Abstract
Background
Increasing research focuses on ethnic differences in Alzheimer’s disease, but such efforts in other neurodegenerative dementias are lacking. Currently, data on the ethnic profile of cognitive impaired persons with Lewy body disease (LBD) is limited, despite Lewy body dementia being the second most common neurodegenerative dementia.
Objective
The study aimed to investigate presenting characteristics among ethnoracially diverse individuals with cognitive impairment secondary to LBD using the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center database.
Methods
Participants self-identified as African American, Hispanic, or White. We used Kruskal-Wallis and Pearson χ2 analyses to investigate group differences in presenting characteristics and linear regression to compare neuropsychological test performance.
Results
Presentation age was similar between groups (median 74–75 years). Compared to Whites (n=1782), African Americans (n=130) and Hispanics (n=122) were more likely to be female and single, have less educational attainment, report more cardiovascular risk factors, describe less medication use, and perform worse on select cognitive tests. Hispanics reported more depressive symptoms.
Conclusion
Cohorts differences highlight the need for population-based LBD studies with racial-ethnic diversity. Culturally-sensitive neuropsychological tests are needed to determine whether observed differences relate to cultural, social, testing, or disease-related factors. More research is needed regarding how social and biological factors impact LBD care among diverse populations.
Keywords: Lewy body disease, dementia, mild cognitive impairment, demography, ethnic groups, Alzheimer’s disease centers
INTRODUCTION
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) research highlights ethnoracial differences in prevalence rates [1–3], neuropathological disease characteristics [4–6], progression [7, 8], and treatment [3, 9]. Data suggests that African Americans are twice as likely to have AD compared to White non-Hispanics, while Hispanics are 1.5 times more likely to have AD than White non-Hispanics [3]. Further, Hispanics and African Americans often show a more severe AD profile compared to White non-Hispanics, including earlier age of onset and greater severity of cognitive impairment at initial presentation [10]. Efforts to explain observed differences suggest that differences can be attenuated, or even eliminated, when adjusting for factors that may influence and/or interact with ethnicity in AD risk and presentation [1, 3, 11]. Research shows that disparities in the prevalence, presentation, and disease progression among Hispanics and African American can be impacted by biological risk factors, such as genetics (e.g. APOE ε4 status) and greater cardiovascular disease, as well as sociodemographic variables and social determinants of health (e.g. lower educational attainment, less access to quality of care, etc) [2, 3, 12].
Several organizations, including the National Institute of Health [13] and the Alzheimer’s Association International Society to Advance Alzheimer’s Research and Treatment [12], identify increasing diversity as an immediate need for advancing AD/AD related dementia (ADRD) research. Although understanding of how ethnoracial factors interfaces with AD expression and treatment is incomplete, such knowledge exceeds data on ethnoracial influence in other neurodegenerative diseases. The most recent ADRD Summit acknowledged that data regarding the epidemiology and clinical course of ADRD subtypes (e.g. Lewy body diseases, frontotemporal dementia) are limited within underrepresented minorities [14].
Lewy Body Diseases (LBDs) are neurodegenerative diseases associated with alpha-synuclein protein aggregations. LBDs with cognitive impairment include dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), a prodromal DLB state with mild cognitive impairment (MCI; DLB-MCI), Parkinson’s Disease (PD) with MCI, and PD with dementia (PDD). Together, DLB and PDD are termed Lewy body dementia. As a single entity, Lewy body dementia is the second most common neurodegenerative dementia after AD [15]. The associated MCI populations are sometimes termed “pre-dementia LBD” [14].
The ADRD Summit 2019 Report [14] identifies the need for observational studies with diverse populations across ADRDs and increased availability and use of culturally- and linguistically-valid assessment tools in aging populations. Additionally, the Report promotes diverse longitudinal clinical cohorts and clinical trial populations in Lewy body dementia. Currently, LBD ethnic profile data is scant, limiting understanding of risk factors, etiology, prognosis, and other variables that may affect disease expression among diverse populations. In view of national priorities targeting increased diversity in LBD cohorts, we aimed to identify the presentation of diverse ethnoracial groups diagnosed with cognitive impairment suspected secondary to LBD using data from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Uniform Data Set (UDS) (NACC-UDS).
METHODS
Setting
Data for the current study were obtained from the NACC-UDS, a database composed of longitudinal data that comprised of over 35,000 participants at the time we received our file from Alzheimer’s Disease Center (ADC) programs in the United States [16]. Study approval was obtained from the Institutional Review Board for each participating NACC site. NACC recruitment and data collection has been described previously [16–19]. Data for the current analysis represents data collected from September 2005 to the December 2018 data freeze.
Participants
Inclusion criteria for the current analysis were: (1) clinical diagnosis of MCI or dementia, (2) presumptive etiologic diagnosis of LBD as the primary or contributing cause of cognitive impairment at the participant’s most recent study visit, (3) age 45 years or older, and (4) participant self-identification as Hispanic, White, or African American. We extracted data from the first visit associated with a diagnosis of MCI or dementia. For NACC participants who were cognitively healthy at baseline and progressed to MCI/dementia, we utilized data from the first visit where a diagnosis of MCI or dementia (for those who went from cognitively normal to dementia, skipping a MCI diagnosis) was entered.
Demographics & Measures
Participants in the current analysis participated in data collection through NACC-UDS versions 1–3. Participants were classified as White non-Hispanic, African American non-Hispanic, or Hispanic (associated with White or African-American race) based on self-report. Other collected demographics included: age at visit, sex, educational attainment, visit year, marital status, and additional lifestyle factors (e.g. living situation, type of residence, and independence status). Extracted medical data included self/informant-report of recent or remote history of diabetes, stroke, hypertension, and hypercholesterolemia and current antidepressant, antipsychotic, antiparkinson agent, cholinesterase inhibitor, and memantine use.
Clinical variables of interest included functional, psychiatric, and cognitive data. The Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ) [20] informed functional status; the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) motor subscale [21] assessed parkinsonism. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory- Questionnaire (NPI-Q) [22] and the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [23] assessed psychiatric symptoms. Cognitive measures included the CDR® Dementia Staging Instrument [24], Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [25], Boston Naming Test (BNT) [26], Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Logical Memory subtest [27], Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – R (WAIS-R) Digit Span Forward and Backward subtests [28], WAIS-R Digit Symbol Coding subtest [28], Trail Making Test (TMT) A & B [29], and Animals and Vegetable Fluency [30].
Statistical analyses
Continuous and categorical variables were summarized by medians and proportions, respectively. Kruskal-Wallis and Pearson χ2 analyses assessed between-group differences for continuous and categorical variables. Significant main effects were followed up by pairwise comparisons using Bonferroni-correction. We conducted simultaneous linear regressions to explore associations of ethnoracial differences on neuropsychological performance adjusting for significant group demographic differences, including education (continuous), sex (female/male; reference group: male), and cardiovascular disease risk factors (hypertension, diabetes; reference group: no reported history of risk factor). To account for proportion differences in diagnostic severity (MCI vs. dementia) between group, we used the CDR® Dementia Staging Instrument global score (continuous, 0–3) to control for cognitive severity. Neuropsychological scores were modeled as unstandardized continuous variables. Ethnoracial status was dummy-coded prior to analysis and “White” was the comparison group. Multicollinearity was checked for all study variables by using correlations, tolerances, and variance inflation factors (VIF). Correlations were sufficiently low (r < .31), tolerance scores were greater than .8, and VIF scores were below 2. Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.0001 to adjust for multiple comparisons. Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 25.0 (Armonk, NY).
RESULTS
Data from 1782 Whites, 130 African Americans, and 122 Hispanics were available for analysis. Hispanics with dementia self-identified from Mexican origin (29%), followed by Puerto Rican (15%), Cuban (14%), Dominican (12%), South American (11%), and Central American (8%) origin; 11% of the sample did not specify country of origin. Hispanics with MCI identified themselves from Mexican (26%), Cuban (23%), Dominican (16%), Central American (13%), South American (13%), Puerto Rican (3%), and unspecified (6%) origin. Seventy two percent of Hispanics (N=88) identified Spanish as their primary language, compared to 99% of African Americans and Whites who identified English as their primary language. Fifty percent of (N=62) Hispanics were administered the Spanish version of the UDS neuropsychological test battery. Of the total sample, 65% had a dementia diagnosis. Table 1 includes breakdown of cognitive severity for each ethnoracial group. ADC site stratified by ethnoracial group is available in Supplemental Table 1.
Table 1.
White | African American | Hispanic | P value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Female, % | 26a | 60b | 41c | <0.0001 |
Age at visit, median (range) | 74(46–100) | 75(52–91) | 75(50–93) | .553 |
Visit year, median (range) | 2008(2005–2018) | 2009(2005–2018) | 2009(2005–2018) | .203 |
Education, % | <0.0001 | |||
<12 | 4a | 22b | 41c | |
12 | 18 | 22 | 22 | |
>12 | 77a | 56b | 37c | |
Cognitive status, % | .005 | |||
mild cognitive impairment | 36.5 | 28 | 25 | |
dementia | 63.5 | 72 | 75 | |
Marital status, % | <0.0001 | |||
Married | 80a | 43b | 57b | |
Widowed | 11a | 34b | 18a | |
Divorced/separated | 4a | 16b | 16b | |
Other or unknown | 5 | 7 | 9 | |
Living situation, % | <0.0001 | |||
alone | 9a | 23b | 12a | |
with spouse | 78a | 41b | 56b | |
with relative or friend | 5a | 30b | 22b | |
group | 4 | 3 | 7 | |
other | 5 | 2 | 4 | |
Type of residence, % | 0.848 | |||
single | 90 | 90 | 88 | |
other | 10 | 10 | 12 | |
Independence status, % | .005 | |||
independent | 31 | 30 | 24 | |
assistance w/ IADLS | 40 | 28 | 37 | |
assistance w/ ADLS | 20 | 30 | 23 | |
completely dependent | 10 | 13 | 17 | |
Health history, % | ||||
diabetes | 10a | 26b | 21b | <0.0001 |
stroke | 5 | 10 | 10 | .008 |
hypertension | 48a | 78b | 62c | <0.0001 |
hypercholesterolemia | 49 | 54 | 56 | .245 |
Note: White education n = 1773, Hispanic education n = 121, White diabetes n = 1754, African American diabetes n = 129, Hispanic diabetes n = 118, White stroke n = 1743, African American stroke n = 129, Hispanic stroke n = 118, White hypertension n = 1744, Hispanic hypertension n = 117, White hypercholesterolemia n = 1735, African American hypercholesterolemia n = 127, Hispanic hypercholesterolemia n = 117, White living situation n = 1775, African American living situation n = 129, Hispanic living situation n = 121, White type of residence n = 1749, African American type of residence n = 125, Hispanic type of residence n = 119, White independence status n = 1765, African American independence status n = 128, Hispanic independence status n = 119
Note: Proportions with different alphabetic superscript are statistically significant using post hoc Bonferroni-correction. Identical alphabetic superscript means they did not significantly differ
Sample characteristics
The average age at presentation with a cognitive diagnosis was similar across cohorts (Table 1). African Americans and Hispanics were more likely to be female than White participants. African Americans and Hispanics also had less educational attainment, were more likely to be single (widowed or divorced/separated), and were more likely to live alone than White participants, though functional status was not statistically different between groups. African-Americans and Hispanics reported a history of diabetes and hypertension more commonly than White participants.
Psychiatric symptoms
Ethnoracial groups did not significantly differ across individual symptoms or total symptom severity measured by the NPI-Q (Table 2). However, Hispanics self-reported more depressive symptoms on the GDS compared to African Americans and Whites. Additionally, a larger percentage of Hispanics fell within the range of suggestive depression (GDS score of ≥6) than Whites and African Americans.
Table 2.
White n = 1722 | African American n=126 | Hispanic n = 116 | p value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
NPI-Q total severity, median (range) | 3(0–28) | 4(0–25) | 5(0–20) | .002 |
NPI-Q =>4, % | 46 | 50 | 58 | .037 |
NPI symptom | ||||
Delusion, % | 18 | 25 | 28 | .016 |
Hallucination, % | 25 | 29 | 35 | .027 |
Agitation, % | 31 | 37 | 38 | .111 |
Depression, % | 44 | 44 | 60 | .006 |
Anxiety, % | 42 | 33 | 55 | .002 |
Euphoria, % | 4 | 6 | 6 | .543 |
Apathy, % | 46 | 44 | 44 | .848 |
Disinhibition, % | 18 | 18 | 16 | .927 |
Irritability, % | 39 | 37 | 40 | .866 |
Motor, % | 19 | 27 | 26 | .035 |
Sleep, % | 47 | 48 | 51 | .735 |
Appetite and eating, % | 28 | 33 | 35 | .095 |
GDS total, median (range) | 3(0–15)a | 2(0–15)a | 4(0–15)b | <0.0001 |
GDS => 6; % | 21a | 19a | 39b | <0.0001 |
Abbreviation: NPI, Neuropsychiatric Inventory- Questionnaire; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale.
White NPI-Q total n = 1708; White delusions, hallucination, euphoria n = 1720; White agitation, disinhibition, irritability, appetite n = 1721; White sleep n = 1719; White GDS n = 1548; African American GDS n = 108, Hispanic GDS n = 108
Note: Proportions with different alphabetic superscript are statistically significant using post hoc Bonferroni-correction. Identical alphabetic superscript means they did not significantly differ
Cognition
After adjusting for sex, education, hypertension, diabetes, and cognitive severity, Hispanics performed worse on Digit Span Forward and the BNT, whereas African Americans performed worse on Digit Symbol, TMT A, BNT, and animal fluency compared to Whites (Table 3). Full linear regression model results are available in Supplemental Table 2. Stratifying the sample using clinical diagnosis (MCI or dementia), rather than using the CDR® Dementia Staging Instrument to account for proportion differences in clinical diagnosis categories, did not meaningful change outcomes, except for having less precision relating to smaller sample sizes (Supplemental Tables 3, 4).
Table 3.
Measure | Group* | B | SE B | β | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
MMSE | African American | −.751 | .406 | −.030 | .065 |
Hispanic | −.301 | .451 | −.011 | .504 | |
DSF | African American | −.245 | .229 | −.025 | .284 |
Hispanic | −1.588 | .246 | −.157 | <0.0001 | |
DSB | African American | −.630 | .208 | −.072 | .003 |
Hispanic | −.501 | .222 | −.055 | .024 | |
Digit Symbol | African American | −5.384 | 1.396 | −.097 | <0.0001 |
Hispanic | −2.442 | 1.518 | −.042 | .108 | |
TMTA | African American | 16.430 | 3.928 | ..094 | <.0001 |
Hispanic | 9.954 | 4.293 | .053 | .021 | |
TMTB | African American | 28.955 | 11.142 | .073 | .009 |
Hispanic | 21.908 | 11.897 | .051 | .066 | |
BNT | African American | −5.633 | .531 | −.217 | <.0001 |
Hispanic | −3.799 | .576 | −.139 | <.0001 | |
Animals fluency | African American | −2.068 | .510 | −.084 | <0.0001 |
Hispanic | −.781 | .543 | −.031 | .151 | |
Vegetable fluency | African American | −.518 | .359 | −.030 | .149 |
Hispanic | −1.045 | .385 | −.059 | .007 | |
Immediate memory | African American | −.204 | .434 | −.011 | .639 |
Hispanic | −.074 | .467 | −.004 | .874 | |
Delayed memory | African American | −.341 | .435 | −.019 | .433 |
Hispanic | −.195 | .473 | −.010 | .680 |
Abbreviation: MMSE, Mini-Mental Status Exam; DSF, Digit Span Forward; DSB, Digit Span Backward; TMTA, Trail Making Test A, TMTB, Trail Making Test B; BNT, Boston Naming Test
White cohort was inputted as the referent group
Other symptoms
No ethnoracial differences were evidence across everyday functional abilities (FAQ) (median scores: White-16, African-American-17, Hispanic-17; p=.468), or degree of motor symptoms (UPDRS) (median scores: White-16, African-American-15, Hispanic-14; p=.845).
Treatment
The only treatment difference observed between groups was that Whites were significantly more likely to report current use of an antiparkinson agent (Table 4).
Table 4.
White (n =1769) | African American (n=127) | Hispanic (n=120) | p value | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Antidepressant | 45 | 28 | 46 | .001 |
Antipsychotic | 14 | 17 | 21 | .121 |
Alzheimer’s medications | 55 | 41 | 46 | .001 |
Antiparkinson agent | 34a | 18b | 23b | <0.0001 |
Note: Proportions with different alphabetic superscript are statistically significant using post hoc Bonferroni-correction. Identical alphabetic superscript means they did not significantly differ
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to describe how ethnically diverse NACC participants diagnosed with LBD compare sociodemographically and clinically. Compared to Whites, African Americans and Hispanics were more likely to be female and single, and have less educational attainment, higher self-reported rates of diabetes and hypertension, less Parkinson medication use, and worse performance on attention, naming and executive functioning measures. In addition, Hispanics were more likely to report depressive symptoms. Group differences were present between Hispanics and African Americans, but more frequently observed between White and ethnic minority participants.
Sex differences varied dramatically by ethnoracial background, with females representing 60% of the African American NACC LBD cohort, 41% of the Hispanic cohort, and only 26% of the White cohort. A less prominent sex difference was reported in a NACC pathology study including non-Hispanic African American and White cohorts with various dementias (64.5% and 42.5% female, respectively) [31]. However, our finding is in contrast to prior data reporting that LBD is more common in men in both clinical [32] and pathological [15, 33, 34] cohorts, largely reflecting White populations [15, 32, 34]. Differences in our findings compared to LBD literature could reflect our inclusion of all cases where LBD was suspected as a primary or contributing cause of cognitive impairment. Females are 1.5 times more likely than males to have a documented diagnosis of probable AD [35] and African-Americans and Hispanics are more frequently diagnosed with AD [3], potentially skewing our study demographics. Gender differences in dementia are partly attributed to the longer life expectancy of women in the United States (81.3 vs 76.3 years), differences which vary by racial-ethnic background (White 81.5 vs 76.7 years; African-American 78.5 vs 72.3 years; Hispanic 85.4 vs 80.2 years) [36]. Particularly given the average age of participants in our cohorts, survival differences alone are an unlikely explanation for the marked gender differences between groups, but may contribute. Additionally, these findings may reflect that NACC is a convenience rather than population-based sample, and thus differences may occur for non-biologic reasons. Existing research shows no clear link between subject or caregiver gender and willingness to enroll an individual in research,[37, 38] but this area of research remains largely unexplored. While the observed sex differences may be partly explainable by these or other factors, the observed differences emphasize the need for diverse enrollment in LBD cohorts and for population-based studies performed in diverse communities [14].
While our study did not investigate co-informant type, we found that Hispanic and African American participants were more likely to be single and living alone. This is similar to a study of dementia caregiving across racial-ethnic groups, where the frequency of spouse caregivers was highest for White participants. Adult children and other family were more common caregivers for Mexican American and African American participants [39]. Our observation of lower educational attainment in Hispanic and African American populations compared to White populations is consistent with prior clinical and population-based studies [10, 40]. Similarly, our finding that Hispanics and African Americans have higher reported rates of hypertension and diabetes compared to White non-Hispanics matches prior reports [1, 41, 42].
Hispanics had slightly higher GDS scores and more individuals who fell within the suggestive depression range using GDS cutoff score >5. While qualitative inspection of data revealed that the degree of difference was relatively small, these results are consistent with prior data that Hispanics with AD experience more depressive and neuropsychiatric symptoms than White and African American populations [43]. We found no difference in NPI-Q total and subscale scores between groups, but a prior NACC analysis performed using data from Hispanic and White non-Hispanic participants with normal cognition or dementia suggested that NPI-Q scores may not be comparable across these ethnic groups due to lack of ethnic-group scalar invariance [44]. NPI-Q scores were higher in Hispanic than White participants in that study and authors theorized differences could relate to ethnocultural differences (including perceived stigma, informant reporting), differences in education, and/or language barriers [44].
Cohort differences in antidepressant use were not statistically significant, though the point frequency for antidepressant use was much lower for African-Americans than the other cohorts. Antipsychotic medication use was also similar between cohorts, in contrast to a prior NACC study where Hispanics had a greater use of antipsychotic medications compared to White non-Hispanics [45]. White participants had a statistically higher frequency of antiparkinsonian medication use and a non-significant higher use of medications for dementia in our cohorts. These results are consistent with studies suggesting that ethnic minorities, particularly African Americans, are less likely to receive anti-dementia pharmacological treatment than White patients [9, 46–48].
Hispanics and African Americans had poorer performance in naming, attention, and executive functioning compared to White participants after adjusting for sex, educational attainment, hypertension, diabetes, and cognitive severity. Whether these differences relate to test characteristics, analysis, biopsychosocial factors, comorbidities, or differences in LBD expression is uncertain. Research suggests that adjusting for years of education has limited utility in correcting for educational inequities across ethnic groups [11]. One test with notably lower performance across Hispanics and African Americans compared to Whites was the BNT. BNT performance is significantly influenced by culture [49], a construct for which race and ethnicity are often a proxy. Based on our findings that African Americans and Hispanics reported more frequent cerebrovascular risk factors, it is plausible that cerebrovascular disease played a role in performance on processing speed and executive function measures such as TMT and Digit Span backwards [50]. However, including reported history of hypertension and diabetes did not eliminate the effect of ethnicity on performance. While reported history of cardiovascular disease risk factors may be a weak proxy of cerebrovascular burden, inclusion of neuroimaging parameters was outside the scope of the current analysis. It is also possible that poorer performance on attention and executive functioning among African Americans and Hispanics is indicative of the limits on statistical adjustments for disease severity since the ethnic minority groups had more individuals presenting at dementia vs MCI stages, though the differences between groups were non-significant (see Table 1).
To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine differences in ethnically diverse individuals diagnosed with LBD. Study results emphasize how sociodemographic variables may influence the clinical presentation of LBD among ethnically diverse populations. However, this is a largely descriptive study based on NACC data. NACC represents a convenience sample from 39 ADCs serving various populations and with different local foci, rather than a representative sample of dementia cases in the U.S. Location, specialty, gender, race/ethnicity, and other factors may affect recruitment patterns. Ethnic minorities account for only 10–11% of the total sample, hindering statistical analysis and the generalizability of the study findings. In addition, 72% of Hispanics reported Spanish as their primary language, but only 50% of Hispanics completed the neuropsychological evaluation in Spanish. It is possible that this affected performance on some neuropsychological tasks, particularly relating to fluency. We used categorized groups based off self-reported ethnoracial status, yet race/ethnicity can be a proxy for biological and social variables, like acculturation, genetic markers, and geographic factors. Categorizing Hispanics as a homogenous group does not account for the various countries of origin represented that might better characterize homogenous subgroups. Additionally, diagnosing LBD is a challenge that can require multiple physicians over multiple office visits to render an accurate diagnosis [51] and we did not limit our study to pathologically-confirmed cases. Lastly, although NACC published a LBD module in 2017, the data from that module remain unavailable for analysis due to a small sample size. Similarly, Version 3 of the UDS is currently in use, which adopted four new non-proprietary neuropsychological tests to replace similar tests that were previously used under licensing restrictions. Although the four new measures can be equated to their previous counterparts using equipercentile equating [52], we did not compare cognitive performance on measures implemented in Version 3 since only 14% Whites, 6% African Americans, and 19% Hispanics had these data available. As available data increases, additional analyses can explore group differences using more sensitive or appropriate variables for LBD cohorts.
Despite limitations, this study is highly relevant to ADCs, whose research is critical for advancing the detection, diagnosis, treatment, and prevention of neurodegenerative diseases. Differences between cohorts, such as sex, highlight the need for population-based studies in LBD engaging individuals from diverse racial-ethnic backgrounds [14]. Future research should also explore whether observed sex difference relate to NACC recruitment, social/cultural differences between different racial/ethnic backgrounds, or LBD differences between racial-ethnic groups. Research is needed regarding optimal culturally-sensitive neuropsychological testing and whether observed differences relate to cultural, social, testing, or disease-related factors. Overall, there is a need for more research in investigating how social and biological factors interplay, ultimately impacting the detection, evaluation, and treatment of LBD among ethnically diverse populations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The NACC database is funded by NIA/NIH Grant U01 AG016976. NACC data are contributed by the NIA-funded ADCs: P30 AG019610 (PI Eric Reiman, MD), P30 AG013846 (PI Neil Kowall, MD), P30 AG062428-01 (PI James Leverenz, MD) P50 AG008702 (PI Scott Small, MD), P50 AG025688 (PI Allan Levey, MD, PhD), P50 AG047266 (PI Todd Golde, MD, PhD), P30 AG010133 (PI Andrew Saykin, PsyD), P50 AG005146 (PI Marilyn Albert, PhD), P30 AG062421-01 (PI Bradley Hyman, MD, PhD), P30 AG062422-01 (PI Ronald Petersen, MD, PhD), P50 AG005138 (PI Mary Sano, PhD), P30 AG008051 (PI Thomas Wisniewski, MD), P30 AG013854 (PI Robert Vassar, PhD), P30 AG008017 (PI Jeffrey Kaye, MD), P30 AG010161 (PI David Bennett, MD), P50 AG047366 (PI Victor Henderson, MD, MS), P30 AG010129 (PI Charles DeCarli, MD), P50 AG016573 (PI Frank LaFerla, PhD), P30 AG062429-01(PI James Brewer, MD, PhD), P50 AG023501 (PI Bruce Miller, MD), P30 AG035982 (PI Russell Swerdlow, MD), P30 AG028383 (PI Linda Van Eldik, PhD), P30 AG053760 (PI Henry Paulson, MD, PhD), P30 AG010124 (PI John Trojanowski, MD, PhD), P50 AG005133 (PI Oscar Lopez, MD), P50 AG005142 (PI Helena Chui, MD), P30 AG012300 (PI Roger Rosenberg, MD), P30 AG049638 (PI Suzanne Craft, PhD), P50 AG005136 (PI Thomas Grabowski, MD), P30 AG062715-01 (PI Sanjay Asthana, MD, FRCP), P50 AG005681 (PI John Morris, MD), P50 AG047270 (PI Stephen Strittmatter, MD, PhD).
Disclosure statement:
AMK receives research support from the University of Florida Dorothy Mangurian Headquarters for Lewy Body Dementia. She has received pilot grant funding from the 1Florida ADRC (AG047266). These disclosures are not conflicts of interest regarding the submitted publication.
GES receives research support from the Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute (CER-1306-01897), the state of Florida (9AZ15), and the NIH including the 1Florida ADRC (AG047266). He receives royalties from two books. These disclosures are not conflicts of interest regarding the submitted publication.
MJA is supported by an ARHQ K08 career development award (K08HS24159); receives compensation from the AAN for work as an evidence-based medicine methodology consultant; is on the level of evidence editorial board for Neurology® and related publications (uncompensated); and receives publishing royalties for Parkinson’s Disease: Improving Patient Care (Oxford University Press, 2014). She serves as an investigator for a Lewy Body Dementia Association Research Center of Excellence. She has received pilot grant funding from the 1Florida ADRC (AG047266). These disclosures are not conflicts of interest regarding the submitted publication.
Footnotes
MGM reports no disclosures
References
- [1].Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, Jacobs DM, Small S, Bell K, Merchant C, Lantigua R, Costa R, Stern Y (2001) Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean hispanics, and caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology 56, 49–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Chin AL, Negash S, Hamilton R (2011) Diversity and disparity in dementia: The impact of ethnoracial differences in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 25, 187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].(2018) 2018 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 14, 367–429. [Google Scholar]
- [4].Morris JC, Schindler SE, McCue LM, Moulder KL, Benzinger TLS, Cruchaga C, Fagan AM, Grant E, Gordon BA, Holtzman DM, Xiong C (2019) Assessment of Racial Disparities in Biomarkers for Alzheimer DiseaseAssessment of Racial Disparities in Biomarkers for Alzheimer DiseaseAssessment of Racial Disparities in Biomarkers for Alzheimer Disease. JAMA Neurology 76, 264–273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Howell JC, Watts KD, Parker MW, Wu J, Kollhoff A, Wingo TS, Dorbin CD, Qiu D, Hu WT (2017) Race modifies the relationship between cognition and Alzheimer’s disease cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers. Alzheimer’s research & therapy 9, 88–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Burke SL, Rodriguez MJ, Barker W, Greig-Custo MT, Rosselli M, Loewenstein DA, Duara R (2018) Relationship between Cognitive Performance and Measures of Neurodegeneration among Hispanic and White Non-Hispanic Individuals with Normal Cognition, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 24, 176–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Santos OA, Pedraza O, Lucas JA, Duara R, Greig-Custo MT, Hanna Al-Shaikh FS, Liesinger AM, Bieniek KF, Hinkle KM, Lesser ER, Crook JE, Carrasquillo MM, Ross OA, Ertekin-Taner N, Graff-Radford NR, Dickson DW, Murray ME (2019) Ethnoracial differences in Alzheimer’s disease from the FLorida Autopsied Multi-Ethnic (FLAME) cohort. Alzheimer’s & dementia : the journal of the Alzheimer’s Association 15, 635–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Mehta KM, Yaffe K, Pérez-Stable EJ, Stewart A, Barnes D, Kurland BF, Miller BL (2008) Race/ethnic differences in AD survival in US Alzheimer’s Disease Centers. Neurology 70, 1163–1170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Mehta KM, Yin M, Resendez C, Yaffe K (2005) Ethnic differences in acetylcholinesterase inhibitor use for Alzheimer disease. Neurology 65, 159–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Livney MG, Clark CM, Karlawish JH, Cartmell S, Negrón M, Nuñez J, Xie SX, Entenza-Cabrera F, Vega IE, Arnold SE (2011) Ethnoracial differences in the clinical characteristics of Alzheimer’s disease at initial presentation at an urban Alzheimer’s disease center. The American journal of geriatric psychiatry : official journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry 19, 430–439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Manly JJ, Jacobs DM, Touradji P, Small SA, Stern Y (2002) Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 8, 341–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Babulal GM, Quiroz YT, Albensi BC, Arenaza-Urquijo E, Astell AJ, Babiloni C, Bahar-Fuchs A, Bell J, Bowman GL, Brickman AM, Chételat G, Ciro C, Cohen AD, Dilworth-Anderson P, Dodge HH, Dreux S, Edland S, Esbensen A, Evered L, Ewers M, Fargo KN, Fortea J, Gonzalez H, Gustafson DR, Head E, Hendrix JA, Hofer SM, Johnson LA, Jutten R, Kilborn K, Lanctôt KL, Manly JJ, Martins RN, Mielke MM, Morris MC, Murray ME, Oh ES, Parra MA, Rissman RA, Roe CM, Santos OA, Scarmeas N, Schneider LS, Schupf N, Sikkes S, Snyder HM, Sohrabi HR, Stern Y, Strydom A, Tang Y, Terrera GM, Teunissen C, Melo van Lent D, Weinborn M, Wesselman L, Wilcock DM, Zetterberg H, O’Bryant SE (2019) Perspectives on ethnic and racial disparities in Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias: Update and areas of immediate need. Alzheimer’s & Dementia 15, 292–312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Corriveau RA, Koroshetz WJ, Gladman JT, Jeon S, Babcock D, Bennett DA, Carmichael ST, Dickinson SLJ, Dickson DW, Emr M, Fillit H, Greenberg SM, Hutton ML, Knopman DS, Manly JJ, Marder KS, Moy CS, Phelps CH, Scott PA, Seeley WW, Sieber B-A, Silverberg NB, Sutherland ML, Taylor A, Torborg CL, Waddy SP, Gubitz AK, Holtzman DM (2017) Alzheimer’s Disease-Related Dementias Summit 2016: National research priorities. Neurology 89, 2381–2391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Schneider J, Jeon S, Gladman JT, Corriveau RA (2019) ADRD Summit 2019 Report to the National Advisory Neurological Disorders and Stroke Council. https://www.ninds.nih.gov/sites/default/files/2019_adrd_summit_recommendations_508c.pdf Last updated 16 September 2019. Accessed 13 May 2020.
- [15].Barker WW, Luis CA, Kashuba A, Luis M, Harwood DG, Loewenstein D, Waters C, Jimison P, Shepherd E, Sevush S (2002) Relative frequencies of Alzheimer disease, Lewy body, vascular and frontotemporal dementia, and hippocampal sclerosis in the State of Florida Brain Bank. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders 16, 203–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Besser L, Kukull W, Knopman DS, Chui H, Galasko D, Weintraub S, Jicha G, Carlsson C, Burns J, Quinn J, Sweet RA, Rascovsky K, Teylan M, Beekly D, Thomas G, Bollenbeck M, Monsell S, Mock C, Zhou XH, Thomas N, Robichaud E, Dean M, Hubbard J, Jacka M, Schwabe-Fry K, Wu J, Phelps C, Morris JC, Neuropsychology Work Group DaCClotNIoA-fUSAsDC (2018) Version 3 of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center’s Uniform Data Set. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 32, 351–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Beekly D, M Ramos E, W Lee W, D Deitrich W, E Jacka M, Wu J, L Hubbard J, D Koepsell T, C Morris J, Kukull W (2007) The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) database: the Uniform Data Set. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Morris JC, Weintraub S, Chui HC, Cummings J, DeCarli C, Ferris S, Foster NL, Galasko D, Graff-Radford N, Peskind ER (2006) The Uniform Data Set (UDS): clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from Alzheimer Disease Centers. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders 20, 210–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Beekly DL, Ramos EM, van Belle G, Deitrich W, Clark AD, Jacka ME, Kukull WA (2004) The National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center (NACC) Database: an Alzheimer disease database. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 18, 270–277. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH, Chance JM, Filos S (1982) Measurement of Functional Activities in Older Adults in the Community. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Fahn S, Elton RL (1987) UPDRS program members. Unified Parkinsons disease rating scale. Recent developments in Parkinson’s disease 2, 153–163. [Google Scholar]
- [22].Kaufer D, L. Cummings J, Ketchel P, Smith V, MacMillan A, Shelley T, L. Lopez O, DeKosky S (2000) Validation of the NPI-Q, a Brief Clinical Form of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Yesavage JA, Sheikh JI (1986) 9/Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Clinical Gerontologist 5, 165–173. [Google Scholar]
- [24].Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412–2414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975) “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12, 189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintraub S (1983) Boston naming test, Lea & Febiger, Philadelphia. [Google Scholar]
- [27].Wechsler D (1987) WMS-R : Wechsler Memory Scale--Revised : manual, Psychological Corp. : Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, San Antonio. [Google Scholar]
- [28].Wechsler D (1981) Wechler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; ). [Google Scholar]
- [29].Reitan RM (1958) Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and motor skills 8, 271–276. [Google Scholar]
- [30].Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Fischer JS (2004) Neuropsychological assessment, Oxford University Press, USA. [Google Scholar]
- [31].Graff-Radford NR, Besser LM, Crook JE, Kukull WA, Dickson DW (2016) Neuropathologic differences by race from the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center. Alzheimers Dement 12, 669–677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Savica R, Grossardt BR, Bower JH, Boeve BF, Ahlskog JE, Rocca WA (2013) Incidence of Dementia With Lewy Bodies and Parkinson Disease Dementia. JAMA Neurology 70, 1396–1402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Kane JPM, Surendranathan A, Bentley A, Barker SAH, Taylor J-P, Thomas AJ, Allan LM, McNally RJ, James PW, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O’Brien JT (2018) Clinical prevalence of Lewy body dementia. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy 10, 19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Barnes LL, Lamar M, Schneider JA (2019) Sex differences in mixed neuropathologies in community-dwelling older adults. Brain Research 1719, 11–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Ronquillo JG, Baer MR, Lester WT (2016) Sex-specific patterns and differences in dementia and Alzheimer’s disease using informatics approaches. Journal of women & aging 28, 403–411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Life Expectancy all races, World Life Expectancy, https://www.worldlifeexpectancy.com/usa/life-expectancy, Accessed 21 Nov 2019.
- [37].Cary MS, Rubright JD, Grill JD, Karlawish J (2015) Why are spousal caregivers more prswevalent than nonspousal caregivers as study partners in AD dementia clinical trials? Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 29, 70–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Karlawish J, Rubright J, Casarett D, Cary M, Ten Have T, Sankar P (2009) Older Adults’ Attitudes Toward Enrollment of Non-competent Subjects Participating in Alzheimer’s Research. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 182–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Rote SM, Angel JL, Moon H, Markides K (2019) Caregiving Across Diverse Populations: New Evidence From the National Study of Caregiving and Hispanic EPESE. Innovation in aging 3, igz033–igz033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Ryan CL, Siebens J (2012) Educational Attainment in the United States: 2009 Population Characteristics. Current Population Reports. P20–566. US Census Bureau. [Google Scholar]
- [41].Brickman AM, Schupf N, Manly JJ, Luchsinger JA, Andrews H, Tang MX, Reitz C, Small SA, Mayeux R, DeCarli C, Brown TR (2008) Brain Morphology in Older African Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Whites From Northern Manhattan. JAMA Neurology 65, 1053–1061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Noble JM, Manly JJ, Schupf N, Tang M-X, Luchsinger JA (2012) Type 2 diabetes and ethnic disparities in cognitive impairment. Ethnicity & disease 22, 38–44. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Salazar R, Dwivedi AK, Royall DR (2016) Cross-ethnic differences in the severity of neuropsychiatric symptoms in persons with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 29, 13–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Sayegh P, Knight BG (2014) Functional assessment and neuropsychiatric inventory questionnaires: measurement invariance across Hispanics and non-Hispanic whites. The Gerontologist 54, 375–386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Xiong GL, Filshtein T, Beckett LA, Hinton L (2015) Antipsychotic Use in a Diverse Population With Dementia: A Retrospective Review of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center Database. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences 27, 326–332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Koller D, Hua T, Bynum JPW (2016) Treatment Patterns with Antidementia Drugs in the United States: Medicare Cohort Study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 64, 1540–1548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Zuckerman IH, Ryder PT, Simoni-Wastila L, Shaffer T, Sato M, Zhao L, Stuart B (2008) Racial and ethnic disparities in the treatment of dementia among Medicare beneficiaries. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences 63, S328–S333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Hernandez S, McClendon MJ, Zhou X-HA, Sachs M, Lerner AJ (2010) Pharmacological treatment of Alzheimer’s disease: effect of race and demographic variables. Journal of Alzheimer’s disease : JAD 19, 665–672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Welsh KA, Fillenbaum G, Wilkinson W, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Stern Y, Harrell LM, Edland SD, Beekly D (1995) Neuropsychological test performance in African-American* and white patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 45, 2207–2211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Schneider BC, Gross AL, Bangen KJ, Skinner JC, Benitez A, Glymour MM, Sachs BC, Shih RA, Sisco S, Manly JJ, Luchsinger JA (2014) Association of Vascular Risk Factors With Cognition in a Multiethnic Sample. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B 70, 532–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Galvin JE, Duda JE, Kaufer DI, Lippa CF, Taylor A, Zarit SH (2010) Lewy body dementia: The caregiver experience of clinical care. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 16, 388–392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Monsell SE, Dodge HH, Zhou X-H, Bu Y, Besser LM, Mock C, Hawes SE, Kukull WA, Weintraub S, Neuropsychology Work Group Advisory to the Clinical Task F (2016) Results From the NACC Uniform Data Set Neuropsychological Battery Crosswalk Study. Alzheimer disease and associated disorders 30, 134–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.