Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2017 Aug 28;24(13):1857–1866. doi: 10.1177/1087054717727352

Creativity in ADHD: Goal-Directed Motivation and Domain Specificity

Nathalie Boot 1,, Barbara Nevicka 1, Matthijs Baas 1
PMCID: PMC7543022  PMID: 28845720

Abstract

Objective: This research aimed to provide explanations for the inconsistent findings regarding creativity in ADHD. Method: In Study 1, we assessed real-world creative achievements and intrinsic motivation during idea generation in adults with ADHD and compared these with controls. In Study 2, we manipulated competition during idea generation to investigate effects on idea originality in adults with ADHD versus controls, and assessed creativity in specific domains. Results: Adults with ADHD reported more real-world creative achievements. We did not observe differences in intrinsic motivation during idea generation between groups, but adults with ADHD generated more original ideas when competing for a bonus. Moreover, adults with ADHD rated themselves as more creative in specific creative domains. Conclusion: Our findings suggest that goal-directed motivation may drive the enhanced real-world creative achievements of people with ADHD. Moreover, people with ADHD may selectively engage and excel in creative domains that fit their skills and preferences.

Keywords: ADHD, adult, creativity, motivation, divergent thinking

Introduction

In recent years, an increasing number of people have been diagnosed with ADHD, a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by distractibility, hyperactivity, and impulsivity (e.g., Garfield et al., 2012). Although ADHD is usually first diagnosed in childhood, symptoms often persist into adulthood (Biederman, Petty, Evans, Small, & Faraone, 2010). The disorder may cause considerable problems in people’s lives: ADHD is associated with lower academic and occupational success, and an increased risk of developing depression, anxiety disorders, and addiction (Biederman et al., 2006; Faraone et al., 2000). However, ADHD may have certain benefits in situations that require people to be creative.

Previous studies indicate that creativity, the ability to generate ideas that are both novel and useful (Amabile, 1996), benefits from defocused attention and a reduced ability to ignore task-irrelevant stimuli in the environment (Baird et al., 2012; Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, 2003). The defocused processing of task-unrelated information during creative tasks may activate uncommon associations, resulting in original combinations of information. Moreover, high energy, impulsivity, and risk-taking seem to facilitate creativity (Barron & Harrington, 1981; Feist, 1998). Thus, people who are easily distracted, hyperactive, and impulsive, such as those with ADHD, may be more creative than people who do not experience such symptoms. Indeed, studies show that adults with ADHD (vs. controls), as well as healthy people who experience a high number of ADHD symptoms, report more publically recognized creative achievements in daily life, such as receiving a patent for an invention or publishing a book (Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017; White & Shah, 2011; Zabelina, Condon, & Beeman, 2014). However, the precise cognitive and motivational mechanisms underlying this observation remain unclear. The purpose of this research is to address these mechanisms to better understand creativity in ADHD.

First, real-world creativity is a complex construct that relies on the novel and appropriate combination of existing knowledge through several lower-level cognitive processes. For example, creative problems are often complex and ill-defined and the creative process must start with a restructuring of the context and ultimate goals of the problem at hand. Then, a person may generate multiple potential solutions for the identified problem and evaluate the feasibility of those solutions (e.g., Cropley, 2006; Mumford, Baughman, Threlfall, Supinski, & Costanza, 1996). These processes can be assessed using standardized laboratory tasks. For example, the Alternate Uses Task (AUT; Guilford, 1967) measures divergent thinking (i.e., the generation of multiple ideas in response to open-ended questions) by asking people to generate new, original uses for common objects, such as a brick. Some studies show that adults with ADHD (vs. controls) generate more original ideas on such tasks (White & Shah, 2006, 2011, 2016), but others do not find enhanced divergent thinking in ADHD (Abraham, Windmann, Siefen, Daum, & Güntürkün, 2006; Barkley, Murphy, & Kwasnik, 1996; Murphy, Barkley, & Bush, 2001). However, people with ADHD may outperform people without the disorder on other aspects of the creative process (Abraham et al., 2006; Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017). For example, ADHD symptoms have been associated with enhanced originality (but reduced usefulness) of problem reconstructions (Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017). Thus, when investigating creativity in ADHD, it is important to look at different creative processes.

Second, real-world creativity also relies on the motivation to turn existing knowledge into creative output through the aforementioned cognitive processes (Amabile, 1996). For instance, intrinsic motivation, the drive to engage in a task because it is interesting and enjoyable in itself, facilitates creativity (Hennessey & Amabile, 2010). People with ADHD seem to prefer the “loose,” unstructured idea generation process of creativity over more analytical processes (White & Shah, 2011), and thus may be more intrinsically motivated during idea generation. ADHD, however, is generally associated with motivational deficits (e.g., Toplak, Jain, & Tannock, 2005) and ADHD symptoms may actually result from a decreased motivation to perform tasks that are not (immediately) reinforced by external rewards (Barkley, 1997; Volkow et al., 2011). Thus, what drives people with ADHD to be more creative might be their (extrinsic) motivation to obtain desired outcomes, such as the reception of praise and external rewards, rather than the sheer enjoyment of performing creative tasks. Some evidence indicates that monetary rewards and competition with others indeed improves cognitive control in children with ADHD (Geurts, Luman, & van Meel, 2008; Kohls, Herpertz-Dahlmann, & Konrad, 2009). When external reinforcement is absent, the rather simple ideation tasks used in the laboratory may not encourage people to fully engage in the process of generating their most original ideas. This may explain why increased divergent thinking is not consistently observed in ADHD. Moreover, people with ADHD may experience greater motivation during daily creative activities in domains of their own choosing, but not during standardized laboratory tasks. Thus, it is important to assess how motivation and reinforcement during creative tasks influence creative performance and whether people with ADHD are only creative in domains of their choosing.

Because real-world creativity relies on cognitive and motivational processes, another important factor to examine is medication use in ADHD, as medication use may affect these processes. A range of different drugs are available to alleviate ADHD symptoms, the most widely used ones being amphetamines and methylphenidate (Advokat, 2010). These drugs increase dopamine and noradrenaline levels in the brain, thereby normalizing frontostriatal activity in people with ADHD (Arnsten & Dudley, 2005; Rubia et al., 2009). This, in turn, improves executive functions, including response inhibition (Aron, Dowson, Sahakian, & Robbins, 2003) and sustained attention (Advokat, 2010). While improving the ability to focus, such medication may decrease the cognitive flexibility that many creative processes require (e.g., seeing associations between remotely related concepts, divergent thinking). Interestingly, methylphenidate may likewise target the motivational deficits observed in ADHD by increasing task salience (Liddle et al., 2011; Volkow et al., 2004). A recent study showed that methylphenidate administration influenced creativity in a sample of healthy adults (Gvirts et al., 2017), but whether medication also affects creativity in people with ADHD is unclear. In one study on creativity in adults with ADHD, medication use did not affect divergent thinking (White & Shah, 2011). However, other studies did not control for medication use or explicitly excluded participants who used medication from the study (Abraham et al., 2006; White & Shah, 2006, 2016).

Here, we present two studies in which we investigated these factors as explanations for the inconsistent findings in previous studies. In Study 1, we assessed the role of intrinsic motivation in idea generation in people with ADHD versus healthy controls. Moreover, we compared performance in these groups during creative problem reconstruction and explored the effects of medication use on these processes. In Study 2, we manipulated motivation and task engagement during idea generation by providing participants with an opportunity to win a bonus by competing with others. Moreover, we assessed whether people with ADHD (vs. controls) consider themselves to be more creative in specific domains of creativity.

Study 1

We had four objectives in Study 1. First, we aimed to replicate the finding that people with ADHD (vs. controls) report more real-world creative achievements (White & Shah, 2011). Second, because real-world creativity builds on several creative problem solving stages, we assessed whether performance of people with ADHD differed from controls during two of those stages: initial problem reconstruction and subsequent idea generation. We hypothesized that ADHD participants would outperform controls on aspects related to originality but not usefulness (Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017). Third, we assessed the role of intrinsic motivation in creative idea generation. If people with ADHD enjoy idea generation more than people without ADHD (White & Shah, 2011), participants with ADHD (vs. controls) may be more motivated to perform well during an idea generation task. We expected that people with ADHD would generate more original ideas because of increased intrinsic motivation. Finally, we explored whether creative processes were influenced by ADHD medication by comparing the performance of people with ADHD who did, and who did not, use such medication.

Method

Design, participants, and procedure

We recruited 71 participants with ADHD to participate in this online study via a local institution specialized in the treatment of ADHD. All participants had been diagnosed by a clinical psychologist or psychiatrist. Of these participants, 42 used some type of medication to treat their ADHD symptoms at the time of testing (medicated ADHD group), whereas 29 others did not (nonmedicated ADHD group). In addition, we recruited 36 healthy control participants who matched participants in the ADHD groups on age, gender, and education. On average, participants were 26.77 years old (SD = 7.85; range = 18-54; 61% female). The two ADHD groups and the control group did not differ in terms of gender, χ2(2, N = 107) = 2.66, p = .265; age, F(2, 104) = .14, p = .868, ηp2 < .01; or education level, F(2, 104) = 1.43, p = .243, ηp2 < .03. Participation was voluntary and participants provided written consent. Participants received €5 for their participation. During the study, participants first completed an ADHD symptom checklist followed by an idea generation task, a creative achievement questionnaire, and a problem construction task (see below). Finally, participants provided demographic information and participants in the ADHD groups answered some questions about their ADHD diagnosis and medication use.

Measures

ADHD

Current ADHD symptoms, such as hyperactivity and distractibility, were measured using the 23-item ADHD rating scale for adults (Kooij et al., 2005). For each ADHD symptom, participants rated its frequency in the past 6 months (1 = never or rarely, 5 = very often). Sample items include “forgetful in daily activities,” “make careless mistakes in work,” and “talk excessively” (α = .92; M = 3.25, SD = 0.77).

Creative achievements

We assessed publically recognized and concrete creative achievements in 10 domains (e.g., creative writing, theater, culinary arts) using the Creative Achievement Questionnaire (Carson, Peterson, & Higgins, 2005). For each domain, participants were presented with eight rank-ordered statements ranging from 0 (“I have no training or recognized talent in this area”) to 7 (“I have won a national prize in this area”) and indicated which of these applied to them. The marked ranks were summed together to yield a creative achievement score (M = 13.04; SD = 14.80, range = 0-82). Because participants’ scores were strongly skewed, these were log-transformed prior to data analysis (Silvia, Wigert, Reiter-Palmon, & Kaufman, 2012).

Divergent thinking

We assessed divergent thinking using the AUT (Guilford, 1967). In two separate 2-min trials, we asked participants to generate as many new, original ways to use a fork and a newspaper as they could think of. Three independent and trained coders scored participants’ ideas in terms of fluency (the number of nonredundant ideas), flexibility (the number of conceptual categories the ideas belong to) and originality (the extent to which an idea is novel). To obtain a measure of flexibility, ideas were categorized into different conceptual categories. For example, for the fork topic, the idea “to defend yourself” was coded in the category “as a weapon,” whereas the idea “use as a screwdriver” was coded in the category “as a tool.” To obtain a measure of originality, coders scored each idea for the extent to which it was novel and uncommon on a 5-point scale (1 = not original at all, 5 = very original). We averaged originality ratings across all ideas an individual generated to correct for differences in fluency. Interrater reliability for flexibility (ICCfork = .97, p < .001; ICCnewspaper = .98, p < .001) and originality (ICCfork = .81, p < .001; ICCnewspaper = .84, p < .001) was good. We averaged fluency, flexibility, and originality ratings for both objects and across the three coders. On average, participants generated 8.30 ideas per trial (SD = 2.95, range = 1.50-17.50) using 6.49 different categories (SD = 1.99, range = 1.5-11.67), with a mean originality of 1.66 (SD = 0.29, range = 1.09-2.75).

Following each trial, participants answered four questions regarding their intrinsic motivation (e.g., “I enjoyed generating new uses for a newspaper/fork”; 1 = I do not agree at all, 5 = I completely agree; α = .88, M = 3.60, SD = 0.90).

Problem construction

We measured participants’ ability to restructure complex, ill-defined problems using a problem construction task (Mumford et al., 1996). During this task, participants read a short description of four problematic situations such as “You are the principal of an elementary school. One of the students has brought in a snake, but now it is missing.” After reading each description, we asked participants to redefine the problem in terms of (a) diagnostic information, (b) alternative goals, (c) alternative procedures, and (d) constraints. For each of these aspects, participants could choose from four problem definitions that varied in usefulness (high vs. low) and originality (high vs. low). In the above example, an alternative goal high in both usefulness and originality would be: “How can I turn this into a learning experience for the students?”; whereas, an alternative goal of low usefulness and low originality would be: “How can I keep from being held directly accountable?” We subsequently counted the number of high-usefulness and high-originality options that participants selected. Participants obtained a mean usefulness score of 12.38 (SD = 2.14, range = 3-16) and a mean originality score of 7.07 (SD = 2.38, range = 0-15).

Medication use

Participants in the ADHD groups indicated whether they used (now or in the past) medication to treat their symptoms. If so, they indicated what kind of medication they used, the dosage, and how often they used medication in the preceding 6 months.

Results

Descriptive statistics and group differences in ADHD symptoms

Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for all variables. ADHD symptoms correlated positively with creative achievements and negatively with the usefulness of problem construction. AUT fluency and flexibility were positively correlated, but originality did not correlate with either fluency or flexibility. Creative achievement scores correlated positively with AUT originality and originality of problem construction. As expected, self-reported ADHD symptoms differed between groups, F(2, 104) = 60.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .54. Planned contrasts showed that both the medicated (M = 3.69, SD = 0.41) and unmedicated (M = 3.62, SD = 0.54) ADHD groups reported more severe ADHD symptoms than controls (M = 2.46, SD = 0.64), t(104) = 10.89, p < .001, d = 2.26, but symptoms did not differ between the two ADHD groups, t(104) = 0.55, p = .583, d = 0.15.

Table 1.

Correlations Between ADHD Symptoms and Creativity Measures in Study 1.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. ADHD symptoms
2. Creative achievements .22*
3. AUT fluency −.12 .15
4. AUT flexibility −.09 .12 .88**
5. AUT originality −.03 .19* .05 .04
6. Problem usefulness −.36** −.02 −.05 −.02 .04
7. Problem originality .11 .21* −.07 −.10 .10 .06
8. Intrinsic motivation −.06 .19 .11 .19 .40** .14 .01

Note. N = 107; AUT = Alternate Uses Task.

*

p < .05. **p < .01.

Group differences in creativity

We observed differences between the ADHD and control groups on two of the creativity measures. First, creative achievements scores differed between groups, F(2, 104) = 6.30, p = .003, ηp2 = .11. Planned contrasts showed that participants in the medicated (M = 2.35, SD = 0.94) and nonmedicated (M = 2.58, SD = 0.87) ADHD groups reported more real-world creative achievements than healthy controls (M = 1.86, SD = 0.70; t(104) = 3.47, p = .001, d = 0.71), but creative achievement scores did not differ between the two ADHD groups, t(104) = 1.14, p = .257, d = 0.25. Moreover, the usefulness of problem reconstruction differed between groups, F(2, 104) = 5.56, p = .005, d = .10. In line with our hypothesis, ADHD participants picked fewer useful problem definitions than controls (M = 13.25, SD = 1.66; t(104) = 3.23, p = .002, d = 0.70). Usefulness scores of medicated (M = 12.17, SD = 2.04) and nonmedicated (M = 11.59, SD = 2.51) ADHD participants did not differ from each other, t(104) = 1.16, p = .247, d = 0.25. There was no significant group difference for the originality of problem construction, F(2, 104) = 1.12, p = .330, ηp2 = .02; AUT fluency, F(2, 104) = 0.39, p = .678, ηp2 = .01; AUT flexibility, F(2, 104) = 1.09, p = .341, ηp2 = .02; and AUT originality, F(2, 104) = 0.19, p = .827, ηp2 < .01.

Motivation during creative idea generation

Participants’ intrinsic motivation ratings during the AUT correlated positively with idea originality during this task, but not with flexibility or fluency (Table 1). Moreover, motivation ratings did not differ between the three groups, F(2, 104) = .02, p = .977, ηp2 < .01.

Discussion and Introduction to Study 2

In Study 1, we found that people with ADHD (vs. controls) reported more real-world creative achievements, in line with findings by White and Shah (2011). However, people with ADHD did not outperform controls on the creative processes on which creative outcomes and achievements rely: problem reconstruction and idea generation. Although higher intrinsic motivation ratings were associated with more original ideas during an idea generation task, neither intrinsic motivation nor performance differed between groups. This is inconsistent with findings suggesting that people with ADHD enjoy generating ideas more than control participants (White & Shah, 2011). Moreover, group differences during creative problem reconstruction do not seem to account for the enhanced creative achievements in ADHD, as ADHD participants (vs. controls) selected fewer useful (and equally original) problem reconstructions. Finally, we did not find performance differences between medicated and unmedicated ADHD participants for any of the creativity measures.

In Study 2, we further investigated the role of motivation in the enhanced real-world creative achievements in ADHD. Given the motivational deficits associated with ADHD, it is possible that people with ADHD need additional reinforcement that triggers their motivation to perform well during tasks that are not immediately rewarding (Shaw & Giambra, 1993; Volkow et al., 2011). Therefore, we manipulated motivation by presenting participants with an opportunity to win a bonus in a competitive setting. We hypothesized that people with ADHD (vs. controls) would generate more original ideas on trials in which they could win money by competing with others, but not on trials without such competition. We specifically focused on originality of ideas, because originality can be seen as the hallmark of creativity (Amabile, 1996).

We also investigated whether people with ADHD (vs. controls) are more creative in specific domains. Such domain specificity could indicate that people with ADHD tend to select creative activities or environments that they like and/or fit their qualities (Verheul et al., 2015, 2016). Although an advantage of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire is that it measures recognized achievements in various domains rather than participants’ subjective judgment of their own creativity, a disadvantage of this scale is that many people report low scores in most domains. As a result, it is difficult to assess group differences in performance in specific domains using this scale. Therefore, in Study 2, we included a different self-report measure of creativity—one that assesses everyday creative abilities in different domains.

Method

Design, participants, and procedure

We recruited 46 participants with ADHD and 44 healthy controls to participate in this study for money or course credit via advertisements posted around campus. Participation was voluntary and participants provided written consent. On average, participants were 22.93 years old (SD = 3.41; range = 18-36; 62% female). The ADHD and control group did not differ on gender, χ2(2, N = 107) = 1.30, p = .254; or education, t(88) = 0.57, p = .570, d = 0.12. However, the ADHD group was significantly older than the control group (MADHD = 23.98, SDADHD = 3.87; Mcontrol = 21.84, SDcontrol = 2.44), t(88) = −3.12, p = .002, d = 0.66. Participants were seated in individual cubicles behind a computer, which displayed all materials and recorded all responses. First, they completed a divergent thinking task, during which we manipulated competition (within-subjects design) by providing participants with an opportunity to win a bonus. Subsequently, participants completed an ADHD symptom scale and a creativity scale. Finally, participants provided demographic information and those with ADHD additionally provided information about their ADHD diagnosis, medication use, and comorbidity with any other disorder(s) (e.g., major depression, anxiety disorders). Participants also completed an achievement goal questionnaire that is not relevant for the present research questions.

Measures

ADHD

We used the same scale as in Study 1 (Kooij et al., 2005; α = .93; M = 3.04, SD = 0.79).

Divergent thinking

As in Study 1, we measured divergent thinking using the AUT (Guilford, 1967). Participants were asked to generate as many new, original uses for four objects (a belt, book, tin can, and towel) during four separate 1-min trials. The objects were presented in random order. Again, three independent and trained coders scored participants’ ideas in terms of fluency, flexibility, and originality. Interrater reliability for both flexibility (all ICCs > .95, all ps < .001) and originality (all ICCs > .80, all ps < .001) was good. On average, participants generated 5.44 ideas per object (SD = 2.26, range = 1.00-12.75) in 4.31 different categories (SD = 1.44, range = 1.00-7.58), with a mean originality of 1.79 (SD = 0.39, range = 0.84-2.75).1 As in Study 1, participants answered three questions concerning their motivation to perform well following each trial.

Competition manipulation

We manipulated competition by providing participants with an opportunity to win an additional €5 during certain trials. We told participants that the originality of their ideas on two of the trials would be compared with the performance of another randomly selected participant from the sample. If their ideas were found to be more original on those trials, they would win €5. If the ideas of the other participant were more original, the other participant would win €5. On the other two “non-competition” trials, participants’ ideas were not compared with other participants’ ideas. We told participants that their ideas on those trials would only be considered to assess their individual performance. Prior to each trial, participants were told whether or not their ideas on that particular trial were to be compared with those of another participant. Competition and no-competition trials were presented in random order. As an instructional check, after each trial, participants indicated whether their ideas on the preceding trial were to be considered for the bonus.

Manipulation check

Immediately after the final trial, we assessed participants’ subjective experience of competition during the task using five items (e.g., “I felt like I was participating in a competition,” “I wanted to be more creative that the other”) on a 5-point scale (1 = I do not agree at all, 5 = I totally agree; α = .84, M = 3.73, SD = 0.56).

Creativity in different domains

We assessed self-perceived creative abilities using the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS; Kaufman, 2012). This scale consists of 50 items assessing creativity in five domains: self/everyday, scholarly, performance, mechanical/scientific, and artistic creativity. For each item, participants indicated how creative they considered themselves to be compared with other people (1 = much less creative, 5 = much more creative). Items included “Planning a trip or event with friends that meets everyone’s needs” (self/everyday domain; α = .66), “Shooting a fun video to air on YouTube” (performance domain; α = .82), “Constructing something out of metal, stone, or similar material” (mechanical/scientific domain; α = .78), “Drawing a picture of something I’ve never actually seen (like an alien)” (artistic domain; α = .66), and “Arguing a side in a debate that I do not personally agree with” (scholarly domain; α = .67).

Medication use and comorbidity

As in Study 1, we asked participants in the ADHD group questions about their present and past medication use, type of medication, and dosage. Moreover, participants indicated whether a psychologist or psychiatrist had diagnosed them with any other disorder(s) (e.g., major depression, anxiety disorders) that was still relevant at the time of participation. Such disorders are common in ADHD and may affect creativity in this group (Baas, Nijstad, Boot, & De Dreu, 2016; Faraone et al., 2000). Twenty-six percent of ADHD participants reported a comorbid mental disorder.

Results

Descriptive statistics and group differences in ADHD symptoms

Table 2 shows the means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for all variables. ADHD symptoms correlated positively with AUT originality and self-reported creativity in the mechanical/scientific, artistic, and performance domains, but not with any of the other creativity indicators. As in Study 1, AUT fluency and flexibility were positively correlated, but originality did not correlate with either fluency or flexibility. As expected, the ADHD group (M = 3.62, SD = 0.49) reported more severe ADHD symptoms than the control group (M = 2.44, SD = 0.79), t(88) = 10.69, p < .001, d = 1.80.

Table 2.

Correlations Between ADHD Scales and Creativity Measures in Study 2.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Age
2. ADHD symptoms .21*
3. AUT fluency .08 .09
4. AUT flexibility .08 .11 .90**
5. AUT originality −.05 .28** −.01 −.07
6. K-DOCS self/everyday .00 .00 .12 .07 .26**
7. K-DOCS mechanical/scientific .03 .33** .02 .05 .01 −.15
8. K-DOCS artistic .14 .26* .13 .13 .15 .07 .30**
9. K-DOCS performance .06 .34** .20 .16 .19 .11 .13 .45**
10. K-DOCS scholarly .08 .11 −.02 .02 .18 .20 .19 .16 .34**

Note. N = 90; AUT = Alternate Uses Task; K-DOCS = Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale.

*

p < .05. **p < .01.

Manipulation check

On average, participants made an error on the instructional check questions on 13% of the trials. However, including the percentage of errors made as a covariate in the analyses did not change the results. A 2 (Group: ADHD vs. controls) × 2 (Competition: competition vs. no competition) repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed that, during the AUT, participants were more motivated under competition (M = 3.80, SD = 0.61) than in the absence of competition (M = 3.69, SD = 0.58), F(1, 88) = 7.51, p = .007, ηp2 = .08, suggesting that our manipulation was successful. Motivation ratings did not differ between groups, nor was there an interaction between group and competition (Fs < 2.45, ps > .121).

Group differences in idea generation and the effects of competition

To assess group differences in creativity in the presence or absence of competition, we conducted a series of repeated-measures ANOVAs with the above factors. Because age differed between groups and was also related to the subjective experience of competition, r(90) = −.34, p = .001, we entered age as a covariate in these analyses. For AUT fluency, we did not find a main effect of competition, F(1, 87) = 0.67, p = .415, ηp2 = .01, or group F(1, 87) = 0.00, p = .997, ηp2 < .01, nor did we find a significant interaction between group and competition, F(1, 87) = 2.41, p = .124, ηp2 = .03. Similarly, we did not find any main or interaction effects for flexibility (Fs < 1.61, ps > .207). However, we found a significant main effect of competition, F(1, 87) = 12.89, p = .001, ηp2 = .13, for the originality of ideas. Participants generated more original ideas under competition (M = 1.86, SD = 0.56) than in the absence of competition (M = 1.72, SD = 0.33). We did not find a main effect of group on originality, F(1, 87) = 1.07, p = .304, ηp2 = .01, but group interacted with competition, F(1, 87) = 4.67, p = .034, ηp2 = .05. Pairwise comparisons showed that people with ADHD were not significantly more original compared with controls in either condition: competition, F(1, 87) = 2.71, p = .103, ηp2 = .03; no competition, F(1, 87) = 0.09, p = .767, ηp2 < .01. However, ADHD participants became more original under competition (Mcompetition = 1.92, Mno competition = 1.72), F(1, 87) = 12.24, p = .001, ηp2 = .12, whereas control participants did not (Mcompetition = 1.79, Mno competition = 1.71), F(1, 87) = 0.10, p = .749, ηp2 < .01.2

Self-reported creativity in different domains

A multivariate ANOVA showed that K-DOCS scores differed significantly between groups, F(5, 84) = 3.80, p = .006, Wilk’s Λ = 0.83, ηp2 = .18 (see Table 3 for means and standard deviations). After correction for multiple comparisons (adjusted α = .01), we found significant group differences in the performance, F(1, 88) = 8.72, p = .004, ηp2 = .09; and mechanical/scientific domain, F(1, 88) = 8.50, p = .005, ηp2 = .09. No group differences were observed in the everyday, F(1, 88) = 0.54, p = .464, ηp2 = .01; scholarly, F(1, 88) = .59, p = .443, ηp2 = .01; and artistic domains, F(1, 88) = 2.82, p = .097, ηp2 = .03.

Table 3.

Means and Standard Deviations of Self-Reported Creativity in Five Different Domains for the ADHD and Control Groups.

ADHD
Control
p
M SD M SD
Self/everyday 3.75 0.60 3.67 0.49 .464
Mechanical/scientific 2.89 0.81 2.40 0.81 .005
Artistic 3.32 0.65 3.08 0.69 .097
Performance 3.30 0.82 2.83 0.69 .004
Scholarly 3.72 0.64 3.62 0.56 .443

Discussion

In the present studies, we investigated several potential explanations for the inconsistent evidence regarding creativity in ADHD. First of all, we replicated findings showing that people with ADHD report more real-world creative achievements (Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017; White & Shah, 2011). While we did not observe increased intrinsic motivation during creative idea generation in ADHD, people with ADHD did generate more original ideas when competing for a bonus, but not in the absence of such competition. Competition did not affect the creative performance of control participants. Moreover, we found that ADHD participants (vs. controls) rated themselves as more creative in specific creative domains. People with ADHD reported higher creative abilities in the performance (e.g., playing music in public, acting in a play) and mechanical/scientific domain (e.g., setting up experiments, programming), but, for instance, not in the artistic domain. Enhanced performance during the early stages of creative problem solving does not seem to play a role in the enhanced real-world creativity observed in ADHD. Participants in the ADHD (vs. control) group selected fewer useful (and equally original) problem reconstructions. Finally, we did not find any performance differences between medicated and unmedicated participants with ADHD.

Theoretical Implications

Our finding that people with ADHD generated more original ideas under competition than in the absence of competition is in line with studies showing that rewards or social comparison can improve cognitive performance in people with ADHD (Geurts et al., 2008; Kohls et al., 2009; Luman, Oosterlaan, & Sergeant, 2005). In Study 1, in which we did not manipulate competition, we did not observe increased (intrinsic) motivation during idea generation in ADHD participants, contrary to findings showing that people with ADHD enjoy idea generation more than people without ADHD (White & Shah, 2011). Our findings do suggest that people with ADHD can be motivated to exert more goal-directed effort when a desired outcome, either in the form of a monetary reward or the possibility to beat an opponent, is introduced. Although the ideas of people with ADHD were more original under competition compared with when competition was absent, their ideas were not more original than those of control participants in either condition (possibly due to a lack of power). Moreover, it is unclear whether the observed effects were driven by the prospect of obtaining a monetary reward, the opportunity to outperform an opponent, or both.

We did not find group differences in originality during problem reconstruction, an early stage of the creative process. In fact, participants with ADHD (vs. controls) selected fewer useful problem reconstructions, supporting findings showing that people with ADHD have problems with aspects of creativity relating to practicality (Abraham et al., 2006; Boot, Nevicka, & Baas, 2017). Possibly, people with ADHD need additional reinforcement to perform well during idea generation as well as initial problem reconstruction. Alternatively, people with ADHD may outperform controls in stages following problem reconstruction and idea generation. Creative ideas may evoke a sense of inspiration, a motivational state that drives people to actualize those ideas (Thrash, Maruskin, Cassidy, Fryer, & Ryan, 2010). This drive seems to be especially strong in people scoring high on approach temperament, a strong sensitivity to appetitive stimuli and reinforcement, such as people with ADHD (Boot, Baas, Van Gaal, Cools, & De Dreu, 2017; Mitchell, 2010). Thus, future studies could assess whether motivation during the actualization, rather than generation of ideas underlie the enhanced real-world creativity observed in ADHD.

The finding that participants with ADHD (vs. controls) report being more creative in specific creative domains suggests that people with ADHD are not more creative overall, but that they may select creative tasks and environments that they like and/or fit their abilities. People with ADHD strive to maintain control over their environment in a generally chaotic life. The ability to do so contributes to their psychological well-being (Toner, O’Donoghue, & Houghton, 2006; Wilmshurst, Peele, & Wilmshurst, 2011). People with (symptoms of) ADHD are more likely to be self-employed than people without such symptoms, possibly because they prefer to work in informal environments in which they experience high autonomy (Mannuzza, Klein, Bessler, & LaPadula, 1993; Verheul et al., 2015, 2016). The good fit between characteristics of ADHD and such environmental factors may increase job satisfaction and commitment (Verheul et al., 2015). Thus, people with ADHD may simply invest more time and energy in creative activities and domains that match their preferences and skills. As a result, they may develop a high level of expertise in these domains, enabling them to generate more original ideas (Baer, 2015). In our study, ADHD participants (vs. controls) reported higher creativity specifically in the performance and mechanical/scientific domain. Exactly why people with ADHD are drawn to these domains is unclear and should be assessed in future studies. Moreover, the reliability of the subscales for which we did not observe group differences was suboptimal, which may have influenced the results.

When interpreting our findings regarding domain-specific creativity, it is important to note that these are based on self-reported abilities rather than actual creative performance. A strength of the K-DOCS is that it assesses creative abilities in a wide range of situations. However, this may limit the experience that participants have with each of these situations, making it difficult to judge one’s own creativity. Moreover, people’s subjective judgment of their own creativity is not always accurate and may be strongly influenced by self-esteem, which tends to be reduced in ADHD (Kaufman, 2012; Shaw-Zirt, Popali-Lehane, Chaplin, & Bergman, 2005). Thus, the present findings may underestimate the (self-perceived) creativity of people with ADHD. At the same time, scientific creativity in ADHD may be overestimated in our sample of mainly university students. More generally, the education level of participants in our samples limits the generalizability of our results. Our samples consisted of high-functioning people with ADHD who managed to do well in educational settings despite their symptoms, while people with ADHD are generally less likely to obtain a university degree than people without ADHD (Wilmshurst et al., 2011). Thus, future studies should verify these findings in a more heterogeneous sample.

We did not find any differences in creativity between medicated and unmedicated participants in the present studies. However, because of the large individual variability, we did not take the type and dosage of medication into account. Future studies could more systematically assess medication effects on creativity in ADHD by manipulating medication use, similar to several studies conducted with children with ADHD (Douglas, Barr, Desilets, & Sherman, 1995; González-Carpio Hernández & Serrano Selva, 2016). Similarly, comorbidity with other mental disorders in the ADHD group was too diverse to allow for meaningful exploration of its association with creativity, although it is likely that comorbid disorders influence creative processes in ADHD (e.g., Baas et al., 2016).

Conclusion

In the present studies, we showed that people with ADHD generate more original ideas when competing for rewards with others than when such competition is absent. Although the exact mechanism underlying this effect remains to be uncovered in future studies, our findings suggesting that goal-directed motivation may drive the enhanced real-world creative achievements of people with ADHD. Moreover, we found that people with ADHD report enhanced creativity in specific domains, indicating that people with ADHD may excel in specific creative tasks that match their preferences and abilities. Encouraging them to engage in such tasks and rewarding their creative work may help both themselves, as well as the people around them, to benefit from the positive side of ADHD.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Maartje Rodenhuis, Sifra Verburg, Geert Nagel Joseph, Eveline Maan, Nancy Kluvers, and Yvonne van der Plas for their help with data collection.

Author Biographies

Nathalie Boot is currently a PhD student at the University of Amsterdam. Her research focuses on the neural and cognitive correlates of creativity.

Barbara Nevicka is an assistant professor at the Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam. Research interests include narcissism in social contexts in general, with a focus on leadership.

Matthijs Baas is an assistant professor at the Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam. His research focuses on the social, personality, and cognitive correlates of creativity.

1.

One participant did not generate any valid ideas for one of the objects and thus obtained an average originality score below 1.

2.

Because we did not find any differences between medicated and unmedicated participants in Study 1, we did not include medication use as a main factor of interest in Study 2. In Study 2, 22 participants in the ADHD group used medication to treat their symptoms, whereas 24 did not. Creative performance did not differ between medicated and unmedicated participants (all Fs < 0.157, all ps > .694). Moreover, performance did not differ between ADHD participants with (N = 15) and without comorbid disorders (N = 31; all Fs < 0.843, all ps > .366).

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was facilitated by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO-451-12-023) to Matthijs Baas.

References

  1. Abraham A., Windmann S., Siefen R., Daum I., Güntürkün O. (2006). Creative thinking in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 12, 111-123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Advokat C. (2010). What are the cognitive effects of stimulant medications? Emphasis on adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 1256-1266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amabile T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Arnsten A. F. T., Dudley A. G. (2005). Methylphenidate improves prefrontal cortical cognitive function through α2 adrenoceptor and dopamine D1 receptor actions: Relevance to therapeutic effects in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1, Article 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aron A. R., Dowson J. H., Sahakian B. J., Robbins T. W. (2003). Methylphenidate improves response inhibition in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1465-1468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baas M., Nijstad B. A., Boot N., De Dreu C. K. W. (2016). Mad genius revisited: Vulnerability to psychopathology, biobehavioral approach-avoidance, and creativity. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 668-692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baer J. (2015). The importance of domain-specific expertise in creativity. Roeper Review, 37, 165-178. [Google Scholar]
  8. Baird B., Smallwood J., Mrazek M. D., Kam J. W. Y., Franklin M. S., Schooler J. W. (2012). Inspired by distraction: Mind wandering facilitates creative incubation. Psychological Science, 23, 1117-1122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barkley R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barkley R. A., Murphy K., Kwasnik D. (1996). Psychological adjustment and adaptive impairments in young adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 41-54. [Google Scholar]
  11. Barron F., Harrington D. M. (1981). Creativity, intelligence, and personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 439-476. [Google Scholar]
  12. Biederman J., Monuteaux M. C., Mick E., Spencer T., Wilens T. E., Silva J. M., . . . Faraone S. V. (2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 36, 167-179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Biederman J., Petty C. R., Evans M., Small J., Faraone S. V. (2010). How persistent is ADHD? A controlled 10-year follow-up study of boys with ADHD. Psychiatry Research, 177, 299-304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Boot N., Baas M., Van Gaal S., Cools R., De Dreu C. K. W. (2017). Creative cognition and dopaminergic modulation of fronto-striatal networks: Integrative review and research agenda. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 78, 13-23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boot N., Nevicka B., Baas M. (2017). Subclinical symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are associated with specific creative processes. Personality and Individual Differences, 114, 73-81. [Google Scholar]
  16. Carson S. H., Peterson J. B., Higgins D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499-506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Carson S. H., Peterson J. B., Higgins D. M. (2005). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 37-50. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cropley A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 391-404. [Google Scholar]
  19. Douglas V. I., Barr R. G., Desilets J., Sherman E. (1995). Do high doses of stimulants impair flexible thinking in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 877-885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Faraone S. V., Biederman J., Spencer T., Wilens T., Seidman L. J., Mick E., Doyle A. E. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: An overview. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 9-20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Feist G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290-309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Garfield C. F., Dorsey E. R., Zhu S., Huskamp H. A., Conti R., Dusetzina S. B., . . . Alexander G. C. (2012). Trends in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ambulatory diagnosis and medical treatment in the United States, 2000-2010. Academic Pediatrics, 12, 110-116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Geurts H. M., Luman M., van Meel C. S. (2008). What’s in a game: The effect of social motivation on interference control in boys with ADHD and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 848-857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. González-Carpio Hernández G., Serrano Selva J. P. (2016). Medication and creativity in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Psicothema, 28, 20-25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Guilford J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gvirts H. Z., Mayseless N., Segev A., Lewis D. Y., Feffer K., Barnea Y., . . . Shamay-Tsoory S. G. (2017). Novelty-seeking trait predicts the effect of methylphenidate on creativity. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 31, 599-605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hennessey B. A., Amabile T. M. (2010). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 569-598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kaufman J. C. (2012). Counting the muses: Development of the Kaufman Domains of Creativity Scale (K-DOCS). Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 298-308. [Google Scholar]
  29. Kohls G., Herpertz-Dahlmann B., Konrad K. (2009). Hyperresponsiveness to social rewards in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, Article 20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kooij J. J. S., Buitelaar J. K., van den Oord E. J., Furer J. W., Rijnders C. A. T., Hodiamont P. P. G. (2005). Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine, 35, 817-827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Liddle E. B., Hollis C., Batty M. J., Groom M. J., Totman J. J., Liotti M., . . . Liddle P. F. (2011). Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: Effects of motivation and methylphenidate. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 761-771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Luman M., Oosterlaan J., Sergeant J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: A review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 183-213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mannuzza S., Klein R. G., Bessler A., LaPadula M. (1993). Adult outcome of hyperactive boys: Educational achievement, occupational rank, and psychiatric status. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 565-576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mitchell J. T. (2010). Behavioral approach in ADHD: Testing a motivational dysfunction hypothesis. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 609-617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mumford M. D., Baughman W. A., Threlfall K. V., Supinski E. P., Costanza D. P. (1996). Process-based measures of creative problem-solving skills: I. Problem construction. Creativity Research Journal, 9, 63-76. [Google Scholar]
  36. Murphy K. R., Barkley R. A., Bush T. (2001). Executive functioning and olfactory identification in young adults with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology, 15, 211-220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rubia K., Halari R., Cubillo A., Mohammad A.-M., Brammer M., Taylor E. (2009). Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naïve children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology, 57, 640-652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Shaw G. A., Giambra L. (1993). Task-unrelated thoughts of college students diagnosed as hyperactive in childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 9, 17-30. doi: 10.1080/87565649309540541 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shaw-Zirt B., Popali-Lehane L., Chaplin W., Bergman A. (2005). Adjustment, social skills, and self-esteem in college students with symptoms of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8, 109-120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Silvia P. J., Wigert B., Reiter-Palmon R., Kaufman J. C. (2012). Assessing creativity with Self-Report Scales: A review and empirical evaluation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6, 19-34. [Google Scholar]
  41. Thrash T. M., Maruskin L. A., Cassidy S. E., Fryer J. W., Ryan R. M. (2010). Mediating between the muse and the masses: Inspiration and the actualization of creative ideas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 469-487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Toner M., O’Donoghue T., Houghton S. (2006). Living in chaos and striving for control: How adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder deal with their disorder. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53, 247-261. [Google Scholar]
  43. Toplak M. E., Jain U., Tannock R. (2005). Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1, Article 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Verheul I., Block J., Burmeister-Lamp K., Thurik R., Tiemeier H., Turturea R. (2015). ADHD-like behavior and entrepreneurial intentions. Small Business Economics, 45, 85-101. [Google Scholar]
  45. Verheul I., Rietdijk W., Block J., Franken I., Larsson H., Thurik R. (2016). The association between attention-deficit/hyperactivity (ADHD) symptoms and self-employment. European Journal of Epidemiology, 31, 793-801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Volkow N. D., Wang G.-J., Fowler J. S., Telang F., Maynard L., Logan J., . . . Swanson J. M. (2004). Evidence that methylphenidate enhances the saliency of a mathematical task by increasing dopamine in the human brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1173-1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Volkow N. D., Wang G.-J., Newcorn J. H., Kollins S. H., Wigal T. L., Telang F., . . . Swanson J. M. (2011). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Molecular Psychiatry, 16, 1147-1154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. White H. A., Shah P. (2006). Uninhibited imaginations: Creativity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1121-1131. [Google Scholar]
  49. White H. A., Shah P. (2011). Creative style and achievement in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 673-677. [Google Scholar]
  50. White H. A., Shah P. (2016). Scope of semantic activation and innovative thinking in college students with ADHD. Creativity Research Journal, 28, 275-282. [Google Scholar]
  51. Wilmshurst L., Peele M., Wilmshurst L. (2011). Resilience and well-being in college students with and without a diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 11-17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zabelina D. L., Condon D., Beeman M. (2014). Do dimensional psychopathology measures relate to creative achievement or divergent thinking? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Attention Disorders are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES