Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Oct 14;10:17281. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74279-3

The impact of handling technique and handling frequency on laboratory mouse welfare is sex-specific

Federica Sensini 1, Dragos Inta 2,3, Rupert Palme 4, Christiane Brandwein 2, Natascha Pfeiffer 2, Marco Andrea Riva 1, Peter Gass 2, Anne Stephanie Mallien 2,
PMCID: PMC7560820  PMID: 33057118

Abstract

Handling is a well-known source of stress to laboratory animals and can affect variability of results and even compromise animal welfare. The conventional tail handling in mice has been shown to induce aversion and anxiety-like behaviour. Recent findings demonstrate that the use of alternative handling techniques, e.g. tunnel handling, can mitigate negative handling-induced effects. Here, we show that technique and frequency of handling influence affective behaviour and stress hormone release of subjects in a sex-dependent manner. While frequent tail handling led to a reduction of wellbeing-associated burrowing and increased despair-like behaviour in male mice, females seemed unaffected. Instead, they displayed a stress response to a low handling frequency, which was not detectable in males. This could suggest that in terms of refinement, the impact in handling could differ between the sexes. Independently from this observation, both sexes preferred to interact with the tunnel. Mice generally explored the tunnel more often than the tail-handling hands of the experimenter and showed more positively rated approaches, e.g. touching or climbing, and at the same time, less defensive burrowing, indicating a strong preference for the tunnel.

Subject terms: Behavioural methods, Biological models, Biomarkers

Introduction

Handling is described as the physical interaction between a laboratory animal and a human operator—be it during maintenance or scientific procedures. Different techniques of handling are practiced in laboratories all over the world. It is what researchers experience in their daily work life. And so do the subjects. The handling technique can influence the wellbeing of the animals and even the outcome of experiments, particularly those concerning spontaneous behaviours1,2. Recent research points out that the handling process is a major modulator of the welfare of laboratory mice36.

The conventional practice for handling mice is to lift them by taking the base of the tail between thumb and forefinger. However, this tail-handling technique has been questioned increasingly. Hurst and West4 were the first to show that tail handling induces anxiety-like behaviour and aversion to human contact. Further studies confirmed that tail handled mice avoid potential aversive situations, e.g. the open arms of the elevated plus-maze or in proximity of the hand of the experimenter35,7,8. This reduced urge to explore novel environments or situations can confound any tests based on explorative behaviour including light–dark paradigm, open field and hole-board test913. Despite the evidences that tail handling impairs the emotional state of mice and may confound the results of behavioural experiments14,15, this method is still predominant. It is often regarded as quick, easy to learn for new users and can be performed without cooperation of the animals4.

Alternative methods such as handling tunnels mitigate handling induced effects4 by eliminating the need for direct contact with the subject. The mouse is gently guided into a tunnel, which can then be lifted. Unlike tail handled mice, tunnel handled mice display an increase in voluntary interaction with the handler and a progressive habituation to the human contact5. Additionally, the mice show less anxiety and stress markers such as urination and defecation during human contact4. Therefore, whenever it is crucial to minimize anxiety in experimental mice, the tunnel method5 seems to be preferable over tail handling5. Apparently, non-aversive handling methods are a valuable tool to refine animal experiments, as requested by the 3Rs principles. Nevertheless, tunnel handling has rarely been implemented in the daily routine of laboratories16. The major criticism is on feasibility, since implementing non-aversive handling methods is often considered too time-costly: especially the initial habituation is thought to be labour-intensive and sometimes even stressful for the animal. Routines such as cage changes can be more time-consuming, particularly during the initial training to the handling technique17. Often, tunnel handling is also falsely associated with the necessity of a permanent tunnel as environmental enrichment in the home cage. Although environmental enrichment is generally endorsed to refine animal experiments, it might be advisable to limit enrichment under certain circumstances18. Hence, the experimenters are hesitant to use the tunnel merely as a handling device, although tunnel handling is equally well accepted by mice, also when the tunnel is solely used as a handling device without being a part of their home cage environment5.

Another important parameter for handling is habituation. While the frequent exposure to handling can lead to habituation effects19, a repetitive exposure to stress can induce depression-like symptoms20,21. We hypothesized that the frequently inflicted anxiogenic experience of regular tail handling might induce chronic stress with the result of despair behaviour—a depression-associated behaviour, as chronic exposure to stress depicts a commonly used model for depression in rodents22. The chronic exposure to various and often unpredictable mild stressors, however, would probably lead to much higher severity in the chronic mild stress models of depression. Yet, a recent study already demonstrated an anhedonia-like phenotype in tail handled mice—another hallmark of depression. In this, Clarkson et al. found that the hedonic consumption of a palatable sucrose solution was significantly decreased after tail handling3. We therefore decided to investigate here the impact of handling frequency on despair. The more frequent (daily) tail handling was expected to elicit a higher stress load and therefore induce a stronger response. We also analysed the stress hormone response via faecal corticosterone metabolites (FCMs) and voluntary burrowing as wellbeing-associated parameters2326.

Additionally, we aimed to reproduce the effects of handling on voluntary interaction with the experimenter to display a potential preference of the subjects towards the tunnel—which would further reinforce the idea of tunnel handling as a suitable way to refine experiments with mice.

Material and methods

Animals and housing

All procedures were performed in the experimental unit of the animal facility of the CIMH. The subjects were 32 males and 32 females naïve C57BL/6NCrl mice (Charles River Laboratory, Sulzfeld, Germany; age: PND 56–62 at arrival, and PND 91–97 at the beginning of burrowing training). Before the onset of experiments, the animals were accustomed to a the housing room with 21–23 °C room temperature and 50–60% humidity and a reversed 12:12 dark–light cycle (lights on at 7 pm) for 2 weeks. They wereindividually housed in Macrolon type II (370 cm2, Tecniplast, Milan, Italy) cages with aspen wood bedding (ABEDD LTE E-002, ssniff-Spezialdiäten, Soest, Germany) and nesting material (cellulose tissue). They were supplied with food pellets (LasQCdiet Rod16-H, LasVendi GmbH, Soest, Germany) and tap water ad libitum. Behavioural experiments were conducted during the active phase of the animals (8–12 pm) unless further specified. All procedures were approved by the German animal welfare authorities (Regierungspräsidium Karlsruhe; 35-9185-81-G-146-13) and performed strictly according to the regulations of animal experimentation within the European Union (European Communities Council Directive 2010/63/EU).

Separated for sex, mice were randomly assigned to one handling procedure: tail or tunnel (for each sex n = 16). We further divided the groups into subgroups, which received different frequencies of handling (n = 8 for each sex): daily (from Monday to Friday) or once per week.

Handling and interaction test

Before the onset of the respective handling, all mice were tail handled once a week during cage change. Mice were familiarized with the respective handling procedure during an adaptation period of 2 weeks, during which they were handled for routine maintenance and during the interaction test (see below). The procedure involved 30 s of handling, 60 s of rest in the home cage and repeated handling for 30 s. Tail handling was performed as follows: the experimenter gently grasped the tail and put the mouse on the sleeve of the laboratory coat for 30 s before returning it to the home cage. For tunnel handling, the mouse was guided into the tunnel (opaque, 130 mm length, 55 mm diameter) and then lifted for 30 s. The experimenter wore Nitrile Powder-free gloves (Abena Classic, Abena A/S, Aabenraa, Denmark) during all handling procedures.

Daily handled mice performed the interaction test for 3 days within 6 days (Monday, Wednesday and Friday), weekly handled only once (Wednesday) The procedure was the following: After the cage lid and nesting material were removed from the home cage, the gloved hand of the experimenter or the handling tunnel was introduced for 60 s. We assessed the voluntary interaction towards the carrier (hand or tunnel) and counted the number of approaches. Subsequently, each mouse was handled for 30 s by the assigned method, followed by 60 s of rest in the home cage without hand or tunnel. Then the interaction procedure was repeated, and the voluntary interaction was assessed again.

We counted sniffing, touching, climbing on the tunnel or the handler’s hand and burrowing as parameters of voluntary interaction. The sniffing behaviour was rated as a neutral exploratory behaviour while touching and climbing behaviours were considered as a more positive interaction, exhibiting the willingness to interact or even socialize. In this constellation, we also evaluate burrowing, as a defensive and negative interaction, similar to the behaviour displayed in the marble burying test, where burrowing is a validated indicator for an anxiety-like response. In fact, while burrowing is considered an indicator of wellbeing in normal circumstances, aversive stimuli can generate defensive burying behaviour towards novel potentially threatening objects27,28.

Burrowing

Burrowing is an innate behaviour in rodents that is frequently used to assess the overall wellbeing of mice2931. A plastic bottle (standard opaque water bottle, 250 ml, 150 mm length, 55 mm diameter) was filled with before mentioned food pellets and introduced into the home cage one hour before the onset of the dark phase. Mice were free to burrow the substrate out of the bottle32. The percentage of burrowed material was measured after 6 h. We familiarized the mice with the burrowing schedule for 5 days before the onset of the respective handling treatment. The burrowing performance was detected 2 days before and after the initial handling adaptation phase.

Forced swim test

The Forced Swim Test measures depressive-like behaviour. Mice were transferred to an experimental room where they were placed into a glass cylinder (23 cm high, 13 cm in diameter) filled with water (22 °C) up to a height of 8 cm. Within 6 min the onset and the percentage of floating were determined as described elsewhere33,34.

Fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCMs)

12 days after the start of the experimental handling technique, fecal samples for stress monitoring were collected from the cages 24 h after the cage change as previously described. This non-invasive method circumvents additional handling and restraint for blood sampling which could confound the experimental desgin. Fecal samples were extracted with methanol using a standard protocol35,36. Briefly, an aliquot (50 mg) of each well-homogenized, dried fecal sample was mixed with 1 ml 80% methanol, vortexed/shaken for 30 min and subsequently centrifuged for 10 min at 2500×g. The supernatant was then analysed using a 5α-pregnane-3β,11β,21-triol-20-one enzyme immunoassay as previously described and successfully validated for use in mice37,38.

Statistics

Statistical analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics 24. Differences were considered significant at p < 0.05. The FST and handling interaction data were analysed using three-way ANOVA with the factors ‘handling technique’, ‘handling frequency’ and ‘sex’ and, when appropriate, using repeated-measures ANOVA. Mann–Whitney U-Tests for independent samples and Wilcoxon test for related samples were used to analyze burrowing behaviour, as well as FCM concentrations. We additionally analysed the development of interaction behavior over the course of the training using linear mixed models (LMM) with parameter ~ time*condition + (1|ID). ID was considered a random effect. The repeated covariance type was set to scaled identity. The intercept for the default model was 5.0 s (pre)6 and 6.19 s (post) seconds for sniffing, 3.00 s (pre) and 3.75 s (post) for touching, 1.53 s (pre) and 1.81 s (post) for climbing and 0.81 s (pre) and 1.75 (post) for burrowing. Post-hoc analyses were Šidák corrected.

No animals were excluded from the study of from any statistical analyses. The single animal served as an experimental unit.

Results

Body weight was not affected by handling technique

Mice showed increase of body weight over time in the weekly assessments (before, during and after interaction assessment: F(4,224) = 82.417, p < 0.001) and the typical sex difference (F(1,56) = 169.824, p < 0.001). Additionally, males gained weight more quickly (time*sex F(4,224) = 12.172, p < 0.001). We did not detect treatment related differences Another interaction was between sex and intensity, where males weight less when handled daily (sex*intensity: F(1,56) = 3.543, p = 0.065).

Male mice showed stronger impairments in affective behaviour after tail handling

We detected the effects of the handling technique on depressive-like behaviour in the FST and reduced activity in the burrowing test (Fig. 1A,C).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Different dimensions of handling influenced the assessed parameters: (A) handling style influenced the depression-associated behaviour in males; (B) handling frequency influenced the FCM concentration in females; (C) handling technique influenced the burrowing behaviour in males. Handling evoked alterations in (A) depressive-like behaviour, (B) FCM concentrations and (C) burrowing performance. Males showed a significant increase in immobility and a decrease of burrowing due to tail handling, which indicates an impaired wellbeing. These parameters were unaltered in females. However, the FCM concentration of males remained unchanged, while the stress hormone response was more sensitive to the intense daily handling procedure, regardless of the applied technique of handling. Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

The handling technique significantly influenced the immobility in the FST (technique: F(1,56) = 4.458, p = 0.039) and, although no sex-effect was apparent, an interaction between sex and handling technique was revealed (technique*sex: F(1,56) = 5.080, p = 0.028), where tail handling was linked to more immobile behaviour in males (males: technique F(1,30) = 10.107, p = 0.003). Females showed no significant behavioural alterations. Handling frequency did not significantly influence immobility.

We did not observe differences linked to sex, technique or frequency of handling on burrowing behaviour. However, we did find a significant decrease in burrowing in males after daily tail handling for 2 weeks (Wilcoxon z = − 2.100 p = 0.036). The performance of females was not altered by any handling treatment.

Concentrations of fecal corticosterone metabolites (FCMs) were lower in more frequently handled mice

Higher concentrations of FCMs were found in female mice (sex U(1,41) = 95.000, p < 0.001; Fig. 1B). Sex-dependent analysis revealed no differences due to handling frequency for males, but a significant increase due to weekly handling in females (females: frequency U(1,22) = 34.000, p = 0.028). The handling technique did not influence FCM concentrations in both sexes.

Mice exhibit more positive interaction towards the tunnel

During the first two weeks of the respective handling techniques, we conducted the interaction test. We evaluated exploratory behaviours both before and after the handling (Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Voluntary interactions of daily handled mice with the handler before and after the handling session in the course of the initial handling. Left: explorative and positive behaviours (sniffing, touching, climbing). Right: defensive burrowing behaviour. N = 16, Data are represented as means ± SEM. *Differences of exploratory behaviours; s: sniffing approaches s < 0.05; t: touching approaches t < 0.05, tt < 0.01, ttt < 0.001; c: climbing approaches c < 0.05, cc < 0.01, ccc < 0.001; $: differences between pre and post handling.

In general, the tunnel handling led to more initial (= pre handling) positive interactions, such as touching (technique: F(1,90) = 60,399, p < 0.001) and climbing (technique: F(1,90) = 46.735, p < 0.001). Sex-specific differences were only observed in session 6 (sex: post F(1,28) = 8.958, p = 0.006) when females showed less approaches. In general, exploratory sniffing was not affected, but tail handled mice showed only a few touch responses and nearly no climbing. The defensive burrowing behaviour, on the other hand, was more pronounced in tail handled mice (technique: F(1,90 = 12,417, p < 0.001).We found differences between the test sessions for sniffing (session: F(2,90) = 4.627, p = 0.01, post hoc day 3 vs 6 p = 0.058) and climbing (session: F(2,90) = 4.401, p = 0.015, post hoc day 3 vs 6 p = 0.016). Both parameters increased comparing session 3 and 6.

The differences between the techniques was also prominent after the handling in the interaction test: touching (technique F(1,90) = 51.653, p < 0.001) and climbing (technique F(1,90) = 28.310, p < 0.001) was more prominent in tunnel handled mice. Interestingly, the measurement after the handling detected a difference between the techniques (F(1,90) = 22.672, p < 0.001). Sniffing remained unaltered. With respect to development of interaction over sessions, we detected a trend for increasing sniffing behavior (session: F(2,90) = 2.752, p = 0.069) and a significant effect for touching (session: F(2,90) = 5.258, p = 0.007, post hoc day 1 vs day 3 p = 0.037 and day 1 vs day 6 p = 0.010).

Differences in exploratory behaviour before and after handling were only detected after tail handling. In session 1 tail handling evoked reduced exploration (handling intervention: F(1,14) = 19.386 p = 0.001), which was reversed in session 3 (handling intervention: F(1,14) = 5.458 p = 0.035) and finally non-present in session 6. Defensive behaviour increased after the handling intervention in both handling techniques throughout the sessions—except for the tunnel handling in the final session.

Discussion

Tail handling is considered aversive to rodents as it resembles the feeling of being caught by a predator4. The evolutionary instinct is to flee and avoid. Handling with a tunnel or other alternative handling techniques like cup handling do not provoke such an instinctive reaction17. We hypothesized that tunnel handling circumvents potential stress for laboratory mice, preventing negative effects on affective behaviour and thus refining experimental procedures. In this study, we focused on the initial adaptation to tunnel handling in the most widely used mouse strain in medical and behavioural research: C57BL/6. We found that both stress hormone release and affective behaviour were influenced by the handling technique and handling frequency in a sex-dependent manner.

Tunnel handling has already been validated as a tool to reduce anxiety-like behaviour in male and female mice4,5. Clarkson and Dwyer also demonstrated that tunnel handling led to less anhedonia-like behaviour compared to tail handling3. Our results corroborate the observation of a depressive-like phenotype of tail handled mice in the FST for males. The anhedonia study did not assess female mice, so their anhedonic status remains unknown. We did not detect despair-like behaviour in females. Further research is necessary to see, if possibly females are simply less sensitive to tail handling.

In males, tail handling triggered both anhedonic and despair behaviours which are typical characteristics of animal models of depression, e.g. stress-based models such as chronic mild stress (CMS)22,39. However, while CMS protocols often include a great variety of stressors, the daily tail handling was limited to one type of stressor and, unlike many of the CMS protocols4042, the daily handling stress was predictable. Anxiety might play a role in the establishment of CMS-induced depression, but some stressors are known to be aversive without being anxiety-focused, e.g. tilted cages, changes in circadian rhythms by continuous illumination or heat stress43, while tail handling is known to induce anxiety10. Perhaps the permanent exposition to the anxiogenic handling is one explanation for the specific effect on males—as some studies showed how female rodents typically display less anxiety-like behaviour44,45. The stress load of handling could be too small to affect females on a behavioural level. Males however could have a lower threshold of reacting to this stimulus. Interestingly, other studies showed no differences in anxiety-like behaviour due to tail handling5,8. Therefore, the origin of the sex-dependent effect is hard to pin down with the current results. A direct comparison of daily handling with a non-anxiogenic stressor could be used to confirm this idea.

Apart from depression-like behaviour, tail handling also reduced the performance in burrowing in the home cage setting. This parameter is commonly used as a marker for rodents’ wellbeing29. However, the handling frequency influenced the outcome: wellbeing-associated parameters were only affected in those animals which were handled daily. This indicates that depressive-like behaviour does not necessarily lead to abnormal burrowing behaviour, i.e. reduced wellbeing. Therefore a burrowing mouse may have impairments in affective behaviours, but a mouse that does not burrow is more likely to display such impairments. Apparently, the affective state of a mouse cannot be detected by a single wellbeing parameter, but it can be still used as an indicator16,29,46,47. More research is necessary to illuminate the full connection between burrowing behaviour and the affective state46. Nonetheless, we found burrowing to be a sensitive tool for the assessment of effects in males induced by daily tail handling. In female mice, however, we did not detect any welfare or affective impairments.

FCM concentrations reflect adrenocortical activity and thus the stress hormone response, well48. Some studies have already observed a sex-dependent corticosterone increase, where females appeared to have higher corticosterone levels after stressful events49,50. Generally, FCM levels are higher in females due to differences in the metabolism37,38,49,50. Unfortunately, many studies solely used males, falsely assuming that the utilization of females always necessitates the observation of the oestrous cycle and inevitably increases the variability in results51. As we did include females in our study, we were able to observe higher FCM concentrations in weekly handled female mice. FCM concentrations of daily handled females and all males, however, appeared undisturbed. One explanation could be that the habituation to the low-frequency handling scheme was not as quick for females as it was for males and hence, this interaction protocol induced a stress hormone response. Repeated contact with the handler is known to habituate rodents to handling, when performed with procedures which are as stress-free as possible5 or follow an escalating protocol, in which mice are introduced to tail handling over several days starting with the simple presentation of the hand and short interactions. This introduction to the handling process led to a better initial performance in a cognitive test, possibly due to reduced stress and anxiety52. This observation is in line with the reduced FCM concentrations of the daily handled female mice in our study, underlining the benefit of habituation on the stress response. In general, females, irrespective of the handling method, showed higher concentrations of plasma corticosterone53, proposing that females may be more sensitive to handling as for other types of stressors54,55. The analysed faecal samples were collected 12 days after the onset of the respective handling, which might have been a sufficient time frame to adapt to the handling stress for all males and the daily handled females, but not for the weekly handled females. We think it is particularly noteworthy that, although the FCM concentrations of males were not influenced by handling technique or handling frequency, we confirm significant stress-associated affective and wellbeing associated impairments with an increase in the despair behaviour56,57 and a reduced burrowing in the home cage29,58, while females remained unaffected regarding behaviours in these domains. This supports the fact that in most murine models females exhibit less anxiety in behavioural tests than males59.

Both sexes demonstrated more interaction with the tunnel than with the hand. We further specified the interaction between the subject and the handling instrument into different categories, to analyse the behaviour more in detail. Sniffing was categorized as neutral exploration. In contrast, we classified touching and climbing onto the handling instrument as a positive interaction, demonstrating the willingness for voluntary physical exposition and signaling a feeling of safety. Lastly, we identified burrowing during the interaction test as a defensive behaviour, comparable to the one seen in the novelty-induced anxiety, in which this defensive burrowing behaviour takes place after the exposure to a novel yet harmless object60,61. While both, the exploration and the positive behaviours, were prominently enhanced towards the tunnel in comparison to the hand, defensive burrowing was more pronounced towards the experimenter’s hand, especially directly after the interaction. In line with other studies5,7, the hand appears more aversive to the animal. In summary, the tunnel was explored more often and the quality of approaches was more positive and less defensive17,62.

Our findings are limited to individually housed experimental mice. Mice are social animals and are preferably housed in groups6365. There are however, situations in which individual housing can be appropriate, especially in male mice, where single housing avoids aggressiveness towards conspecifics—which is a welfare issue. Besides the obvious injuries and pain, the frequent exposure to social distress can lead to depression-like states in attacked lower ranked animals66. In female mice aggression is very rare and hence this argumentation does not hold up. But for comparability reasons between the groups they might be socially isolated as well. A systematic evaluation whether group-housed females might be a suitable control group to males is not available. In general, social isolation is considered a burden for female mice and from this perspective it is interesting to see, that the females in our study showed no alteration in behaviour, but only an elevated FCM response to weekly handling independent from the handling technique. Single housed males were sensitive to tail handling with respect to despair and burrowing. However, Mertens et al.67 showed that tail handling can still be an appropriate handling technique for male mice, as it reduces aggressiveness after the transfer to new cages in group-housed C57BL6/N. Since tail handling did not negatively influence the mice compared to tunnel handling—except for anxiety-related behaviour—they even propose tail handling as a refinement to prevent aggression, which promotes the beneficial group housing over social isolation.

The ethical principle of the 3R (replace, reduce, refine) is to minimize the number of animal experiments while maximizing the welfare of laboratory animals and the produced results68. Scientists have largely committed to it and it is now integrated into legislation in the EU directive 2010/63. Especially concerning refinement, handling techniques have been a matter of intense debate in recent years. The most common procedure, tail handling, has been shown to induce not only anxiety but also depression-like behaviour. Alternative techniques to circumvent these strains and stresses are valuable, but only sparsely implemented in daily routines nowadays. The feasibility of tunnel handling is still being discussed. Many researchers see the tunnel solely as a tool for environmental enrichment, which is implemented into the home cage. And while environmental enrichment is a valuable tool for increasing the welfare of animals69, it can also become a confounding factor in experiments70. Therefore, some researchers might balk at the idea of an additional factor to consider in their experimental design. However, it is not necessary to enrich the home cage in order to handle them with the tunnel. Gouveia and Hurst demonstrated that the use of a tunnel, which served as a permanent enrichment within the home cage of the animals, was initially preferred over a tunnel, to which the animals were only exposed to during handling5. But in the long run, both tunnel-based approaches showed similar responses in the elevated plus-maze, when compared to a tail handled group, signifying the higher impact of the handling technique over the familiarity of the handling instrument5. Moreover, the welfare effect of tunnel handling might be difficult to detect in wild type mice, while it might be easier in disease models. In a chronic kidney disease model, for instance, the severity of symptoms ameliorated in female mice49.

Some studies have found evidence for reduced experimental variation due to tunnel handling. The mean variation is one of the parameters that define the statistically necessary group size. Lower variation can lead to the reduction of animals needed to achieve meaningful scientific results71. Fridgeirsdottir and Hillered52 found that the gentle habituation of escalating handling led to reduced variation in the Morris water maze test. Nakamura and Suzuki8 found decreased variation in the open field and elevated O-maze test in tunnel handled compared to tail handled mice. In our study, we did not observe reduced variability in the results of our experiments. Further and repeated experiments would be necessary to fine-tune the method and to analyse whether handling can also be successfully used to reduce the number of animals, independent of the experimenter and lab. Another concern is that changing the handling technique can be an incisive process as it may complicate comparison to former results, by introducing this additional factor. On the other hand, environmental stressors as tail handling can reduce reliability and reproducibility14,72.

A conversion to tunnel handling as a standard operation procedure in also in large scaled facilities is accompanied with substantial alterations of processes with new steps of procedure, e.g. cleaning of tunnels. It can also raise costs for purchasing of new material, especially if the permanent home enrichment is put in place, and follow-up costs, e.g. by cleaning5,17. In addition, this new materials needs storage capacity. Hence, economic evaluations need to be considered to estimate whether the investment is worthwhile in each particular case. Various economic tools are available according to the objective of the analysis; cost–benefit analysis (CBA) and cost-effectiveness analysis (CEA) are the most relevant73. In fact, while CBA uses monetary terms for evaluating costs and outcomes to provide information about the financial feasibility of the project, CEA considers the relation between costs and effectiveness of two different interventions. Both can be useful to highlight the potential benefit of non-aversive handling in terms of allocation of resources and comparison with the state of the art techniques74,75. However, merely economic evaluations are not sufficient to justify the adoption or rejection of a new procedure, while animal wellbeing and experiment reproducibility should be the main drivers. Indeed, more research is necessary to understand how handling influences the phenotype to prevent misleading scientific results and feasibility evaluation of whether or not to use tunnels instead of tail handling should be assessed not only comprising the potentially initial higher workload and costs, but also focusing on the wellbeing of the subjects. Apart from the obvious ethical aspects, impaired wellbeing is a confounding factor for many readouts in animal experiments. Whenever the affective state of the experimental animals plays a role in the research question of a study, the usage of an alternative handling method (like tunnel handling) seems an advisable approach.

Acknowledgements

We thank Edith Klobetz-Rassam for her excellent technical support and EIA analysis and Donata Dominoni for her support in the data acquisition. This work was supported by grants from the DFG (Forschergruppe 2591 ‘Severity assessment in animal based research’, Project P05) to P.G. and by grants from the Italian Ministry of University and Research (PRIN 2017AY8BP4 and PON “Ricerca e Innovazione” PerMedNet Project ARS01_01226) to M.A.R. D.I. acknowledges grant support by the German Research Foundation (DFG, Grant DI 168/3-1), the Ingeborg Ständer Foundation, the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF, the ERA-NET NEURON program, NMDAR-PSY, Grant 01EW1807A) and the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF, the Sinergia, Grant 186346).

Author contributions

A.M., D.I., R.P., M.R. and P.G. conceived the idea for the study. P.G., D.I. and M.R. acquired funding. A.M., R.P., C.R. and N.P. designed the study and protocols. F.S., N.P., C.B. and A.M. carried out experiments. A.M. analysed data. F.S. and A.M. drafted the manuscript, and all authors commented on the manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Deacon RM. Assessing nest building in mice. Nat. protoc. 2006;1:1117–1119. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wurbel H. Ideal homes? Housing effects on rodent brain and behaviour. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:207–211. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01718-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Clarkson JM, Dwyer DM, Flecknell PA, Leach MC, Rowe C. Handling method alters the hedonic value of reward in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:2448. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20716-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hurst JL, West RS. Taming anxiety in laboratory mice. Nat. Methods. 2010;7:825–826. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Reducing mouse anxiety during handling: effect of experience with handling tunnels. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e66401. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Optimising reliability of mouse performance in behavioural testing: the major role of non-aversive handling. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:44999. doi: 10.1038/srep44999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ghosal S, et al. Mouse handling limits the impact of stress on metabolic endpoints. Physiol. Behav. 2015;150:31–37. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Nakamura Y, Suzuki K. Tunnel use facilitates handling of ICR mice and decreases experimental variation. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2018;80:886–892. doi: 10.1292/jvms.18-0044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Deacon RM. Housing, husbandry and handling of rodents for behavioral experiments. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:936–946. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Novak J, Bailoo JD, Melotti L, Rommen J, Würbel H. An exploration based cognitive bias test for mice: effects of handling method and stereotypic behaviour. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0130718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bourin M, Hascoët M. The mouse light/dark box test. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2003;463:55–65. doi: 10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01274-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Schneider P, et al. Altered synaptic phospholipid signaling in PRG-1 deficient mice induces exploratory behavior and motor hyperactivity resembling psychiatric disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 2018;336:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.08.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Brown GR, Nemes C. The exploratory behaviour of rats in the hole-board apparatus: is head-dipping a valid measure of neophilia? Behav. Process. 2008;78:442–448. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Mandillo S, et al. Reliability, robustness, and reproducibility in mouse behavioral phenotyping: a cross-laboratory study. Physiol. Genom. 2008;34:243–255. doi: 10.1152/physiolgenomics.90207.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Bailey KR, Rustay NR, Crawley JN. Behavioral phenotyping of transgenic and knockout mice: practical concerns and potential pitfalls. ILAR J. 2006;47:124–131. doi: 10.1093/ilar.47.2.124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jirkof P, Rudeck J, Lewejohann L. Assessing affective state in laboratory rodents to promote animal welfare: what is the progress in applied refinement research? Animals. 2019 doi: 10.3390/ani9121026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gouveia K, Hurst JL. Improving the practicality of using non-aversive handling methods to reduce background stress and anxiety in laboratory mice. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:20305. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56860-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ. Enriched environments, experience-dependent plasticity and disorders of the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:697–709. doi: 10.1038/nrn1970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hånell A, Marklund N. Structured evaluation of rodent behavioral tests used in drug discovery research. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;8:252. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zhao Y, et al. A mouse model of depression induced by repeated corticosterone injections. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008;581:113–120. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kim KS, Kwon HJ, Baek IS, Han PL. Repeated short-term (2hx14d) emotional stress induces lasting depression-like behavior in mice. Exp. Neurobiol. 2012;21:16–22. doi: 10.5607/en.2012.21.1.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Willner P. The chronic mild stress (CMS) model of depression: history, evaluation and usage. Neurobiol. Stress. 2017;6:78–93. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gjendal K, Ottesen JL, Olsson IAS, Sorensen DB. Effect of repeated exposure to isoflurane on nest building and burrowing in mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2020;59:30–36. doi: 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-19-000027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Abdelrahman A, et al. A novel multi-parametric analysis of non-invasive methods to assess animal distress during chronic pancreatitis. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:14084. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50682-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Falkenberg MK, et al. Clinical, physiologic, and behavioral evaluation of permanently catheterized NMRI mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2019;58:380–389. doi: 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-18-000060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Palme R. Non-invasive measurement of glucocorticoids: Advances and problems. Physiol. Behav. 2019;199:229–243. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Thomas A, et al. Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204:361–373. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1466-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.de Brouwer G, Fick A, Harvey BH, Wolmarans W. A critical inquiry into marble-burying as a preclinical screening paradigm of relevance for anxiety and obsessive-compulsive disorder: mapping the way forward. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2019;19:1–39. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00653-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jirkof P. Burrowing and nest building behavior as indicators of well-being in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;234:139–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hohlbaum K, et al. Severity classification of repeated isoflurane anesthesia in C57BL/6JRj mice: assessing the degree of distress. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0179588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mallien AS, et al. Systematic analysis of severity in a widely used cognitive depression model for mice. Lab. Anim. 2020;54:40–49. doi: 10.1177/0023677219874831. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Deacon RM. Burrowing in rodents: a sensitive method for detecting behavioral dysfunction. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:118–121. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lima-Ojeda JM, et al. Pharmacological blockade of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors induces antidepressant-like effects lacking psychotomimetic action and neurotoxicity in the perinatal and adult rodent brain. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;45:28–33. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kronenberg G, et al. Exofocal dopaminergic degeneration as antidepressant target in mouse model of poststroke depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2012;72:273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mallien AS, et al. Daily exposure to a touchscreen-paradigm and associated food restriction evokes an increase in adrenocortical and neural activity in mice. Horm. Behav. 2016;81:97–105. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Palme R, Touma C, Arias N, Dominchin MF, Lepschy M. Steroid extraction: get the best out of faecal samples. Vet. Med. Austria. 2013;100:238–246. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Touma C, Palme R, Sachser N. Analyzing corticosterone metabolites in fecal samples of mice: a noninvasive technique to monitor stress hormones. Horm. Behav. 2004;45:10–22. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Touma C, Sachser N, Mostl E, Palme R. Effects of sex and time of day on metabolism and excretion of corticosterone in urine and feces of mice. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2003;130:267–278. doi: 10.1016/S0016-6480(02)00620-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Antoniuk S, Bijata M, Ponimaskin E, Wlodarczyk J. Chronic unpredictable mild stress for modeling depression in rodents: meta-analysis of model reliability. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019;99:101–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Strekalova T, Spanagel R, Bartsch D, Henn FA, Gass P. Stress-induced anhedonia in mice is associated with deficits in forced swimming and exploration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2007–2017. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gosselin T, et al. Fluoxetine induces paradoxical effects in C57BL6/J mice: comparison with BALB/c mice. Behav. Pharmacol. 2017;28:466–476. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hill MN, Hellemans KG, Verma P, Gorzalka BB, Weinberg J. Neurobiology of chronic mild stress: parallels to major depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012;36:2085–2117. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Liu LL, Li JM, Su WJ, Wang B, Jiang CL. Sex differences in depressive-like behaviour may relate to imbalance of microglia activation in the hippocampus. Brain Behav. Immun. 2019 doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2019.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Luine V. Sex differences in chronic stress effects on memory in rats. Stress. 2002;5:205–216. doi: 10.1080/1025389021000010549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.An XL, et al. Strain and sex differences in anxiety-like and social behaviors in C57BL/6J and BALB/cJ mice. Exp. Anim. 2011;60:111–123. doi: 10.1538/expanim.60.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hohlbaum K, et al. Systematic assessment of well-being in mice for procedures using general anesthesia. J. Vis. Exp. 2018 doi: 10.3791/57046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Trevarthen AC, et al. Measuring affect-related cognitive bias: do mice in opposite affective states react differently to negative and positive stimuli? PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0226438. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0226438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bamberg E, Palme R, Meingassner JG. Excretion of corticosteroid metabolites in urine and faeces of rats. Lab. Anim. 2001;35:307–314. doi: 10.1258/0023677011911886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ono M, et al. Does the routine handling affect the phenotype of disease model mice? Jpn. J. Vet. Res. 2016;64:265–271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Harizi H, Homo-Delarche F, Amrani A, Coulaud J, Mormede P. Marked genetic differences in the regulation of blood glucose under immune and restraint stress in mice reveals a wide range of corticosensitivity. J. Neuroimmunol. 2007;189:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2007.06.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Prendergast BJ, Onishi KG, Zucker I. Female mice liberated for inclusion in neuroscience and biomedical research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2014;40:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Fridgeirsdottir GA, Hillered L, Clausen F. Escalated handling of young C57BL/6 mice results in altered Morris water maze performance. Ups. J. Med. Sci. 2014;119:1–9. doi: 10.3109/03009734.2013.847511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Aoki M, Shimozuru M, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y. Sex differences in behavioral and corticosterone responses to mild stressors in ICR mice are altered by ovariectomy in peripubertal period. Zool. Sci. 2010;27:783–789. doi: 10.2108/zsj.27.783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Marchette RCN, Bicca MA, Santos ECDS, de Lima TCM. Distinctive stress sensitivity and anxiety-like behavior in female mice: strain differences matter. Neurobiol. Stress. 2018;9:55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2018.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bondar NP, Lepeshko AA, Reshetnikov VV. Effects of early-life stress on social and anxiety-like behaviors in adult mice: sex-specific effects. Behav. Neurol. 2018;2018:1538931. doi: 10.1155/2018/1538931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Commons KG, Cholanians AB, Babb JA, Ehlinger DG. The rodent forced swim test measures stress-coping strategy, not depression-like behavior. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 2017;8:955–960. doi: 10.1021/acschemneuro.7b00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lam VYY, et al. Chronic stress alters behavior in the forced swim test and underlying neural activity in animals exposed to alcohol prenatally: sex- and time-dependent effects. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:42. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Shepherd AJ, Cloud ME, Cao YQ, Mohapatra DP. Deficits in burrowing behaviors are associated with mouse models of neuropathic but not inflammatory pain or migraine. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2018;12:124. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Johnston AL, File SE. Sex differences in animal tests of anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 1991;49:245–250. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90039-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Taylor GT, Lerch S, Chourbaji S. Marble burying as compulsive behaviors in male and female mice. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 2017;77:254–260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Wolmarans D, Stein DJ, Harvey BH. Of mice and marbles: novel perspectives on burying behavior as a screening test for psychiatric illness. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2016;16:551–560. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0413-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Roughan JV, Sevenoaks T. Welfare and scientific considerations of tattooing and ear tagging for mouse identification. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2019;58:142–153. doi: 10.30802/AALAS-JAALAS-18-000057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kalliokoski O, Teilmann AC, Jacobsen KR, Abelson KS, Hau J. The lonely mouse: single housing affects serotonergic signaling integrity measured by 8-OH-DPAT-induced hypothermia in male mice. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e111065. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0111065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Arakawa H. Ethological approach to social isolation effects in behavioral studies of laboratory rodents. Behav. Brain Res. 2018;341:98–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Van Loo PL, Van de Weerd HA, Van Zutphen LF, Baumans V. Preference for social contact versus environmental enrichment in male laboratory mice. Lab. Anim. 2004;38:178–188. doi: 10.1258/002367704322968867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kappel S, Hawkings P, Mendl MT. To group or not to group? Good practice for housing male laboratory mice. Animals. 2017 doi: 10.3390/ani7120088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Mertens S, et al. Effect of three different forms of handling on the variation of aggression-associated parameters in individually and group-housed male C57BL/6NCrl mice. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0215367. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Russell WMS, Burch RL. The Principles of Humane Experimental Technique. New York: Methuen; 1959. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Olsson IA, Dahlborn K. Improving housing conditions for laboratory mice: a review of "environmental enrichment". Lab. Anim. 2002;36:243–270. doi: 10.1258/002367702320162379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Bayne K. Potential for unintended consequences of environmental enrichment for laboratory animals and research results. ILAR J. 2005;46:129–139. doi: 10.1093/ilar.46.2.129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bodden C, et al. Heterogenising study samples across testing time improves reproducibility of behavioural data. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:8247. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-44705-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Wahlsten D. Standardizing tests of mouse behavior: reasons, recommendations, and reality. Physiol. Behav. 2001;73:695–704. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00527-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bala MV, Zarkin GA, Mauskopf JA. Conditions for the near equivalence of cost-effectiveness and cost-benefit analyses. Value Health. 2002;5:338–346. doi: 10.1046/j.1524-4733.2002.54134.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bennett AJ, Corcoran CA, Hardy VA, Miller LR, Pierre PJ. Multidimensional cost-benefit analysis to guide evidence-based environmental enrichment: providing bedding and foraging substrate to pen-housed monkeys. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 2010;49:571–577. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.BaboMartins S, Rushton J. Cost-effectiveness analysis: adding value to assessment of animal health welfare and production. Rev. Sci. Technol. 2014;33:681–689. doi: 10.20506/rst.33.3.2312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES