Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2020 Apr 9;28(2):284–307. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2020.1749228

Differences in cognitive performance between informal caregivers and non-caregivers

Francesca Falzarano 1, Karen L Siedlecki 1
PMCID: PMC7544647  NIHMSID: NIHMS1594152  PMID: 32270735

Abstract

Extensive literature exists documenting the relationship between stress and cognition. Caregiving for an individual with Alzheimer’s disease can be aunique and chronic stress experience due to the increasing dependency of the care-recipient as the disease progresses. The current study examines the relationship between stress and cognitive performance in 47 dementia caregivers compared to 47 noncaregiver control participants matched on age, gender, and education. Participants completed measures assessing stress (measured via the Perceived Stress Scale) and seven domains of cognition including episodic memory, working memory, executive functioning, attention, visuospatial processing, processing speed, and implicit memory. Results showed that caregivers had poorer performance than non-caregivers on certain measures of episodic memory, working memory, and executive functioning; while no significant differences were observed on measures of attention, visuospatial processing, processing speed, or implicit memory. In addition, when controlling for general stress, caregiver performance on measures of processing speed and visuospatial processing was also poorer than non-caregivers. By controlling for levels of general stress that may not be related to caregiving, these results show that differences in cognitive performance are unlikely to be explained by general stress alone.

Keywords: Caregivers, cognition, stress, Alzheimer’s disease, dementia


Informal caregivers of persons living with chronic illnesses and disabilities experience circumstances considered to be inherently stressful in a role that may be prolonged over a period of years. Caregivers of individuals living with Alzheimer’s disease (AD)/dementia face particularly unique challenges due to the progressive nature of the illness in which the care-recipient loses the ability to communicate, recognize loved ones, and provide basic care for oneself. Caregivers arguably are chronically stressed and given the amount of unpaid health services they provide to a growing population of persons with dementia, it is important to determine the adverse impacts that caregivers may experience as a result of stress. Caregivers do not only provide assistance with activities of daily living, such as bathing and transferring, they are also tasked with navigating the health care system, making medical decisions, and facilitating communication with formal care providers (Wolff et al., 2016). Moreover, caregivers simultaneously experience the loss of a relationship with a loved one, hence why they are often referred to as the concealed victims of the disease (Brodaty & Donkin, 2009; Clyburn et al., 2000; Zarit et al., 1985). Because informal caregivers provide approximately 17.7 billion hours of unpaid care services worth an estimated 220.2 USD billion (Alzheimer’s Association, 2014), a value that greatly exceeds the national spending for home health care, examining caregiver functioning in the context of stress is a pertinent issue not only at the individual level, but at the societal level as well. The stress associated with caregiving can negatively impact a caregiver’s health, well-being, and quality of life. Additionally, any interference with a caregiver’s ability to provide effective support and care would create additional strain on the formal health care system at a crucial time when demand continues to increase (Gillespie et al., 2015). Furthermore, because the caregiving role requires complex organization and problem-solving skills, impaired cognitive functioning may impact the quality of care and safety of the care-recipient (Willis et al., 1992).

Substantial research exists that demonstrates the impact of stress on cognitive functioning (e.g., Arnsten, 2009; Bremner, 1999; Lupien et al., 2009; McEwen, 1998; McEwen & Magarinos, 1997; McEwen & Sapolsky, 1995). In particular, research has shown that chronic stress results in the over secretion of glucocorticoids, such as cortisol, and can potentially have long-lasting effects on the brain and body, such as impaired immune functioning, cardiovascular disease, and accelerated cognitive decline (Cacioppo et al., 1998; S. Lupien et al., 1994; S. J. Lupien et al., 2007, 2009; McEwen, 1998). In particular, studies have found that cumulative exposure to high levels of glucocorticoids result in damage to the hippocampus (Gianaros et al., 2007; S. J. Lupien et al., 2009, 2018; Marshall et al., 2015) and that greater levels of stress have been associated with smaller hippocampal volume (Zimmerman et al., 2016)

S. Lupien et al. (1994) found a relationship between high levels of cortisol in the hippocampus and impaired declarative memory. Group differences, however, were not found on non-declarative implicit memory measures. There is evidence to suggest that a double dissociation exists between the implicit and explicit memory systems, such that implicit memory assessments are not dependent on the medial temporal regions of the brain (e.g., Henson, 2003; Preston & Gabrieli, 2008; Schacter & Buckner, 1998; Voss & Paller, 2008). Research also suggests that there is a strong connection between the hippocampus and the frontal lobe (Preston & Eichenbaum, 2013; Thierry et al., 2000; Vertes, 2006) and that damage to the hippocampus can lead to downstream effects on other brain regions, along with the cognitive functions they support, such as working memory or processing speed (Cabeza et al., 2004; Nichols et al., 2006). This may indicate that stress can impact performance not only on purely hippocampal-dependent tasks, but also on tasks that are associated with the recruitment of the hippocampus.

Unlike the vast research available on the physical and psychological effects of informal caregiving, much less is known about the relationship between stress and cognitive functioning in caregivers. Mild stress has been shown to be associated with everyday memory failures, such as misplacing one’s keys or forgetting to take medication (Mahoney et al., 1998). Chronic stress and caregiver burnout have been linked to impairments in processing speed, episodic memory, learning, and attention (Caswell et al., 2003; Mackenzie et al., 2007, 2009; Oken et al., 2011).

Mackenzie et al. (2007) examined cognitive functioning in 27 caregivers of palliative patients and found that caregivers exhibited impairments in attention regulation and learning/memory, however, no deficits were observed in working memory. In a follow-up study, Mackenzie et al. (2009) found that caregivers reported clinically significant levels of distress and burden and had poorer performance on measures of learning and episodic/working memory compared to noncaregiver control participants.

Vitaliano et al. (2005) found that spousal caregivers of individuals with dementia showed a significant decline in vocabulary ability compared to controls, with differences more pronounced among caregivers who reported greater hostility with their care recipient and possessed higher metabolic risk factors. Moreover, spousal caregivers, who are usually close in age to the care-recipient and may already process information at a slower rate due to the normal aging process, are also at risk for accelerated age-related cognitive decline (Caswell et al., 2003). Similarly, Caswell et al. (2003) found that caregivers of spouses with AD performed significantly worse on measures of processing speed and attention (measured via the digit symbol task) compared to age-matched spousal controls. As part of the same study, Vitaliano et al. (2009) longitudinally assessed digit symbol performance and found that individuals who provided care for an average of 44 months at Time 1 had a mean digit symbol score that was roughly 3.30 points lower than noncaregivers (d =.29), and caregivers scored approximately 4.50 points lower than controls at Time 3 (d = .38). De Vugt et al. (2006) also noted deficits in digit symbol and delayed recall performance. Likewise, in a telephone-based study using the Nurses’ Health Study Cohort, Lee et al. (2004) found significant impairments in both immediate and delayed recall, verbal fluency, and digit-span backwards in female caregivers compared to noncaregivers.

In a cross-sectional study, Oken et al. (2011) investigated which aspects of cognition were most impacted by caregiver-related stress in a sample of 31 caregivers between the ages of 45–85, compared to 25 age-matched controls. Differences were observed between caregivers and noncaregivers on reaction time, attention, and executive functioning, but no differences were found on verbal memory performance. It is not entirely clear why these results are inconsistent with other research that has identified a link between stress and memory performance, however it may be necessary to include multiple measures of memory to determine which types of memory are most impacted by chronic stress.

Many explanations have been proposed to explain why long-term caregivers may have poorer cognitive performance than non-caregivers. Although the process of caregiving for an individual with AD may be a chronic stress experience, considerable individual differences exist. The discrepancy between the presence of stressful events and how people react to them is a fundamental principle underlying the stress process model (Haley et al., 1987; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Mittelman et al., 2004; Pearlin et al., 1990), a common theoretical framework used in caregiver research, which attempts to explain the various paths by which stress can differentially impact individuals. Within the model, caregiver stress is divided into four domains (in addition to the caregiver’s background characteristics): primary stressors (e.g., stressors that arise directly from the care environment, such a problematic behaviors of the care-recipient and caregiver overload), secondary stressors (stressors that arise in domains outside of the care environment, such as role strains), mediators, and outcomes. Vitaliano et al. (2009) posit that as AD progresses and communication deficits in the care-recipient worsen, in conjunction with the caregiver’s increasing isolation from normal social activities, the caregiver’s environment can become less intellectually stimulating, which may further exacerbate cognitive decline. Moreover, this can also lead to depressive symptoms, which may serve as a mediator between caregiver status and poorer cognitive performance.

Despite evidence supporting the relationship between caregiving and poorer cognitive functioning, another line of research has demonstrated support for the healthy caregiver hypothesis, arguing that caregivers may have better outcomes compared to noncaregivers because healthier older adults are more likely to become and remain caregivers (Bertrand et al., 2012). High-intensity caregivers are believed to engage in more physical activity (e.g., bathing, assistance with mobility and transport) as part of their role (Bertrand et al., 2012; Fredman et al., 2008) and research has found that caregivers had lower rates of mortality and functional impairment compared to noncaregiver control participants (e.g., Fredman et al., 2010; O’Reilly et al., 2008; Roth et al., 2015). To investigate this in the context of cognition, Bertrand et al. (2012) tested performance on verbal learning and processing speed in 5,592 women over 65 who assisted a relative or friend with at least one ADL and/or IADL and found that continuous caregivers exhibited better cognitive performance than noncaregivers. However, the participants did not specify what their care responsibilities consisted of and whether they were primary, full-time caregivers. The participants were also required to assist with at least one ADL/IADL, which may suggest that the care-recipient could have maintained a significant level of functional independence. Furthermore, Pertl et al. (2015) used data from the Health and Retirement Study (HRS) to examine whether having a spouse with dementia was associated with poorer cognitive functioning. Although no differences were identified between caregivers and controls on any of the cognitive domains assessed, the HRS does not explicitly examine caregiving. Instead, it was presumed that participants who have (and live with) a spouse with dementia were caregivers, although it was unknown how much care the participants provided. A more recent study argued that sampling bias may overestimate the negative effects of caregiver stress that prior literature has identified. O’Sullivan et al. (2019) compared caregivers’ cognitive performance to two demographically matched control samples and found that caregivers and noncaregivers exhibited comparable performance on multiple cognitive tests, with caregivers even outperforming control participants on tests of processing speed and reaction time despite scoring higher on measures of stress and depression. Overall, however, the evidence regarding cognitive performance in caregivers is mixed.

With the changing demographic of society and an increase in older adults developing age-related pathological illnesses, it is important to investigate the effects that diseases such as AD can have on the functioning of caregivers. Although research has found that caregivers exhibit worse cognitive performance compared to control participants, previous work examining cognition in caregivers has focused on a limited number of cognitive variables. Thus, the aim of the current study is to extend prior work by examining performance in caregivers and noncaregiver control participants across seven domains of cognition.

Method

Participants

The current study included a sample of 47 individuals who served as primary, informal caregivers to an individual with AD/dementia and 53 control participants, 47 of which were matched on age, education, and gender to caregiver participants to form the final sample. Control participants and caregivers were recruited via Research Match, an online research registry, and CraigsList.com. Caregivers were also recruited through flyers and announcements made at caregiver support groups. Study information was also distributed at an adult day center and via certified home health aides at a large skilled nursing facility in New York City. Fordham University’s Institutional Review Board approved all study protocol and procedures.

Inclusion criteria

Participants in the caregiver group were required to be the primary caregiver of an individual with AD/dementia still living in the community and spoke English as their primary language. Because age is an important predictor of cognition (e.g., Murman, 2015), participants under the age of 40 were not eligible to participate. Participants were excluded if the care-recipient resided in an assisted living or long-term care facility. Participants in the control group were matched to caregivers based on age, gender, and education. Control participants did not have (or previously have) a caregiver role for a disabled/ill family or friend and did not provide care for an individual in a nursing home (see Appendix A for an overview of measures completed by control participants). For both groups, the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein etal., 1975), an assessment of global cognitive functioning, was administered as a screening measure. There were no participants who exhibited cognitive impairment in the current sample (e.g., by obtaining a score of less than 24; Tombaugh & McIntyre, 1992).

Measures

Caregiver-specific measures

Data used in the current analysis are part of a larger study examining caregiving and cognition in the context of Pearlin et al.’s (1990) stress process model. Participants in the caregiving group responded to questions inquiring about caregiving history (e.g., length of time the person has been a caregiver), relationship to the care-recipient, hours of care provided per week, and use of/frequency of home health services. Based on the stress process model, caregivers were also administered questionnaires pertaining to primary stressors (e.g., health, behavior, and functional capacity of the care-recipient, caregiver overload, relational deprivation) and secondary stressors (e.g., family conflict, occupational and financial strain, role captivity, and loss of self). See Appendix B for full list of measures.

Perceived stress

The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen et al., 1983) is a 10-item general stress measure that assesses the degree to which individuals appraise situations as uncontrollable, unpredictable, and overwhelming over the past month. Sample items included: “In the last month, how often have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?” and “In the last month, how often have you felt confident about your ability to handle personal problems?” Participants responded to each statement on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (very often). In the current study, Cronbach’s α = .90 in the caregiver subsample and α = .91 in the control subsample. In the overall sample, Cronbach’s α = .90.

Cognitive outcomes

Working memory

The n-back test is a measure of working memory that was administered via PsyToolKit (Stoet, 2010, 2017), a psychological experiment software. In this task, participants were presented with a sequence of letters one at a time. As each letter appeared, the participant was asked to determine whether the stimulus shown is the same one that was presented n trials ago. In the first portion of the task, participants determined whether the target presented was the same as the one that was shown two letters prior (2-back task). The second portion of the task (3-back task) required the participant to discern whether the letter shown was the same as the target presented three letters prior. Twenty trials were administered in total for each part. Performance was assessed via reaction times (RTs), accuracy for each trial, and percentage of wrongly reported n-back items.

Episodic memory

Episodic memory was assessed via the Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition (WMS-III) word recall task (Wechsler, 1997). Participants were read a list of 12 unrelated words and were given 90 seconds to recall the words in any order. The same procedure was repeated with the same list of words three additional times. After four trials were completed, participants were then read a list of new words and were asked to recall the words in any order. The final trial instructed participants to recall as many words from the first list. The task was scored by summing the number of words correctly recalled on trials 1–4 (with higher numbers indicating better recall). Number of intrusions across trials 1–4 were also summed to form an overall score.

Attention

The Stroop Color-Word Interference Test was administered online via PsyToolKit (Stoet, 2010, 2017) to assess attention (e.g., Dunsky et al., 2017; Fahimi et al., 2018; MacLeod, 1992; McGuinness et al., 2010; Melara & Algom, 2003). In this task, participants were presented with a series of words printed in four colors and were asked to indicate the color of the ink in which the word is printed. Participants completed 100 trials (25 trials for each color) of congruent and incongruent targets. Performance was assessed via an interference score, which is the difference in the amount of time taken to correctly respond to congruent versus incongruent stimuli. According to Siegrest (1995), the Stroop Color-Word Interference Test exhibits high reliability (.88).

Processing speed

Processing speed was assessed via three measures: pattern comparison, letter comparison, and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) digit symbol task (Wechsler, 2008). Task administration of pattern and letter comparison followed the format as described in Salthouse and Babcock (1991). Participants were given two sheets of paper and were given 30 seconds per page to determine whether two patterns of lines were the same or different, with a total of 60 trials; participants were instructed to write the letter “S” if the two pairs were the same or a “D” if the two were different. The letter comparison task was then administered, and participants determined whether two strings of three, six, or nine letters were the same or different. As in pattern comparison, the letter “S” was used to indicate that the two pairs are the same or a “D” if they were different. Both comparison tasks were scored based on the number of correct responses minus the number of incorrect responses within the 30-second time limit.

In the WAIS-IV digit symbol task, participants were given a sheet of paper with a key located at the top. The key included numbers (1–9) with corresponding symbols. Participants were instructed to fill in the correct symbol below each digit within a −90 second time limit. The digit symbol task was scored by summing the number of correct items, with higher scores indicating better performance.

Executive functioning

The connections task (Salthouse et al., 2000), which is a variant of the trail making task, was comprised of two parts with two conditions each. Participants were given a sheet of a paper with 49 circles and were given 20 seconds to complete each condition. In the first condition of part A, the circles on the page were numbered from 1–49, and participants were instructed to draw lines to connect the numbers in ascending order. In condition 2 of part A, the task requirements were similar, and participants were presented with a sheet of letters and were instructed to connect letters in alphabetical order. In part B, participants were asked to connect alternating numbers and letters from lowest to highest (e.g., 1, A, 2, B, 3, C). The final condition of part B required participants to alternate between letters and numbers (e.g., A, 1, B, 2, C, 3). Participants’ scores were assessed by subtracting the number of correct connections minus the number of incorrect connections. Scores from both conditions in part A and both conditions in part B were averaged to form two composite variables, the ratio of conditions B/A was then calculated.

Visuospatial processing

Visuospatial processing was assessed using the mental rotation task (Shephard & Metzler, 1971) administered via PsyToolKit (Stoet, 2010, 2017). In this task, which consisted of 24 trials, three 3-dimensional stimuli were presented in each trial. A gray stimulus was presented at the top of the screen, and participants were asked to imagine what the image would look like if it was rotated. The angle of rotation of the image was increased for each trial (e.g., 0, 60,120, and 180 degrees). Two options were given, and participants were asked to choose the stimulus that matches the sample in gray. Performance was assessed via reaction time (change in reaction time from the shortest to the longest angle), as well as with a percent correct score, with higher scores indicating better performance.

Implicit memory

Implicit memory was assessed using the word-stem completion task, which is a measure of perceptual priming and incidental learning. This measure followed the format used by Roediger et al. (1992). In phase I (study phase), participants were presented with a list of 68 words and were instructed to read and construct a visual image of each word. Then, participants were asked to rate the pleasantness of each image on a scale from 0 (extremely unpleasant) to 7 (extremely pleasant). After all questionnaires and cognitive tasks were completed, participants were administered phase II (test phase) of the task and were presented with 34 word-stem puzzles. Each puzzle contained the first three letters of a possible word (with five possible completions) and participants were instructed to fill in the word-stems with the first word that comes to mind. Participants were not informed of any relation between the test phase and the study phase that occurred at the beginning of the study.

Procedure

The study was conducted at Fordham University as a part of a larger study. Testing sessions took approximately 90 minutes, in which participants were asked to complete questionnaires and a series of tasks measuring the cognitive abilities described above. For a full description of the tasks and questionnaires administered, please see Appendices A and B. All participants were compensated with a gift card to Amazon.com.

Data analysis plan

Prior to conducting data analyses, all variables were examined for normality and homogeneity. In terms of descriptive statistics, means and standard deviations or percentages were calculated for all demographic, cognitive, and questionnaire measures. Propensity score matching was used to match caregiver and control participants based on age, education, and gender. Mean group comparisons between control and caregiving groups were analyzed via t-tests and ANCOVAs.

Results

Propensity score matching

To ensure that groups could be compared at baseline, propensity score matching using the Propensity Score Matching R plug-in for SPSS Statistics 25.0 was used to match caregiver to control participants on age, education, and gender (Thoemmes, 2012). One-to-one nearest neighbor matching without replacement was used to match 47 controls to 47 caregiver participants. Based on prior literature, logistic regression and an a priori caliper of .25 standard deviations (d = .25) of the logit transformation of the propensity score was used to match participants (Stuart, 2010; Stuart & Rubin, 2008; Thoemmes & Kim, 2011). In order to determine the effectiveness of the propensity scores, the matched cases were assessed by examining two multivariate tests of covariate balance to assess group differences. Balance is assumed to be achieved when there are no significant differences between groups. The linear combination of variables using a X2 distribution (Hansen & Bowers, 2008) were examined and showed that the covariates were well-balanced due to its nonsignificant result (X2 (3) = .19, p = .98). Based on the standards proposed by Iacus et al. (2011), the second test compared the L statistic, a test that assesses covariate balance while including interaction effects. According to Thoemmes (2012), the L statistic should produce a value smaller in the matched sample than the unmatched sample. In the current analysis, the L statistic was reduced from an initial pre-matching value of .25 to .22, which suggests that the covariate balance had been improved following matching. Additionally, no covariates in the dataset displayed a large imbalance (> d = 25). Finally, standardized group mean differences on the propensity score were examined before (d = .07) and after matching (d = .01). According to Rubin (2001), standardized mean differences in propensity scores should be close to zero (d < .20). Based on these guidelines, the results of the current propensity score analyses indicate well-matched groups.

Descriptive statistics

Sociodemographic variables

The overall sample consisted of 94 participants between the ages of 40–71 (M = 52.94, SD = 6.66). The mean age of the caregiving group was 53.00 years (SD = 6.26), and the control group had a mean age of 52.87 years (SD = 7.11). Within each group, 46.8% (n = 22) of the sample consisted of males and 53.2% (n = 25) were females. Independent samples t-tests were performed to assess differences between caregiver and control participants on age, gender, education, race/ethnicity, marital status, number of children and grandchildren, along with scores on the Perceived Stress Scale, and there were no significant differences between caregiver and control participants.

Demographic characteristics for caregiver participants

The caregiver sample identified as predominantly White (51.10%). Demographic information for caregiver participants is presented in Table 1. All caregiver participants reported residing with the care-recipient. Participants reported a mean of 4.77 (SD = 4.05) years spent in the caregiving role and provided an average of 65.82 (SD = 54.87) hours of care per week. Most of the caregiver participants reported being a child of the care recipient (85.1%, n = 40), while 7 participants (14.9%) reported being spousal caregivers. An independent samples t-test was conducted to determine if differences exist between child and spousal caregivers. No differences emerged on any measures.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics for caregivers (n = 47) and controls (n = 47).

Caregivers
Controls
Sociodemographic Variable % M SD % M SD
Sex (Male) 46.80 46.80
 Race/Ethnicity
 Asian 2.10 2.10
 Black or African American 27.70 27.70
 Hispanic or Latino 14.90 4.30
 White 51.10 66.00
 More than one 4.60 0.00
Highest Level of Education
 Less than a High School Degree 2.10 2.10
 High School Degree or Equivalent 14.90 14.90
 Associate Degree 12.80 12.80
 Bachelor’s Degree 36.20 36.20
 Graduate Degree 27.70 27.70
 Other Advanced Degree 6.40 6.40
Subjective Health 1.72 0.54 1.57 0.61
Marital Status
 Married 34.00 44.70
 Widowed 0.00 0.00
 Divorced 8.50 6.40
 Separated 4.30 0.00
 Not Married, but in a committed relationship 14.90 6.40
 Single 38.30 42.60
Number of Children 0.87 1.03 1.06 1.39
Number of Grandchildren 0.21 1.03 0.29 0.83
Employment
 Employed Full-Time 44.70 34.00
 Employed Part-Time 27.70 29.80
 Unemployed 8.50 12.80
 Retired 8.50 17.00
 Other 10.60 6.40
Perceived Stress Scale 21.00 7.50 18.42 7.47
Length of Time as a Caregiver 4.77 4.05 n/a
 One year 23.40
 Two Years 14.90
 Three Years 12.80
 Four Years 10.60
 Five Years 6.40
 Six Years 6.40
 Seven Years 4.30
 Eight Years 6.40
 Ten Years 2.10
 Twelve Years 6.40
 Fourteen Years 2.10
 Fifteen Years 4.30
Hours Spent Caregiving Per Week 65.82 54.87 n/a
Use of Home Health Aide (1 = yes) 38.30 n/a
 Home Health Aide Hours Per Week 21.52 41.28
Receives Medicaid Compensation for Caregiving 12.80 n/a

Additionally, 38.30% (n = 18) of caregivers reported having a home health aide to assist with caregiving responsibilities. Of the caregivers who reported having a home health aide, the care recipients received approximately 45.35 (SD = 50.84) hours of home health care per week, with care ranging from one hour a week to 168 hours a week (24 hours a day, seven days a week). Independent samples t-tests were conducted to examine differences between caregivers of individuals who receive home health services and those who do not. The only significant finding was that caregivers of individuals with home health services reported worse care-recipient cognitive status compared to those who did not receive home health care (t(45) = 2.38, p < .05).

Demographic characteristics for control participants

Demographic information for control participants is presented in Table 1. Thirty-one (66%) participants identified as White. The remaining racial/ethnic background of the control participants consisted of thirteen (27.70%) Black or African American participants, two (4.30%) Hispanic/Latino participants, and one Asian (2.1%) participant.

Comparing caregiver and control participants

Table 2 summarizes the results of the independent samples t-tests used to examine differences between caregiver and control participants on cognitive performance. Results show that caregivers performed significantly worse than control participants on a number of cognitive tests. In the 2-back condition of the n-back test, caregivers had a fewer number of correctly identified items compared to controls. In the 3-back condition, caregivers had a higher percentage of wrongly reported items, a lower number of correctly identified items, and a slower reaction time compared to controls.

Table 2.

Independent samples t-test results comparing caregivers and controls on cognitive outcomes (n = 94).

Caregivers
Control participants
M SD M SD t-test Cohen’s d
Working Memory
 N-Back: 2-Back Condition
  Percentage of items wrongly reported 1.58 0.29 1.25 0.33 4.67*** 1.06
  Number of items correctly identified 0.93 0.31 1.17 0.17 −4.53*** −.96
  Average reaction time 1403.92 334.67 1118.80 297.36 4.37 .90
 N-Back: 3-Back Condition
  Percentage of items wrongly reported 1.40 0.97 1.21 0.30 2.63** .26
  Number of items correctly identified 1.05 0.22 1.18 0.09 3.71*** −.77
  Average reaction time 1188.55 337.26 963.60 244.48 −3.70*** .76
Episodic Memory
 WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest (Sum of Trials 1–4) 29.25 6.56 35.51 4.51 −5.39*** −1.11
 WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest Intrusions (Sum of Trials 1–4) 0.27 0.24 0.22 0.28 0.62 .19
Attention
 Stroop Test 2.12 0.24 2.01 0.37 1.71 .35
Processing Speed
 Pattern Comparison 34.17 8.59 43.45 7.08 −5.71 −1.17
 Letter Comparison 23.25 6.01 29.55 5.33 −5.37 −1.11
 Digit Symbol 54.76 12.38 66.23 10.65 −4.81 −.99
Executive Functioning
 Connections 0.52 0.17 0.59 0.12 −2.18*** −.47
Visuospatial Processing
 Mental Rotation: Percent correct 79.61 7.95 83.32 6.63 −2.45 −.50
 Mental Rotation: Average reaction time 3.55 0.17 3.52 0.17 0.86 .17
Implicit Memory
 Word-Stem Completion Test 10.46 3.78 11.17 4.06 −0.87 −.18
*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

As seen in Table 2, caregivers also performed significantly worse on the WMS-III word recall subtest and the connections task. No significant group differences were identified on any of the processing speed tasks (pattern comparison, letter comparison, and digit symbol; ps = .90, .23, and .18, respectively), attention (stroop test; p = .20), visuospatial processing (mental rotation; p = .38), and implicit memory (word-stem completion test; p = .39). Although group differences were not significant, almost all of the relationships were in the direction hypothesized, with the exception of the stroop task – in which caregiver participants scored slightly higher, although the effect size was small (d = .35). Alternatively, for the three processing speed tasks measured via the pattern comparison, letter comparison, and digit symbol tasks, control participants exhibited higher mean scores with large effect sizes (ds = −1.17, −1.11, −.99, respectively). Control participants also scored higher on the mental rotation (d = −.50) and word-stem completion tasks (d = −.18), although the effect sizes were not large.

Analyses of covariates

To determine group differences while controlling for perceived stress, a series of ANCOVAs were conducted for each cognitive variable (See Table 3).

Table 3.

ANCOVA results controlling for perceived stress (n = 94).

Caregivers
Control participants
Measure M Adjusted M SD M Adjusted M SD F p Partial Eta Squared d
Working Memory
N-Back: 2-Back Condition
Percentage of items wrongly reported 1.57 1.57 0.29 1.25 1.25 0.35 20.12 0.00  0.21 .99
 Perceived Stress 0.22 0.64  0.003
Number of items correctly identified 0.93 0.93 0.31 1.17 1.17 0.17 19.14 0.00  0.18 −.95
 Perceived Stress 0.06 0.80  0.001
Average reaction time 1403.92 1413.39 334.67 1118.80 1109.33 297.36 21.46 0.00  0.19 .96
 Perceived Stress 2.84 0.09  0.03
N-Back: 3-Back Condition
Percentage of items wrongly reported 1.40 1.40 0.37 1.21 1.22 0.30 6.38 0.01  0.07 .53
 Perceived Stress 0.13 0.72  0.001
Number of items correctly identified 1.06 1.06 0.22 1.18 1.18 0.09 12.11 0.001  0.12 −.71
 Perceived Stress 1.15 0.29  0.01
Average reaction time 1188.55 1192.00 337.26 963.60 960.15 244.48 14.05 0.00  0.13 .78
 Perceived Stress 0.42 0.52  0.005
Episodic Memory
WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest (Sum of Trials 1–4) 29.26 29.10 6.56 35.51 35.70 4.51 31.57 0.00  0.26 −1.17
 Perceived Stress 2.46 0.12  0.03
WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest Intrusions (Sum of Trials 1–4) 0.27 0.27 0.24 0.22 0.22 0.28 0.37 0.55  0.01 .19
 Perceived Stress 0.002 0.97  0.00
Attention
Stroop Test 2.13 2.12 0.24 2.01 2.02 0.37 2.41 0.12  0.03 .32
 Perceived Stress 0.34 0.56  0.004
Processing Speed
Pattern Comparison 34.17 34.33 8.59 43.45 43.30 7.09 29.60 0.00  0.25 −1.13
 Perceived Stress 1.30 0.26  0.01
Letter Comparison 23.26 23.23 6.01 29.55 29.60 5.33 28.15 0.00  0.24 −1.12
 Perceived Stress 0.05 0.82  0.001
Digit Symbol 54.80 54.82 12.39 66.23 66.17 10.66 21.79 0.00  0.19 −.98
 Perceived Stress 0.08 0.77  0.001
Executive Functioning
Connections 0.53 0.53 0.17 0.60 0.60 0.12 4.70 0.03  0.05 −.47
 Perceived Stress 0.03 0.85  0.00
Visuospatial Processing
Mental Rotation: Percent Correct 79.62 79.55 7.95 83.32 83.45 6.63 6.20 0.01  0.06 −.53
Perceived Stress 0.23 0.63  0.003
Mental Rotation: Average reaction time 3.55 3.55 0.17 3.52 3.51 0.17 1.32 0.25  0.01 .23
 Perceived Stress 3.00 0.09  0.03
Implicit Memory
Word-Stem Completion Test 10.47 10.42 3.79 11.17 11.22 4.06 0.93 0.34  0.01 −.20
Corrected Model 0.58 0.56  0.01
 Perceived Stress 0.42 0.52  0.01

d = Cohen’s d. Bolded p-values indicate significance.

The results of the ANCOVAs yielded consistent findings with those reported from the t-tests above. Furthermore, after controlling for perceived stress, additional significant findings emerged. Although average reaction time on the 2-back test was not significant before controlling for perceived stress as covariate, significant differences emerged such that caregivers had a longer reaction time compared to controls, however, perceived stress was not a statistically significant predictor. When examining processing speed between caregiver and control participants, significant differences emerged only after controlling for perceived stress. For the pattern comparison, letter comparison, and digit symbol tasks, there were significant group differences with caregivers performing worse compared to control participants. Group differences were also identified on the mental rotation task, which assesses visuospatial processing with caregivers scoring lower than controls. Group differences also emerged on the Stroop test. However, caregivers scored higher than controls on this measure. There were no significant differences that emerged for average reaction time on the mental rotation task and performance on the word-stem completion test after controlling for perceived stress.

Discussion

In the current study, as expected, control participants exhibited better performance across multiple cognitive domains, such as working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning. In addition, analyses showed consistent results with what was found in the original t-test analyses, although additional significant differences emerged after controlling for perceived stress. Caregivers performed worse on both conditions of the working memory task, as well as measures of processing speed, visuospatial processing, executive functioning, and episodic memory. Interestingly, caregiver and control participants did not differ on levels of stress as identified in previous studies (e.g., Oken et al., 2011). It is thus possible that the sample of caregivers included in the current study may have experienced less burden and stress compared to other populations of caregivers. Furthermore, by examining whether caregiver and control participants perform differently across various cognitive assessments, while controlling for levels of general stress, these results show that differences in performance cannot be explained by general stress alone, and that other factors that may perhaps be unique to the caregiving experience (e.g., sleep disruptions, decreased psychological well-being, lack of social support) may help to explain why caregivers performed more poorly.

These results are generally consistent with the little work that has been done on caregiving and cognitive functioning. It is unclear, however, why differences emerged on certain measures and not others. Mackenzie et al. (2007, 2009) found that caregivers exhibited poorer performance on measures of attention, learning, and episodic memory compared to a healthy normative sample, but, unlike the current study, they did not find differences in working memory performance. Furthermore, other studies have found that caregivers displayed poorer performance on processing speed, along with other measures such as verbal fluency, and short-term memory using the digit span backwards task, as compared to control participants (Caswell et al., 2003; De Vugt et al., 2006). The discrepancies between the current findings and prior research are only in terms of significance in the t-tests, and not effect sizes. The three measures of processing speed in the current study showed large effects (e.g., digit symbol performance yielded a Cohen’s d of −.98, pattern comparison yielded a Cohen’s d of −1.13, and letter comparison yielded a Cohen’s d of −1.12), such that control participants outperformed caregiver participants and differences in processing speed measures did, in fact, emerge after controlling for perceived stress.

Additionally, an important point to consider are the various tasks used to operationalize different cognitive functions. Mackenzie et al. (2007, 2009) used the Ruff 2 & 7 Selective Attention Task and the WMS-III Working Memory Index, while Caswell et al. (2003) used the digit symbol task to simultaneously examine attention and processing speed performance. Moreover, working memory incorporates executive functioning processes and differences were also identified between caregiver and control participants on the connections task, a measure of executive functioning, in the current study. Executive functioning performance is associated with the pathways between the hippocampus and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, while processing speed performance is mainly associated with the structural integrity of white matter tracts in the frontal, parietal, and temporal cortices (Eckert et al., 2010;Turken et al., 2008). Although processing speed is an essential component that contributes to higher order functions such as working memory, it is possible that no differences initially emerged in the processing speed domain due to the reduced complexity of the tasks compared to the n-backtest. However, although this is a possible explanation for the current findings, the large effect sizes evident in the current sample point to an underpowered sample as a potential reason for the lack of significant relationships.

Previous research supports the impact of chronic stress on performance on complex cognitive measures, such as episodic memory tasks, which is associated with the hippocampus, and may also compromise prefrontal cortex functioning and working memory performance (Arnsten, 2009; Cabeza et al., 2004; Leszczynski, 2011). Thus, it is not surprising that differences were observed primarily on working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning measures, with caregivers exhibiting worse performance on these measures. This is an important finding, and although examining the unique influences of caregiver stressors on cognition was beyond the scope of the current study, cognition in caregivers is an important topic that warrants further research given the complexity of responsibilities required to effectively carry out the caregiving role.

Limitations and future directions

Several limitations must be considered in interpreting study findings. Most notably, the sample size (n = 47 for each group) may have limited the study’s power. Furthermore, cognitive functioning was assessed cross-sectionally, which does not allow for conclusions regarding how caregiving is associated with changes in cognition over time. Additionally, the conclusions drawn in the current study are limited by the characteristics of the caregiving sample. Data were only collected on whether caregivers were caring for an individual with Alzheimer’s/dementia and not on specific type of dementia and thus, we were not able to determine whether cognitive performance varies across different types of dementia caregivers. Given the difficulty in recruiting caregivers to participate in research, the individuals who participated in the current study can be considered a specialized group of caregivers. That is, these caregivers are more likely to seek out research opportunities, thus this sample may not be representative of all dementia caregivers. Additionally, given that stress did not differ between caregiver and control participants, it is also possible that the current findings, along with caregiver levels of stress, could be overestimated as noted above, or perhaps underestimated compared to caregivers who do not utilize formal services or seek out resources. An additional limitation is that participants also exhibited better performance on the 3-back version of the n-back test compared to the 2-back condition. This is an unexpected finding, given that the 3-back test is more difficult and places greater constraints on working memory. It is possible that practice effects influenced n-back performance, such that participants gained increased familiarity with the measure and had a better understanding of the directions required, which is common in cognitive assessments (Goldberg et al., 2015). Future research should incorporate multiple measures of working memory to determine if the effects observed in the current study are specific to the n-back paradigm. Another limitation is the lack of adjustment of p-values to control for multiple comparisons in the analyses, which increases the risk of type I error; as a result, some of the significant findings reported may be due to chance. However, adjustments were not made in order to reduce type II errors, which are more common in studies with similarly small sample sizes. Not adjusting for multiple comparisons is a commonly used method to reduce type II error, due to the conservative nature of multiple comparison adjustments (Feise, 2002; Rothman, 1990). Additionally, adjustments are not needed if all comparisons are reported (Feise, 2002). Lastly, a majority of the cognitive assessments used in the current study do not have normative data to assess performance. Future research should include cognitive assessments that contain available normative data in order to determine if the differences observed between caregiver and control participants are clinically significant.

Conclusion

This study examined the differences between caregivers and noncaregivers on performance across seven domains of cognition. The results showed that caregivers performed significantly worse compared to control participants on multiple domains of cognition including working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning. Furthermore, by controlling for levels of general stress that may not be related to caregiving, these results show that differences in performance cannot be explained by general stress alone, and that other factors (e.g., well-being, health behaviors, and levels of social support) may help to explain why caregivers performed more poorly. With the increasing older adult population, and the subsequent increase in age-related pathological illnesses, the number of individuals providing care to a loved one will continue to increase, and additional research is needed to further examine the possible effects of caregiving to identify mechanisms to target in intervention and support efforts.

Acknowledgments

Funding

Dr. Falzarano acknowledges support in the development of this manuscript from an NIA-funded T32 training program grant (AG049666).

Appexdix

Appendix A.

Description of measures completed by control participants.

Domain Variable Description Source
Screening Mini Mental State Exam Brief cognitive screening assessing global cognition across five domains Folstein et al. (1975)
Depression Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale Self-report questionnaire assessing depressive symptoms within the past week Radloff (1977)
Social Support ENRICHD Social Support Inventory Questionnaire assessing perceived availability of emotional, informational, and practical support. Mitchell et al. (2003)
Coping Cope Inventory Self-report questionnaire asking participants to report how they cope in stressful situations Carver et al. (1989)
Perceived Stress Perceived Stress Scale Questionnaire assessing situational stress appraisal within the past month Cohen et al. (1983)
Working Memory N-back Test Decide whether presented letter is the same as 2- or 3- trials back Kirchner (1958), Stoet (2010, 2017)
Episodic Memory (immediate and delayed recall) WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest Number of words recalled across 5 trials of a word list Wechsler (1997)
Attention Stroop Color-Word Interference Test Respond to color of ink that word is printed in Stoet (2010, 2017)
Processing Speed WAIS-IV Digit Symbol Test Use a code legend to indicate the correct symbol underneath each digit Weschler (2008)
Processing Speed Pattern Comparison Make same/different judgments of pairs of line patterns Salthouse and Babcock (1991)
Processing Speed Letter Comparison Make same/different judgments of pairs of letter strings Salthouse and Babcock (1991)
Executive Functioning Connections Connect circles in order (e.g., numerical order, alphabetical order, alternating between numerical and alphabetical order, and alternating between alphabetical and numerical order) Salthouse et al. (2000)
Visuospatial Processing Mental Rotation Task Choose rotated stimulus that matches target stimulus Shephard and Metzler (1971), Stoet (2010, 2017)
Implicit Memory Word-Stem Completion Task Phase I (study phase): Read word-list, construct visual image of each word, and rate the pleasantness of the image. Phase II (test phase): fill in word-stems Roediger et al. (1992)

Appendix B.

Description of measures completed by caregiver participants.

Stress Domain (based on the stress process model; Pearlin et al., 1990) Variable Description Source

Background and Context Demographics Self-report items assessing age, gender, race, marital status, highest level of education, and socioeconomic status Pearlin et al. (1990)
Primary Stressors: Objective Cognitive Status Assesses cognitive impairment in care-recipient and their level of difficulty in carrying out specific tasks Pearlin et al. (1990)
Primary Stressors: Objective Problematic Behaviors Frequency of problematic behaviors exhibited by the care-recipient over the past week Pearlin et al. (1990)
Primary Stressors: Objective Katz Index of Independence in Activities of Daily Living (ADLs) Assesses care-recipients ability to carry out functions such as bathing, dressing, and feeding Katz et al. (1963)
Primary Stressors: Objective Lawton and Brody’s Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADLs) Scale Assesses care-recipients ability to carry out complex activities such as grocery shopping, food preparation, and financial management Lawton and Brody (1969)
Primary Stressors: Subjective Caregiver Overload Assesses caregivers’ energy level and amount of time it takes to complete daily activities Pearlin et al. (1990)
Primary Stressors: Subjective Relational Deprivation Assesses loss related to intimacy exchange and activity deprivation due to care-recipient’s illness Pearlin et al. (1990)
Secondary Stressors: Role Strain Family Conflict Assesses level of disagreement within the family due to the seriousness/safety of the care-recipient, family members’ attitudes toward the care-recipient, and family members’ attitudes toward the caregiver Pearlin et al. (1990)
Secondary Stressors: Role Strain Financial Strain Assessed via 3 items asking caregivers to report how their total household income has changed compared to prior to the onset of care, how their monthly expenses have changed since the onset of care, and how finances work out at the end of the month Pearlin et al. (1990)
Secondary Stressors: Role Strain Occupational Strain Assesses conflict in providing care and maintaining employment Pearlin et al. (1990)
Secondary Stressors: Role Strain Modified Caregiver Strain Index Assesses strain/burn related to caregiving responsibilities across five domains: employment, financial, physical, social, and time Thornton and Travis (2003)
Secondary Stressors: Intrapsychic Strain Role Captivity Assesses the extent to which caregivers feel trapped as a result of their role Pearlin et al. (1990)
Secondary Stressors: Intrapsychic Strain Loss of Self Assesses loss of identity due to caregiving role Pearlin et al. (1990)

Domain Variable Description Source

Depression Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale Self-report questionnaire assessing depressive symptoms within the past week Radloff (1977)
Social Support ENRICHD Social Support Inventory Questionnaire assessing perceived availability of emotional, informational, and practical support. Mitchell et al. (2003)
Coping Cope Inventory Self-report questionnaire asking participants to report how they cope in stressful situations Carver et al. (1989)
Perceived Stress Perceived Stress Scale Questionnaire assessing situational stress appraisal within the past month Cohen et al. (1983)
Working Memory N-back Test Decide whether presented letter is the same as 2- or 3- trials back Kirchner (1958), Stoet (2010, 2017)
Episodic Memory (immediate and delayed recall) WMS-III Word-Recall Subtest Number of words recalled across 5 trials of a word list Wechsler (1997)
Attention Stroop Color-Word Interference Test Respond to color of ink that word is printed in Stoet (2010, 2017)
Processing Speed WAIS-IV Digit Symbol Test Use a code legend to indicate the correct symbol underneath each digit Weschler (2008)
Processing Speed Pattern Comparison Make same/different judgments of pairs of line patterns Salthouse and Babcock (1991)
Processing Speed Letter Comparison Make same/different judgments of pairs of letter strings Salthouse and Babcock (1991)
Executive Functioning Connections Connect circles in order (e.g., numerical order, alphabetical order, alternating between numerical and alphabetical order, and alternating between alphabetical and numerical order) Salthouse et al. (2000)
Visuospatial Processing Mental Rotation Task Choose rotated stimulus that matches target stimulus Shephard and Metzler (1971), Stoet (2010, 2017)
Implicit Memory Word-Stem Completion Task Phase I (study phase): Read word-list, construct visual image of each word, and rate the pleasantness of the image. Phase II (test phase): fill in word-stems Roediger et al. (1991)

Footnotes

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  1. Alzheimer’s Association. (2014). 2014 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 10(2), e47–e92. 10.1016/jjalz.2014.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnsten AF (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410–422. 10.1038/nrn2648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bertrand RM, Saczynski JS, Mezzacappa C, Hulse M, Ensrud K, & Fredman L (2012). Caregiving and cognitive function in older women: Evidence for the healthy caregiver hypothesis. Journal of Aging and Health, 24(1), 48–66. 10.1177/0898264311421367 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bremner JD (1999). Does stress damage the brain? Biological Psychiatry, 45(7), 797–805. 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00009-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brodaty H, & Donkin M (2009). Family caregivers of people with dementia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 11(2), 217–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cabeza R, Daselaar SM, Dolcos F, Prince SE, Budde M, & Nyberg L (2004). Task-independent and task-specific age effects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention and episodic retrieval. Cerebral Cortex, 14(4), 364–375. 10.1093/cercor/bhg133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cacioppo JT, Poehlmann KM, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Malarkey WB, Burleson MH, Berntson GG, & Glaser R (1998). Cellular immune responses to acute stress in female caregivers of dementia patients and matched controls. Health Psychology, 17(2), 182–189. 10.1037/0278-6133.17.2.182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carver CS, Scheier MF, & Weintraub JK (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(2), 267–283. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Caswell LW, Vitaliano PP, Croyle KL, Scanlan JM, Zhang J, & Daruwala A (2003). Negative associations of chronic stress and cognitive performance in older adult spouse caregivers. Experimental Aging Research, 29(3), 303–318. 10.1080/03610730303721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clyburn LD, Stones MJ, Hadjistavropoulos T, & Tuokko H (2000). Predicting caregiver burden and depression in Alzheimer’s disease. Journals of Gerontology Series B, 55(1), S2–S13. 10.1093/geronb/55.1.S2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cohen S, Kamarck T, & Mermelstein R (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396. 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. De Vugt ME, Jolles J, Van Osch L, Stevens F, Aalten P, Lousberg R, & Verhey FR (2006). Cognitive functioning in spousal caregivers of dementia patients: Findings from the prospective MAASBED study. Age and Ageing, 35(2), 160–166. 10.1093/ageing/afj044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dunsky A, Abu-Rukun M, Tsuk S, Dwolatzky T, Carasso R, & Netz Y (2017). The effects of a resistance vs. an aerobic single session on attention and executive functioning in adults. PloS One, 12(4), e0176092 10.1371/journal.pone.0176092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Eckert MA, Keren NI, Roberts DR, Calhoun VD, & Harris KC (2010). Age-related changes in processing speed: Unique contributions of cerebellar and prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4(10), 1–14. 10.3389/neuro.09.010.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fahimi F, Goh WB, Lee TS, & Guan C (2018). EEG predicts the attention level of elderly measured by RBANS. International Journal of Crowd Science, 2(3), 272–282. 10.1108/IJCS-09-2018-0022 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Feise RJ (2002). Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment?. Bmc Medical Research Methodology, 2(1), 8–11. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-2-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, & McHugh PR (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198. 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fredman L, Cauley JA, Hochberg M, Ensrud KE, & Doros G, & Study of Osteoporotic Fractures. (2010). Mortality associated with caregiving, general stress, and caregiving-related stress in elderly women: Results of caregiver-study of osteoporotic fractures. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 58(5), 937–943. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02808.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fredman L, Cauley JA, Satterfield S, Simonsick E, Spencer SM, Ayonayon HN, & Harris TB (2008). Caregiving, mortality, and mobility decline: The health, aging, and body composition (Health ABC) study. Archives of Internal Medicine, 768(19), 2154–2162. 10.1001/archinte.168.19.2154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, & Matthews KA (2007). Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. Neuroimage, 35(2), 795–803. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gillespie P, O’Shea E, Cullinan J, Buchanan J, Bobula J, Lacey L, … Lawlor B (2015). Longitudinal costs of caring for people with Alzheimer’s disease. International Psychogeriatrics, 27(5), 847–856. 10.1017/S104161021400206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goldberg TE, Harvey PD, Wesnes KA, Snyder PJ, & Schneider LS (2015). Practice effects due to serial cognitive assessment: Implications for preclinical Alzheimer’s disease randomized controlled trials. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, 7(1), 103–111. 10.1016/j.dadm.2014.11.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Haley WE, Levine EG, Brown SL, & Bartolucci AA (1987). Stress, appraisal, coping, and social support as predictors of adaptational outcome among dementia caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 2(4), 323–330. 10.1037/0882-7974.2A323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hansen BB, & Bowers J (2008). Covariate balance in simple, stratified and clustered comparative studies. Statistical Science, 23(2), 219–236. 10.1214/08-STS25 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Henson RN (2003). Neuroimaging studies of priming. Progress in Neurobiology, 70(1), 53–81. 10.1016/S0301-0082(03)00086-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Iacus SM, King G, & Porro G (2011). Multivariate matching methods that are monotonic imbalance bounding. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 706(493), 345–361. 10.1198/jasa.2011.tm09599 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Katz S, Ford AB, Moskowitz RW, Jackson BA, & Yaffe MW (1963). The index of ADL: A standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. Jama, 785(12), 914–919. doi: 10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kirchner WK (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kirchner WK (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 352–358. doi: 10.1037/h0043688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lawton MP, & Brody EM (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3 3 Part 1), 179–186. doi: 10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lazarus RF, & Folkman SCS (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer. [Google Scholar]
  32. Lee S, Kawachi I, & Grodstein F (2004). Does caregiving stress affect cognitive function in older women? The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 792(1), 51–57. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000106000.02232.30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Leszczynski M (2011). How does hippocampus contribute to working memory processing? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(37), 1–2. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lupien S, Lecours AR, Lussier I, Schwartz G, Nair N, & Meaney MJ (1994). Basal cortisol levels and cognitive deficits in human aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 74(5), 2893–2903. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-05-02893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lupien SJ, Juster RP, Raymond C, & Marin MF (2018). The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: From neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinologyt 49, 91–105. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lupien SJ, Maheu F, Tu M, Fiocco A, & Schramek TE (2007). The effects of stress and stress hormones on human cognition: Implications for the field of brain and cognition. Brain and Cognition, 65(3), 209–237. 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, & Heim C (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434–445. 10.1038/nrn2639 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Mackenzie CS, Smith MC, Hasher L, Leach L, & Behl P (2007). Cognitive functioning under stress: Evidence from informal caregivers of palliative patients. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 10(3), 749–758. 10.1089/jpm.2006.0171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Mackenzie CS, Wiprzycka UJ, Hasher L, & Goldstein D (2009). Associations between psychological distress, learning, and memory in spouse caregivers of older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64(6), 742–746. 10.1093/geronb/gbp076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. MacLeod CM (1992). The Stroop task: The “gold standard” of attentional measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(1), 12–14. 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Mahoney AM, Dalby JT, & King MC (1998). Cognitive failures and stress. Psychological Reports, 82(3_suppl), 1432–1434. 10.2466/pr0.1998.82.3c.1432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Marshall AC, Cooper NR, Segrave R, & Geeraert N (2015). The effects of long-term stress exposure on aging cognition: A behavioral and EEG investigation. Neurobiology of Aging, 36(6), 2136–2144. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.02.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. McEwen BS (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840(1), 33–44. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. McEwen BS, & Magarinos A (1997). Stress effects on morphology and function of the hippocampus. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 821(1), 271–284. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48286.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McEwen BS, & Sapolsky RM (1995). Stress and cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 5(2), 205–216. 10.1016/0959-4388(95)80028-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McGuinness B, Barrett SL, Craig D, Lawson J, & Passmore AP (2010). Attention deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 81(2), 157–159. 10.1108/IJCS-09-2018-0022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Melara RD, & Algom D (2003). Driven by information: A tectonic theory of Stroop effects. Psychological Review, 110(3), 422–471. 10.1037/0033-295X.1103.422 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mitchell PH, Powell L, Blumenthal J, Norten J, Ironson G, Pitula CR, Froelicher ES, Czajkowski S, Youngblood M, Huber M, & Berkman LF (2003). A short social support measure for patients recovering from myocardial infarction: The ENRICHD Social Support Inventory. Journal of Cardiopulmonary Rehabilitation and Prevention, 23(6), 398–403. 10.1097/00008483-200311000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mittelman MS, Roth DL, Haley WE, & Zarit SH (2004). Effects of a caregiver intervention on negative caregiver appraisals of behavior problems in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: Results of a randomized trial. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(1), P27–P34. 10.1093/geronb/59.1P27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Murman DL (2015). The impact of age on cognition. Thieme Medical Publishers, 36(3), 111–121. 10.1055/s-0035-1555115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nichols EA, Kao YC, Verfaellie M, & Gabrieli JD (2006). Working memory and long-term memory for faces: Evidence from fMRI and global amnesia for involvement of the medial temporal lobes. Hippocampus, 16(7), 604–616. 10.1002/hipo.20190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. O’Reilly D, Connolly S, Rosato M, & Patterson C (2008). Is caring associated with an increased risk of mortality? A longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine, 67(8), 1282–1290. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.06.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. O’Sullivan M, Brennan S, Lawlor BA, Hannigan C, Robertson IH, & Pertl MM (2019). Cognitive functioning among cognitively intact dementia caregivers compared to matched self-selected and population controls. Aging & Mental Health, 23(5), 566–573. 10.1080/13607863.2018.1428937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Oken BS, Fonareva I, & Wahbeh H (2011). Stress-related cognitive dysfunction in dementia caregivers. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 24(4), 191–198. 10.1177/0891988711422524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Pearlin LI, Mullan JT, Semple SJ, & Skaff MM (1990). Caregiving and the stress process: An overview of concepts and their measures. The Gerontologist, 30(5), 583–594. https://doi.org/0.1093/geront/30.5.583 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Pertl MM, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, & Brennan S (2015). Risk of cognitive and functional impairment in spouses of people with dementia: Evidence from the health and retirement study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 28(4), 260–271. 10.1177/0891988715588834 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Preston AR, & Eichenbaum H (2013). Interplay of hippocampus and prefrontal cortex in memory. Current Biology, 23(17), R764–R773. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.05.041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Preston AR, & Gabrieli JD (2008). Dissociation between explicit memory and configural memory in the human medial temporal lobe. Cerebral Cortex, 18(9), 2192–2207. 10.1093/cercor/bhm245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Radloff LS (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385–401. 10.1177/014662167700100306 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Roediger HL, Weldon MS, Stadler ML, & Riegler GL (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(6), 1251–1269. 10.1037//0278-7393.18.6.1251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Roediger HL, Weldon MS, Stadler ML, & Riegler GL (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(6), 1251–1269. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.6.1251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Roth DL, Fredman L, & Haley WE (2015). Informal caregiving and its impact on health: A reappraisal from population-based studies. The Gerontologist, 55(2), 309–319. 10.1093/geront/gnu177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rothman KJ (1990). No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology, 1(1), 43–46. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199001000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rubin DB (2001). Using propensity scores to help design observational studies: Application to the tobacco litigation. Health Services and Outcomes Research Methodology, 2(3–4), 169–188. 10.1023/A:1020363010465 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Salthouse TA, & Babcock RL (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 763–776. 10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.763 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Salthouse TA, Toth J, Daniels K, Parks C, Pak R, Wolbrette M, & Hocking KJ (2000). Effects of aging on efficiency of task switching in a variant of the trail making test. Neuropsychology, 14(1), 102–111. 10.1037/0894-4105.14.1.102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Schacter DL, & Buckner RL (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20(2), 185–195. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80448-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Shephard RN, & Metzler J (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171 (3972), 701–703. https://doi.org/0.1126/science.171.3972.701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Siegrist M (1995). Reliability of the stroop test with single-stimulus presentation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 81 (3_suppl) 1295–1298. doi: 10.2466/pms.1995.81.3f.1295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Stoet G (2010). PsyToolkit - A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behavior Research Methods, 42(4), 1096–1104. 10.3758/BRM.42A1096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stoet G (2017). PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology, 44(1), 24–31. 10.1177/0098628316677643 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stuart EA (2010). Matching methods for causal inference: A review and a look forward. Statistical Science: A Review Journal of the Institute of Mathematical Statistics, 25(1), 1–21. https://doi.org/0.1214/09-STS313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Stuart EA & Rubin DB (2008). Best practices in quasi-experimental designs: Matching methods for causal inference In Osborne J, ed., Best practices in quantitative methods, (p. 155–176). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.. [Google Scholar]
  74. Thierry AM, Gioanni Y, Degenetais E, & Glowinski J (2000). Hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: Anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus, 10(4), 411–419. doi.10.1002/1098–1063(2000)10:4<411::AID-HIPO7>3.0.CO;2-A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Thornton M, & Travis SS (2003). Analysis of the reliability of the modified caregiver strain index. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(2), S127–S132. 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Thoemmes F (2012). Propensity score matching in SPSS. University of Tubingen. [Google Scholar]
  77. Thoemmes FJ, & Kim ES (2011). A systematic review of propensity score methods in the social sciences. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 46(1), 90–118. 10.1080/00273171.2011.540475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tombaugh TN, & McIntyre NJ (1992). The mini-mental state examination: A comprehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(9), 922–935. 10.1111/jM532-5415.1992.tb01992.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Turken U, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bammer R, Baldo JV, Dronkers NF, & Gabrieli JD (2008). Cognitive processing speed and the structure of white matter pathways: Convergent evidence from normal variation and lesion studies. Neuroimage, 42(2), 1032–1044. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Vertes RP (2006). Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in the rat. Neuroscience, 142(1), 1–20. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Vitaliano PP, Echeverria D, Yi J, Phillips PE, Young H, & Siegler IC (2005). Psychophysiological mediators of caregiver stress and differential cognitive decline. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 402–411. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Vitaliano PP, Zhang J, Young HM, Caswell LW, Scanlan JM, & Echeverria D (2009). Depressed mood mediates decline in cognitive processing speed in caregivers. The Gerontologist, 49(1), 12–22. 10.1093/geront/gnp004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Voss JL, & Paller KA (2008). Brain substrates of implicit and explicit memory: The importance of concurrently acquired neural signals of both memory types. Neuropsychologia, 46(13), 3021–3029. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wechsler D (1997). Wechsler memory scale (WMS-III) (Vol. 14). Psychological corporation. [Google Scholar]
  85. Wechsler D (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). NCS Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  86. Wechsler Wechsler D.. Adult Intelligence Scale-Fourth Edition (WAIS-V) San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  87. Willis SL, Jay GM, Diehl M, & Marsiske M (1992). Longitudinal change and prediction of everyday task competence in the elderly. Research on Aging, 14(1), 68–91. 10.1177/0164027592141004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wolff JL, Spillman BC, Freedman VA, & Kasper JD (2016). A national profile of family and unpaid caregivers who assist older adults with health care activities. JAMA Internal Medicine, 176 (3), 372–379. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2015.7664 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zarit S, Orr NK, & Zarit JM (1985). The hidden victims of Alzheimer’s disease: Families under stress. NYU Press. [Google Scholar]
  90. Zimmerman ME, Ezzati A, Katz MJ, Lipton ML, Brickman AM, Sliwinski MJ, & Lipton RB (2016). Perceived stress is differentially related to hippocampal subfield volumes among older adults. PloS One, 11(5), 1–10. 10.1371/journal.pone.0154530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES