cohort members with ADHD and the randomly selected population controls is shown in Fig. 1.

Table 1.

Distribution of polygenic risk score, parental history of mental disorders and socioeconomic factors for the ADHD cases and the randomly drawn population controls.

ADHD cases Controls
N (%) N (%)
Sex
    Male 10,120 (73.73) 10,203 (50.64)
    Female 3605 (26.27) 9944 (49.36)
Polygenic risk scorea
    50 513 (3.74) 339 (1.68)
    40 267 (1.95) 384 (1.91)
    30 240 (1.75) 406 (2.02)
    20 248 (1.81) 427 (2.12)
    10 274 (2.00) 400 (1.99)
    1 154 (1.12) 433 (2.15)
Mother History of mental disorderb
    Schizophrenia and related disorders 69 (0.50) 50 (0.25)
    Mood disorders 105 (0.77) 88 (0.44)
    Neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders 309 (2.25) 204 (1.01)
    Other Psychiatric disorder 276 (2.01) 183 (0.91)
    No psychiatric disorder 12,966 (94.47) 19,622 (97.39)
Highest obtained education
    Primary schoolc 6413 (46.72) 5983 (29.70)
    High school or vocational education 5368 (39.11) 8830 (43.83)
    Short- or medium-cycle higher education 1736 (12.65) 4437 (22.02)
    Long-cycle higher education or PhD 208 (1.52) 897 (4.45)
Working status
    Unemployed or otherwise outside the labor marked 3851 (28.06) 3506 (17.40)
    Student in education 485 (3.53) 505 (2.51)
    Blue collar worker 5421 (39.50) 6464 (32.08)
    Self-employed 174 (1.27) 335 (1.66)
    Clerical worker or Leading wage-earner 3794 (27.64) 9337 (46.34)
Income
    Lowest quintile 3741 (27.26) 4017 (19.94)
    Second quintile 3143 (22.90) 4024 (19.97)
    Third quintile 2735 (19.93) 4030 (20.00)
    Fourth quintile 2362 (17.21) 4032 (20.01)
    Highest quintile 1744 (12.71) 4044 (20.07)
Father History of mental disorderb
    Schizophrenia and related disorders 76 (0.55) 51 (0.25)
    Mood disorders 58 (0.42) 55 (0.27)
    Neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders 176 (1.28) 134 (0.67)
    Other Psychiatric disorder 355 (2.59) 198 (0.98)
    No psychiatric disorder 13,060 (95.15) 19,709 (97.83)
Highest obtained education
    Primary schoolc 5853 (42.64) 5244 (26.03)
    High school or vocational education 6364 (46.37) 10,356 (51.40)
    Short- or medium-cycle higher education 1119 (8.15) 3080 (15.29)
    Long-cycle higher education or PhD 389 (2.83) 1467 (7.28)
Working status
    Unemployed or otherwise outside the labor marked 2028 (14.78) 1657 (8.22)
    Student in education 166 (1.21) 264 (1.31)
    Blue collar worker 8000 (58.29) 9673 (48.01)
    Self-employed 641 (4.67) 1437 (7.13)
    Clerical worker or Leading wage-earner 2890 (21.06) 7116 (35.32)
Income
    Lowest quintile 4020 (29.29) 4014 (19.92)
    Second quintile 3064 (22.32) 4016 (19.93)
    Third quintile 2673 (19.48) 4028 (19.99)
    Fourth quintile 2186 (15.93) 4037 (20.04)
    Highest quintile 1782 (12.98) 4052 (20.11)

aThe polygenic risk score of ADHD divided into fifty groups 1 to 50, here only showing counts for selected groups

bNone of the controls had a mother with ADHD and 9 controls had a father with ADHD. Among the individuals with ADHD, 15 had a mother with ADHD and 49 had a father with ADHD. Definition of ADHD among the parents: ICD-10 diagnosis: F90 or ICD-8 diagnosis: 308.01

cThis category includes individuals with missing information on parental education

Fig. 1. ADHD Polygenic Risk Score Distributions.

Fig. 1

Comparison between individuals with ADHD and uniform randomly population-based individuals.

Column 1 in Table 2 lists the crude odds ratios for ADHD (adjusted for sex and year of birth), related to the PRS for ADHD and the risk factors representing the psychosocial environment. Both the PRS for ADHD and the psychosocial factors were strongly associated with the risk of developing ADHD. Specifically, individuals with a PRS among the 2% highest values had a six fold increased risk of ADHD (6.03 (95% CI, 4.74–7.70)) compared to those with a PRS among the lowest 2%. The risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment (parental history of mental disorder, low parental education level, parental unemployment, and low parental income) were all associated with increased risk of ADHD at the statistically significant level. Column 2 lists odds ratios for ADHD for the risk factors representing the psychosocial environment after adjustment for the PRS, sex, and year of birth. This adjustment let to very subtle attenuations of the associations. Column 3 contains results from the mutually adjusted model. This led to modest attenuation of the association between the PRS and the risk for ADHD. For instance, the odds ratio associated with highest versus lowest 2% genetic liability decreased from 6.03 (95% CI, 4.74–7.70) to 4.23 (95% CI, 3.30–5.45). The crude and adjusted associations between the PRS and the risk of ADHD are shown in Fig. 2. The mutually adjusted odds ratios pertaining to the psychosocial environment should be interpreted with caution, as these factors are unlikely to be independent (e.g. labor market affiliation and income).

Table 2.

Main effect of the polygenic risk score (PRS) and psychosocial risk factors on the risk of ADHD.

Crudea Prs adjustedb Adjustedc
OR (95% CL) OR (95% CL) OR (95% CL)
Sex
    Male 2.69 (2.57;2.82) 2.71 (2.58;2.84) 2.75 (2.61;2.89)
    Female 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Polygenic risk scored
    50 6.03 (4.74;7.70) 4.23 (3.30;5.45)
    40 2.77 (2.15;3.57) 2.33 (1.79;3.03)
    30 2.23 (1.73;2.88) 1.89 (1.46;2.47)
    20 2.19 (1.70;2.83) 1.77 (1.37;2.31)
    10 2.32 (1.81;2.98) 1.96 (1.52;2.54)
    1 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Mother History of mental disorder
    Schizophrenia and related disorders 1.99 (1.37;2.92) 1.97 (1.35;2.91) 1.23 (0.83;1.84)
    Mood disorders 1.67 (1.24;2.25) 1.66 (1.23;2.24) 1.40 (1.03;1.90)
    Neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders 2.29 (1.90;2.76) 2.28 (1.90;2.75) 1.70 (1.41;2.07)
    Other Psychiatric disorder 2.31 (1.90;2.81) 2.23 (1.83;2.72) 1.51 (1.23;1.86)
    No psychiatric disorder 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Highest obtained education
    Primary schoole 5.90 (5.04;6.94) 5.63 (4.80;6.63) 2.24 (1.87;2.70)
    High school or vocational education 2.84 (2.43;3.33) 2.75 (2.35;3.23) 1.54 (1.29;1.85)
    Short- or medium-cycle higher education 1.85 (1.57;2.18) 1.83 (1.56;2.17) 1.39 (1.17;1.66)
    Long-cycle higher education or PhD 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Working status
    Unemployed or otherwise outside the labor marked 2.76 (2.59;2.94) 2.69 (2.52;2.86) 1.45 (1.34;1.57)
    Student in education 2.29 (2.00;2.62) 2.27 (1.98;2.60) 1.43 (1.23;1.67)
    Blue collar worker 1.98 (1.87;2.09) 1.94 (1.83;2.05) 1.24 (1.16;1.33)
    Self-employed 1.24 (1.02;1.50) 1.22 (1.00;1.48) 1.00 (0.81;1.22)
    Clerical worker or Leading wage-earner 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Income
    Lowest quintile 2.67 (2.47;2.88) 2.61 (2.42;2.82) 1.20 (1.09;1.31)
    Second quintile 2.12 (1.96;2.28) 2.07 (1.91;2.23) 1.12 (1.02;1.22)
    Third quintile 1.79 (1.66;1.94) 1.76 (1.63;1.90) 1.12 (1.03;1.22)
    Fourth quintile 1.47 (1.35;1.59) 1.45 (1.34;1.57) 1.13 (1.04;1.23)
    Highest quintile 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Father History of mental disorder
    Schizophrenia and related disorders 2.22 (1.54;3.23) 2.22 (1.54;3.24) 1.51 (1.02;2.25)
    Mood disorders 1.78 (1.21;2.62) 1.78 (1.20;2.62) 1.46 (0.96;2.21)
    Neurotic, stress-related, and somatoform disorders 1.88 (1.49;2.38) 1.86 (1.47;2.35) 1.27 (0.99;1.62)
    Other Psychiatric disorder 2.90 (2.42;3.48) 2.84 (2.37;3.42) 1.71 (1.41;2.08)
    No psychiatric disorder 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Highest obtained education
    Primary schoole 4.59 (4.07;5.19) 4.35 (3.86;4.93) 1.94 (1.68;2.24)
    High school or vocational education 2.43 (2.16;2.74) 2.34 (2.08;2.65) 1.40 (1.22;1.61)
    Short- or medium-cycle higher education 1.37 (1.20;1.56) 1.34 (1.17;1.54) 1.10 (0.95;1.26)
    Long-cycle higher education or PhD 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Working status
    Unemployed or otherwise outside the labor marked 3.18 (2.93;3.45) 3.07 (2.83;3.33) 1.34 (1.21;1.48)
    Student in education 1.51 (1.23;1.85) 1.52 (1.24;1.87) 0.94 (0.75;1.18)
    Blue collar worker 1.99 (1.88;2.10) 1.95 (1.84;2.06) 1.20 (1.12;1.28)
    Self-employed 1.10 (0.99;1.22) 1.10 (0.99;1.22) 0.80 (0.71;0.89)
    Clerical worker or Leading wage-earner 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)
Income
    Lowest quintile 2.51 (2.33;2.70) 2.44 (2.27;2.63) 1.21 (1.10;1.32)
    Second quintile 1.92 (1.78;2.07) 1.89 (1.75;2.05) 1.14 (1.05;1.24)
    Third quintile 1.61 (1.49;1.74) 1.59 (1.47;1.72) 1.11 (1.02;1.20)
    Fourth quintile 1.26 (1.16;1.36) 1.24 (1.15;1.35) 0.98 (0.90;1.06)
    Highest quintile 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref) 1.00 (ref)

aAdjustment for sex and year of birth

bAdjustment for sex, year of birth and the polygenic risk score for ADHD

cAll estimates are mutually adjusted and adjusted for year of birth

dThe polygenic risk score of ADHD divided into fifty groups 1 to 50, here only showing estimates for selected groups (see Fig. 2 for more details)

eThis category includes individuals with missing information on parental education

Fig. 2. The crude and adjusted effect of the polygenic risk score for ADHD on the risk of ADHD.

Fig. 2

*crude adjustment: the estimates are only adjusted for sex and year of birth. The polygenic risk score for ADHD was divided into 50 groups each representing 2% of the distribution of the PRS for ADHD among the randomly drawn population controls.

The population marginal means of the PRS for ADHD adjusted for sex and year of birth are displayed in Figure S1, and show that the polygenic liability was higher among individuals with ADHD than controls across all of the risk factors pertaining to the psychosocial environment. These differences were less apparent for those having parents with mental disorders, but more pronounced across the other environmental factors with a weak tendency to a lower liability in subjects whose parents had longer educational attainment or higher income.

The combined effect of the PRS for ADHD in quintiles and each of the psychosocial risk factors (mutually adjusted for) is shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3. Polygenic Risk Score and Parental Psychosocial Environment.

Fig. 3

Odds ratio for ADHD across levels of parental risk factors and polygenic risk score quartiles.

These plots are predominantly indicative of independence between the effects of the PRS for ADHD and the psychosocial risk factors upon the risk of ADHD (the p-values for interaction range from 0.06 to 0.48).

The results listed in Table S1, Table S2, Figs. S2, S3, and S4 in the supplementary material are analog to those in Tables 1 and 2, Figs. 1, 2 and 3; however, without excluding the ancestral principal components outliers. The results in these two sets of analyses are practically identical. As a representative example, the odds ratio for ADHD associated with the highest versus the lowest 2% genetic liability was 6.01 (95%CI, 4.77–7.60) prior to excluding the ancestral principal components outliers, and 6.03 (95%CI, 4.74–7.70) after excluding these outliers.

Discussion

In this population-based study, we found the expected main effects of both polygenic liability for ADHD and of risk factors related to the psychosocial environment (maternal/paternal education, maternal/paternal work status, maternal/paternal income, and maternal/paternal history of mental disorder) upon the risk of ADHD. Furthermore, the associations with the PRS for ADHD and paternal education, paternal income, and maternal work status were largely independent with little indication of any gene–environment interaction.

That the PRS for ADHD is strongly associated with the development of ADHD in this sample is consistent with a substantial body of evidence suggesting that ADHD is among the most heritable conditions among the mental disorders1113. Also, it is in line with the recent GWAS, which identified the first genome-wide significant loci for ADHD15. Notably, the association between the PRS for ADHD and the risk for ADHD reported here remained almost unchanged when adjusting for the psychosocial risk factors, which included the maternal and paternal history of mental disorder. Thus, the PRS effects were not explained by parental psychopathology or socioeconomic factors, and furthermore, the impacts of the psychosocial factors were not mediated the genetic liability for ADHD. This suggests that manifest mental disorder in the parents is not a requirement for transmission of genetic risk for ADHD12,13.

Psychosocial adversity operationalized in a vast variety of ways has been associated with increased risk of ADHD in a large number of studies1721. The present study corroborates these findings using a relatively broad definition of psychosocial adversity, which likely taps into a “background” environment where some of the more specific insults that have been associated with subsequent development of ADHD—such as early severe deprivation55, maltreatment56, and inconsistent parenting24—are also more likely to occur.

In this study, we found no support for interaction between the polygenic liability for ADHD and the psychosocial environment upon the risk of ADHD. This is in contrast with results from prior studies that have investigated the role of gene–environment interaction in the etiology of ADHD15,2529. However, these prior studies have (i) tended to be based on small case-healthy control samples, (ii) used self-reported historical data on psychosocial environment, and (iii) focused on individual candidate genes (predominantly in the dopaminergic and serotonergic system) that were not replicated in the recent GWAS of ADHD15,2529. For these reasons, the results of the present study are most likely more internally valid than those from the prior studies on this topic.

The results of this study raise the question as to which specific genetic pathways are responsible for the observed dose-response association between the PRS for ADHD and development of ADHD. Based on the results from the recent GWAS of ADHD15, genes involved in synapse formation (FOXP2)57,58, neuroplasticity (SORCS3)59,60, and dopaminergic homeostasis (DUSP6)61,62 may be playing an important role. Determining whether this is indeed the case, will require larger GWAS of ADHD to allow for sufficient estimation of PRSs for specific genetic pathways.

There are limitations to this study, which must be taken into account by the reader. First, due to the biobank- and register-based nature of this study, we were only able to include individuals with ADHD who received a diagnosis of ADHD during inpatient or outpatient treatment at a psychiatric hospital in Denmark32. Hence, children who were diagnosed with ADHD outside psychiatric hospital settings, e.g. by private practicing psychiatrists or by pediatricians, do not appear as “cases” in this study (false negatives)—as these practitioners do not report diagnoses to the Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register, which provided the diagnostic data for this study. Therefore, our results might not generalize to patients with less severe ADHD than those diagnosed at psychiatric hospitals. On the other hand, an advantage of using register data is that the validity of the ADHD diagnoses in the register has been evaluated and found to be appropriate for research purposes63,64. Secondly, gene–environment correlation may threaten the internal validity of the results65. Specifically, the parents of the cohort members may create a family environment that is correlated with the cohort members’ genotype—an effect of so-called genetic nurture, in which environmental influences are misidentified as genetic66 In order to counteract this effect, we have adjusted the analyses involving the PRS for ADHD for the parents’ history of diagnosed mental disorder. However, since this adjustment only takes relatively severe psychopathology into account, which has led to treatment at psychiatric hospitals, this does not rule out the possibility that passive, or evocative67, gene–environment correlation may have led to an overestimation of the effect of gene–environment interactions on the risk of ADHD due to confounding65. Third and finally, it is broadly recognized that PRSs are not yet very informative at the level of the individual and therefore not clinically useful in psychiatry68,69. However, this may change with the results of future GWAS that will likely to explain a larger fraction of the polygenic liability for ADHD and other mental disorders.

In conclusion, based on a study of 13,725 individuals with ADHD and 20,147 population controls, we report strong and independent main effects of genetic liability and psychosocial adversity upon the risk of ADHD—in the expected direction. In contrast, we found no significant interactions between the polygenic liability and the psychosocial environment upon the risk of ADHD, and the genetic liability in individuals with ADHD was increased across all psychosocial factors. Future studies should address if specific genetic pathways and environmental factors are responsible for these results. This line of research may lead to identification of targets for both treatment and preventive measures.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Material (651KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

The study was funded by unrestricted research grants from the Lundbeck Foundation (R165-2013-15320, R102-A9118, R155-2014-1724 and R248-2017-2003). The Danish National Biobank resource is supported by the Novo Nordisk Foundation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41398-020-01019-6).

References

  • 1.Polanczyk G, de Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2007;164:942–948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Thomas R. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics (Evanst.). 2015;135:e994–e1001. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn. (American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC. 2013).
  • 4.Ostergaard SD, Dalsgaard S, Faraone SV, Munk-Olsen T, Laursen TM. Teenage parenthood and birth rates for individuals with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide cohort study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2017;56:578–584.e573. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dalsgaard S, Ostergaard SD, Leckman JF, Mortensen PB, Pedersen MG. Mortality in children, adolescents, and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a nationwide cohort study. Lancet. 2015;2:702–709. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00271-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dalsgaard S, Mortensen PB, Frydenberg M, Thomsen PH. Long-term criminal outcome of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Crim. Behav. Ment. Health.: Cbmh. 2013;23:86–98. doi: 10.1002/cbm.1860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Daley D, Jacobsen RH, Lange AM, Sorensen A, Walldorf J. The economic burden of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a sibling comparison cost analysis. Eur. Psychiatry.: J. Assoc. Eur. Psychiatrists. 2019;61:41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.06.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM, Sjolander A, Larsson H. Serious transport accidents in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effect of medication: a population-based study. JAMA psychiatry. 2014;71:319–325. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Harpin VA. The effect of ADHD on the life of an individual, their family, and community from preschool to adult life. Arch. Dis. Child. 2005;90:i2–i7. doi: 10.1136/adc.2004.059006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Roy, A. et al. Effects of Childhood and Adult Persistent Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder on Risk of Motor Vehicle Crashes: Results From the Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD (Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Baltimore, 2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 11.Faraone SV, Larsson H. Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Mol. Psychiatry. 2019;24:562–575. doi: 10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Freitag CM, Rohde LA, Lempp T, Romanos M. Phenotypic and measurement influences on heritability estimates in childhood ADHD. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;19:311–323. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0097-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bienvenu OJ, Davydow DS, Kendler KS. Psychiatric ‘diseases’ versus behavioral disorders and degree of genetic influence. Psychological Med. 2011;41:33–40. doi: 10.1017/S003329171000084X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Demontis D, et al. Whole-exome sequencing reveals increased burden of rare functional and disruptive variants in candidate risk genes in individuals with persistent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;55:521–523. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Demontis D, et al. Discovery of the first genome-wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:63–75. doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0269-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Satterstrom, F. K. et al. Autism Spectrum Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Have a Similar Burden of Rare Protein-truncating Variants. Vol 22, 1961–1965 (Nature Publishing Group, New York, NY, 2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 17.Ostergaard SD, et al. Predicting ADHD by assessment of rutter’s indicators of adversity in infancy. PloS ONE. 2016;11:e0157352. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Biederman J, Faraone SV, Monuteaux MC. Differential effect of environmental adversity by gender: Rutter’s index of adversity in a group of boys and girls with and without ADHD. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2002;159:1556–1562. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.9.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Biederman J, et al. Family-environment risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. A test of Rutter’s indicators of adversity. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1995;52:464–470. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950180050007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Biederman J, Petty CR, Clarke A, Lomedico A, Faraone SV. Predictors of persistent ADHD: an 11-year follow-up study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2011;45:150–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Thapar A, Cooper M, Eyre O, Langley K. What have we learnt about the causes of ADHD? J. child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013;54:3–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02611.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nigg J, Nikolas M, Burt SA. Measured gene-by-environment interaction in relation to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2010;49:863–873. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Thapar A, Cooper M, Jefferies R, Stergiakouli E. What causes attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Arch. Dis. Child. 2012;97:260–265. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2011-300482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Martel MM, et al. The dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) moderates family environmental effects on ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2011;39:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9439-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Laucht M, et al. Interacting effects of the dopamine transporter gene and psychosocial adversity on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms among 15-year-olds from a high-risk community sample. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2007;64:585–590. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Li JJ, Lee SS. Interaction of dopamine transporter (DAT1) genotype and maltreatment for ADHD: a latent class analysis. J. Child Psychol. psychiatry, allied Discip. 2012;53:997–1005. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02563.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kumsta R, et al. 5HTT genotype moderates the influence of early institutional deprivation on emotional problems in adolescence: evidence from the English and Romanian Adoptee (ERA) study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2010;51:755–762. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02249.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gould KL, Coventry WL, Olson RK, Byrne B. Gene-Environment Interactions in ADHD: The Roles of SES and Chaos. J. Abnorm. child Psychol. 2018;46:251–263. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0268-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nikolas, M., Friderici, K., Waldman, I., Jernigan, K. & Nigg, J. T. Gene x environment interactions for ADHD: synergistic effect of 5HTTLPR genotype and youth appraisals of inter-parental conflict. Behav. Brain Funct.6, 23 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 30.Schwartz S, Susser E. The use of well controls: an unhealthy practice in psychiatric research. Psychological Med. 2011;41:1127–1131. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Baldwin JR. Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:584. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pedersen CB, et al. The iPSYCH2012 case—cohort sample: new directions for unravelling genetic and environmental architectures of severe mental disorders. Mol. Psychiatry. 2017;23:6–14. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Mors O, Perto GP, Mortensen PB. The Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:54–57. doi: 10.1177/1403494810395825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Munk-Jorgensen P, Ostergaard SD. Register-based studies of mental disorders. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:170–174. doi: 10.1177/1403494810390728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Privé, F., Aschard, H., Ziyatdinov, A. & Blum, M. G. B. Efficient Analysis of Large-scale Genome-wide Data with Two R Packages: Bigstatsr and Bigsnpr. Vol 34, 2781–2787 (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 36.Maronna RA, Zamar RH. Robust estimates of location and dispersion for high-dimensional datasets. Technometrics. 2002;44:307–317. doi: 10.1198/004017002188618509. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wimberley T, et al. Genetic liability to ADHD and substance use disorders in individuals with ADHD. Addiction. 2020;115:1368–1377. doi: 10.1111/add.14910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Pedersen CB. The danish civil registration system. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:22–25. doi: 10.1177/1403494810387965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jensen VM, Rasmussen AW. Danish Education Registers. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:91–94. doi: 10.1177/1403494810394715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Petersson F, Baadsgaard M, Thygesen LC. Danish registers on personal labour market affiliation. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:95–98. doi: 10.1177/1403494811408483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Baadsgaard M, Quitzau J. Danish registers on personal income and transfer payments. Scand. J. Public Health. 2011;39:103–105. doi: 10.1177/1403494811405098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Norgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard DM. Storage policies and use of the Danish Newborn Screening Biobank. J. Inherit. Metab. Dis. 2007;30:530–536. doi: 10.1007/s10545-007-0631-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics C. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–427. doi: 10.1038/nature13595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Laursen TM, et al. Filicide in offspring of parents with severe psychiatric disorders: a population-based cohort study of child homicide. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2011;72:698–703. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05508gre. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gebara CF, et al. Psychosocial factors associated with mother-child violence: a household survey. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2017;52:77–86. doi: 10.1007/s00127-016-1298-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Elrud R, Friberg E, Alexanderson K, Stigson H. Sickness absence, disability pension and permanent medical impairment among 64 000 injured car occupants of working ages: a two-year prospective cohort study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2019;127:35–41. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2019.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Morgan, N., Christensen, K., Skedros, G., Kim, S. & Schliep, K. Life Stressors, Hypertensive Disorders of Pregnancy, and Preterm Birth. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynaecol.1-9 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 48.Webb RT, Pickles AR, Appleby L, Mortensen PB, Abel KM. Death by unnatural causes during childhood and early adulthood in offspring of psychiatric inpatients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2007;64:345–352. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.3.345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nilsson E, et al. Schizophrenia and offspring’s risk for adverse pregnancy outcomes and infant death. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2008;193:311–315. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.045146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Cubbin C, Smith GS. Socioeconomic inequalities in injury: critical issues in design and analysis. Annu Rev. Public Health. 2002;23:349–375. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Pedersen CB, et al. A comprehensive nationwide study of the incidence rate and lifetime risk for treated mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71:573–581. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Searle SR, Speed FM, Milliken GA. Population marginal means in the linear model: an alternative to least squares means. Am. Statistician. 1980;34:216–221. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Martin AR, et al. Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2017;100:635–649. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Martin AR, et al. Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nat. Genet. 2019;51:584–591. doi: 10.1038/s41588-019-0379-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stevens SE, et al. Inattention/overactivity following early severe institutional deprivation: presentation and associations in early adolescence. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008;36:385–398. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Capusan AJ, et al. Childhood maltreatment and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adults: a large twin study. Psychological Med. 2016;46:2637–2646. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tsui D. FoxP2 regulates neurogenesis during embryonic cortical development. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:244–258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1665-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Schreiweis C, et al. Humanized Foxp2 accelerates learning by enhancing transitions from declarative to procedural performance. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:14253–14258. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414542111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Breiderhoff T, et al. Sortilin-related receptor SORCS3 is a postsynaptic modulator of synaptic depression and fear extinction. PloS ONE. 2013;8:e75006. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Oetjen S, Mahlke C, Hermans-Borgmeyer I, Hermey G. Spatiotemporal expression analysis of the growth factor receptor SorCS3. J. Comp. Neurol. 2014;522:3386–3402. doi: 10.1002/cne.23606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Mortensen OV, Larsen MB, Prasad BM, Amara SG. Genetic complementation screen identifies a mitogen-activated protein kinase phosphatase, MKP3, as a regulator of dopamine transporter trafficking. Mol. Biol. cell. 2008;19:2818–2829. doi: 10.1091/mbc.e07-09-0980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Caunt CJ, Keyse SM, Dual-specificity MAP. kinase phosphatases (MKPs): shaping the outcome of MAP kinase signalling. FEBS J. 2013;280:489–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-4658.2012.08716.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Linnet KM, et al. Coffee consumption during pregnancy and the risk of hyperkinetic disorder and ADHD: a prospective cohort study. Acta Paediatrica. 2009;98:173–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00980.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Dalsgaard S, Hansen N, Mortensen PB, Damm D, Thomsen PH. Reassessment of ADHD in a historical cohort of children treated with stimulants in the period 1969-1989. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2001;10:230–239. doi: 10.1007/s007870170012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Jaffee SR, Price TS. Gene-environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Mol. psychiatry. 2007;12:432–442. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001950. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kong, A. et al. The Nature of Nurture: Effects of Parental Genotypes. Vol 359, 424–428 (New York, NY, 2018). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 67.Schoeler T, et al. Multi-polygenic score approach to identifying individual vulnerabilities associated with the risk of exposure to bullying. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:730–738. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0310. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Visscher PM, et al. 10 Years of GWAS discovery: biology, function, and translation. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2017;101:5–22. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Martin AR, Daly MJ, Robinson EB, Hyman SE, Neale BM. Predicting polygenic risk of psychiatric disorders. Biol. psychiatry. 2019;86:97–109. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Material (651KB, docx)

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES