Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Oct 13;10:17085. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72896-6

Perceptual inference is impaired in individuals with ASD and intact in individuals who have lost the autism diagnosis

Sagi Jaffe-Dax 1,, Inge-Marie Eigsti 2
PMCID: PMC7554034  PMID: 33051465

Abstract

Beyond the symptoms which characterize their diagnoses, individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show enhanced performance in simple perceptual discrimination tasks. Often attributed to superior sensory sensitivities, enhanced performance may also reflect a weaker bias towards previously perceived stimuli. This study probes perceptual inference in a group of individuals who have lost the autism diagnosis (LAD); that is, they were diagnosed with ASD in early childhood but have no current ASD symptoms. Groups of LAD, current ASD, and typically developing (TD) participants completed an auditory discrimination task. Individuals with TD showed a bias towards previously perceived stimuli—a perceptual process called “contraction bias”; that is, their representation of a given tone was contracted towards the preceding trial stimulus in a manner that is Bayesian optimal. Similarly, individuals in the LAD group showed a contraction bias. In contrast, individuals with current ASD showed a weaker contraction bias, suggesting reduced perceptual inferencing. These findings suggest that changes that characterize LAD extend beyond the social and communicative symptoms of ASD, impacting perceptual domains. Measuring perceptual processing earlier in development in ASD will tap the causality between changes in perceptual and symptomatological domains. Further, the characterization of perceptual inference could reveal meaningful individual differences in complex high-level behaviors.

Subject terms: Perception, Language, Human behaviour

Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is characterized by impairments in social communication and by the presence of repetitive, perseverative or stereotyped behaviors1. Beyond core diagnostic criteria, individuals with ASD also exhibit distinct perceptual aptitudes. For example, in the auditory domain, people with ASD display an enhanced ability to distinguish the pitch of pairs of simple tones2 and the pitch of pairs of spoken word and nonword speech3. In addition to fine-grained processing of the auditory signal, these tasks also require the listener to make perceptual inferences about stored representations of sounds. That is, the listener compares a stored representation to the current observation. Following the logic of Bayesian inference, the sounds presented in a given trial (t) are perceived as more similar to prior stimuli (t−1) than if they were presented in isolation; this perceptual change is described as a “contraction” towards the stored representation47. Depending on the task, reduced perceptual inferencing (and specifically contraction) may either enhance or, more often, impede performance on perceptual discrimination tasks8,9. The current study utilized an auditory “Same-Different” task, in which perceptual inferencing impedes performance, to test whether impaired Bayesian inference could underlie the frequently-reported auditory perceptual advantages characteristic of ASD.

Loss of diagnosis

While ASD was originally considered a lifelong disorder, research indicates that between 8 and 20% of individuals with ASD will present with no symptoms by the time they reach adolescence10. A growing literature has documented many aspects of the loss of autism diagnosis (LAD; formerly, “optimal outcome”) phenomenon, in which LAD individuals are largely indistinguishable from their typically developing (TD) peers, with both groups differing from individuals who remain on the spectrum. Studies have examined behavior using standardized clinical assessments of social and communication skills11, restricted and repetitive behaviors12, psychiatric comorbidities13, language and verbal memory14, executive functions15, and academic skills16. Across each of these studies, participants in the LAD group scored in the average range or higher, with performance similar to (or higher than) that of the TD group. Experimental behavioral studies have revealed similar performance in LAD and TD groups for ratings of likeability and broader autism phenotype17, and for a variety of pragmatic language abilities1821. This behavioral work provides a foundation for further exploration of the range of possible outcomes (including questions about how to define an optimal outcome22), and the nature of the neural systems that support such sharp changes in developmental trajectories23.

While it is clear that clinically meaningful improvements in social and communication skills, and the absence of repetitive, stereotyped and perseverative behaviors and interests, are observed in a significant subgroup of individuals with ASD, there are more open questions than answers. One of the exciting unstudied issues centers on the strengths that characterize ASD24. The diagnosis is associated with a remarkable set of perceptual and cognitive strengths in visuospatial processing25, musical skills26, solving puzzles27, etc. The current paper asks whether perceptual strengths in LAD are effectively normalized, or whether they are retained even when the clinical symptoms of the disorder have remitted. In the absence of longitudinal data, it is not certain that individuals with the LAD outcome originally displayed perceptual strengths.

Sequential discrimination tasks provide a sensitive means of evaluating how representations of stimuli are impacted by prior information—a process often termed perceptual Bayesian inference. For example, in two-tone pitch discrimination tasks, a participant encounters a long sequence of trials containing similar stimuli from a single category (i.e., pure tones), and is asked to make a same/different judgment about each pair. The representation of the first stimulus in each trial is noisier, or less robust, than the representation of the second (more recent) stimulus, at the time of the response28. To compensate for this degradation, findings suggest that the representation of the first stimulus is merged with prior representations; thus, at the point of comparison, the perceived difference between its representation and the second stimulus differs from the physical difference between the stimuli. The level of divergence between the inferred perception and the physical stimuli, as measured by accuracy judgments, can inform us about the degree to which an individual relies implicitly on prior information29. Performance thus also provides an index of how much an individual relies on the most recent versus prior stimuli30.

Recent research on ASD has probed inferencing in perceptual discrimination tasks. Some studies indicate a weaker reliance on the most recent stimuli3134 (but see ref.35). For example, in a two-tone frequency discrimination task, high-functioning adults with ASD showed a reduced contraction of the perceived stimulus towards their representation of the most recent item36,37.

The current paper presents a novel re-analysis of previously-described data38 to examine how ASD, and more specifically, LAD, is associated with perceptual inference. A prior study examining d’ in this task indicated heightened (better) pitch discrimination in ASD; in contrast, the LAD group’s abilities did not differ from those of TD controls, but showed an intermediate pattern of performance between the ASD and the TD groups38. The current study employs a perceptual inference analysis (as described in ref.30) to test whether individuals who no longer have an ASD diagnosis will maintain ASD-like reduced perceptual inferencing, or will present with perceptual processes that look more like those of TD peers. Given their previously reported poorer overall performance38 we predicted that the LAD group would have more contraction towards recently-presented stimuli than that of the ASD group. A greater contraction could account for their observed poorer performance. The current manuscript provides a novel evaluation of whether the loss of ASD diagnosis entails a more typical pattern of perceptual inferencing; it also sheds further light on global statistical learning of stimulus priors in ASD.

Methods

Individuals with LAD (n = 27), ASD (n = 29), and TD (n = 23) were assessed in a same-different two-tone discrimination task. All procedures were approved by the University of Connecticut Institutional Review Board and were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from the participant or legal guardian prior to the study. Groups did not significantly differ on full-scale IQ and chronological age. Participants ranged in age from 8 to 21 years; all had cognitive abilities in the normal range. Additional details are shown in Table 1; see also refs.38,39. In each of 120 trials, two 100 ms tones were presented, and participants were asked to indicate whether the two tones were “same” or “different”. The first tone was randomly chosen to be 500 Hz, 750 Hz, 1000 Hz, or 1500 Hz. The second was of the same frequency (“same” trials; 50%) or a frequency of 1%, 2% or 3% above the first tone (“different trials”; 50%). The inter-tone interval was 1 s, and participants had unlimited time to respond; the inter-trial interval was 500 ms. The experiment included 120 trials across three blocks of 40 trials per block (20 same and 20 different). Blocks were ordered by increasing difficulty (decreasing frequency difference in the “different” trials), with first tone frequency presented in random order. The first block contained trials that differed by 3% of total frequency, the second by 2%, and the third by 1%. Prior to task administration, participants completed a short training block with feedback (16 trials total; 8 at 4% and 8 at 1% frequency difference levels). Training was repeated until participants reached an accuracy level of 75% (no participant required more than 16 trials of training). Trial data and analysis scripts are available from the authors on request.

Table 1.

Demographic information for ASD, Loss of ASD Diagnosis (LAD), and typically developing (TD) groups.

ASD M (SD) LAD M (SD) TD M (SD) χ2 or F p η2p
N (M:F) 29 (25:4) 27 (21:6) 23 (17:6) 0.93 0.63
Chron. age (yrs) 12.3 (2.3) 12.5 (3.6) 13.7 (2.9) 1.37 0.26 0.002
8–17 8–21 9–21
Nonverbal IQa 111 (14) 112 (14) 115 (12) 0.60 0.55 0.002
78–147 92–142 89–139
Verbal IQa 104 (13)* 113 (13) 113 (12) 4.91 0.01 0.16
81–133 91–137 99–136
Fullscale IQa 109 (13) 116 (12) 116 (11) 2.97 0.06 0.07
80–138 96–139 101–142
ADOS Com + Socb 10.3 (3.0)* 1.7 (2.1) 0.8 (1.1) 155.48  < .001 0.82
7–19 0–5 0–4
ADOS Repetitivec  1.2 (1.1)  0.4 (0.6)  0 (0)  10.8 < 0.001  0.3
0–3 0–2 0–0
SCQ Total (Lifetime)d 23.0 (5.9)* 16.5 (6.6)* 1.4 (1.3)* 91.57  < .001 0.24
10–33 5–28 0–4
Age of first words 21.0 (11.2) 26.9 (11.6) 3.32 0.08 0.10
(months) 6–54 8–48

Asterisks identify means that differ significantly from other means in the comparison not sharing that superscript. Data are presented as M(SD), range.

aWechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI47) Nonverbal, Verbal, and Fullscale IQ.

bAutism Diagnostic Observation Schedule48. Communication plus social domain summed score. Cutoff is 7 for ASD and 10 for autistic disorder.

cRestrictive and repetitive behaviors domain score.

dSocial Communication Questionnaire49; for Lifetime scale, 15 is the ASD cutoff.

We analyzed each participant’s response as a function of the tones presented in the preceding trial (t − 1). Analyzing this impact captures a recency effect, where, based on previous work37, individuals with ASD should differ from TD individuals. Given the smaller number of trials in the current work, we could not evaluate the separate contribution of all previous trials in addition to the contribution of the most recent trial. We contrasted performance in two trial types. In Bias + trials, the pitch of the initial tone in the current trial (t) is flanked by the pitch of the tones of pair t − 1 (the previous stimulus) and the pitch of the second tone in pair t (example trial in Fig. 1A). Thus, in Bias + trials, contraction of the first tone in trial t towards the stimulus in trial t − 1 increases the perceived difference between the two tones in trial t (red arrow in Fig. 1A), making it easier to judge the pair as “different.” In Bias-trials, the pitch of the second tone in trial t is flanked by the pitch of the tones in trial t − 1 and the pitch of the first tone in trial t (example trial in Fig. 1B); Thus, in Bias- trials, contraction of the first tone toward the previous stimulus decreases the perceived difference between the two tones in the current trial (red arrow in Fig. 1B), making it harder to judge the pair as “different.” Trials in which the correct response should be “same” were also included in the Bias- group, since contraction of the first tone towards the previous stimulus decreased the chance of making a correct judgment.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Schematic illustration of trial types. (A) Example of Bias + trial. The tones are denoted by blue rectangles. The beginning of each trial is represented by dashed lines. The first tone in the current trial (trial t) is flanked by the second tone and the tones in the previous trial (trial t − 1). The perceptual representation of the first tone is degraded compared to the representation of the second tone and is more contracted towards previously perceived stimuli (denoted by the red arrow). In the Bias + trials, this contraction increases the perceived difference between the two tones in the current trial and eases the identification of these trials as “different”. (B) Example of Bias- trial. The second tone in the current trial (trial t) is flanked by the first tone in the current trial and the tones in the previous trial (trial t − 1). Perceptual contraction of the first tone towards the tones in the previous trial decreases the perceived difference between the tones in the current trial and increases the chances of incorrectly identifying this trial as “same”.

We hypothesized that the difference between trial types would be larger for the TD and LAD groups than for the ASD group, reflecting a larger impact of the previous stimulus on current perception individuals among the TD and LAD groups, compared to individuals with ASD. In the psychophysics literature, the perceptual “contraction” of the representation of the first tone of the stimulus pair in trial t towards the stimulus presented in trial t − 1 is thought to measure sequential dependency29,40. This is also thought to reflect a central tendency or summary statistical learning4,5,8; that is, global statistical learning of the stimulus prior. In the current perceptual inference task, Bias + trials should induce facilitatory contraction, and Bias- trials should cause inhibitory contraction. Given this operationalization of perceptual inference, the two predictions above led to the following specific hypotheses: (1) Because its members have impaired perceptual inference, the ASD group should perform worse on Bias + trials, and better on Bias- trials, than the TD group, due to reduced facilitatory and inhibitory contraction on these trials. (2) The LAD group should perform better on Bias + trials and worse on Bias- trials than the ASD group due to typical facilitatory and inhibitory contraction on these trials.

Following prior work36,37, we included only individuals whose task performance was significantly above chance (> 60% accuracy in the physically easiest condition of 3% frequency difference, as determined by assuming binomial distribution of random responses in these 30 trials). The logic behind this exclusion criterion is that error analysis (such as bias comparison) is only meaningful if there was a cognitive difference between correct and incorrect responses, i.e., when the difference between the tone was above the individual’s limen. Participants who were generally at chance may not have been engaged in the same task; they may simply have been responding at random. Note that if all participants, whose performance was simply above 50% on the easiest condition, were included in the analyses, results were effectively similar.

All analyses of main effects and interactions were conducted using linear mixed-effects models in Matlab R2018b (Mathworks, MA), with group and trial type as fixed variables and subject as a random variable. For specific post-hoc contrasts between two groups, we included only the comparison groups in the model and corrected for multiple comparisons by dividing the significance threshold by the number of parallel comparisons (Bonferroni correction).

Results

As reported previously38, individuals in the TD group had lower overall accuracy. Twenty-one of 29 from the ASD group (72%), 21 of 27 from the LAD group (78%), and 18 of 23 individuals (78%) from the TD group, were included in all subsequent analyses. This relatively high exclusion rate was expected for participants of this chronological age, for a challenging and fairly tedious task. Groups did not differ on the fraction of participants that were excluded due to chance level performance. The overall accuracy difference between groups was not significant, F(2,4374) = 1.8, p = 0.17; see Fig. 2A. Individuals with typical development had a slightly faster mean reaction time compared to the ASD and LAD groups (915 ± 45 ms, 952 ± 84 ms and 954 ± 79 ms; Mean ± SEM for TD, ASD and LAD, respectively); however, there was no significant group difference, F(2,4374) = 1.1, p = 0.32.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Performance on the two-alternative forced choice frequency discrimination task. (A) Overall accuracy by group. We did not find a significant group difference in overall performance. Dots represent individual results. Bars represent group averages. Error bars represent SEM. (B) Performance as a function of trial type. Lines represent groups’ averages. Error bar represent SEM. Individuals in LAD and TD groups performed better on Bias + trials, where the consideration of the preceding stimulus improved discrimination, relative to Bias- trials, where the previous stimulus distracts from correct discrimination. Mixed effects model (see Methods) results indicate a significant within group effect of trial type: LAD: F(1,1522) = 6.2, p = 0.01. TD: F(1,1310) = 5.4, p = 0.01. These effects remained significant when controlling for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction). There was no significant trial type difference for the ASD group, F(1,1541) = 0.76, p = 0.38.

The final sample size was powered to detect a medium or large effect (Cohen’s d > 0.44 for power of 1β = 0.8). The impact of bias was clearly apparent, given that in the TD group, there was a significant effect of trial type (Bias- versus Bias +) on accuracy, F(1,1310) = 5.4, p = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 0.43. Thus, with our sample size, we had sufficient power to find an effect of trial type.

Turning to the analysis of primary interest—the assessment of perceptual inference—linear mixed effects models suggested no significant main effect on accuracy of either Group, F(2,4373) = 0.97, p = 0.38, or Bias trial type (Bias + vs Bias-), F(1,4373) = 1.5, p = 0.21. Importantly, there was a significant Group X Bias trial type interaction, F(2,4373) = 3.02, p = 0.048. Specifically, there was a significant difference between LAD and ASD groups, F(1,3064) = 19.4, p = 0.00001, such that the LAD group exhibited a larger difference in performance than the ASD group between Bias + and Bias- trials. Similarly, there was a significant difference between the TD and ASD groups in the impact of bias, F(1,2852) = 7.6, p = 0.006. These effects remained significant when controlling for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction). The LAD and TD groups did not significantly differ on the difference between Bias + and Bias- trials, F(1,2833) = 2.2, p = 0.14; results are shown in Fig. 2B.

With regards to reaction times, linear mixed effects models suggested no significant main effect of Group, F(2,4373) = 0.79, p = 0.45, or Bias trial type (Bias + vs Bias-), F(1,4373) = 0.59, p = 0.44. There was no significant Group X Trial type interaction, F(2,4373) = 0.026, p = 0.97. The individual measures reported in Table 1 (age, FSIQ, ADOS and SCQ sores, and age of first words) did not contribute any unique variance to the Group X Trial type interaction, all p’s > 0.1, calculated within groups.

Altogether, results show that participants in the LAD and TD groups were more susceptible to the influence of prior stimuli, relative to the ASD group. This finding suggests that individuals with LAD update their perceptual representations according to prior context, while individuals with ASD have a more veridical perceptual representation. Group differences in sensitivity did not appear to be a simple outcome of speeded responding, given the absence of group differences in RT. If anything, faster responders could have shown a reduced sensitivity to the influence of prior stimuli, which is not the case here.

Discussion

With intensive intervention, some children diagnosed with ASD in early childhood (previously described as having an optimal outcome) later go on to lose the symptoms of this neurodevelopmental disorder. Beyond improvements in communication and social skills, results of the current study suggest that children with a “loss of ASD diagnosis” (LAD) also display typical perceptual inference skills, unlike their peers with ASD. Specifically, similar to typically developing youth, participants in the LAD group were susceptible to the influence of recently-presented stimuli in a sequential discrimination judgement task. This result suggests that individuals in the LAD group update their perceptual representations flexibly and rapidly, in contrast to individuals with ASD.

These findings suggest that weaker perceptual updating of the statistical properties of recent contexts, recently reported for individuals with ASD37, is linked to the symptoms of ASD. That is, when an individual loses the ASD diagnosis, they also show a typical ability to update their statistical representation of recent context. In contrast to previous work37, we did not have sufficient trials to compare the impact of the most recent trial with the impact of all previously-presented stimuli. In general, the current findings are quite consistent with a growing body of literature examining the hypothesis that ASD reflects impairments in harnessing statistical regularities to make predictions and extract generalizations3137.

Updating one’s representation of an auditory stimulus, according to the influence of a prior stimulus, might permit more nuanced perception of (for example) verbal prosody, or of the differences among phonological features, which are the fundamental units of speech sounds. In contrast, less malleable (less immediately updated) perceptual representations might predict the presence of absolute pitch abilities; indeed, such abilities have been extensively documented in ASD4143. Further research might seek to link these perceptual processes to their distinct physiological bases, on the one hand, and to fine-grained processing and encoding of speech and music stimuli, on the other hand.

Limitations

The current findings reflect cross-sectional data; they cannot resolve two alternatives. One possibility is that individuals in the LAD group displayed ASD-typical perceptual updating earlier in development, when their ASD symptoms were salient, but that this perceptual “profile” shifted in concert with broader behavioral changes (possibly reflecting an underlying cognitive change). An alternative possibility is that children in the LAD group displayed a TD-typical perceptual profile from early in development, even while they displayed ASD symptoms; although diagnosed with ASD in early childhood, they did not share the characteristic perceptual profile. This might indicate more fundamental differences between individuals who eventually comprise the LAD and TD groups, and would illuminate more basic characteristics of ASD as a disorder.

It is also possible that perceptual updating abilities played a role in the response to speech and language interventions. Given the latter, if perceptual inferencing abilities were identified early in development, we might test whether these skills are associated with a sharper improvement in speech and language skills, given the relevant intervention. Longitudinal designs that better characterize participants early in development, and that assess perceptual inference abilities before and after intervention, are needed to disentangle these alternative explanations. Further research must also consider the impact of factors such as age and IQ.

One interesting point regarding the task design is that participants had unlimited time to respond. Consistent differences in RT would lead to longer intervals since the preceding stimulus as well as the one before it. The decay of one’s representation of a stimulus is expected to be larger for the most recent stimulus, since representations decay exponentially as a function of time44. In this study, the longer RTs for the ASD group might have contributed to an overestimation of their contraction bias and therefor an underestimated of group differences. A study design encouraging speeded responding (e.g., by time-out) would likely have revealed even larger group differences that might have reflected meaningful individual correlates with other behavioral measures—a point relevant to any future research.

Our groups differed on the gender ratio (the ASD group had a larger proportion of male participants), which could contribute to the group difference; the sample was not sufficiently powered for testing interactions of our main comparison with gender. In addition, the ASD group showed a larger variance in the impact of bias on perception, suggesting that this group might be a combination of multiple sub-groups with different perceptual characteristics.

Finally, the current study was not designed specifically to test the effects of perceptual priming. Some prior results suggest that, at least in the domain of semantic priming, individuals with ASD show reduced susceptibility to priming45, though the relevance of this study to the present is fairly distant. Certainly, the current findings provide an impetus for further research on the nature of priming of all types, and how it may differ in ASD with respect to perceptual processing.

Conclusions

The current findings suggest that the relation between basic perceptual aptitudes and high-level cognitive communication and social skills may provide an exciting basis from which to better understand individual differences in the development of language and social skills. This relationship has been previously demonstrated in the context of dyslexia, the most prevalent learning disability. In a study of dyslexia that used similar experimental methods, reading skill acquisition was related to the ability to compensate for noisy observation by integrating prior knowledge46. Findings in the current study support the possibility that perceptual inference—the ability to weight current observation against prior contextual information—is where social cognition meets perception.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Lauren Emberson for supporting this work. Thanks to support from the Princeton University Library Open Access Fund Program for making this publication open access.

Abbreviations

ASD

Autism spectrum disorder

LAD

Loss of autism diagnosis

TD

Typical development

Author contributions

S.J.-D. conceptualized the methods, analyzed the results, and wrote the manuscript. I.M.E. designed the study, supervised the work, collected the data, secured funding, and wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by NIDCD Grant Number R01 MH076189-01A1 to I.M.E.

Data availability

The datasets during and/or analyzed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 2.Heaton P. Interval and contour processing in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2005;35:787–793. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0024-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Heaton P, Hudry K, Ludlow A, Hill E. Superior discrimination of speech pitch and its relationship to verbal ability in autism spectrum disorders. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2008;25:771–782. doi: 10.1080/02643290802336277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hollingworth HL. The central tendency of judgment. J. Philos. 1910;7:461–469. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vierordt, K. Der zeitsinn nach versuchen (H. Laupp, 1868).
  • 6.Preuschhof C, Schubert T, Villringer A, Heekeren HR. Prior Information biases stimulus representations during vibrotactile decision making. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010;22:875–887. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Berliner JE, Durlach NI, Braida LD. Intensity perception. VII. Further data on roving-level discrimination and the resolution and bias edge effects. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1977;61:1577–1585. doi: 10.1121/1.381471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Woodrow H. Weight-discrimination with a varying standard. Am. J. Psychol. 1933;45:391–416. doi: 10.2307/1415039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bull AR, Cuddy LL. Recognition memory for pitch of fixed and roving stimulus tones. Percept. Psychophys. 1972;11:105–109. doi: 10.3758/BF03212696. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Helt M, et al. Can children with autism recover? If so, how? Neuropsychol. Rev. 2008;18:339–366. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9075-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Orinstein AJ, et al. Social function and communication in optimal outcome children and adolescents with an autism history on structured test measures. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:2443–2463. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2409-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Troyb E, et al. Restricted and repetitive behaviors in individuals with a history of ASDs who have achieved optimal outcomes. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44:3168–3184. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2182-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tyson, K. et al. Psychiatric symptoms and comorbidities in children with a history of autism who achieve an optimal outcome. In International Meeting for Autism Research (2010).
  • 14.Tyson K, et al. Language and verbal memory in individuals with a history of autism spectrum disorders who have achieved optimal outcomes. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44:648–663. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1921-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Troyb E, et al. Executive functioning in individuals with a history of ASDs who have achieved optimal outcomes. Child Neuropsychol. 2014;20:378–397. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.799644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Troyb E, et al. Academic abilities in children and adolescents with a history of autism spectrum disorders who have achieved optimal outcomes. Autism. 2014;18:233–243. doi: 10.1177/1362361312473519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Suh J, et al. Ratings of broader autism phenotype and personality traits in optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016;46:3505–3518. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2868-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Canfield AR, Eigsti I-M, de Marchena A, Fein D. Story goodness in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and in optimal outcomes from ASD. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2014;59:533–545. doi: 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15-0022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Suh J, et al. Narrative performance of optimal outcome children and adolescents with a history of an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014;44:1681–1694. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2042-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fitch A, Fein DA, Eigsti IM. Detail and gestalt focus in individuals with optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015;45:1887–1896. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2347-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Irvine CA, Eigsti IM, Fein DA. Uh, Um, and autism: filler disfluencies as pragmatic markers in adolescents with optimal outcomes from autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016;46:1061–1070. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2651-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Georgiades S, Kasari C. Reframing optimal outcomes in autism. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172:716–717. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Eigsti, I.-M. & Castelluccio, B. Language in autism spectrum disorder: What optimal outcomes can tell us about neural plasticity. In All About Language: Science, Theory, and Practice (ed. E. Grigorenko, Y. Shtyrov, & P. M.) (Brookes, 2019).
  • 24.Dawson M, Soulières I, Gernsbacher MA, Mottron L. The level and nature of autistic intelligence. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:657–662. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01954.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Happe FGE. Studying weak central coherence at low levels: children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1996;37:873–877. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01483.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Heaton P, Williams K, Cummins O, Happé F. Autism and pitch processing splinter skills: a group and subgroup analysis. Autism. 2008;12:203–219. doi: 10.1177/1362361307085270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Happé FGE. Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 1994;35:1461–1471. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01287.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lu Z-L, Williamson J, Kaufman L. Behavioral lifetime of human auditory sensory memory predicted by physiological measures. Science (80-) 1992;258:1668–1670. doi: 10.1126/science.1455246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Raviv O, Ahissar M, Loewenstein Y. How recent history affects perception: the normative approach and its heuristic approximation. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012;8:e1002731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Jaffe-Dax S, Frenkel O, Ahissar M. Dyslexics’ faster decay of implicit memory for sounds and words is manifested in their shorter neural adaptation. Elife. 2017;6:e20557. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pellicano E, Burr D. When the world becomes ‘too real’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012;16:504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sinha P, et al. Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014 doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416797111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Karaminis T, et al. Central tendency effects in time interval reproduction in autism. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:1–13. doi: 10.1038/srep28570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Turi M, Karaminis T, Pellicano E, Burr D. No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:2–8. doi: 10.1038/srep21756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lawson RP, Mathys C, Rees G. Adults with autism overestimate the volatility of the sensory environment. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:4–6. doi: 10.1038/nn.4615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Granot, S., Jaffe-Dax, S. & Ahissar, M. Slow sensory adaptation reveals slow formation of sensory priors in autism. In 6th Meeting of the Israel Society for Cognitive Psychology (2019).
  • 37.Lieder I, et al. Perceptual bias reveals slow-updating in autism and fast-forgetting in dyslexia. Nat. Neurosci. 2019 doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0308-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Eigsti I-M, Fein DA. More is less: pitch discrimination and language delays in children with optimal outcomes from autism. Autism Res. 2013;6:605–613. doi: 10.1002/aur.1324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fein D, et al. Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2013;54:195–205. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dyjas O, Bausenhart KM, Ulrich R. Trial-by-trial updating of an internal reference in discrimination tasks: evidence from effects of stimulus order and trial sequence. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2012;74:1819–1841. doi: 10.3758/s13414-012-0362-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bonnel A, et al. Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2003;15:226–235. doi: 10.1162/089892903321208169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Heaton P, Hermelin B, Pring L. Autism and pitch processing: a precursor for savant musical ability? Music Percept. Interdiscip. J. 1998;15:291–305. doi: 10.2307/40285769. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Mottron L, Peretz I, Belleville S, Rouleau N. Absolute pitch in autism: a case study. Neurocase. 1999;5:485–501. doi: 10.1080/13554799908402744. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Ashourian P, Loewenstein Y. Bayesian inference underlies the contraction bias in delayed comparison tasks. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e19551. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kamio Y, Robins D, Kelley E, Swainson B, Fein D. Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2007;37:1116–1122. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0254-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Jaffe-Dax S, Raviv O, Jacoby N, Loewenstein Y, Ahissar M. A computational model of implicit memory captures dyslexics’ perceptual deficits. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:12116–12126. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1302-15.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wechsler, D. (1999). W. A. S. of I. (WASI) (San A. T. P. C. W. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). The Psychological Corporation (The Psychological Corporation, 1999).
  • 48.Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C. & Risi, S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Western Psychological Services, 2002).
  • 49.Rutter, M., Bailey, A. & Lord, C. The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual. Western Psychological Services (Western Psychological Services, 2003).

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets during and/or analyzed during the current study available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES