Abstract
The paper aims to identify selected predictors of food label use to extend our knowledge about consumer behavior related to food purchases. Two types of information were examined: front-of-package (FOP) and back-of-package (BOP), and two contexts of reading labels were distinguished: during shopping and at home. Various types of potential predictors were tested, including demographic (e.g., age, gender, household size, place of living), socioeconomic (e.g., education, professional activity, income), behavioral (e.g., purchasing certain types of products), and psychographic (e.g., importance attached to various types of information) criteria. The survey was conducted with the use of the CAWI (Computer-Assisted Web Interviews) methodology in a sample of 1051 Polish consumers. Quota sampling was applied based on sex, age, education, place of living (urban vs. rural), and region. Descriptive statistics, t-tests, ANOVAs, Pearson correlation coefficients, and multiple and retrograde step regressions were applied. In retrograde step regression models, only one predictor (self-rated knowledge about nutrition healthiness) turned out to be significant for all four measures of label reading. The remaining predictors were specific to selected measures of reading labels. The importance of the information about the content of fat and that about the health effects of consuming a food product were significant predictors of three types of food label use. This study confirms the necessity to investigate reading labels in fine-grained models, adapted to different types of labels and different contexts of reading. Our results show that demographic or socioeconomic variables are not significant predictors of reading food labels for a large group of Polish consumers.
Keywords: food label use, front-of-package (FOP) labels, back-of-package (BOP) labels, nutrition claims, health claims, nutrition knowledge, consumer behavior
1. Introduction
In previous research, food label use was associated with consumer characteristics, product type, and purchasing context. The following variables were found to affect reading labels: gender [1,2,3,4,5], age [4,6], marital status [7], ethnicity [2], socioeconomic status [2,7], including education level [4,5,7,8,9], professional activity [9], income [5], place of living—rural or urban areas [2,5], Body Mass Index [2,4], being an athlete [10], food-related motivation [11], nutrition knowledge [4,12,13], self-reported health [4,10], having a special diet [4,9], being concerned with a healthy lifestyle [10], attitude towards the health value of the products [13], health orientation [14], taste [9,13], price [9], product specificity [12], buying the product for the first time [12], the amount of time spent shopping [9], and buying organic food [15]. There are various connections between these predictors, e.g., the gender effect is mainly due to differences in nutrition knowledge levels [12]. Health-related variables were the most important group of predictors of food label use, followed by motivating factors and sociodemographic variables. Placing importance on health, healthy eating, and nutritional value of food, perceived vulnerability for diet-related diseases, nutrition knowledge, numeracy, and gender were positively associated with the frequency of food label use [16]. Subjective norms and diet-health concern were significant predictors of intention to use food labels [17]. Self-efficacy and health literacy were predictors of food label use among young adults [18]. Consumers’ level of nutrition knowledge influenced their ability to process food labels. Experts used the central route processing to scrutinize intrinsic cues and make judgments about food products, while novices used the peripheral route processing to make simple inferences about the extrinsic cues in labels [19]. Nutritional knowledge and attitude toward food label use positively predicted food label use through self-efficacy and trust. However, these mediation effects were moderated by gender such that the indirect relationship was stronger among men than women [20]. In their review [21], Soederberg Miller and Cassady found an overreliance on convenience samples relying on younger adults, limiting our understanding of how knowledge supports food label use in later life. This limitation is overcome in the current study, as it is based on a sample representative by age.
It is also important to distinguish between food label use at the point of purchase and at home. Education has a positive impact on the likelihood of using labels at home but not while shopping. The average amount of time spent on grocery shopping per visit affects label use while shopping but not at home. The same applies to the primary source of nutrition information being books, magazines, radio, TV, or newspapers. Moreover, being on a special diet influenced label reading in the shop but not at home. The importance of price while shopping affected reading labels at home only, and the importance of taste was a significant predictor of reading labels while shopping only [9]. These results support the need to study food label use separately while shopping and at home.
Findings consistently support a relationship between label reading and dietary practices [22,23]. Changes in diet quality due to label use were estimated for different types of label information. Consumer label use increased the average Healthy Eating Index. These improvements differed across label information types that were used. Among nutritional panels, serving sizes, nutrient content claims, a list of ingredients, and health claims, the use of health claims on food labels provided the highest level of improvement in diet quality [24]. Search for total fat, saturated fat, and cholesterol information on food labels was less likely among individuals who consumed more of the three nutrients, respectively. The search was also related to perceived benefits and costs of using the label, the perceived capability of using the label, knowledge of nutrition and fats, perceived efficacy of diets in reducing the risk of illnesses, perceived importance of nutrition in food shopping, perceived importance of a healthy diet, and awareness of a linkage between excessive consumption of the nutrients and health problems [25]. Nutrition knowledge had a strong effect on general label use, degree of use, and on use of nutrient content concerning fat, ingredients, and vitamins/minerals [26]. Subjects with chronic diseases were more aware of nutritional recommendations, checked more often for specific nutrients, and used nutrition information on food labels more often than did participants without such diseases. However, label use behavior was inconsistently associated with dietary guideline compliance [27]. The odds of healthy weight loss behaviors were two to four times higher when food labels were used frequently to seek information on calories and nutrients such as total fat, saturated fat, or cholesterol [28]. A recent review [29], by Anastasiou, Miller, and Dickinson, demonstrated that results were inconsistent in reporting a relationship between diet and food label use but indicated that reading the nutrition facts label was associated with healthier diets, measured by food frequency questionnaires and 24 h recalls. However, there is insufficient research on the association between dietary consumption and the use of ingredients lists, serving size information, and front-of-package (FOP) labels. Nutrition label use decreased the risk of poor dietary quality regardless of poverty status [30]. General food label use was the main determinant of diet quality and partly mediated the association between eating behavior traits and diet quality. The stronger mediating effect observed in men suggested they relied more on food labeling when attempting to restrain themselves, which translated into better diet quality [31].
It is also important to recognize that various food label types have different effects on consumer behavior. Compared with individual characteristics, nutrition label types had an increased impact on food product ranking ability [32]. Different food label formats differed in the understanding of consumers. The multiple traffic light (MTL) labels influenced the perceived healthiness of foods most often [33]. In an international, large-scale survey, viewing the MTL provided the most favorable ratings out of five FOP nutrition label formats (health star rating, MTL, Nutri-Score, reference intakes, and warning label) [34]. The perceived usefulness and public support of mandatory implementation were higher for the MTL than for the Nutri-Score label [35]. Nutri-Score, which is a summary, interpretive, polychromatic FOP label, emerged as the most effective in the Bulgarian context [36]. FOP nutrition labels were more likely to be viewed than Nutrition Facts labels [37]. Different degrees of health-orientation were reflected in the diverse use of labeled information. Highly health-oriented consumers were more likely to refer to the extensive information reported on the nutrition facts panel, whereas claims were of main interest for consumers with a low orientation to health [14]. Furthermore, 52.5% of consumers did not read the ingredients’ list written on the food label [38]. Simplified FOP labels can induce healthier purchases compared with when only back-of-package (BOP) nutrition information is available [39]. Clearer BOP labeling is also needed [40]. Nutritional warnings cause a salience bias that makes excessive nutrient content and its negative health consequences more salient in consumers’ minds, especially in the case of products with a particular health-related connotation [41]. Although FOP labels may help shoppers make healthier food choices, those advocating for effective labels must resist opposition from food corporations [42]. Future opportunities for FOP labeling include the potential for integrating nutritional profiles with non-nutrient factors affecting health such as food processing and environmental sustainability [43]. Although FOP labels help consumers to identify healthier products, their ability to nudge consumers toward healthier choices is more limited. Importantly, FOP labels may lead to halo effects, positively influencing not only virtue but also vice products, e.g., interpretive nutrient-specific labels improve health perceptions of both vice and virtue products, yet they influence only the purchase intention of virtues [44].
The paper aims to identify selected predictors of reading food labels with the distinction of the label type (FOP and BOP), and the context of food label use (during shopping and afterward, at home). Therefore, four measures of reading labels were considered in this study: FOP in the shop, BOP in the shop, FOP at home, and BOP at home. The main contribution of this paper lies in systematically analyzing predictors of reading labels from the perspective of two major label types (FOP and BOP) and two main reading contexts (in the shop and at home) based on a survey in a large, representative, nation-wide sample of consumers.
2. Materials and Methods
The survey was conducted with the use of the CAWI (Computer Assisted Web Interviews) methodology. The execution of the survey was commissioned to a specialized market research agency. The respondents were informed that the results would be used only for scientific purposes, respecting the anonymity principle. The sample size amounted to 1051 consumers. The target sample size was set at a comparable level with previous consumer studies conducted at the national level in Poland [45,46,47]. It was aimed to obtain a sample resembling the general population of Polish adults, regarding four criteria: age, sex, education level, and the size of the city of origin (in particular, the urban/rural divide). Quota sampling was applied based on five criteria: sex (men and women), age (the following age intervals: 15–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65 and more), education (primary, secondary and tertiary), place of living (urban and rural areas), and region (all 16 Polish regions). Thanks to this approach, the sample structure was similar to the general population of Polish consumers according to the above-mentioned criteria (Table A1).
The questionnaire was created by the author of this study. It was composed of elements adapted from previous research using validated tools [14,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59]. It consisted of 38 questions. The full questionnaire is included in a book in Polish [60]. The operationalization of the key variables used in this study is provided in Table A2. Reading labels was investigated in 4 configurations: (1) FOP in the point of purchase (FOP shop), (2) BOP in the point of purchase (BOP shop), (3) FOP after purchase (FOP home), (4) BOP after purchase (BOP home). It was operationalized as a percentage share of food products the respondent buys. Some more difficult terms were explained in the questionnaire, in particular health claims, nutrition claims, and quality signs. The research examined general food labels. My dependent variables referred to FOP and BOP labels without specifying which information was put on which label. However, in some questions, I examined the importance attached to particular types of information placed on the labels, such as health claims, nutrition claims, list of ingredients, expiry date, country of origin, cooking recipes, brand, organic certificate, quality signs, recommendations of scientific institutes, price, as well as selected types of nutritional information (energy value, the content of fat, sugar, salt, protein, vitamins, dietary fiber, Omega-3 fatty acids) and health information (lowering cholesterol, reducing the risk of heart diseases, strengthening bones, impact on the digestive system, reducing tiredness and fatigue, maintaining proper vision, proper development of children, and proper functioning of the heart). Various types of potential predictors were tested, including demographic (e.g., age, gender, household size, place of living), socioeconomic (e.g., education, professional activity, income), behavioral (e.g., purchasing certain types of products), and psychographic (e.g., importance attached to various types of information) criteria. For most of the questions, 5-point Likert scales were used.
First, I tested the differences in reading food labels depending on selected demographic and socioeconomic criteria: sex, age, place of living, education, professional activity, income, household size, and the number of children. Second, I checked whether reading labels differed depending on some purchasing habits: buying dietary supplements, organic food, functional food, and fair trade products. Third, I examined label use according to selected criteria related to one’s health and diet. Fourth, I examined the correlations of food label use with the evaluation of the quantity, understandability, and credibility of selected information on the food packaging.
Next, I tested the associations between food label use and the importance attached to selected types of information placed on the labels. Four questions were devoted to this issue. The first one concerned general food labels. Second, I focused on the importance of information at the first purchase of a given product. This question referred to a product that was new to the consumer and not necessarily to the entire market Third, I took into consideration typical types of nutritional information. Last, I examined the correlations with selected types of health-related information.
Furthermore, the associations between food label use and the importance attached to marketing communication instruments were identified. The following marketing communication options were put forward for the respondents’ evaluation: TV commercials, producer website, retailer website, producer social media profile, retailer social media profile, consumer opinions in social media, mobile applications, recommendations of family or friends, recommendations of a dietician, outdoor advertising (e.g., billboards), advertising newsletters of retailers, articles in press and magazines, TV culinary programs, culinary blogs, product packaging, and shop assistant.
Regarding information used in the marketing communication for food products, the following options were considered: health effects of consuming a given product, care for the natural environment, supporting producers (e.g., farmers), low price, national origin of the product, the utility of the product in a certain diet, above-average quality of the product, and traditional method of production.
Based on statistical differences revealed in t-tests and ANOVAs as well as the most significant Pearson correlations, 32 independent variables were selected to be analyzed in multiple regressions. Separate regression models were constructed for each type of reading labels.
In the data analysis, descriptive statistics, t-tests, analyses of variance (ANOVAs), Pearson correlation coefficients, and multiple and retrograde step regressions were applied. T-tests were used to compare two quantitative results, ANOVAs were used to compare multiple quantitative results, Pearson correlation coefficients were used to examine the linear correlations between quantitative variables, the multiple regression models were used to examine the simultaneous influence of all independent variables separately on the 4 types of reading labels, and the retrograde step regressions were used to narrow down the set of independent variables to those that remained significant at the p < 0.05 level. Independent variables that differentiated any measure of reading labels in a statistically significant way were included in separate multiple regression models for FOP shop, BOP shop, FOP home, and BOP home. To obtain more parsimonious models, only those predictors that reached statistical significance at the level of p < 0.05 were accepted in retrograde step regressions. The analyses were conducted in Statistica 12 (TIBCO Software Inc., Palo Alto, CA, USA). I report continuous p-values instead of thresholds, following the recent guidelines of statisticians [61]. Results that are statistically significant at p < 0.05 are boldfaced to increase their visibility.
3. Results
Women read food labels more often than men, but the sex differences reached statistical significance only for reading BOP labels at the point of purchase (BOP shop) and FOP labels after the purchase at home (FOP home) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Reading Labels (%) | Women | Men | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
FOP shop | 56.72 | 53.77 | 1.726 | 0.085 |
BOP shop | 52.59 | 47.69 | 2.888 | 0.004 |
FOP home | 51.49 | 46.29 | 2.763 | 0.006 |
BOP home | 52.86 | 50.04 | 1.502 | 0.133 |
A series of ANOVAs demonstrated that age (measured in 10-year intervals) did not differentiate any measure of reading labels (Table 2).
Table 2.
Reading Labels (%) | Age Intervals | ANOVA | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
15–24 | 25–34 | 35–44 | 45–54 | 55–64 | ≥65 | F | p | |
FOP shop | 59.75 | 55.15 | 53.17 | 53.87 | 54.50 | 55.81 | 1.160 | 0.327 |
BOP shop | 51.94 | 50.57 | 50.34 | 47.75 | 50.70 | 50.24 | 0.372 | 0.868 |
FOP home | 48.80 | 49.17 | 47.55 | 47.26 | 51.84 | 49.57 | 0.479 | 0.792 |
BOP home | 53.63 | 51.99 | 51.38 | 48.10 | 53.68 | 50.46 | 0.746 | 0.589 |
Reading labels was not affected by the place of living (defined as rural areas, towns up to 50,000, cities of 50–500,000, and cities of more than 500,000) either (Table 3).
Table 3.
Reading Labels (%) | Place of Living | ANOVA | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rural Areas | <50,000 | 50,000–500,000 | >500,000 | F | p | |
FOP shop | 54.20 | 58.34 | 55.30 | 54.66 | 1.016 | 0.385 |
BOP shop | 48.89 | 51.78 | 51.16 | 50.45 | 0.639 | 0.590 |
FOP home | 48.91 | 51.13 | 48.57 | 47.81 | 0.409 | 0.746 |
BOP home | 50.87 | 54.08 | 50.75 | 51.67 | 0.581 | 0.627 |
As far as the education level is concerned, only the FOP shop was influenced in a significant way (Table 4). Respondents who had tertiary education read food labels in shops more often than the other education groups.
Table 4.
Reading Labels (%) | Education | ANOVA | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Primary | Vocational | Secondary | Tertiary | F | p | |
FOP shop | 55.89 | 52.50 | 55.96 | 59.08 | 2.750 | 0.042 |
BOP shop | 51.16 | 48.21 | 51.21 | 52.11 | 1.209 | 0.305 |
FOP home | 48.77 | 48.31 | 50.38 | 48.51 | 0.310 | 0.818 |
BOP home | 51.72 | 49.19 | 54.45 | 51.15 | 1.787 | 0.148 |
As far as the professional activity is concerned, it affected reading labels at the point of purchase—both FOP and BOP—but not after the purchase at home. White-collar workers read food labels most frequently in-store (both FOP and BOP) (Table 5).
Table 5.
Reading Labels (%) | Professional Activity | ANOVA | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | F | p | |
FOP shop | 61.16 | 52.52 | 48.49 | 57.98 | 54.05 | 55.68 | 2.725 | 0.019 |
BOP shop | 56.15 | 48.03 | 40.80 | 51.93 | 49.57 | 50.58 | 2.924 | 0.013 |
FOP home | 51.44 | 48.34 | 43.73 | 47.15 | 47.72 | 50.73 | 0.820 | 0.535 |
BOP home | 55.81 | 50.34 | 45.41 | 53.95 | 50.55 | 51.31 | 1.209 | 0.303 |
Notes: 1—white-collar worker, 2—blue-collar worker, 3—unemployed, 4—student, 5—not working and taking care of one’s family, 6—old age pensioner or disability pensioner; the category “other” was excluded from the ANOVA.
Reading food labels was associated with household income only for the FOP shop. It was highest among respondents living in households with middle income (3001–4000 PLN) and the highest income (over 6000 PLN per month) (Table 6).
Table 6.
Reading Labels (%) | Income Intervals | ANOVA | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | F | p | |
FOP shop | 48.92 | 55.33 | 58.52 | 55.58 | 55.01 | 57.83 | 2.509 | 0.029 |
BOP shop | 46.66 | 50.50 | 50.65 | 49.49 | 52.43 | 53.53 | 1.034 | 0.396 |
FOP home | 48.15 | 49.38 | 51.31 | 44.96 | 50.93 | 50.15 | 1.081 | 0.369 |
BOP home | 47.96 | 51.54 | 52.87 | 52.03 | 51.56 | 53.03 | 0.586 | 0.711 |
Notes: the average monthly disposable income of one’s household; 1—below 2000 PLN, 2—2001–3000 PLN, 3—3001–4000 PLN, 4—4001–5000 PLN, 5—5001–6000 PLN, 6—over 6000 PLN.
Only FOP label reading (in the shop and at home) correlated positively and significantly with the size of one’s household (Table 7).
Table 7.
Number of Persons | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Household | 0.076 | 0.013 | 0.043 | 0.164 | 0.066 | 0.031 | 0.031 | 0.320 |
Children | 0.043 | 0.164 | 0.054 | 0.081 | 0.039 | 0.211 | 0.040 | 0.199 |
Notes: household—the number of people in one’s household, children—the number of children in one’s household.
Reading food labels was associated with purchasing certain types of products, namely dietary supplements, organic food, functional food, and fair trade products (Table 8). Buying dietary supplements increased the frequency of reading labels on the front of the package in the shop and at home as well as on the back of the package at home only. Buying organic food affected all measures of reading labels in a highly significant way. Buying functional food influenced all kinds of reading labels except for the FOP shop. Buying fair trade products differentiated significantly reading only the BOP information.
Table 8.
Reading Labels (%) | Yes | No | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Buying dietary supplements | ||||
FOP shop | 57.71 | 53.11 | 2.698 | 0.007 |
BOP shop | 51.84 | 48.85 | 1.759 | 0.079 |
FOP home | 51.40 | 46.86 | 2.410 | 0.016 |
BOP home | 54.17 | 49.08 | 2.716 | 0.007 |
Buying organic food | ||||
FOP shop | 58.41 | 51.73 | 3.926 | <0.001 |
BOP shop | 54.71 | 45.11 | 5.715 | <0.001 |
FOP home | 52.92 | 44.52 | 4.486 | <0.001 |
BOP home | 56.05 | 46.24 | 5.273 | <0.001 |
Buying functional food | ||||
FOP shop | 56.41 | 54.75 | 0.930 | 0.353 |
BOP shop | 54.15 | 48.18 | 3.385 | 0.001 |
FOP home | 52.03 | 47.43 | 2.341 | 0.019 |
BOP home | 57.11 | 48.48 | 4.434 | <0.001 |
Buying fair trade products | ||||
FOP shop | 57.80 | 54.41 | 1.769 | 0.077 |
BOP shop | 54.31 | 48.78 | 2.913 | 0.004 |
FOP home | 50.35 | 48.57 | 0.840 | 0.401 |
BOP home | 56.80 | 49.56 | 3.454 | 0.001 |
Note: the category “No” includes direct “No” answers and “I don’t know” answers.
Reading food labels was correlated with self-rated healthiness of one’s diet, one’s knowledge about healthy nutrition, and self-rated health but not BMI (Body Mass Index) (Table 9).
Table 9.
Self-Rated Measures | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Diet | 0.072 | 0.019 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.075 | 0.014 | 0.091 | 0.003 |
Knowledge | 0.089 | 0.004 | 0.118 | <0.001 | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.097 | 0.002 |
Health | 0.072 | 0.020 | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.063 | 0.042 | 0.076 | 0.014 |
BMI | −0.029 | 0.344 | −0.049 | 0.109 | 0.026 | 0.401 | −0.020 | 0.512 |
Note: BMI—Body Mass Index (kg/m2).
However, none of the investigated measures of reading labels depended on being on a special diet for health reasons (Table 10).
Table 10.
Reading Labels (%) | Yes | No | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
FOP shop | 52.71 | 55.87 | −1.378 | 0.168 |
BOP shop | 50.83 | 50.19 | 0.279 | 0.780 |
FOP home | 48.55 | 49.16 | −0.242 | 0.809 |
BOP home | 53.13 | 51.23 | 0.753 | 0.452 |
Reading BOP labels was related to the evaluation of the quantity of selected information (health claims, nutrition claims, and quality signs) on food labels (Table 11). Those who considered that there was too little such information tended to read food labels more often. Moreover, all measures of reading labels were associated with the self-evaluated understandability of information put on the labels (namely health claims, nutrition claims, quality signs, functional food, and organic food). Finally, reading labels correlated with the perceived credibility of such information (health claims, nutrition claims, list of ingredients, expiry date, organic certificate, and quality signs). Those who trusted information placed on the labels more tended to read them more often both in the shop and at home.
Table 11.
Reading Labels (%) | Quantity | Understandability | Credibility | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | |
FOP shop | 0.033 | 0.287 | 0.080 | 0.009 | 0.083 | 0.007 |
BOP shop | 0.143 | <0.001 | 0.079 | 0.010 | 0.072 | 0.019 |
FOP home | 0.051 | 0.098 | 0.073 | 0.018 | 0.080 | 0.010 |
BOP home | 0.125 | <0.001 | 0.080 | 0.010 | 0.074 | 0.017 |
Reading food labels depended on the importance attached to selected information put on them (Table 12). The subjective importance of almost all types of information correlated positively and significantly with reading FOP and BOP food labels both at the point of purchase and at home, except cooking recipes, the importance of which was correlated significantly only with the FOP home.
Table 12.
Types of Information | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Health claims | 0.078 | 0.011 | 0.103 | 0.001 | 0.076 | 0.013 | 0.102 | 0.001 |
Nutrition claims | 0.081 | 0.009 | 0.111 | <0.001 | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.104 | 0.001 |
List of ingredients | 0.091 | 0.003 | 0.141 | <0.001 | 0.084 | 0.007 | 0.122 | <0.001 |
Expiry date | 0.093 | 0.002 | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.070 | 0.024 | 0.081 | 0.009 |
Country of origin | 0.067 | 0.029 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.080 | 0.009 | 0.088 | 0.004 |
Cooking recipes | 0.045 | 0.147 | 0.053 | 0.088 | 0.082 | 0.008 | 0.059 | 0.055 |
Brand | 0.070 | 0.024 | 0.072 | 0.020 | 0.080 | 0.009 | 0.074 | 0.016 |
Organic certificate | 0.081 | 0.008 | 0.102 | 0.001 | 0.090 | 0.003 | 0.088 | 0.004 |
Quality signs | 0.084 | 0.006 | 0.086 | 0.005 | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.094 | 0.002 |
Recommendations * | 0.081 | 0.009 | 0.092 | 0.003 | 0.097 | 0.002 | 0.092 | 0.003 |
Price | 0.075 | 0.015 | 0.070 | 0.023 | 0.074 | 0.016 | 0.078 | 0.011 |
Average** | 0.079 | 0.010 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.085 | 0.006 | 0.092 | 0.003 |
Notes: * of scientific institutes; ** average evaluation of the importance of all types of information mentioned in this column of the table.
As far as the importance of information at the first purchase of a food product (excluding price) is concerned, it differentiated only BOP label reading, both in the shop and at home. The respondents indicating the list of ingredients read the highest share of BOP labels in both contexts (Table 13).
Table 13.
Reading Labels (%) | Most Important at First Purchase | ANOVA | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
COO | NI | HI | LI | ED | F | p | |
FOP shop | 53.52 | 53.94 | 54.25 | 58.68 | 55.84 | 1.260 | 0.284 |
BOP shop | 47.04 | 53.80 | 44.34 | 56.82 | 47.67 | 7.257 | <0.001 |
FOP home | 52.08 | 47.30 | 54.56 | 49.61 | 48.33 | 1.145 | 0.334 |
BOP home | 52.18 | 54.49 | 49.47 | 56.64 | 47.93 | 3.878 | 0.004 |
Notes: COO—the country of origin, NI—nutritional information (e.g., about the content of fat or dietary fiber), HI—information on health effects (e.g., good for the bones, lowering cholesterol), LI—list of ingredients, ED—expiry date; the reading labels indicators were measured in percentages of food products the consumer bought; the responses “other” and “I don’t know” were excluded from the ANOVAs.
All the investigated nutritional information types correlated significantly with all four measures of reading labels (Table 14).
Table 14.
Types of Information | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Energy value (calories) | 0.078 | 0.011 | 0.120 | <0.001 | 0.093 | 0.003 | 0.111 | <0.001 |
Content of fat | 0.090 | 0.004 | 0.128 | <0.001 | 0.106 | 0.001 | 0.130 | <0.001 |
Content of sugar | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.135 | <0.001 | 0.097 | 0.002 | 0.127 | <0.001 |
Content of salt | 0.071 | 0.022 | 0.118 | <0.001 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.116 | <0.001 |
Content of protein | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.127 | <0.001 | 0.100 | 0.001 | 0.120 | <0.001 |
Content of vitamins | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.123 | <0.001 | 0.102 | 0.001 | 0.123 | <0.001 |
Content of dietary fiber | 0.096 | 0.002 | 0.123 | <0.001 | 0.106 | 0.001 | 0.116 | <0.001 |
Content of Omega-3 fatty acids | 0.082 | 0.008 | 0.120 | <0.001 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.116 | <0.001 |
All of the investigated health information types correlated significantly with all the measures of reading labels (Table 15).
Table 15.
Information about the Impact of the Product on: | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Lowering cholesterol | 0.071 | 0.021 | 0.084 | 0.007 | 0.109 | <0.001 | 0.100 | 0.001 |
Reducing the risk of heart diseases | 0.079 | 0.010 | 0.084 | 0.006 | 0.100 | 0.001 | 0.094 | 0.002 |
Strengthening bones | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.097 | 0.002 | 0.099 | 0.001 | 0.099 | 0.001 |
The digestive system | 0.082 | 0.008 | 0.101 | 0.001 | 0.094 | 0.002 | 0.103 | 0.001 |
Reducing tiredness and fatigue | 0.091 | 0.003 | 0.097 | 0.002 | 0.105 | 0.001 | 0.100 | 0.001 |
Maintaining proper vision | 0.084 | 0.007 | 0.094 | 0.002 | 0.104 | 0.001 | 0.102 | 0.001 |
Proper development of children | 0.075 | 0.016 | 0.080 | 0.009 | 0.093 | 0.002 | 0.085 | 0.006 |
Proper functioning of the heart | 0.080 | 0.009 | 0.090 | 0.004 | 0.096 | 0.002 | 0.100 | 0.001 |
Furthermore, reading food labels was associated with the importance attached to all investigated promotion instruments for food products with health and nutrition claims, with some minor exceptions for particular food label use measures (Table 16).
Table 16.
Promotion Instruments | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
TV commercials | 0.068 | 0.028 | 0.055 | 0.073 | 0.075 | 0.015 | 0.056 | 0.072 |
Producer website | 0.071 | 0.020 | 0.070 | 0.023 | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.077 | 0.012 |
Retailer website | 0.064 | 0.038 | 0.064 | 0.038 | 0.078 | 0.012 | 0.067 | 0.029 |
Producer social media profile | 0.067 | 0.031 | 0.055 | 0.073 | 0.084 | 0.007 | 0.067 | 0.029 |
Retailer social media profile | 0.071 | 0.022 | 0.061 | 0.048 | 0.086 | 0.005 | 0.068 | 0.028 |
Consumer opinions in social media | 0.077 | 0.012 | 0.076 | 0.014 | 0.083 | 0.007 | 0.083 | 0.007 |
Mobile applications | 0.059 | 0.058 | 0.062 | 0.043 | 0.080 | 0.010 | 0.067 | 0.031 |
Recommendations of family or friends | 0.081 | 0.009 | 0.090 | 0.004 | 0.080 | 0.010 | 0.092 | 0.003 |
Recommendations of a dietician | 0.074 | 0.016 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.073 | 0.018 | 0.093 | 0.002 |
Outdoor advertising (e.g., billboards) | 0.076 | 0.014 | 0.063 | 0.040 | 0.090 | 0.004 | 0.073 | 0.018 |
Advertising newsletters of retailers | 0.062 | 0.045 | 0.064 | 0.039 | 0.063 | 0.040 | 0.062 | 0.043 |
Articles in press and magazines | 0.068 | 0.027 | 0.066 | 0.032 | 0.081 | 0.009 | 0.069 | 0.025 |
TV culinary programs | 0.085 | 0.006 | 0.065 | 0.034 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.072 | 0.019 |
Culinary blogs | 0.075 | 0.015 | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.095 | 0.002 | 0.089 | 0.004 |
Product packaging | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.077 | 0.013 | 0.078 | 0.012 | 0.084 | 0.007 |
Shop assistant | 0.076 | 0.014 | 0.053 | 0.087 | 0.077 | 0.012 | 0.057 | 0.065 |
All kinds of reading labels were associated the most strongly with the importance attached to the health effects of consuming a given product (Table 17).
Table 17.
Information Type | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
Health effects of consuming a given product | 0.099 | 0.001 | 0.109 | <0.001 | 0.101 | 0.001 | 0.116 | <0.001 |
Care for the natural environment | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.098 | 0.001 | 0.092 | 0.003 | 0.088 | 0.004 |
Supporting producers (e.g., farmers) | 0.067 | 0.031 | 0.073 | 0.018 | 0.092 | 0.003 | 0.082 | 0.008 |
Low price | 0.083 | 0.007 | 0.059 | 0.056 | 0.085 | 0.006 | 0.062 | 0.043 |
Polish origin of the product | 0.057 | 0.064 | 0.077 | 0.012 | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.084 | 0.007 |
Utility of the product in a certain diet | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.097 | 0.002 | 0.099 | 0.001 | 0.085 | 0.006 |
Above average quality of the product | 0.088 | 0.004 | 0.096 | 0.002 | 0.083 | 0.007 | 0.094 | 0.002 |
Traditional method of production | 0.071 | 0.021 | 0.099 | 0.001 | 0.089 | 0.004 | 0.099 | 0.001 |
It is also worth noting that all the analyzed types of reading labels correlated significantly with the willingness to pay (WTP) a higher price for food products with health claims and nutrition claims compared to conventional products (Table 18). The strongest correlation was observed between the BOP shop and the WTP for nutrition claims.
Table 18.
WTP | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
WTP HC | 0.073 | 0.018 | 0.079 | 0.011 | 0.077 | 0.012 | 0.091 | 0.003 |
WTP NC | 0.075 | 0.015 | 0.087 | 0.005 | 0.077 | 0.012 | 0.096 | 0.002 |
The multiple regression models explained better reading BOP labels (BOP shop R2 = 0.173, BOP home R2 = 0.143) than FOP labels (FOP shop R2 = 0.091, FOP home R2 = 0.089) (Table 19). In the full multiple regression model, reading FOP labels at the point of purchase was determined by only three predictors: self-rated knowledge about healthy nutrition, the importance of the expiry date information on the food packaging, and the importance of the information about the health effects of consuming a given product. Second, reading BOP information in the shop turned out to depend on five variables. It was positively associated with the importance attached to the list of ingredients, self-rated nutrition knowledge, indicating the list of ingredients as the most important information at the first purchase, and average assessment of the quantity of selected information on the food packaging: health claims, nutrition claims, and quality signs. It was negatively related to the consumer education level. The third regression model reported in this table refers to reading FOP labels at home. Six variables had a significant impact on this measure, four out of which were positively related: self-rated nutrition knowledge, the importance attached to culinary blogs, the importance of information about the health effects of consuming the product, and the importance of the information that the product reduces tiredness and fatigue. On the other hand, it was negatively associated with the information about the impact of the product on the digestive system and the importance attached to the recommendations of a dietician. The last regression model concerned the predictors of reading BOP information after the purchase. It was found to depend on five variables, all of which contributed positively: the importance of the information about the content of fat, the importance of the list of ingredients, the importance of the information about the health effects of consuming a product, the evaluation of the quantity of information on the label, and the importance of the list of ingredients when one buys a food product for the first time.
Table 19.
Independent Variables | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p | β | p | β | p | β | p | |
Woman | −0.004 | 0.899 | 0.020 | 0.525 | 0.045 | 0.161 | −0.020 | 0.515 |
Education | 0.031 | 0.362 | −0.070 | 0.033 | −0.024 | 0.490 | −0.042 | 0.205 |
White-collar | 0.050 | 0.131 | 0.045 | 0.149 | 0.012 | 0.706 | 0.029 | 0.358 |
Student | 0.020 | 0.556 | −0.039 | 0.222 | −0.022 | 0.521 | −0.003 | 0.924 |
Income | 0.041 | 0.208 | 0.042 | 0.175 | 0.024 | 0.462 | 0.026 | 0.405 |
Household | −0.009 | 0.776 | −0.032 | 0.306 | −0.041 | 0.218 | −0.054 | 0.092 |
Dietary suppl. | 0.035 | 0.259 | −0.001 | 0.961 | 0.021 | 0.507 | 0.027 | 0.365 |
Organic | 0.036 | 0.298 | 0.039 | 0.237 | 0.065 | 0.060 | 0.011 | 0.747 |
Functional | −0.034 | 0.304 | 0.018 | 0.562 | 0.012 | 0.712 | 0.042 | 0.193 |
Fair trade | 0.010 | 0.763 | 0.032 | 0.314 | −0.025 | 0.453 | 0.043 | 0.183 |
Diet | −0.041 | 0.292 | 0.035 | 0.347 | −0.029 | 0.454 | 0.030 | 0.420 |
Knowledge | 0.117 | 0.002 | 0.131 | <0.001 | 0.110 | 0.004 | 0.062 | 0.094 |
Health | −0.002 | 0.955 | 0.060 | 0.067 | −0.005 | 0.879 | 0.044 | 0.180 |
Quantity | 0.025 | 0.459 | 0.067 | 0.037 | 0.021 | 0.523 | 0.069 | 0.034 |
Understandability | 0.042 | 0.253 | 0.026 | 0.452 | 0.011 | 0.760 | 0.054 | 0.127 |
Credibility | 0.056 | 0.144 | −0.063 | 0.088 | 0.037 | 0.336 | −0.033 | 0.380 |
List of ingredients | −0.022 | 0.565 | 0.170 | <0.001 | −0.033 | 0.400 | 0.103 | 0.006 |
Expiry date | 0.112 | 0.001 | −0.002 | 0.956 | 0.003 | 0.941 | 0.012 | 0.727 |
Institutes | 0.017 | 0.643 | −0.014 | 0.690 | 0.053 | 0.147 | −0.005 | 0.890 |
First-time LI | 0.048 | 0.124 | 0.085 | 0.004 | −0.006 | 0.852 | 0.060 | 0.048 |
Sugar | −0.049 | 0.318 | 0.056 | 0.228 | −0.037 | 0.455 | 0.045 | 0.344 |
Fat | 0.038 | 0.466 | 0.055 | 0.266 | 0.091 | 0.079 | 0.108 | 0.031 |
Dietary fiber | 0.043 | 0.338 | 0.016 | 0.701 | 0.033 | 0.462 | −0.031 | 0.481 |
Cholesterol | −0.045 | 0.325 | −0.071 | 0.098 | 0.077 | 0.089 | 0.010 | 0.813 |
Digestive system | −0.070 | 0.172 | 0.008 | 0.865 | −0.108 | 0.037 | −0.027 | 0.590 |
Fatigue | 0.070 | 0.128 | 0.018 | 0.685 | 0.091 | 0.046 | −0.001 | 0.988 |
Dietician | −0.058 | 0.130 | 0.003 | 0.933 | −0.107 | 0.005 | −0.018 | 0.628 |
Blogs | 0.028 | 0.461 | 0.033 | 0.364 | 0.099 | 0.010 | 0.039 | 0.293 |
Packaging | 0.055 | 0.116 | −0.035 | 0.296 | 0.020 | 0.574 | 0.008 | 0.814 |
Health effects | 0.110 | 0.009 | 0.055 | 0.166 | 0.094 | 0.025 | 0.104 | 0.010 |
WTP HC | −0.004 | 0.941 | −0.029 | 0.594 | −0.011 | 0.847 | −0.007 | 0.900 |
WTP NC | −0.027 | 0.640 | −0.006 | 0.907 | −0.071 | 0.214 | 0.000 | 0.994 |
R2 | 0.091 | 0.173 | 0.089 | 0.143 |
According to the final retrograde step regression models (Table 20), the FOP shop increased with being a white-collar worker, having better (self-rated) knowledge about healthy nutrition, evaluating the credibility of information on labels higher, attaching importance to the expiry date, and health effects in the marketing communication for food products. Second, the BOP shop increased with one’s knowledge about a healthy diet, better self-rated health, evaluating the quantity of information on labels as insufficient, attaching importance to the list of ingredients in general and during first-time purchases, and attaching importance to the content of fat. Third, reading FOP labels at home increased with the knowledge about a healthy diet, attaching importance to the content of fat and to the impact of the product on lowering cholesterol, attaching importance to culinary blogs, and to health effects in the marketing communications but decreased with the importance attached to the recommendations of a dietician. Finally, reading BOP labels at home depended on buying functional food, one’s knowledge about a healthy diet, evaluation of the quantity of information on food labels, the importance attached to the list of ingredients, to the content of fat, and to health effects in the marketing communication.
Table 20.
Independent Variables | FOP Shop | BOP Shop | FOP Home | BOP Home | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p | β | p | β | p | β | p | |
White-collar | 0.072 | 0.017 | x | x | x | x | x | x |
Functional | x | x | x | x | x | x | 0.062 | 0.039 |
Knowledge | 0.115 | <0.001 | 0.168 | 0.031 | 0.105 | 0.031 | 0.107 | 0.001 |
Health | x | x | 0.065 | 0.030 | x | x | x | x |
Quantity | x | x | 0.076 | 0.029 | x | x | 0.062 | 0.037 |
Credibility | 0.066 | 0.040 | x | x | x | x | x | x |
List of ingredients | x | x | 0.175 | 0.033 | x | x | 0.120 | <0.001 |
Expiry date | 0.116 | <0.001 | x | x | x | x | x | x |
First-time LI | x | x | 0.086 | 0.029 | x | x | x | x |
Fat | x | x | 0.094 | 0.033 | 0.081 | 0.036 | 0.113 | 0.001 |
Cholesterol | x | x | x | x | 0.086 | 0.036 | x | x |
Dietician | x | x | x | x | −0.092 | 0.037 | x | x |
Blogs | x | x | x | x | 0.107 | 0.035 | x | x |
Health effects | 0.096 | 0.003 | x | x | 0.081 | 0.037 | 0.106 | 0.002 |
R2 | 0.066 | 0.152 | 0.068 | 0.124 |
Note: only predictors significant at p < 0.05 are included in these models.
4. Discussion
The main contribution of this paper is the identification of variables that influence food label use in four configurations: FOP in the shop, BOP in the shop, FOP at home, and BOP at home, based on a survey in a large, representative, nation-wide sample of consumers. In retrograde step regression models, only one predictor (self-rated knowledge about healthy diet) turned out to be significant for all four measures of label reading. Two more predictors (importance attached to the content of fat and health effects in marketing communication) were significant in three out of the four investigated forms of reading labels. The remaining independent variables were specific to one or two forms only. It confirms the necessity to study reading labels in fine-grained models, adapted to different types of labels and different contexts of reading. Moreover, this study demonstrated that sociodemographic and behavioral characteristics have limited power of explaining reading labels in multivariate models (except being a white-collar worker for FOP shop and buying functional food for BOP home). Most of the relevant predictors were psychographic, as they concerned the importance attached to selected information types put on the labels or used in the marketing communication as well as the evaluation of the quantity and credibility of information placed on the labels. It is also worth noting that most of the predictors were related to the nutrition and health aspects of communication.
My results differed from some previous findings. In a sample of US adults, significant differences in food label use were observed across all demographic characteristics examined [62]. This difference may stem from: (1) international differences in food label use, (2) a different operationalization of food label use as well as (3) the inclusion of a wide set of psychographic criteria in my multivariate models, which turned out to be better predictors. In another study, determinants of food label use differed by sex. Age and diet quality perception were significant predictors of food label use for both men and women, but ethnicity was significant for males only. Similar to my findings, women checked food label components more often than men [63], but the current study distinguishes various forms and contexts of food label use. Significant differences between men and women were observed for reading BOP labels in the shop and FOP labels at home. Reading food labels has different predictors than the importance attached to some types of information placed on the labels. For instance, another study based on the same dataset demonstrated that the importance attached to salt content information depended on sex and age [64]. Following most previous studies, my results confirmed the impact of nutrition knowledge on food label use. However, interestingly, this relationship may be treated as bidirectional. Cavaliere et al. found that food label use increased the nutritional knowledge of consumers, which in turn favored a healthy diet [65]. It is also important to realize that the valuation of information on the labels is heterogeneous across consumer segments. For instance, Ballco and De Magistris [66] identified three consumer segments: “health-claims oriented”, “nutritional- and health-claim oriented”, and “indifferent”. There is a complexity of targeting nutrition labels because a nonlinear effect of health attitude on the selection of products with increased nutrients content was revealed in previous research [67].
Information may not always be effective in improving food choices. One explanation is that nutrition information is complex and difficult to convey in a clear, actionable manner. In addition, knowledge, while necessary, may not be sufficient to motivate behavior change [68]. The online shopping environment offers new promising tools, such as dynamic food labels with real-time feedback [69], which are not available offline. Even though food label use was associated with improved dietary factors, it was not sufficient alone to modify behavior ultimately leading to improved health outcomes [62]. The perceived credibility of nutrition claims, agreeing that the availability of health-related information is not sufficient for the vast majority of consumers to change their food preferences, and believing that foods carry an excessive number of nutrition claims affected the importance of nutrition claims among food processors and distributors [70]. There is also evidence of mistrust in health claims, as indicated by the negative relationship between the consideration of such claims and the stated importance of “quality” and perceived need to “change dietary quality”—the more discerning shoppers were the least likely to consider health claims [71].
There are a few limitations to this study. The first one is that it is based on self-reported data only. A known limitation of self-report instruments such as surveys and questionnaires is their susceptibility to socially desirable responding. Socially desirable responding is the tendency to give answers that make the respondent look good, or the tendency ‘‘to stretch the truth in an effort to make a good impression’’ [72]. In my survey, I minimized this bias by ensuring the survey anonymity and confidentiality of answers. At the beginning of the questionnaire, the respondents were informed that the survey was anonymous and the results would be used for scientific purposes only. Both self-reported and objective measures of food label use were positively associated with dietary quality. However, self-reported measures appeared to capture a greater motivational component of food label use than did more objective measures [73]. Second, the determination coefficients in my regression models were relatively low, especially for reading FOP labels. This means that there may be other important predictors, which were not included in my study. Nevertheless, taking into consideration the wide range of potential predictors that were examined in my models, it may be also due to the difficulty of assessing the percentage of products the labels of which are read by the given respondent. I opted for the percentage measure rather than a Likert-type scale of frequency because of its higher objectivity. Responses such as “often” or “seldom” can be understood by different consumers differently, e.g., for some people reading 30% of labels may mean “often” and for others “seldom”. That is why I preferred the percentage measures, bearing in mind also the weakness of this measure related to the different numerical skills of respondents. This shortcoming of my study may be overcome by applying a combination of observational methods (ethnography) and surveys in future research.
There are several implications of my results. First, nutrition knowledge improvement programs should be developed, as higher nutrition knowledge was found to translate into a higher interest in reading all kinds of food labels (FOP and BOP) in both contexts (during the purchase and after the purchase), and food label use leads to more favorable food choices for public health. Second, food processors and retailers should more often consider the possibility of emphasizing the health effects of a given product alongside or even instead of other attributes. Third, consumers are particularly sensitive to information about the content of fat. Therefore, the use of nutrition claims and graphical designs of the packaging pointing to the low-fat or zero-fat properties of a product is encouraged. Fourth, retailers should encourage their customers to read the labels at the point of purchase, which may be achieved by allocating special space and offering tools to obtain additional information about the products. Fifth, smartphone apps should be developed to facilitate consumer understanding of information placed on the labels and to facilitate comparisons across products in a given category. Sixth, it is important to pay attention not only to the type of information placed on the labels but also its quantity, and last but not least, credibility. The manufacturers should be selective and focus on the consumer-friendly design of labels. Public authorities should facilitate the use of official nutrition claims and health claims, at the same time keeping high standards of awarding such claims. Public information campaigns should be organized to explain the transparency of the procedures of obtaining health and nutrition claims and the credibility of other types of information on the labels. Finally, public authorities should increase their effort in monitoring the industry so that it adheres to high standards of labeling, including the accuracy, completeness, and visibility of information sought by consumers.
Appendix A
Table A1.
Characteristics | Categories | General Population | Study Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Sex | Women | 52.1 | 53.3 |
Men | 47.9 | 46.7 | |
Age | 15–24 | 12.5 | 15 |
25–34 | 17.8 | 17.3 | |
35–44 | 18.7 | 17.9 | |
45–54 | 14.5 | 13.9 | |
55–64 | 16.5 | 15.7 | |
65 and more | 20 | 20.2 | |
Education | Primary | 49.4 | 47.7 |
Secondary | 31.9 | 31.6 | |
Tertiary | 18.7 | 20.7 | |
Place of living | Urban areas | 60.7 | 61.7 |
Rural areas | 39.3 | 38.3 | |
Region | Dolnośląskie | 7.6 | 7.6 |
Kujawsko-pomorskie | 5.4 | 5.6 | |
Lubelskie | 5.6 | 5.6 | |
Lubuskie | 2.6 | 2.7 | |
Łódzkie | 6.5 | 6.7 | |
Małopolskie | 8.7 | 8.6 | |
Mazowieckie | 13.9 | 13.5 | |
Opolskie | 2.6 | 2.8 | |
Podkarpackie | 5.5 | 5.6 | |
Podlaskie | 3.2 | 3.3 | |
Pomorskie | 5.9 | 6 | |
Śląskie | 11.9 | 11.4 | |
Świętokrzyskie | 3.4 | 3.4 | |
Warmińsko-mazurskie | 3.7 | 3.8 | |
Wielkopolskie | 9 | 9 | |
Zachodniopomorskie | 4.5 | 4.4 |
Note: general population refers to the inhabitants of Poland aged 15 and more.
Table A2.
Variable | Operationalization | Measurement Scale and Coding |
---|---|---|
FOP shop | While shopping, you read the information on the front of the packaging for what share of food products you buy? | % |
BOP shop | While shopping, you read the information on the back of the packaging for what share of food products you buy? | % |
FOP home | At home, you read the information on the front of the packaging for what share of food products you have bought? | % |
BOP home | At home, you read the information on the back of the packaging for what share of food products you have bought? | % |
Woman | Sex | Woman—1, Man - 0 |
Education | Education level | Primary—1, Vocational—2, Secondary—3, Tertiary—4 |
White-collar | Professional activity—a single-choice question with the following catalog of answers: (a) white-collar worker, (b) blue-collar worker, (c) unemployed, (d) student, (e) I don’t work and I take care of the family, (f) old age pensioner/disability pensioner, (g) other. What? | White-collar worker selected—1, White-collar worker not selected—0 |
Student | Professional activity—a single-choice question with the following catalog of answers: (a) white-collar worker, (b) blue-collar worker, (c) unemployed, (d) student, (e) I don’t work and I take care of the family, (f) old age pensioner/disability pensioner, (g) other. What? | Student selected—1, Student not selected—0 |
Income | What is the total monthly net, i.e., disposable, income of all members of your household? A single-choice question with the following catalogue of answers: (a) below 2000 PLN, (b) 2001–3000 PLN, (c) 3001–4000 PLN, (d) 4001–5000 PLN, (e) 5001–6000 PLN, (f) over 6000 PLN |
(a) below 2000 PLN—1, (b) 2001–3000 PLN—2, (c) 3001–4000 PLN—3, (d) 4001–5000 PLN—4, (e) 5001–6000 PLN—5, (f) over 6000 PLN—6 |
Household | Size of your household | Number of persons |
Dietary suppl. | Do you buy the following products? Dietary supplements | Yes—1, No—0, I don’t know—0 |
Organic | Do you buy the following products? Organic food | Yes—1, No—0, I don’t know—0 |
Functional | Do you buy the following products? Functional food | Yes—1, No—0, I don’t know—0 |
Fair trade | Do you buy the following products? Fair trade products | Yes—1, No—0, I don’t know—0 |
Diet | How do you evaluate your diet? | Very healthy—5, Rather healthy—4, Average—3, Rather unhealthy—2, Very unhealthy—1 |
Knowledge | How do you evaluate your knowledge about healthy nutrition? | Very big—5, Rather big—4, Average—3, Rather small—2, Very small—1 |
Health | How do you evaluate your health status? | Very good—5, rather good—4, average—3, rather poor—2, very poor—1 |
Quantity | How do you assess the quantity of the following information on food product packages? | Measured separately for health claims, nutrition claims, and quality signs Too much—1, Appropriate—2, Too little—3 The arithmetical mean |
Understandability | How understandable for you is the following information on food product packages? | Measured separately for health claims, nutrition claims, quality signs, functional food, and organic food Very understandable—5, Rather understandable—4, Average—3, Rather not understandable—2, Completely not understandable—1 The arithmetical mean |
Credibility | How credible for you is the following information on food product packaging? | Measured separately for health claims, nutrition claims, list of ingredients, expiry date, organic certificate, and quality signs Very credible—5, Rather credible—4, Average—3, Rather not credible—2, Definitely not credible—1 The arithmetical mean |
List of ingredients | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? List of ingredients | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Expiry date | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Expiry date | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Institutes | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Recommendations of scientific institutes | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
First-time LI | What constitutes the most important information on a food product label when you buy it for the first time (excluding price)? A single-choice question with the following answer options: (a) country of origin, (b) nutritional information (e.g., about the content of fat or dietary fiber), (c) information about health effects (e.g., good for the bones, lowering cholesterol), (d) list of ingredients, (e) expiry date, (f) other, (g) I don’t know | List of ingredients selected—1, List of ingredients not selected—0 |
Sugar | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Content of sugars | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Fat | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Content of fats | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Dietary fiber | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Dietary fiber | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Cholesterol | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Impact of the product on lowering cholesterol | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Digestive system | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Impact of the product on the digestive system | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Fatigue | How important for you is the following information on food product packages? Impact of the product reducing tiredness and fatigue | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Dietician | In your opinion, how important are the following ways of promoting food products with health and nutrition claims? Recommendations of a dietician | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Blogs | In your opinion, how important are the following ways of promoting food products with health and nutrition claims? Culinary blogs | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Packaging | In your opinion, how important are the following ways of promoting food products with health and nutrition claims? Food packages | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
Health effects | How important for you is the following information concerning food products? Health effects of consuming a given product | Very important—5, Rather important—4, Average—3, Rather not important—2, Without any importance—1 |
WTP HC | Are you willing to pay more for products with health claims (compared to similar products without such claims)? | Definitely yes—5, Rather yes—4, I don’t know—3, Rather not—2, Definitely not—1 |
WTP NC | Are you willing to pay more for products with nutrition claims (compared to similar products without such claims)? | Definitely yes—5, Rather yes—4, I don’t know—3, Rather not—2, Definitely not—1 |
Funding
This research was funded by the National Science Centre, Opus grant numbers 2017/25/B/HS4/00031 and 2015/17/B/HS4/00253. The APC was funded by the National Science Centre, Opus grant number 2017/25/B/HS4/00031.
Conflicts of Interest
The author declares no conflict of interest. The funder had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.
References
- 1.Mandal B. Use of food labels as a weight loss behaviour. J. Consum. Aff. 2010;44:516–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2010.01181.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chen X., Jahns L., Gittelsohn J., Wang Y. Who is missing the message? Targeting strategies to increase food label use among US adults. Public Health Nutr. 2012;15:760–772. doi: 10.1017/S1368980011002242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Smith S., Taylor J., Stephen A. Use of food labels and beliefs about diet-disease relationships among university students. Public Health Nutr. 2000;3:175–182. doi: 10.1017/S1368980000000203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ozimek I., Tomaszewska-Pielacha M. Czynniki wpływające na czytanie przez konsumentów informacji zamieszczanych na opakowaniach produktów żywnościowych. Studies Proc. Polish Assoc. Knowl. Manag. 2011;52:26–35. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Niewczas M. Kryteria wyboru żywności. Żywność. Nauka. Technologia. Jakość. 2013;6:204–219. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Macon J., Oakland M., Jensen H., Kissack P. Food label use by older Americans. J. Nutr. Elderly. 2004;24:35–52. doi: 10.1300/J052v24n01_03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Besler H., Buyuktuncer Z., Uyar M. Consumer understanding and use of food and nutrition labeling in Turkey. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2012;44:584–591. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2012.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Vemula S., Gavaravarapu S., Mendu V., Mathur P., Avula L. Use of food label information by urban consumers in India – a study among supermarket shoppers. Public Health Nutr. 2014;17:2104–2114. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Nayga R., Lipinski D., Savur N. Consumers’ use of nutritional labels while food shopping and at home. J. Consum. Aff. 1998;32:106–120. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.1998.tb00402.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Moreira M., García-Díez J., de Almeida J., Saraiva C. Evaluation of food labelling usefulness for consumers. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2019;43:327–334. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mulders M., Corneille O., Klein O. Label reading, numeracy and food & nutrition involvement. Appetite. 2018;128:214–222. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nayga R. Nutrition knowledge, gender, and food label use. J. Consum. Aff. 2005;34:97–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2000.tb00086.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Al-Barqi R., Al-Salem Y., Mahrous L., Abu Abat E., Al-Quraishi R., Benajiba N. Understanding barriers towards the use of food labels among Saudi female college students. Mal J Nutr. 2020;26:19–30. doi: 10.31246/mjn-2019-0117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cavaliere A., De Marchi E., Banterle A. Does consumer health-orientation affect the use of nutrition facts panel and claims? An empirical analysis in Italy. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016;54:110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.07.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nestorowicz R., Pilarczyk B. Wyzwania wobec komunikacji marketingowej na rynku żywności ekologicznej w Polsce – w świetle badań konsumentów. Marketing i Rynek. 2014;8:583–589. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hess R., Visschers V., Siegrist M. The role of health-related, motivational and sociodemographic aspects in predicting food label use: A comprehensive study. Public Health Nutr. 2012;15:407–414. doi: 10.1017/S136898001100156X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vijaykumar S., Lwin M., Chao J., Au C. Determinants of food label use among supermarket shoppers: A Singaporean perspective. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2013;45:204–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2012.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cha E., Kim K., Lerner H., Dawkins C., Bello M., Umpierrez G., Dunbar S. Health literacy, self-efficacy, food label use, and diet in young adults. Am. J. Health Behav. 2014;38:331–339. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.38.3.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Walters A., Long M. The effect of food label cues on perceptions of quality and purchase intentions among high-involvement consumers with varying levels of nutrition knowledge. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2012;44:350–354. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2011.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Limbu Y., McKinley C., Gautam R., Ahirwar A., Dubey P., Jayachandran C. Nutritional knowledge, attitude, and use of food labels among Indian adults with multiple chronic conditions: A moderated mediation model. Brit. Food J. 2019;121:1480–1494. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-09-2018-0568. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Soederberg Miller L., Cassady D. The effects of nutrition knowledge on food label use. A review of the literature. Appetite. 2015;92:207–216. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kreuter M., Brennan L., Scharff D., Lukwago S. Do nutrition label readers eat healthier diets? Behavioral correlates of adults’ use of food labels. Am. J. Prev. Med. 1997;13:277–283. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30175-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Amuta-Jimenez A., Lo C., Talwar D., Khan N., Barry A. Food label literacy and use among US adults diagnosed with cancer: Results from a national representative study. J. Cancer Educ. 2019;34:1000–1009. doi: 10.1007/s13187-018-1403-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kim S., Nayga R., Capps O. Food label use, self-selectivity, and diet quality. J. Consum. Aff. 2005;35:346–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2001.tb00118.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lin C., Lee J., Yen S. Do dietary intakes affect search for nutrient information on food labels? Soc. Sci. Med. 2004;59:1955–1967. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.02.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Drichoutis A., Lazaridis P., Nayga R. Nutrition knowledge and consumer use of nutritional food labels. Eur. Rev. Agric. Econ. 2005;32:93–118. doi: 10.1093/erae/jbi003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lewis J., Arheart K., LeBlanc W., Fleming L., Lee D., Davila E., Cabán-Martinez A., Dietz N., McCollister K., Bandiera F., et al. Food label use and awareness of nutritional information and recommendations among persons with chronic disease. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009;90:1351–1357. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Laz T., Rahman M., Berenson A. Association of frequent use of food labels with weight loss behaviors among low-income reproductive-age women. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2015;34:73–79. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2014.891957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Anastasiou K., Miller M., Dickinson K. The relationship between food label use and dietary intake in adults: A systematic review. Appetite. 2019;138:280–291. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wilson M., Ramírez A., Arsenault J., Miller L. Nutrition label use and its association with dietary quality among Latinos: The roles of poverty and acculturation. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2018;50:876–887. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2018.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Jacob R., Drapeau V., Lamarche B., Doucet É., Pomerleau S., Provencher V. Associations among eating behaviour traits, diet quality and food labelling: A mediation model. Public Health Nutr. 2020;23:631–641. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019003203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ducrot P., Méjean C., Julia C., Kesse-Guyot E., Touvier M., Fezeu L., Hercberg S., Péneau S. Objective understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels among nutritionally at-risk individuals. Nutrients. 2015;7:7106–7125. doi: 10.3390/nu7085325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Borgmeier I., Westenhoefer J. Impact of different food label formats on healthiness evaluation and food choice of consumers: A randomized-controlled study. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:184. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Talati Z., Egnell M., Hercberg S., Julia C., Pettigrew S. Consumers’ perceptions of five front-of-package nutrition labels: An experimental study across 12 countries. Nutrients. 2019;11:1934. doi: 10.3390/nu11081934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Hagmann D., Siegrist M. Nutri-Score, multiple traffic light and incomplete nutrition labelling on food packages: Effects on consumers’ accuracy in identifying healthier snack options. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020;83:103894. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103894. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Andreeva V., Egnell M., Handjieva-Darlenska T., Talati Z., Touvier M., Galan P., Hercberg S., Pettigrew S., Julia C. Bulgarian consumers’ objective understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels: A comparative randomized study. Arch. Public Health. 2020;78:35. doi: 10.1186/s13690-020-00416-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Graham D., Heidrick C., Hodgin K. Nutrition label viewing during a food-selection task: Front-of-package labels vs. Nutrition Facts labels. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015;115:1636–1646. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2015.02.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Goyal R., Deshmukh N. Food label reading: Read before you eat. J. of Education and Health Promotion. 2018;7:56. doi: 10.4103/jehp.jehp_35_17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Elshiewy O., Boztug Y. When back of pack meets front of pack: How salient and simplified nutrition labels affect food sales in supermarkets. J. Public Policy Mark. 2018;37:55–67. doi: 10.1509/jppm.16.100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Temple N., Fraser J. Food labels: A critical assessment. Nutrition. 2014;30:257–260. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2013.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ares G., Antúnez L., Ottrbring T., Curutchet M., Galicia L., Moratorio X., Bove I. Sick, salient and full of salt, sugar and fat: Understanding the impact of nutritional warnings on consumers’ associations through the salience bias. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020;86:103991. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103991. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Temple N. Front-of-package food labels: A narrative review. Appetite. 2020;144:104485. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.El-Abbadi N., Taylor S., Micha R., Blumberg J. Nutrient profiling systems, front of pack labeling and consumer behavior. Curr. Atheroscler. Rep. 2020;22:36. doi: 10.1007/s11883-020-00857-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ikonen I., Sotgiu F., Aydinli A., Verlegh P. Consumer effects of front-of-package nutrition labeling: An interdisciplinary meta-analysis. J. Acad. Market. Sci. 2020;48:360–383. doi: 10.1007/s11747-019-00663-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ozimek I., Żakowska-Biemans S., Gutkowska K. Polish consumers’ perception of food-related risks. Pol. J. Food Nutr. Sci. 2009;59:189–192. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Jeżewska-Zychowicz M., Jeznach M., Kosicka-Gębska M. Consumers’ interests in sweets with health-promoting properties and their selected determinants. Pol. J. Food Nutr. Sci. 2013;63:43–48. doi: 10.2478/v10222-012-0062-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Bryła P. Organic food consumption in Poland: Motives and barriers. Appetite. 2016;105:737–746. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.07.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bryła P. Marketing Regionalnych I Ekologicznych Produktów Żywnościowych. Perspektywa Sprzedawcy I Konsumenta. Lodz University Press; Lodz, Poland: 2015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cavaliere A., Ricci E., Banterle A. Nutrition and health claims: Who is interested? An empirical analysis of consumer preferences in Italy. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015;41:44–51. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.11.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Grunert K., Scholderer J., Rogeaux M. Determinants of consumer understanding of health claims. Appetite. 2011;56:269–277. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hung Y., Grunert K., Hoefkens C., Hieke S., Verbeke W. Motivation outweighs ability in explaining European consumers’ use of health claims. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017;58:34–44. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kozirok W., Marciszewicz E., Babicz-Zielińska E. Postawy i zachowania kobiet wobec żywności prozdrowotnej. Studia i Prace WNEiZ US. 2016;43:199–208. doi: 10.18276/sip.2016.43/3-18. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mackison D., Wrieden W., Anderson A. Validity and reliability testing of a short questionnaire developed to assess consumers’ use, understanding and perception of food labels. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010;64:210–217. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2009.126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Menrad K., Sparke K. Consumers’ Attitudes And Expectations Concerning Functional Food. University of Applied Sciences of Weihenstephan; Straubing, Germany: 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Nestorowicz R. Asymetria Wiedzy A Aktywność Informacyjna Konsumentów Na Rynku Produktów Żywnościowych. University of Economics in Poznan; Poznan, Poland: 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Ravoniarison A., Gollety M. L’effet «I can do it!»: Rôle du sentiment d’efficacité personnelle dans la satisfaction à l’égard des aliments santé à orientation fonctionnelle. Décisions Marketing. 2017;85:29–47. doi: 10.7193/DM.085.29.47. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Riley M., Bowen J., Krause D., Jones D., Stonehouse W. A survey of consumer attitude towards nutrition and health statements on food labels in South Australia. Funct. Foods in Health Dis. 2016;6:809–821. doi: 10.31989/ffhd.v6i12.306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Strijbos C., Schluck M., Bisschop J., Bui T., de Jong I., van Leeuwen M., von Tottleben M., van Breda S. Consumer awareness and credibility factors of health claims on innovative meat products in a cross-sectional population study in the Netherlands. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016;54:13–22. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.06.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Van Trijp H., van der Lans I. Consumer perceptions of nutrition and health claims. Appetite. 2007;48:305–324. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bryła P. Oświadczenia Zdrowotne I Żywieniowe na Rynku Produktów Żywnościowych. Lodz University Press; Lodz, Poland: 2020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Wasserstein R., Schirm A., Lazar N. Moving to a world beyond “p<0.05”. Am. Stat. 2019;73:1–19. doi: 10.1080/00031305.2019.1583913. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Ollberding N., Wolf R., Contento I. Food label use and its relation to dietary intake among US adults. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011;111:S47–S51. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2011.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Stran K., Knol L. Determinants of food label use differ by sex. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2013;113:673–679. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.12.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bryła P. Selected predictors of the importance attached to salt content information on the food packaging (a study among Polish consumers) Nutrients. 2020;12:293. doi: 10.3390/nu12020293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cavaliere A., Siletti E., Banterle A. Nutrition information, Mediterranean diet, and weight: A structural equation approach. Agric. Econ. Czech. 2020;66:10–18. doi: 10.17221/25/2019-AGRICECON. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ballco P., De Magistris T. Spanish consumer purchase behaviour and stated preferences for yoghurts with nutritional and health claims. Nutrients. 2019;11:2742. doi: 10.3390/nu11112742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Almli V., Asioli D., Rocha C. Organic consumer choices for nutrient labels on dried strawberries among different health attitude segments in Norway, Romania, and Turkey. Nutrients. 2019;11:2951. doi: 10.3390/nu11122951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Guthrie J., Mancino L., Lin C. Nudging consumers toward better food choices: Policy approaches to changing food consumption behaviors. Psychol. Market. 2015;32:501–511. doi: 10.1002/mar.20795. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Shin S. The effects of dynamic food labels with real-time feedback on diet quality: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Nutrients. 2020;12:2158. doi: 10.3390/nu12072158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Bryła P. Selected antecedents of the importance of nutrition claims for food processors and distributors. J. Agribus. Rural Dev. 2019;52:103–110. doi: 10.17306/J.JARD.2019.01117. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Petrovici D., Fearne A., Nayga R., Drolias D. Nutritional knowledge, nutritional labels, and health claims on food: A study of supermarket shoppers in the South East of England. Brit. Food J. 2012;114:768–783. doi: 10.1108/00070701211234318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Martin C., Nagao D. Some effects of computerized interviewing on job applicant responses. J. Appl. Psychol. 1989;74:72–80. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.1.72. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Soederberg Miller L., Cassady D., Applegate E., Beckett L., Wilson M., Gibson T., Ellwood K. Relationships among food label use, motivation, and dietary quality. Nutrients. 2015;7:1068–1080. doi: 10.3390/nu7021068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]