Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Oct 6.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2019 Aug;25(7):740–749. doi: 10.1017/S1355617719000444

Influence of Methylphenidate on Long-Term Neuropsychological and Everyday Executive Functioning After Traumatic Brain Injury in Children with Secondary Attention Problems

Elizabeth LeBlond 1,2, Julia Smith-Paine 1,2, Jacqlyn J Riemersma 3, Paul S Horn 3,4,5, Shari L Wade 1,2,3, Brad G Kurowski 2,3,6,7
PMCID: PMC7536786  NIHMSID: NIHMS1627697  PMID: 31178001

Abstract

Objective:

To investigate the effects of methylphenidate on long-term executive and neuropsychological functioning in children with attention problems following TBI, as well as the relationship between methylphenidate associated changes in lab-based neuropsychological measures of attentional control, processing speed, and executive functioning and parent- or self-report measures of everyday executive functioning.

Method:

26 children aged 6–17 years, who were hospitalized for moderate-to-severe blunt head trauma 6 or more months previously, were recruited from a large children’s hospital medical center. Participants were randomized into a double-masked, placebo-controlled cross-over clinical trial. Participants completed a comprehensive neuropsychological battery and parent- and self-report ratings of everyday executive functioning at baseline, and at 4 weeks and 8 weeks following upward titration of medication to an optimal dose or while administered a placebo.

Results:

Methylphenidate was associated with significant improvements in processing speed, sustained attention, and both lab-based and everyday executive functioning. Significant treatment-by-period interactions were found on a task of sustained attention. Participants who were randomized to the methylphenidate condition for the first treatment period demonstrated random or erratic responding, with slower and more variable response times when given placebo during the second period.

Conclusion:

Results indicate that methylphenidate treatment is associated with positive outcomes in processing speed, sustained attention, and both lab-based and everyday measures of executive functioning compared to placebo group. Additionally, results suggest sustained attention worsens when discontinuing medication.

Keywords: Attentional control, processing speed, cross-over, reaction time, pediatric, ADHD

INTRODUCTION

Traumatic brain injury (TBI) is one of the most common causes of acquired morbidity and mortality in children (Taylor et al., 2017). Because these injuries occur at an early age, often before certain skills have emerged, they can result in residual deficits as the individual continues to develop (Anderson et al., 2009; Babikian et al., 2015), including psychiatric and neurobehavioral problems (Bryant et al., 2010; Dennis et al., 1995; Fenwick & Anderson, 1999; Massagli et al., 2004). Attention problems are prevalent following injury and can negatively impact school functioning, social relationships, emotional well-being, self-esteem, and quality of life (Anderson et al., 2009; Catroppa et al., 2007; Kaufmann et al., 1993). Many children with no premorbid diagnosis of ADHD meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD post-TBI. This condition, referred to as secondary ADHD (SADHD), is one of the most common secondary diagnoses in children after TBI (Bloom et al., 2001) with approximately 16% of individuals meeting diagnostic criteria 6 months post-injury (Yeates et al., 2005), significantly higher than the estimated prevalence of primary ADHD in the United States of 7.8% (Bonfield, Lam, Lin, & Greene, 2013).

The use of stimulant medications in children with primary ADHD has been extensively studied, but there is a paucity of studies evaluating the use of stimulants in children with attention problems after brain injury (Pangilinan, Giacoletti-Argento, Shellhaas, Hurvitz, & Hornyak, 2010). Previous studies and clinical trials evaluating the use of stimulants for attention problems in children who sustained TBI have yielded inconsistent results regarding efficacy (Backeljauw & Kurowski, 2014; Dougall, Poole, & Agrawal, 2015; Warden et al., 2006). Research in a variety of neurological conditions (e.g., spina bifida, epilepsy, and among survivors of pediatric brain tumor) has revealed similar attention deficits, as well as mixed findings regarding efficacy of stimulant treatments (D’Agati, Cerminara, Casarelli, Pitzianti, & Curatolo, 2012; De la Torre, Martin, Cervantes, Guil, & Mestre, 2017; Mulhern, Merchant, Gajjar, Reddick, & Kun, 2004; Parisi, Moavero, Verrotti, & Curatolo, 2010; Wasserman, Stoner, Stem, & Holmbeck, 2016). Research investigating the use of stimulant medication in primary ADHD has found extensive evidence that long-term treatment with methylphenidate significantly improves attention-related behaviors (Abikoff et al., 2004) (Shaw et al., 2012). Side effects, including neurological (e.g., headache and dizziness), psychiatric (e.g., depressed or elevated mood), tic disorders, psychotic symptoms (e.g., hallucination), cardiovascular (e.g., blood pressure and pulse changes, racing heart), gastrointestinal (e.g., loss of appetite, weight loss during prolonged treatment), and dermatological (e.g., hives) events, are typically not disruptive to the child’s overall functioning (Graham et al., 2011) and are potentially controlled by so-called “drug holidays” of interrupted use (Graham et al., 2011).

Executive dysfunction is one of the most common neurocognitive sequelae after pediatric TBI (Horton, Soper, & Reynolds, 2010). Executive function (EF) has been broadly defined as a superordinate, managerial capacity for directing more modular abilities (e.g., language, memory, psychomotor skills, and perception) with the objective of setting, managing, or attaining goals (Levin & Hanten, 2005). Deficits in EF differ between children who experience SADHD, those who have pre-injury ADHD, and those who sustain a TBI without SADHD. Particularly, children with SADHD demonstrate greater impairments in attention and working memory compared to those with pre-injury ADHD and TBI-only; these children also showed impaired psychomotor speed at both 6- and 12-month follow-ups, while attention deficits emerged at 12 months, suggesting that deficits in attention are not simply due to slowed cognition (Ornstein et al., 2014). SADHD is associated with slower response times and poorer planning abilities than those with primary ADHD. These findings indicate different underlying mechanisms between pre-injury ADHD and SADHD and suggest that different treatments may be necessary for SADHD (Ornstein et al., 2014). While it may be possible to differentiate between primary and secondary ADHD with neuropsychological testing, studies have found that children with both pre- and post-injury ADHD have increased difficulties with attention, EF (e.g., reaction time, response inhibition) and memory following TBI (De la Torre et al., 2017; Sinopoli & Dennis, 2012; Sinopoli, Schachar, & Dennis, 2011; Slomine et al., 2005). Executive dysfunction may persist up to 10 years post-injury (Muscara, Catroppa, & Anderson, 2008) and has been implicated as a factor in negative academic and social outcomes in children after TBI (Biederman et al., 2004; Yeates et al., 2004).

The primary goal of this study was to investigate the effects of methylphenidate on long-term executive and neuropsychological functioning in children with attention problems following TBI. Children with ADHD following TBI (either SADHD or primary ADHD plus TBI) have greater problems in these areas compared to those with primary ADHD alone or TBI without SADHD (Ornstein et al., 2014); therefore, it is critical to understand how treatment with stimulants influences EF, which has not previously been examined in this population. Additionally, we wanted to understand the relationship between methylphenidate associated changes in lab-based neuropsychological measures of attentional control, processing speed, and EF and parent- or self-report measures of everyday EF behaviors (i.e., in a real-world setting rather than laboratory-based measures obtained in a controlled environment) (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000) in this population. We hypothesized that methylphenidate will improve everyday EF and that improvements on these dimensions will be associated with lab-based measures of attentional control, processing speed, and EF. Specifically, we hypothesized that improvements in these measures will mediate improvements in everyday EF behaviors.

METHOD

Design

The design was a randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled cross-over clinical trial. An in-depth description of the intervention and efficacy of methylphenidate for the management of attention problems, based upon parent- and self-report of attention rating scales, after pediatric TBI is reported in a separate paper (Kurowski et al., 2018). Following a screening interview and completion of informed consent and assent, participants completed a baseline visit and were randomized to the experimental (methylphenidate) or the control (placebo) condition for 4 weeks (period 1). At the end of 4 weeks, participants immediately crossed over to the other condition for another 4 weeks (period 2). Participants were assessed at baseline and weeks 4 and 8 to examine differences in outcomes between the medication and placebo conditions. The total duration of the trial originally planned for 8 weeks, but took up to 12 weeks for some participants due to scheduling issues, with a total of three full assessment visits (i.e., baseline and optimal visits) and six titration visits (Kurowski et al., 2018). Participants weighing less than 25 kg received 18 mg (low), 27 mg (medium), and 36 mg (high) dosages; participants weighing above 25 kg received 18 mg (low), 36 mg (medium), and 54 mg (high) dosages during the trial. During the placebo condition, participants underwent the same procedures as during the medication condition due to the masked nature of the study. The mean optimal dose of methylphenidate was 40.5 mg (1.00 mg/kg/day) (Kurowski et al., 2018). Testers were masked to the medication status of the child. Participants were instructed to take their medication at a consistent time in the morning; average time of medication administration was 8:22 a.m. Study visits typically occurred in afternoons and evenings; however, there may have been variation depending on the schedules of participants and their families. Further details of the trial procedures and design can be found in a previously published paper (Kurowski et al., 2018).

Participants and Recruitment

Children aged 6–17 years, who were hospitalized for a moderate-to-severe blunt head trauma 6 or more months previously, were recruited from a large children’s hospital medical center. Six years of age was chosen as the lower age limit based on clinical practice guidelines, which recommend the use of stimulants as first-line treatment for ADHD at 6 years and older (Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, 2011). To study the efficacy of methylphenidate following pediatric TBI specifically, the upper age range of 17 years was chosen. Consistent with previous investigations, TBI severity was categorized by the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974) score and neuroimaging (Kurowski et al., 2018). The Vanderbilt ADHD parent rating scales (Wolraich, Feurer, Hannah, & Baumgaertel, 2003) were used to determine eligibility based on a parent report of at least six current symptoms on the inattentive and/or hyperactive subscales. Participants actively receiving medications for attention problems and or other attention related treatments (e.g., attention training) were eligible if treatments were discontinued prior to study enrollment (Kurowski et al., 2018).

Three hundred and twenty-one participants were assessed for the basic criteria of age, diagnoses of a TBI, and time since injury. Of these, 118 declined to participate. Twenty did not want to start or change medications, 26 were not having attention difficulties per their parent and were not screened, 3 did not want to burden their child, 19 declined due to the time commitment, 7 declined due to the distance to the hospital, 4 were not interested in this study, 23 declined all research, and 16 did not give a reason for refusal. One hundred and sixty-three potential participants were excluded from the study. Eighty-eight of these turned 18 prior to contact, 6 turned 18 after initial contact was made, 54 were ineligible due to Vanderbilt scores, 3 were ineligible due to neurological impairments, 2 were excluded due to pre-injury intellectual disability, and two were excluded due not meeting injury severity criteria.

Demographics and baseline assessments

Twenty-six individuals completed baseline assessments and were randomized. Of those randomized, the mean age at injury and baseline visit were 6.3 (SD: 4.1) and 11.5 (SD: 2.8) years, respectively. Mean GCS was 11.9 (SD: 4.2), and 25 (92.6%) of participants had evidence positive neuroimaging findings consistent with brain trauma. Six participants were female and 19 were white. See Table 1 for additional demographics. A consort flow diagram is provided in more detail elsewhere (Kurowski et al., 2018). Mechanisms of injury for the 20 participants who completed the study were falls (n = 6), bicycle accidents (n = 3), motor vehicle accidents (n = 3), sledding accidents (n = 3), other accidents (e.g., assault, running into another child, accidental hit by a baseball bat, n = 3), and being struck by a car (n = 2). Mechanisms of injury for the six participants who did not complete the study were falls (n = 2), motor vehicle accidents (n = 2), being struck by a car (n = 1), and abusive head trauma (n = 1).

Table 1.

Demographics

Intent to treat
Completers
Randomized
(n = 26)
Non-completer
(n = 6)
Completer
(n = 20)
p-
value
Medication first
(n = 10)
Placebo first
(n = 10)
p-
value
Demographics
Female, n (%) 6 (23.1) 1 (16.7) 5 (25.0) 1.0 1 (10.0) 4 (0.0) .30
Caucasian, n (%) 19 (73.1) 3 (50.0) 16 (80.0) .29 8 (80.0) 8 (80.0) 1.0
Household income 9 (34.6) 2 (33.3) 7 (35.0) 1.0 4 (40.0) 3 (30.0) 1.0
 ≥$70,000, n (%)
Mother graduate college, n (%) 8 (30.8) 2 (33.3) 6 (30.0) 1.0 4 (40.0) 2 (20.0) .63
Abnormal CT, n (%) 25 (96.2) 6 (100) 19 (95.0) 1.0 9 (90.0) 10 (100) 1.0
Severe TBI, n (%) 8 (30.8) 2 (33.3) 6 (30.0) 1.0 4 (40.0) 2 (20.0) .63
GCS, M (SD) 11.9 (4.2) 10.7 (4.6) 12.4 (4.1) .45 11.6 (4.1) 13.2 (4.2) .42
Age at injury, M (SD) 6.3 (4.1) 6.9 (3.8) 6.1 (4.3) .68 5.7 (5.2) 6.5 (3.5) .70
Time since injury, M (SD) 5.2 (3.8) 5.2 (4.2) 5.2 (3.8) 1.0 5.2 (4.2) 5.3 (3.7) .97
Age at baseline, M (SD) 11.5 (2.8) 12.1 (2.7) 11.3 (2.8) .57 10.9 (2.9) 11.7 (2.7) .52

To determine ADHD diagnostic status at the time of enrollment, the ADHD portion of the K-SADS-P/L (Kaufman et al., 1997) was administered at baseline. At the time of enrollment, 6 participants had combined presentation ADHD, 18 had predominantly inattentive, and 2 had predominantly hyperactive-type ADHD; ADHD subtype did not differ between those with primary versus secondary ADHD. There were no demographic or baseline assessment differences between groups that received medication versus placebo first.

Measures

The executive and neuropsychological outcomes were obtained at baseline and at each optimal dose visit (week 4 and week 8) for a total of three assessments (Kurowski et al., 2018).

The Behavior Rating Interview of Executive Function (BRIEF) is standardized and validated for use in children ages 5–18 years, including those with pediatric TBI (Gioia & Isquith, 2004; Gioia et al., 2000). Both parent- and self-report (age 11 and older) were used to obtain ratings of everyday EF behaviors. The global executive composite (GEC), behavior regulatory index (BRI), and metacognitive index (MI) T-scores were used in the analyses, with higher scores reflecting poorer EF.

The Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II) was used to assess various aspects of attentional control, including sustained and selective attention/response accuracy, processing speed, reaction time, and inhibitory control (Conners, 2004; Conners, 1995,2000). The CPT II is recommended as a supplemental pediatric TBI Common Data Elements outcome measure (McCauley et al., 2012) has been used previously to assess attentional control after severe TBI in children age 6–18 years (Galbiati et al., 2009) and is relatively unaffected by practice effects (Conners, 2004; Conners, 1995, 2000). The following scores were included in the current analyses: accuracy, coefficient of variation, mean response time, and standard deviation (SD) of response time. Higher accuracy scores reflect better performance; whereas higher scores on the other three measures reflect slower and more variable responses, suggesting difficulties in attention regulation.

The D-KEFS Verbal Fluency (D-KEFS VF) (Delis, Kaplan, & Kaplan, 2001) measures both lexical ability (e.g., letter fluency, category fluency) and executive functioning (e.g., category switching accuracy and total number if correct switching). The D-KEFS is sensitive to TBI severity (Strong, Tiesma, & Donders, 2011) and focal left frontal lesions (Levin, Song, Ewing-Cobbs, Chapman, & Mendelsohn, 2001). Participants were asked to verbalize words beginning with a designated letter according to specific rules (letter fluency), verbalize exemplars of specific categories (category fluency), and verbalize exemplars of semantic category switches (category switching accuracy and total correct), thus increasing the demand on EF. The D-KEFS Trail Making subtest assesses the ability to shift sets, specifically the Number–Letter Sequencing subtest assesses how quickly individuals switch between connecting numbers and letters (number–letter sequencing). Higher scores on the D-KEFS subtests reflect better function.

The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th Edition Processing Speed Index (WISC-IV-PSI) has been designed for children 6–16:11 years of age and provides a measure of processing speed and sustained attention (Wechsler, 2003). It is comprised of two subtests: Coding and Symbol Search. The information processing indexes are highly sensitive to TBI and its severity (Allen, Thaler, Donohue, & Mayfield, 2010; Donders & Janke, 2008; McCauley et al., 2012), with higher scores reflecting better processing speed. One child who was 17 was administered the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th Edition Processing Speed Index (WAIS-IV- PSI; (Wechsler, Coalson, & Raiford, 2008). Because both measures yield highly correlated standard scores (Wechsler, 2003), their score was included with the others in a combined WISC/WAIS processing speed variable.

Analysis

Descriptive statistics were used to characterize the sample. Comparisons between groups were assessed with t tests and Fisher’s Exact tests when appropriate. Repeated measures linear mixed models (Cnaan, Laird, & Slasor, 1997) were used to determine the association of methylphenidate with dependent variables. The dependent variables included the BRIEF (MI, BRI, and GEC), WISC/WAIS PSI (Coding and Symbol Search subtests), the CPT (accuracy, coefficient of variation, mean response time, and SD of response time), and the DKEFS (Trail Making Number–Letter Switching subtest and Verbal Fluency subtests). Other independent variables were the period of the evaluation (week 4 or week 8) and the interaction between period and treatment (this interaction term captures the potential carry over effect). Separate univariate regression analyses were performed to assess potential covariates, including age, sex, time since injury, injury severity (GCS score), race, and socioeconomic status (as measured by maternal education, college graduate or not). Injury factors and socioeconomic status were also considered as covariates because they predict the development of SADHD within the first 24 months after pediatric TBI (Max et al., 2004; Max et al., 2005; Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, 2011) prior to constructing multivariate models, each potential covariate was examined individually with treatment group and period, as well as the baseline value of the measure of interest in order to determine its relationship with the dependent variable (in the presence of group and period effects). Covariates that had a p-value below .05 in this analysis were to be included in the final model; however, no potential covariates significantly contributed to the models. Given the potential profile differences in children with primary versus secondary ADHD (Slomine et al., 2005), diagnosis type was also explored as a covariate in the models, but it was not found to have a significant effect. Therefore, we did not differentiate between diagnosis type in the current analyses. All analyses were based on intention to treat principles (Gupta, 2011). Effect sizes were derived based on least squares (adjusted) mean differences divided by an adjusted estimate of the SD: the standard error of the difference multiplied by the square root of the adjusted degrees of freedom (Lakens, 2013). The mediation effects of processing speed (WISC/WAIS PSI measures) were examined by fitting a regression model (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002; Mackinnon, Warsi, & Dwyer, 1995) of processing speed and treatment effect on responsiveness of parent-reported everyday EF behaviors. The coefficients of the treatment effect in the model were compared and we estimated the effect and the related standard error of the mediation effect. The direct and indirect effects were derived using software that was written specifically for two-treatment two-period crossover designs, as we have in this study (MEMORE for SAS version 1.1, Copyright 2016, AK Montoya and AF Hayes). All analyses were conducted using the SAS ® statistical software package version 9.3 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Statistical significance was defined as a p-value of .05. Magnitude of effect sizes are reported to assist with interpretation. Due to the exploratory nature of this medication trial, we did not apply multiple testing corrections.

RESULTS

Neuropsychological Outcomes

Methylphenidate was associated with significant improvements in processing speed, sustained attention, and EF. WISC/WAIS PSI scores during the optimal dose week of the medication condition were lower than during the optimal dose week of the placebo condition (p = .02, effect size = −0.60; Table 2), driven by the Coding subtest (p = .02, effect size = −0.58; Table 2). No significant differences were found on the Symbol Search subtest (p = .15; Table 2), suggesting that the overall effect was due to improved psychomotor speed rather than visual scanning ability. Similar results were seen on the CPT. The medication condition showed less response variability (Coefficient of Variation; p = .04, effect size = 0.62; Table 2), faster reaction times (p = .02, effect size = 0.44; Table 2), and a lower SD in reaction time (p = .02, effect size 0.43; Table 2).

Table 2.

Neuropsychological outcomes

Placebo, M (SE) Methylphenidate, M (SE) t (df) d
WAIS PSI
 PSI overall (standard score) 91.25 (2.24) 96.05 (2.24) −2.55 (18.0)* −0.60
 Coding overall (scaled) 7.55 (0.48) 8.60 (0.48) −2.47 (18.0)* −0.58
 Symbol search overall (scaled) 9.30 (0.56) 9.95 (0.56) −1.49 (18) −0.35
CPT II
 Coefficient of variation – overall 42.21 (3.18) 32.76 (3.07) 2.29 (13.8)* 0.62
 Coefficient of variation – period 1 34.28 (4.20) 35.00 (4.20) −0.12 (30.7) −0.02
 Coefficient of variation – period 2 50.15 (4.78) 30.53 (4.46) 3.00 (31.4)* 0.53
 Mean RT – overall 480.15 (14.16) 430.54 (13.72) 2.51 (32.0)* 0.44
 Mean RT – period 1 463.08 (19.15) 424.31 (19.36) 1.40 (32.0) 0.25
 Mean RT – period 2 497.22 (21.15) 436.78 (20.06) 2.06 (32.0)* 0.36
 SD RT – overall 216.61 (20.92) 145.90 (20.25) 2.42 (32.0)* 0.43
 SD RT – period 1 167.30 (27.84) 155.01 (28.03) 0.31 (32.0) 0.05
 SD RT – period 2 265.93 (31.14) 136.79 (29.32) 3.02 (32.0)** 0.53
D–KEFS VF (scaled score)
 Switch accuracy – overall 7.87 (0.48) 8.24 (0.48) −0.56 (17.0) −0.14
 Switch accuracy – period 1 8.30 (0.66) 6.78 (0.70) 1.58 (32.7) 0.28
 Switch accuracy – period 2 7.44 (0.70) 9.70 (0.66) −2.35 (17.0) −0.41
 Correct switches – overall 8.00 (0.61) 8.82 (0.61) −1.25 (17.0) −0.30
 Correct switches – period 1 8.96 (0.84) 7.38 (0.89) 1.28 (27.1) 0.25
 Correct switches – period 2 7.04 (0.89) 10.26 (0.84) −2.61 (27.1) −0.50
*

p< .05

**

p< .01

***

p< .0001, CPT II = Continuous Performance Test II, RT = Reaction Time, STD RT = Standard Deviation of Reaction Time, DKEFS-VF = D-KEFS Verbal Fluency, PSI = Processing Speed Index, and WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.

Note: “period 1” refers to the first 4 weeks of the study, and “period 2” refers to the final 4 weeks after cross-over.

Three significant treatment-by-period interactions were found on the CPT. When the methylphenidate condition was first, the placebo condition in the second period was associated with higher response variability (coefficient of variation; p = .01, effect size = 0.53; Table 2), slower response times (p = .05, effect size = 0.36; Table 2), and higher SD of response time (p = .005; effect size = 0.53; Table 2). These findings suggest that, after switching from methylphenidate to placebo, participants were slower to respond with more variation in response times, and responded randomly or erratically compared to those treated first by placebo.

While no overall differences were found on the DKEFS measures, there were two significant treatment-by-period interactions. Compared to placebo, the methylphenidate condition in the second period was associated with higher scores on category switch accuracy (p = .03, effect size = −0.30; Table 2) and category switch total correct (p = .01, effect size = −0.50; Table 2) indicating better ability to switch between sets.

Everyday EF Behavior Outcomes

There were overall differences in mean scores between placebo and medication groups at optimal dose visits for the parent-BRIEF GEC (p = .001, effect size = 0.97), MI (p = .03, effect size = 0.57), and BRI scores (p = .001, effect size = 0.89). Three significant treatment-by-period interactions were found on the parent-BRIEF. When the methylphenidate condition was first, the placebo condition in the second period was associated with higher scores on the GEC (p = .01, effect size = 0.55), the MI (p = .04, effect size = 0.38), and the BRI (p = .02, effect size = 0.48), indicating poorer everyday EF behaviors. Overall differences were found on the self-BRIEF GEC and MI scores after controlling for baseline scores, period (i.e., first or second 4 weeks), and interaction term of treatment by period (which was not statistically significant in these cases) (Table 3), with the methylphenidate condition associated with improved EF.

Table 3.

BRIEF outcomes

Placebo, M (SE) Methylphenidate, M (SE) t (df) d
BRIEF parent report (T score)
65.20 (1.30) 60.05 (1.30) 4.10 (18)*** 0.97
1 64.49 (1.84) 61.61 (1.84) 1.10 (26.5) 0.21
2 65.91 (1.84) 58.49 (1.84) 2.85 (26.5)** 0.55
 BRI – overall 58.00 (1.33) 54.30 (1.33) 2.44 (18)* 0.57
57.29 (1.88) 55.51 (1.88) 0.67 (30.6) 0.12
58.71 (1.88) 53.9 (1.88) 2.11 (30.6)* 0.38
67.75 (1.52) 62.40 (1.52) 3.79 (18)** 0.89
67.18 (2.15) 63.82 (2.15) 1.11 (25.8) 0.22
68.32 (2.15) 60.98 (2.15) 2.42 (25.8)* 0.48
BRIEF self report (T score)
 GEC – overall 46.03 (3.53) 41.10 (3.53) 2.71 (7.0)* 1.02
 BRI – overall 44.85 (3.31) 41.33 (3.31) 2.10 (7.0) 0.79
 MI – overall 47.46 (3.52) 41.81 (3.52) 3.09 (7.0)* 1.17
*

p< .05

**

p< .01

***

p< .0001, BRI = Behavioral regulation index, GEC = Global executive composite, and MI = Metacognitive index. Individual period results are not given for the self report measures because the treatment-by-period interaction terms were not statistically significant.

Note: “period 1” refers to the first 4 weeks of the study, and “period 2” refers to the final 4 weeks after cross-over.

Mediation outcomes

No significant mediation effects were found in the models exploring processing speed as a mediator of the treatment response of parent- or self-report ratings of everyday EF behaviors to methylphenidate (all p >.05).

DISCUSSION

This study provides a further understanding of the potential influence of methylphenidate on EF and neuropsychological outcomes in children with attention problems after TBI. The results suggest that methylphenidate treatment is beneficial for processing speed, sustained attention, lab-based measures of EF, and everyday EF behaviors. These results also suggest that when switching off methylphenidate, performance on sustained attention tasks may worsen, leading to slower reactions, more variable reaction times, and more random and erratic responses.

Previous studies of methylphenidate treatment for attentional impairments following TBI have revealed similar results. Many studies have found that methylphenidate treatment enhances processing speed in both adult and pediatric TBI populations (Willmott & Ponsford, 2009). Similar results have been found on measures of attention in both adult and pediatric TBI populations (Kim et al., 2012; Konrad, Gunther, Hanisch, & Herpertz-Dahlmann, 2004; Mahalick et al., 1998). The results of the current study add to the literature by demonstrating the efficacy of using dose titration to determine an optimal dose. Previous studies have utilized only one (Kim et al., 2012; Mahalick et al., 1998; Willmott & Ponsford, 2009) or two (Konrad et al., 2004) weight-dependent doses of methylphenidate. As assessments were administered while participants were taking their optimal dose, these results may be more representative of the performance of individuals being treated with methylphenidate in the long term.

A unique finding from this study is that it suggests there may be a negative cognitive “rebound effect” following discontinuation of or a “drug holiday” from methylphenidate. An international literature review revealed that drug holidays were a common practice with 25%–70% of families partaking in one (Ibrahim & Donyai, 2015). These holidays were more likely to coincide during weekends or school holidays and were thought to help alleviate or mitigate common negative side effects of methylphenidate, such as those affecting growth, sleep, and appetite; however, their effectiveness remains poorly understood (Graham et al., 2011). This decline in performance suggests that the estimated 40% of children who do not take medications when they are not in school (Charach & Gajaria, 2008) may be at risk for increased impaired attention until treatment is resumed. Furthermore, this study suggests there may be negative cognitive effects that should be considered when families and doctors decide to discontinue medication or take a drug holiday.

In addition, because mediation models were not significant, this suggests that improvements in processing speed performance are not the driving force in reported improvements in everyday EF behaviors. This finding is consistent with other literature in the ADHD population, which suggests performance-based measures account for little unique variance in predicting ADHD status and demonstrates little overlap with behavioral ratings (Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2008). A better understanding of neural mechanisms of recovery and treatment is needed to better understand improvements following medication trials. Future research should consider including neuroimaging and other biological markers as part of an assessment for treatment outcomes (Berridge et al., 2006; Hart et al., 2013; Solanto, 1998; Swanson, Baler, & Volkow, 2011; Volkow, Fowler, Wang, Ding, & Gatley, 2002).

Another promising direction for future research is combining medication and behavioral treatments for children following a TBI. Several studies have demonstrated that interventions targeting parenting skills, family distress, and problem-solving abilities post-injury can improve cognitive and behavioral outcomes for children and families after TBI (Antonini et al., 2014; Wade, Kurowski, et al., 2015; Wade, Taylor, et al., 2015). Research from the ADHD population suggests that best practice for treating symptoms of ADHD incorporates both medical and behavioral interventions subcommittee (Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, 2011). Future studies should explore whether a similar approach for children with TBI and SADHD would be beneficial.

Limitations

This study adds to the prior literature on TBI treatment of EF and attentional issues with methylphenidate, but has several limitations to consider when interpreting the findings. Due to the cross-over design of the study, children were given the same neuropsychological measures three times within an 8- to 12-week period. However, the pattern of performance in the current study did not suggest that improvements in scores were due to practice or learning effects because improvement across time, regardless of group, was absent. In addition, the finding that performance on assessments worsened after switching from methylphenidate to placebo would be unexpected if significant practice/leaming effects were present. The study does not account for teacher reports of behavior, which may differ from parent report (Lavigne, Dulcan, LeBailly, & Binns, 2012). Study participants included children with attention problems after injury; however, these were a mix of children with and without primary ADHD. Prior research suggests that there may be different profiles for each group; however, when pre-injury ADHD status was included as a covariate in the models, we did not find a significant influence on findings. Larger sample sizes are likely needed to explore profile differences between primary and secondary ADHD and between differing subtypes of ADHD. The study demographics should also be considered in interpreting the results as the population consisted of approximately 75% males, 75% whites, and primarily moderate injuries with a mean age of injury of approximately 6 years and time since injury at enrollment of approximately 5 years. Finally, further research is needed to understand how individual (e.g., genetics) and environmental factors (e.g., home environment, parenting) interrelate to influence treatment response and determine who is most likely to benefit from medication versus other treatments.

CONCLUSION

Findings from this double-masked, placebo controlled, upward titration, cross-over clinical trial support the use of long-acting methylphenidate for management of neuropsychological and everyday EF behavior problems long-term after pediatric brain injury. There is currently a lack of evidence-based trials for management of the sequelae of pediatric brain injury, as well as possible interventions to help improve common EF deficits following injury. Due to the high incidence of brain injury in children and the significant impact of executive problems, especially in children with SADHD, on functioning, larger trials of stimulant medications are warranted. Further research is also needed to explore “cognitive rebound effects” and how these may impact doctors’ and families’ decisions around taking medication holidays. The relationship between improvements of neuropsychological and everyday EF behavior assessments following medication treatment is currently poorly understood and further research is warranted to understand the relationship. Furthermore, the neural physiology of EF problems and neural mechanisms of treatments need to be better characterized in childhood brain injury populations.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (NICHD) grant (grant number REDACTED FOR BLIND REVIEW) and, in part, by the Cincinnati Children’s Research Foundation; the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award (grant number REDACTED FOR BLIND REVIEW); and the National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research, formerly known as the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research (grant number REDACTED FOR BLIND REVIEW). This study is registered with clinicaltrials.gov, registration number REDACTED FOR BLIND REVIEW. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or other supporting agencies. No competing financial interests exist. All authors of this manuscript have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES

  1. Abikoff H, Hechtman L, Klein RG, Weiss G, Fleiss K, Etcovitch J, Cousins L, Greenfield B, Martin D, & Pollack S (2004). Symptomatic improvement in children with ADHD treated with long-term methylphenidate and multimodal psychosocial treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(7), 802–811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen DN, Thaler NS, Donohue B, & Mayfield J (2010). WISC-IV profiles in children with traumatic brain injury: similarities to and differences from the WISC-III. Psychological Assessment, 22(1), 57–64. doi: 10.1037/a0016056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson V, Brown S, Newitt H, & Hoile H (2009). Educational, vocational, psychosocial, and quality-of-life outcomes for adult survivors of childhood traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 24(5), 303–312. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181ada830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Antonini TN, Raj SP, Obeqohn KS, Cassedy A, Makoroff KL, Fouladi M, & Wade SL (2014). A pilot randomized trial of an online parenting skills program for pediatric traumatic brain injury: improvements in parenting and child behavior. Behavior Therapy, 45(4), 455–168. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Babikian T, Merkley T, Savage RC, Giza CC, & Levin H (2015). Chronic aspects of pediatric traumatic brain injury: review of the literature. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(23), 1849–1860. doi: 10.1089/neu.2015.3971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Backeljauw B, & Kurowski BG (2014). Interventions for attention problems after pediatric traumatic brain injury: what is the evidence? PM R, 6(9), 814–824. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2014.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berridge CW, Devilbiss DM, Andrzejewski ME, Arnsten AF, Kelley AE, Schmeichel B, Hamilton C, &. Spencer RC (2006). Methylphenidate preferentially increases catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefrontal cortex at low doses that enhance cognitive function. Biological Psychiatry, 60(10), 1111–1120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Doyle AE, Seidman LJ, Wilens TE, Ferrero F, Morgan CL, & Faraone SV (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bloom DR, Levin HS, Ewing-Cobbs L, Saunders AE, Song J, Fletcher JM, & Kowatch RA (2001). Lifetime and novel psychiatric disorders after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(5), 572–579. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bonfield CM, Lam S, Lin Y, & Greene S (2013). The impact of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder on recovery from mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurosurgery: Pediatrics, 12(2), 97–102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bryant RA, O’donnell ML, Creamer M, McFarlane AC, Clark CR, & Silove D (2010). The psychiatric sequelae of traumatic injury. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(3), 312–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Catroppa C, Anderson VA, Morse SA, Haritou F, & Rosenfeld JV (2007). Children’s attentional skills 5 years post-TBI. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(3), 354–369. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/js1019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Charach A, & Gajaria A (2008). Improving psychostimulant adherence in children with ADHD. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 8(10), 1563–1571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cnaan A, Laird NM, & Slasor P (1997). Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Statistics in Medicine, 16(20), 2349–2380. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Conners C (2004). Continuous performance test. Technical guide and software manual. North Tonawanda, NY: MultiHealth Systems. [Google Scholar]
  16. Conners CK (1995). Conners’ continuous performance test computer program 3.0 user’s manual. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: MultiHealth Systems. [Google Scholar]
  17. Conners CK (2000). Conners’ continuous performance test II. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: MultiHealth Systems. [Google Scholar]
  18. D’Agati E, Cerminara C, Casarelli L, Pitzianti M, & Curatolo P (2012). Attention and executive functions profile in childhood absence epilepsy. Brain and Development, 34(10), 812–817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. De la Torre GG, Martin A, Cervantes E, Guil R, & Mestre JM (2017). Attention lapses in children with spina bifida and hydrocephalus and children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 39(6), 563–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Delis DC, Kaplan E, & Kaplan J (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive functioning system. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assesments. [Google Scholar]
  21. Dennis M, Wilkinson M, Koski L, & Humphreys RP (1995). Attention deficits in the long term after childhood head injury, In Broman S & Michel ME (Eds.), Traumatic head injury children (pp. 165–187). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Donders J, & Janke K (2008). Criterion validity of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition after pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(4), 651–655. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708080752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dougall D, Poole N, & Agrawal N (2015). Pharmacotherapy for chronic cognitive impairment in traumatic brain injury. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 12, CD009221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fenwick T, & Anderson V (1999). Impairments of attention following childhood traumatic brain injury. Child Neuropsychology, 5(4), 213–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Galbiati S, Recla M, Pastore V, Liscio M, Bardoni A, Castelli E, & Strazzer S (2009). Attention remediation following traumatic brain injury in childhood and adolescence. Neuropsychology, 23(1), 40–49. doi: 10.1037/a0013409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gioia GA, & Isquith PK (2004). Ecological assessment of executive function in traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(1–2), 135–158. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, & Kenworthy L (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Graham J, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Danckaerts M, Dittmann RW, Döpfner M, Hamilton R, Hollis C, Holtmann M, Hulpke-Wette M, Lecendreux M, Rosenthal E, Rothenberger A, Santosh P, Sergeant J, Simonoff E, Sonuga-Barke E, Wong ICK, Zuddas A, Steinhausen H-C, Taylor E, & European Guidelines Group. (2011). European guidelines on managing adverse effects of medication for ADHD. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(1), 17–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gupta SK (2011). Intention-to-treat concept: a review. Perspectives in Clinical Research, 2(3), 109–112. doi: 10.4103/2229-3485.83221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hart J, Kraut MA, Womack KB, Strain J, Didehbani N, Bartz E, Conover H, Mansinghani S, Lu H, & Cullum CM (2013). Neuroimaging of cognitive dysfunction and depression in aging retired National Football League players: a cross-sectional study. JAMA Neurology, 70(3), 326–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Horton AM Jr, Soper HV, & Reynolds CR (2010). Executive functions in children with traumatic brain injury. Applied Neuropsychology, 17(2), 99–103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ibrahim K, & Donyai P (2015). Drug holidays from ADHD medication: international experience over the past four decades. Journal of Attention Disorders, 19(7), 551–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D & Ryan N (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kaufmann PM, Fletcher JM, Levin HS, Miner ME, & Ewing-Cobbs L (1993). Attentional disturbance after pediatric closed head injury. Journal of Child Neurology, 8(4), 348–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kim J, Whyte J, Patel S, Europa E, Wang J, Coslett HB, & Detre JA (2012). Methylphenidate modulates sustained attention and cortical activation in survivors of traumatic brain injury: a perfusion fMRI study. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 222(1), 47–57. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2622-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Konrad K, Günther T, Hanisch C, & Herpertz-Dahlmann B (2004). Differential effects of methylphenidate on attentional functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(2), 191–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kurowski BG, Epstein JN, Pruitt DW, Horn PS, Altaye M, & Wade SL (2018). Benefits of methylphenidate for long-term attention problems after traumatic brain injury in childhood: a randomized, double-masked, placebo-controlled, dose-titration, crossover trial. The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 34(2), E1–E12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lakens D (2013). Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lavigne JV, Dulcan MK, LeBailly SA, & Binns HJ (2012). Can parent reports serve as a proxy for teacher ratings in medication management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(4), 336–342. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31824afea1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Levin HS, & Hanten G (2005). Executive functions after traumatic brain injury in children. Pediatric Neurology, 33(2), 79–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2005.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Levin HS, Song J, Ewing-Cobbs L, Chapman SB, & Mendelsohn D (2001). Word fluency in relation to severity of closed head injury, associated frontal brain lesions, and age at injury in children. Neuropsychologia, 39(2), 122–131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, & Sheets Y (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7(1), 83–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mackinnon DP, Warsi G, & Dwyer JH (1995). A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 30(1), 41. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3001_3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mahalick DM, Carmel PW, Greenberg JP, Molofsky W, Brown JA, Heary RF, Marks D, Zampella E, & Hodosh R (1998). Psychopharmacologic treatment of acquired attention disorders in children with brain injury. Pediatric Neurosurgery, 29(3), 121–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Massagli TL, Fann JR, Burington BE, Jaffe KM, Katon WJ, & Thompson RS (2004). Psychiatric illness after mild traumatic brain injury in children. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 85(9), 1428–1434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Max JE, Lansing AE, Koele SL, Castillo CS, Bokura H, Schachar R, Collings N, & Williams KE (2004). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents following traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(1–2), 159–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Max JE, Schachar RJ, Levin HS, Ewing-Cobbs L, Chapman SB, Dennis M, Saunders A, & Landis J (2005). Predictors of secondary attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents 6 to 24 months after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(10), 1041–1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McCauley SR, Wilde EA, Anderson VA, Bedell G, Beers SR, Campbell TF, Chapman SB, Ewing-Cobbs L, Gerring JP, Gioia GA, Levin HS, Michaud LJ, Prasad MR, Swaine BR, Turkstra LS, Wade SL, Yeates KO, & Pediatric TBI Outcomes Workgroup. (2012). Recommendations for the use of common outcome measures in pediatric traumatic brain injury research. Journal of Neurotrauma, 29(4), 678–705. doi: 10.1089/neu.2011.1838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, & Kun LE (2004). Late neurocognitive sequelae in survivors of brain tumours in childhood. The Lancet Oncology, 5(7), 399–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Muscara F, Catroppa C, & Anderson V (2008). The impact of injury severity on executive function 7-10 years following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(5), 623–636. doi: 10.1080/87565640802171162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Ornstein TJ, Sagar S, Schachar RJ, Ewing-Cobbs L, Chapman SB, Dennis M, Saunders AE, Yang TT, Levin HS, & Max JE (2014). Neuropsychological performance of youth with secondary attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 6-and 12-months after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(10), 971–981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pangilinan PH, Giacoletti-Argento A, Shellhaas R, Hurvitz EA, & Hornyak JE (2010). Neuropharmacology in pediatric brain injury: a review. PM R, 2(12), 1127–1140. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Parisi P, Moavero R, Verrotti A, & Curatolo P (2010). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children with epilepsy. Brain and Development, 52(1), 10–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shaw M, Hodgkins P, Caci H, Young S, Kahle J, Woods AG, & Arnold LE (2012). A systematic review and analysis of long-term outcomes in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of treatment and non-treatment. BMC Medicine, 10(1), 99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Sinopoli KJ, & Dennis M (2012). Inhibitory control after traumatic brain injury in children. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30(3), 207–215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sinopoli KJ, Schachar R, & Dennis M (2011). Traumatic brain injury and secondary attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: the effect of reward on inhibitory control. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(7), 805–819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Slomine BS, Salorio CF, Grados MA, Vasa RA, Christensen JR, & Gerring JP (2005). Differences in attention, executive functioning, and memory in children with and without ADHD after severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(5), 645–653. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Solanto MV (1998). Neuropsychopharmacological mechanisms of stimulant drug action in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review and integration. Behavioural Brain Research, 94(1), 127–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Strong CAH, Tiesma D, & Donders J (2011). Criterion validity of the Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS) fluency subtests after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(2), 230–237. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710001451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management, Wolraich M, Brown L, Brown RT, DuPaul G, Earls M, Feldman HM, Ganiats TG, Kaplanek B, Meyer B, Perrin J, Pierce K, Reiff M, Stein MT, & Visser S (2011). ADHD: clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128(5), 1007–1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Swanson J, Baler RD, & Volkow ND (2011). Understanding the effects of stimulant medications on cognition in individuals with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a decade of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(1), 207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Taylor CA, Bell JM, Breiding MJ, & Xu L (2017). Traumatic brain injury-related emergency department visits, hospitalizations, and deaths – United States, 2007 and 2013. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 66(9), 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Teasdale G, & Jennett B (1974). Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet, 2(7872), 81–84. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Toplak ME, Bucciarelli SM, Jain U, & Tannock R (2008). Executive functions: performance-based measures and the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 15(1), 53–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G, Ding Y, & Gatley SJ (2002). Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: insights from PET imaging studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 6(Suppl 1), S31–S43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wade SL, Kurowski BG, Kirkwood MW, Zhang N, Cassedy A, Brown TM, Nielsen B, Stancin T, & Taylor HG (2015). Online problem-solving therapy after traumatic brain injury: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 135(2), e487–95. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-1386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wade SL, Taylor HG, Cassedy A, Zhang N, Kirkwood MW, Brown TM, & Stancin T (2015). Long-term behavioral outcomes after a randomized, clinical trial of counselor-assisted problem solving for adolescents with complicated mild-to-severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 32(13), 967–975. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3684 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Warden DL, Gordon B, McAllister TW, Silver JM, Barth JT, Bruns J, Drake A, Gentry T, Jagoda A, Katz DI, Kraus J, Labbate LA, Ryan LM, Sparling MB, Walters B, Whyte J, Zapata A, & Zitnay G (2006). Guidelines for the pharmacologic treatment of neurobehavioral sequelae of traumatic brain injury. Journal of Neurotrauma, 25(10), 1468–1501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wasserman RM, Stoner AM, Stern A, & Holmbeck GN (2016). ADHD and attention problems in children with and without Spina Bifida. Topics in Spinal Cord Injury Rehabilitation, 22(4), 253–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Wechsler D (2003). WISC-IV administration manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assesment. [Google Scholar]
  71. Wechsler D Coalson DL, & Raiford SE (2008). WAIS-IV: Wechsler adult intelligence scale. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  72. Willmott C, & Ponsford J (2009). Efficacy of methylphenidate in the rehabilitation of attention following traumatic brain injury: a randomised, crossover, double blind, placebo controlled inpatient trial. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 80(5), 552–557. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.159632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wolraich M, Feurer I, Hannah J, & Baumgaertel A (2003). Vanderbilt ADHD Teacher Rating Scale (VADTRS) and the Vanderbilt ADHD Parent Rating Scale (VADPRS). Oklahoma City, OK: University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center. [Google Scholar]
  74. Yeates KO, Armstrong K, Janusz J, Taylor HG, Wade S, Stancin T, & Drotar D (2005). Long-term attention problems in children with traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(6), 574–584. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000159947.50523.64 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yeates KO, Swift E, Taylor HG, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, & Minich N (2004). Short- and long-term social outcomes following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10(3), 412–426. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704103093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES