Summary
Positive animal emotion (affect) is a key component of good animal welfare [1] and plays an important role in stress-coping and resilience [2]. Methods for reliably inducing and measuring positive affect are critical, but both have been limited in availability. In rats, one promising way of inducing positive affective states is by human-simulated rough and tumble play or ‘tickling’ [3,4]. However, in humans tickling induces both pleasure and displeasure, and neither an established non-verbal indicator of positive affect, the Duchenne smile, nor laughter detects this variation [5,6]. Rats also show individual differences in response to tickling [7], and this variation needs to be readily quantified if we are to ensure that tickling is only implemented where it generates positive affect. Here, we use a validated and objective measure of affective valence, the affective bias test [8], to show that 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations provide a quantifiable and graded measure of positive affect that accurately reflects the positive state induced by this human–rat interaction.
Hinchcliffe and colleagues use the affective bias test to show that rats vary in the level of positive affect induced by simulated social play (‘tickling’), and that 50 kHz call rates provide a graded real-time measure of tickling-induced positive affect.
Main Text
Reliable induction of positive affect in animals is critical if we are to successfully improve animal welfare [1] or generate model systems to investigate the putative benefits of such states [2]. In laboratory rats, ‘tickling’ is a widely advocated approach [3] (see: https://nc3rs.org.uk/news/tickling-rats-social-enrichment-improve-rodent-welfare). Tickling has beneficial effects but individuals vary in their response [7]. Blanket recommendations to implement tickling thus risk having unintended detrimental effects. This can be avoided if easy-to-use quantifiable and graded measures of positive affect allow accurate real-time monitoring of the response to tickling. Here, we ask whether, unlike Duchenne smiles and laughter in humans [5,6], 50 kHz rat ultrasonic vocalizations fulfil this role.
Answering this question requires a validated measure of affective valence that can quantify the strength of positive affect induced by tickling, thus providing a ground truth against which tickling-induced 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations can be gauged. Previous studies have been limited to measures of motivation such as approach to the tickler’s hand [4], or analysis of affective decision-making in rats who either do or don’t vocalize when tickled [9] but without investigating whether call rate reflects the strength of positive affect. Here we employ the affective bias test which has demonstrated the predicted affective valence for a wide range of pharmaceutical, hormonal, immune and environmental manipulations, yielding large effect sizes [8]. Moreover, dose-dependent bias data in drug studies indicate that it provides a highly sensitive, graded measure of the induced state [8].
Male Lister Hooded rats (n = 16)underwent an affective bias test, experiencing four independent training sessions (finding a food reward in a specific digging substrate), two after 30s tickling during which 50 kHz calls were measured (substrate A) and two under control conditions (substrate B), followed by a choice test (A vs B). A hand approach test [4] was carried out a week later (Supplemental Information).
Rats showed a positive choice bias for substrate A (one sample t-test against null hypothesis of 0: t15 = 4.753, p = 0.0003, Figure 1B), indicating that tickling conferred greater reward-value to the digging experience and hence generated a more positive state than the control procedure. Moreover, there was a strong positive correlation between the choice bias score and the mean number of 50 kHz calls emitted during tickling sessions (Pearson’s correlation, r = 0.8911, p < 0.0001, Figure 1C; 50 kHz calls during individual tickling sessions also correlated strongly with choice bias, Supplemental Information), whilst no significant correlation was found between approach latency to the experimenter’s hand and either the number of 50 kHz calls emitted (r = -0.4157, p = 0.1093, Figure 1D) or the affective bias test choice bias score (r = -0.4664, p = 0.0686; Figure 1E).
Our findings show that, at a population level, tickling induced a more positive affective state. However, rats varied in how strongly they preferred the tickling-associated substrate (Figure 1B), just as drug dosage affected preference strength in previous affective bias test studies [8], implying that tickling induces stronger positive affect in some individuals than others. Therefore, not all rats like to be tickled [7] and when employing tickling to enhance affect and welfare, care should be taken to identify these animals. To this end, we show that 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations provide a real-time indicator of (tickling-induced) positive affect and, importantly, that the rate of calling reflects how positively valenced the state is (Figure 1C). 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations could thus be used to monitor the affective state induced by tickling. Latency to approach the experimenter’s hand did not reflect the strength of positive affect induced by tickling (Figures 1D,E; Supplemental Information), suggesting that interaction with the handler alone is not related to affective state and/or that hand approach is not a particularly sensitive measure of positive affect.
Overall, our findings demonstrate that 50 kHz USVs provide an easy-to-use, graded, and real-time measure of positive affect in response to a short-term event (tickling). Our results support the use of tickling to induce positive affect and welfare in rats, whilst confirming that rats, like humans, vary in how rewarding they find it. They also indicate that 50 kHz vocalisations may not be as closely related to tickling-induced human laughter as previously suggested. Unlike human smiles and laughter [5,6], 50 kHz vocalisations directly reflect the animal’s affective experience when being tickled. We suggest that future studies should use the affective bias test to investigate whether this generalises to other contexts in which 50 kHz vocalisations have been recorded e.g. juvenile play, mating, aggression [3]. Because the affective bias test is sensitive to both positive and negative affective states [8], hence addressing a long-standing challenge in studies of animal affect, and especially positive emotion, of establishing a ground truth state against which methods for inducing and measuring affect can be validated, it could also be used to investigate whether rat 22 kHz alarm calls provide a similarly graded measure of negative affective state. Furthermore, it could be implemented in other species to determine whether, as in rats, vocalisations can be veridical signals of affective state or, as in humans and possibly other great apes [10], they also reflect the influence of other social factors.
Acknowledgements
We thank Matthew W. Jones (School of Physiology, Pharmacology and Neuroscience, University of Bristol) for providing access to ultrasonic vocalisation recording equipment and software to analyse the vocalisation results. We also thank two anonymous referees for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper. Funding for this research was supported by a University of Bristol Research Postgraduate Studentship awarded to J.K.H., and a UK Medical Research Council project grant (MR/L011212/1) awarded to E.S.J.R.
Author Contributions
J.K.H., E.S.J.R. and M.M. designed the study. J.K.H. conducted the experiment and collected and analysed data. J.K.H., M.M. and E.S.J.R. wrote the paper.
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Supplemental Information includes one figure, one data file and experimental procedures and can be found with this article online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.038. Data from this study are available online at: https://figshare.com/s/ab8f75ecfdeaea5153b0
Supplemental Information
References
- 1.Boissy A., Manteuffel G., Jensen M.B., Moe R.O., Spruijt B., Keeling L.J., Winckler C., Forkman B., Dimitrov I., Langbein J. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol. Behav. 2007;92:375–397. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kalisch R., Müller M.B., Tüscher O. A conceptual framework for the neurobiological study of resilience. Behav. Brain Sci. 2015;38:e92. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X1400082X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Burgdorf J., Kroes R.A., Moskal J.R., Pfaus J.G., Brudzynski S.M., Panksepp J. Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play, and aggression: behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback. J. Comp. Psychol. 2008;122:357–367. doi: 10.1037/a0012889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Burgdorf J., Panksepp J. Tickling induces reward in adolescent rats. Physiol. Behav. 2001;72:167–173. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00411-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Harris C.R., Alvarado N. Facial expressions, smile types and self-reporting during humour, tickle and pain. Cogn. Emot. 2005;9:655–669. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Harris C.R., Christenfeld N. Humour, tickle, and the Darwin-Hecker hypothesis. Cogn. Emot. 1997;11:103–110. [Google Scholar]
- 7.LaFollette M.R., O’Haire M.E., Cloutier S., Blankenberger W.B., Gaskill B.N. Rat tickling: a systematic review of applications, outcomes, and moderators. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0175320. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hinchcliffe J.K., Stuart S.A., Mendl M., Robinson E.S.J. Further validation of the affective bias test for predicting antidepressant and pro-depressant risk: effects of pharmacological and social manipulations in male and female rats. Psychopharmacol. 2017;234:3105–3116. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4687-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rygula R., Pluta H., Popik P. Laughing rats are optimistic. PLoS One. 2012;7:e51959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ross M.D., Owren M.J., Zimmermann E. Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Curr. Biol. 2010;19:1106–1111. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.