Abstract
A healthy mitochondrial network is essential for the maintenance of neuronal synaptic integrity. Mitochondrial and metabolic dysfunction contributes to the pathogenesis of many neurodegenerative diseases including dementia. OPA1 is the master regulator of mitochondrial fusion and fission and is likely to play an important role during neurodegenerative events. To explore this, we quantified hippocampal dendritic and synaptic integrity and the learning and memory performance of aged Opa1 haploinsufficient mice carrying the Opa1Q285X mutation (B6; C3-Opa1Q285STOP; Opa1+/−). We demonstrate that heterozygous loss of Opa1 results in premature age-related loss of spines in hippocampal pyramidal CA1 neurons and a reduction in synaptic density in the hippocampus. This loss is associated with subtle memory deficits in both spatial novelty and object recognition. We hypothesize that metabolic failure to maintain normal neuronal activity at the level of a single spine leads to premature age-related memory deficits. These results highlight the importance of mitochondrial homeostasis for maintenance of neuronal function during ageing.
Keywords: OPA1, optic atrophy, memory, hippocampus, ageing
Mitochondria generate energy required for memory function. Aged mice having genetically reduced amount of OPA1 protein necessary for energy production have subtle but persistent deficits in behavioural memory. Anatomical examination of the relevant brain region (hippocampal CA1) revealed changes affecting communication between cells and leading to premature age-related memory deficits.
Graphical Abstract
Introduction
Mitochondria play a critical role in cell death and survival (Fieni et al., 2012). Compromised mitochondrial function (Hedskog et al., 2013) is common in neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s, Parkinson's, and Huntington's disease (Schon and Area-Gomez, 2013; Krols et al., 2016; Cai and Tammineni, 2017; Erpapazoglou et al., 2017). While neuronal metabolism accounts for ∼20% of the body’s total energy demand (Yu et al., 2018), mitochondrial respiration, which is least 15 times more efficient at ATP production than glycolysis (Rich, 2003), is largely responsible for meeting this demand. Mitochondrial morphology is controlled by mitochondrial fission and fusion, and in neurons this ensures the correct shape and distribution of mitochondria in relation to dendrite and synapse location. Elongated mitochondria, for example, accelerate the propagation of local changes in membrane potential within a neuron (Skulachev, 2001). Neurons require large amounts of ATP to maintain ionic gradients across cell membranes and support synaptic transmission. The level of ATP demand is specific for each individual spine and synapse. Not surprisingly, many age-related neurodegenerative diseases are characterized by deficient mitochondrial renewal or dynamics as reviewed by Chan (2006). Healthy ageing requires enough metabolic capacity to cope with stress and injury (Atamna et al., 2018). By contrast, reduction in flexibility of cellular transport or inability to meet peak-time metabolic demands results in cellular oxidative stress, senescence, cell type transformation, and, ultimately, cell death (Chauhan et al., 2014; Rattan, 2015).
The mitochondrial fusion protein OPA1 is embedded in the mitochondrial intermembrane space and controls the inner membrane structure (Griparic et al., 2004). OPA1 mutations result in abnormal mitochondrial dynamics (Liao et al., 2017) and neurodegeneration (Alexander et al., 2000; Delettre et al., 2000). The associated disease, autosomal dominant optic atrophy, is characterized by progressive bilateral central vision loss and is accompanied by extra-ophthalmological symptoms in ∼20% of cases. The autosomal dominant optic atrophy disease phenotype can be replicated in animal models (Williams et al., 2011; Carelli et al., 2013; Ham et al., 2019). Heterozygous B6:C3-Opa1Q285STOP (Opa1+/−) mice have normal longevity, but gradually develop visual dysfunction, resembling a slow onset model of the human disease (Davies et al., 2007). The Opa1+/− mutation results in a ∼50% reduction in OPA1 levels, but does not alter its structure (Davies et al., 2007). The initial retinal pathology in Opa1+/− mice; retinal ganglion cell dendritic degeneration, synaptic loss and mitochondrial changes, starts at 10 months, but the visual phenotype is only detectable clinically by 12 months (Davies et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2010). These deficits can be quantified by visual electrophysiology (electroretinography; Barnard et al., 2011) and visual acuity [optokinetic response (OKN) behavioural testing; Davies et al., 2007]. Opa1 haploinsufficiency reduces mitochondrial efficiency (Del Dotto et al., 2018), but the associated pathology is relatively mild in comparison with mutations causing protein misfolding or truncation. In humans, OPA1 protein levels decrease with age causing cases of muscle dystrophy (Tezze et al., 2017) or even systemic neurodegeneration (Sarzi et al., 2012).
In this study, we assessed hippocampal-dependent memory (Tulving et al., 1972) using two tests that could be performed with reduced visual acuity and complement each other (Denninger et al., 2018): one evaluates spatial learning and the other non-spatial learning of object identity. While all our mice were able to perform behavioural tasks, Opa1+/− mice showed some consistent deficits. Following this finding, we examine cellular integrity of CA1, as both tests require intact CA1 hippocampal region.
Materials and methods
Animals and genotyping
All experiments were conducted in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act and the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology (ARVO) guidelines for the use of animals in research. The Cardiff University Biological Standards Committee approved the experimental protocols. We followed the guidelines of the Animal and Plant Health Agency for compliance with Regulation (EC) 1069/2009 and implementing Regulation (EC) 142/2011 for the transport, storage, use and disposal of animal by-products.
The mouse model (B6; C3-Opa1Q285STOP; Opa1+/−) has been described in detail elsewhere (Davies et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2011). The colony was kept in a 12-h light (10 lux)/12-h dark cycle with food and water available ad libitum. All experimental animals were outcrossed to a C57BL6J background to at least five generations. Visual acuity was evaluated by behavioural testing (OKNs) at 14 months of age followed by two memory tests [spatial novelty testing (T-maze) and novel object recognition (NOR) test] at 15 months. An additional control group of animals was tested for visual acuity at 20 months.
All mice taking part in the behavioural memory tests were euthanized at 16 months of age. The genotype was confirmed, and neuronal structure in hippocampal tissue examined by DiOlistics (WT; n = 5, Opa1+/−; n = 6), RT-PCR (WT; n = 3, Opa1+/−; n = 3), western blotting (WT; n = 2, Opa1+/−; n = 2) and immunohistochemistry (WT; n = 5, Opa1+/−; n = 6) to obtain pilot data. We followed up the initial study with a more robust examination of hippocampal tissue (reported here) from 24 mice (16 months of age) with DiOlistics (WT: n = 6; Opa1+/−; n = 6, male/female ratio was 50/50) and western blots (WT; n = 5, Opa1+/−; n = 7).
Behaviour
All behavioural tests were performed in a dedicated behavioural lab. Animals were housed in home cages before and after the procedure; all animals were returned to the home cages for at least 3 min between trials and for at least 1 min between different phases of the same test.
Visual acuity tests
We followed the EMPReSS method (http://empress.har.mrc.ac.uk/) to record OKNs. We tested visual acuity using drifting, full contrast black-and-white square wave gratings (2°, ∼1 cm width) with spatial frequency of 0.25 cycles per degree selected as the known upper limit of spatial frequency for aged Opa1+/− mice (Davies et al., 2007). All mice (WT: n = 12, Opa1+/−: n = 12, 6 animals per group at 14 and 20 months) were habituated to the apparatus (optokinetic drum, with stimuli presented on the inner surface) for 10 min in the dark, then 5 min in light. The illuminance was 250 candelas at the level of the platform. The drum rotated clockwise for 1 min, and after a 30 s rest period, reversed the direction. Each mouse ran the test daily for 3 days in a row, three times a day. We filmed and scored mice behaviour using two independent experts masked in relation to mice age and genotype. We counted the number and duration of episodes when head movements followed the direction and speed of rotating drum (Davies et al., 2007).
Memory tests
We selected memory tests previously suggested for subjects with impaired vision (Brown and Wong, 2007; O’Leary et al., 2011; Wolf et al., 2016): T-maze and a simple variant of the NOR. We purposefully avoided tests based on visually guided navigation such as large open arena or Morris water-maze. While both selected tests exploit the inherent preference of mice for the novelty, they are complimentary regarding brain circuitry involved in the task (Denninger et al., 2018). The T-maze evaluates spatial learning, which relies heavily on hippocampus. The NOR evaluates non-spatial learning of object identity, which relies on multiple brain regions. Each mouse ran the test multiple times on different non-consecutive days.
Spatial novelty (T-maze) test
We followed standard protocols for testing spatial novelty in mice using a T-maze. All mice (WT: n = 10, Opa1+/−: n = 16) were kept in their holding cages before, after and during the testing. Initially, one arm of a T-maze was blocked off and mice were allowed to investigate the maze for 10 min. The door to the blocked off arm was then removed and mice were allowed further 5 min for active exploration. There was only a minimum delay between ‘sampling’ and ‘choice’ phases of the test. Time spent in the base of the maze (TB, also referred as ‘start’), and in each of the arms [novel (TN), also referred as ‘new’ and familiar (TF), also referred as ‘old’] was recorded. Mice were tested for six consecutive days, at the same time of day on each occasion. Standard discrimination (DI) and recognition indices (RI) measured novelty behaviour.
(eq.1) |
(eq.2) |
A positive discrimination index score indicates a preference for novel. The RI score indicates the proportion of time spent investigating novelty relative to the total exploration time.
Novel object recognition test
We followed a previously published protocol (Ennaceur and Delacour, 1988) with minor modifications. All mice (WT: n = 9, Opa1+/−: n = 18) were weighed before testing and kept in their holding cages before, after and during the testing. Mice were first habituated to an empty white walled Perspex arena (60 cm × 60 cm × 15 cm) for 10 min on each of three consecutive days. Each mouse was then placed in the arena with two identical objects for 10 min and allowed to explore (exposure phase). Time spent exploring different objects at exposure phase was noted and is further referred to as ‘contact time’. After 1 min in the home cage, the mouse was placed back in the arena which now contained one original and one novel object. A mouse was allowed further 5 min of exploration (test phase). The time spent exploring the arena base (TB), familiar (TF) and the novel (TN) objects during test phase was recorded. Mice ran the test for six consecutive days, at the same time of day on each occasion. Objects (n = 7) made from different materials (metal, resin, silicone, wood, fabric, slate and glass) were alternated on each day to prevent any possible bias. As with the T-maze, we calculated discrimination index (eq.1) and RI, (eq.2).
Statistical analyses of behavioural data
Time variables in all behavioural tests (average time for following rotation drum in OKN visual acuity test, TB, TN and TF in both T-maze and NOR) were normally distributed and were analysed by parametric statistics. Count values (number of head turns) in OKN, and number of correct choices in T-maze and NOR were not necessary normally distributed, especially when counts were small. Recognition indices (range values) were not normally distributed (skewed unimodal distributions). All not normally distributed data were compared using non-parametric statistics: Kruskal–Willis test as alternative to the one-way ANOVA and the Mann–Whitney is an alternative to the two-sample t-test. To increase the power of the statistical tests, each subject/sample was tested several times and data pooled for the test (with additional adjustment for effective sample size). The between-subject factors and the effect sizes are indicated together with P-values. All plots are presented as mean and standard error. Significance levels marked with *, **, *** for P-values 0.05, 0.01 and 0.001 accordingly.
Tissue analysis
For all tissue analysis presented in this study, the experimenters were masked in relation to mice genotype (published methods Davies et al., 2007) to avoid any possible bias in data processing and analysis.
Quantitative RT-PCR
Tissue analyses were performed by Central Bioscience Services, Cardiff University. Total RNA was isolated with Trizol-Reagent and purified using the RNeasy Clean Up kit (Qiagen). One microgram of total RNA from each sample was reverse transcribed using the high capacity cDNA reverse transcription kit (Applied Biosystems). Quantitative PCR reactions were prepared using TaqMan® Universal PCR Master Mix, No AmpErase® UNG (Applied Biosystems) and mixed with cDNA, Taqman primers and probe gene specific assay mix. TaqMan gene expression assays (Applied Biosystems) were used for Mus musculus Opa1 (Assay ID: Mm00453879_m1), Syp (Mm00436850_m1) and PSD95 (Mm00492193_m1). qPCR was performed using an ABI Prism 7900HT (Applied Biosystems). Assays were carried out in triplicate with the mean Ct values used to calculate the relative gene expression levels after normalizing to 18S RNA levels (endogenous control: VIC/MGB Probe, Primer Limited). Analysis of relative gene expression data was performed using the 2−ΔΔCT method. Statistical analysis was performed on normally distributed ΔΔC values by multivariate ANOVA with Tukey post hoc test. The data are represented as RQ values in folds.
Western blot analysis
We tested two synaptic proteins (PSD95 and synaptophysin), microtubulin protein Tau, and OPA1 protein simultaneously on the same samples to correlate protein changes. The nature of synaptic changes can be interpreted as following: (i) reduction in synaptophysin with no changes in other synaptic proteins would indicate reduced efficacy of pre-synaptic terminal; (ii) reduction in synaptophysin with significant reduction in Tau would signal axonal degeneration; and (iii) PSD95 reduction in combination with modest decline in Tau would signal reduced size or postsynaptic terminals. Since OPA1, PSD95 and Tau had similar molecular weights (∼80kDa) and bands in close proximity, membranes had to be stripped and washed several times to optimize band separation for quantitative analysis (a range of exposure times were used to optimize the appearance of each protein band).
For total protein extraction, mouse hippocampus was sonicated in RIPA buffer containing 1% ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid free protease inhibitors (Roche) on ice. Homogenized hippocampal samples with loading buffer were pre-heated to 95°C for 5 min. Equal amounts (40 μg) of protein extracts were separated by 12% sodium dodecyl sulphate-polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (SDS-PAGE) in Tris/glycine/sodium dodecyl sulphate running buffer (Biorad, Hemel Hempstead, UK). Electrophoresis was run for 1 h 15 min at 100 V; then gels were transferred to 0.2 μm nitrocellulose membrane (Bio-Rad, UK) for 75 min at 250 mA in a cold Tris/glycine transfer buffer (Biorad, Hemel Hempstead, UK) with 20% methanol (v/v). After the transfer, membranes were blocked with 5% bovine serum albumin (BSA, Sigma) in phosphate buffered saline for 1 h at room temperature. Precision Plus Protein Standard (Bio-Rad, UK) was loaded in a volume of 10 μl/lane to show the location of proteins at the distinct size in the gel.
To quantify and distinguish proteins with similar molecular weights we used Gel Stripping by Low pH (2.2) buffer (glycine, sodium dodecyl sulphate and Tween20 in distilled water). After reading out the results of the first set of proteins, gels we incubated in buffer two times, washed in TBS and TBST, and blocked in 5% skimmed milk (1 h). The gels were then re-probed with new primary antibody overnight. Proteins were probed with mouse anti-Tau (ab80579, 1 µg/ml, 1:1000), rabbit anti-synaptophysin (ab14692 1:1000), rabbit anti-PSD95 (ab18258, 1 µg/ml, 1:1000) and rabbit monoclonal to OPA1 (ab157457, 1:1000) (all Abcam, Cambridge, UK) overnight at 4°C. The mouse monoclonal anti-β-actin antibody (A5441, clone AC-74, 1:2000) (Sigma) and mouse monoclonal VDAC1/Porin antibody (ab14734, 1:2000) (Abcam, Cambridge, UK) were used as a loading control.
Following four washes in phosphate buffered saline, rabbit anti-mouse and goat anti-rabbit horseradish peroxidase secondary antibodies (Sigma) were used at 1:1000 and 1:2000, respectively, at room temperature for 2 h before being membrane being developed using ChemiDoc XRS+ imaging system with Image Lab image.
The membranes were cut into two parts; one for the high molecular weight proteins (OPA1, PSD95 and Tau) and one for the smaller molecular weights (β-actin and synaptophysin) to allow optimal transfer conditions.
Bands from multiple western blots were quantified using Bio-Rad Image lab software. We evaluated consistency of both normalization proteins (voltage-dependent anion channel and β-actin) as a function of OPA1 levels. All values were therefore normalized to β-actin because its single band was easily detected and well separated from other bands.
The protein data were normally distributed and compared with parametric statistics. The between-subject factors and the effect sizes (-partial- eta squared values) are indicated together with P-values. All plots are presented as mean and standard error, the significance levels marked with *, **, *** for P-values 0.05, 0.01 and 0.001 accordingly.
DiOlistic cell labelling
Cells were labelled randomly with the density of labelled cells depending primarily on the dye concentration in the bullets. We used a previously published protocol (Williams et al., 2013; Binley et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2016) with a few alterations. In brief, hippocampi were isolated under a dissection microscope and transferred to a McIlwain Tissue Chopper for 200 µm slicing in a septal to temporal direction. To increase the yield of labelled neurons without affecting our ability to trace individual cells, we used two different dyes. Tungsten particles (1.7 µm, 100 mg: BioRad) were coated with 2 mg of DiI (Molecular Probes) and 4 mg of DiO (Molecular Probes) distributed in Tefzel tubing and cut into 1.2 cm ‘bullets’. Hippocampal tissue slices were labelled (two labelling colours green and red) with a Helios gene gun at 120 psi through a 3 µm polyethylene terephthalate membrane filter to prevent dye particle clumping. The total time from dissection to labelling was less than 10 min. Slices were then incubated in Neurobasal Medium (Sigma) at 37°C, 5% CO2 for 30 min to allow dye diffusion. Following incubation, the tissue was transferred to adhesive microscope slides (HistoBond, Fisher Scientific), fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde for 30 min and counterstained with Hoechst nuclei. Slides were mounted in Prolong Gold AntiFade Reagent (Invitrogen) and imaged with confocal microscope within 1 week.
Confocal imaging
Though cells were labelled in all hippocampal regions, our analysis was restricted to CA1 pyramidal neurons. Neurons were imaged at 20× at 0.7 magnification of visual field for analyses of dendrite structure (Z stack interval 1 µm). Selected dendrites were imaged at 40× at full magnification to analyse spine structure (Z stack interval 0.6 µm). We imaged on average at least nine well labelled neurons per subject.
DiOlistic data analysis
Cell dendrites and spines were traced using Imaris 9.2 software (Bitplane, Oxford Instruments). Unprocessed confocal Z stack images were reconstructed in 3D to generate Sholl plots of dendritic complexity (Sholl, 1953) and automated analyses of dendritic branching patterns. All dendritic spines were automatically counted using the FilamentTracer Spine Classifier extension in Imaris, based on their shape (stubby, mushroom and thin). Spine counts were normalized to the length of the imaged dendrite. Proximal and distant apical dendrites were analysed separately. We used combination of parametric and non-parametric statistics. Similar to behavioural data, all cells from all subjects were pooled for the statistical test (with additional adjustment for effective sample size).The between-subject factors and the effect sizes are indicated together with P-values. All plots are presented as mean and standard error, the significance levels marked with *, **, *** for P-values 0.05, 0.01 and 0.001 accordingly.
Data availability
Original and derived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the authors on request.
Results
Vision loss in Opa1 deficient mice (B6; C3-Opa1Q285STOP, Opa1+/−)
We assessed severity of visual deficits using OKNs prior to memory tests (at 14 months) and at 20 months. When an animal resolves the stimulus pattern on the inner wall of optokinetic drum, it follows the movement of the drum with its head to maintain fixation; this reflex response does not depend on previous exposure or training (Thaung et al., 2002). The scores for this visual acuity test were not directly comparable between different ages. Young animals followed the stimulus often, but for a very short time. In contrast, the older animals followed the stimulus for longer with fewer head turns. To avoid any bias, we used both measures and only directly compared animal performance within the same age group.
Opa1+/− mice demonstrated a reduction in the number of tracking episodes and in tracking time per episode compared to age-matched controls (Fig. 1A and B, left; head turns reduced from 13.3 ± 2.1 to 9.8 ± 0.9 at 14 months, Z = −1.84, P = 0.033; and from 10.8 ± 1.9 to 7.4 ± 1.1, Z = −1.68, P = 0.046 at 20 months; total tracking time reduced from 32.4 ± 2.7 to 25.6 ± 2.5 at 14 months, Z = −1.63, P = 0.052 and from 27.7 ± 2.8 to 17.3 ± 1.9, Z = −1.88, P = 0.03 at 20 months of age).
Although the visual acuity in aged Opa1+/− mice was reduced compare to aged matched controls, animals could still see relatively high special frequencies at high contract (0.25 cycles per degree; corresponding to 1 cm wide contrast line at about 20 cm viewing distance). Given that lower spatial frequencies (from 0.02 to 0.04 cycles per degree) excite the most of the primary visual cortex neurons in mice (Ayzenshtat et al., 2016); and a mean optimal spatial frequency in V1 is ca. 0.15 cycles per degree (Niell and Stryker, 2008), we concluded that mice preserved enough vision to manage behavioural performance in memory tasks.
In addition, we performed meta-analyses of visual acuity tests combining results of experiments performed in the current study with previously published results (Davies et al., 2007) where visual acuity tests were performed in the younger mice in the same behavioural laboratory and with the same methodology. The results are detailed in the Supplementary Fig. l. The between-subjects’ factors (gender, age and genotype) were analysed with multivariant ANOVA on combined sample of 123 animals. The genotype emerged as the most significant factor (P < 0.001, F = 12.4; η2 = 0.08, df = 1), followed by age/genotype interaction (P = 0.004, F = 4.6; η2 = 0.09). Gender or gender/age, gender/genotype interactions were not detected as significant factors in relation to vision loss when total time of stimulus tracking was considered. However, analyses of individual tracking episodes showed that males and females used different behavioural strategies to compensate for vision failure (P < 0.001, F = 11.3; η2 = 0.06): when first signs of vision deficits appeared, females opted for more short tracking episodes while males increased duration of each tracking episodes. The total time spent tracking stimulus was not different between the genders.
Learning and memory
The T-maze test based on the willingness of rodents to explore a new environment (reviewed in Dudchenko, 2004). When novel arm was open for the first time, all animals spent some time at the base of the maze (TB) exploring/hesitating before entering a maze arm. The data are provided in full in Supplementary Fig. 2A–C. For wild type (WT) animals, TB just exceeded 10 s [all: 11.9 ± 1.9 s; M (males): 13.6 ± 4.1 s; F (females): 11.0 ± 2.6 s] and animals chose the novel arm in ∼70% of trials (all: 71.6%; M: 66.7%; F: 74.4%). Some animals that initially had chosen the familiar arm later returned to investigate the novel arm (all: 28.4%; M: 100%; F: 36.4%). This returning behaviour differed between sexes, with males usually visiting both arms on each trial. The RI (Fig. 1C) represents a proportion of time spent in a novel part of the maze and did not differ between the groups (all: 0.71 ± 0.2; M: 0.69 ± 0.3; F: 0.72 ± 0.3). All mutant animals spent more time (TB) exploring/hesitating before making a choice (all: 23.1 ± 2.3 s; M: 17.2 ± 2.5 s; F: 31.7 ± 4.1 s; F = 8.62, P < 0.01; η2 = 0.14; ANOVA and Turkey HSD). The difference was significant for both sexes (males: Q = 5.2, P < 0.01, females: Q = 7.4, P < 0.01, with additional differences between sexes statistically confirmed Q = 4.9, P < 0.01). The preference for the novel arm was at the chance level (all: 46.9%; M: 51.8%; F: 40%); the females seemed to prefer familiar rather than novel environment. As in WTs, some animals that had initially chosen the familiar arm, returned later to investigate the novel arm; this behaviour was more prevalent in males (all: 28.1%; M: 81.5%; F: 20.8%). This behaviour helped mutant males to overcome deficits as judged by RI measures (all: 0.54 ± 0.2; M: 0.59 ± 0.2; F: 0.35 ± 0.3; KW and Dunn; H = 14.1; P = 0.003; η2 = 0.10); with difference confirmed between sexes (T = 2.7; P = 0.03; M: T = 1.16; P = 0.74; F: T = 2.9; P < 0.01).
The RI is shown in Fig. 1C (right); the control data are in good correspondence with the results from an ageing study by Von Bohlen Und Halbach et al. (2006).
In novel object recognition test (NOR), all animals spent some time exploring arena before approaching a particular object (TB) and this time was just above 10 s in the WT group (all: 10.7 ± 1.5s; M: 11.0 ± 1.9s; F: 10.4 ± 2.1s). The data are provided in full in Supplementary Fig. 2D–G. WT animals chose novel object in more than 75% of trials (all: 77.7%; M: 79.5%; F: 76.0%). Mutant animals spent more time exploring (TB) before making a choice (all: 30.1 ± 2.1s; M: 29.9 ± 2.8 s; F: 30.4 ± 3.1 s) with preference for the novel object almost at chance level (all: 56.6%; M: 50.9%; F: 66.4%). ANOVA and Turkey HSD (F = 12.4; P < 0.001; η2 = 0.17) attributed this difference to mutation rather than sex difference. All WT animals spent little time investigating familiar objects (TF) (all: 6.7 ± 1.2 s; M: 6.6 ± 1.3 s; F: 6.8 ± 1.8 s) and more time investigating novel objects (TN) (all: 21.5 ± 1.9 s; M: 26.0 ± 2.6 s; F: 18.0 ± 2.6 s). In contrast, mutant males spent similar amount of time investigating both objects [novel (TN): 13.5 ± 1.7 s; familiar (TF): 13.0 ± 1.8 s]; by contract, mutant females spent little time investigating familiar object (TF) (similar to WT), and more time investigating novel objects (TN) (less than WT females) [novel (TN): 12.63 ± 1.7 s; familiar (TF): 6.3 ± 1.7 s]. The differences were confirmed statistically: time for exploration of familiar object increased in males (F = 3.7; P = 0.011; η2 = 0.01; Q = 4.09, P = 0.02) and time investigating novel objects reduced in all mutants (F = 2.7; P = 0.047; η2 = 0.001) without differences between sexes.
This behavioural pattern was reflected in RI: WT (all: 0.76 ± 0.02 s; M: 0.8 ± 0.03; F: 0.72 ± 0.03) and mutants (all: 0.56 ± 0.01; M: 0.51 ± 0.03; F: 0.66 ± 0.03). Statistical analyses (KW, Dunn) confirms difference between genotypes (H = 14.01; P = 0.003; η2 = 0.87); with partial effect only confirmed for males (M: T = 3.9; P < 0.01; F: T = 0.98; P = 0.81). Taken together, our data suggest that both sexes had mutation-related deficit in this task. The observed changes in RI are shown in Fig. 1D (right); our control data are in good correspondence with ageing study by Wahl et al. (2018). We estimated contact time for different objects used in this study using measurements of time spent exploring an object during exploration phase (Supplementary Fig. 2F). As Fig.1E suggests, mutant group spent less time exploring objects, but the contact times were not statistically different. For both groups, mean contact time was within the range accepted to be sufficient to form object memory There was no significant difference in time spent investigating individual objects (Fig. 1E), made from different materials (metal, resin, silicone, wood, fabric, slate and glass). Our animal performance in behavioural memory tests indicate subtle but persistent learning and memory deficits in Opa1+/− mice that have not been previously reported.
Anatomy of CA1 region
DiOlistic labelling was used for the rapid structural visualization of large numbers of multiple neurons within blocks of neural tissue.
Figure 2A shows a schematic representation of a caudal hippocampal section and Fig. 2B a composite confocal image of a DiOlistically labelled section, indicating the quantity and quality of neuronal labelling. Figure 2C gives an example of labelled CA1 neurons and traced apical dendrite from WT and Opa1+/− mice, respectively. We have analysed on average nine neurons from each subject (range from 2 to 14, three subjects of each gender for each group). Reconstructions of the apical CA1 dendritic field allowed analysis dendritic integrity, Fig. 2D and E. Although some of CA1 neurons in Opa1 +/− animals had reduced dendritic complexity, individual variability within each subject was large and on average we did not detect any significant changes in dendritic complexity. We concluded that dendritic integrity is largely maintained in Opa1+/− mutants. We also analysed multiple morphological aspects of dendritic structure (data are not shown on the figure) which did not reveal any difference between groups. Such as: individual dendrite lengths (WT 37.23 ± 1.415 µm, Opa1+/− 38.41 ± 1.040 µm), total dendritic field length (WT 2639 ± 125.6 µm, Opa1+/− 2610 ± 110.9 µm) or the total number of branch points (WT 36.26 ± 2.030, Opa1+/− 34.88 ± 1.802). The segmentation of dendritic field based on dendrite branching levels (1° to 5°) revealed that the sum of dendritic length at each level was maintained both in close proximity to the cell soma and distal dendrites between mutant and control animals.
In Opa1+/− female mice the apical dendrites of CA1 neurons were shorter, with the total dendritic length reduced by ∼20% compared to males, from 2896 ± 153.9 µm in males to 2417 ± 133.2 µm in females (Fig. 2E, ANOVA, P < 0.01, df = 3, F = 4.03, η2 = 0.097, with non-equal variance post hoc test, P = 0.046, between female mutants and controls, P = 0.032, between male and female mutants). When analysing branching levels we noted that the sum of primary and secondary branches was also reduced in females compared to males by more than 30% (34% (p = 0.0132) for primary dendrites, and 33% (P = 0.0145) for secondary dendrites). Dendritic lengths at all other levels remained consistent with controls.
All spines in the selected regions were classified into filopodia, stubby, mushroom and thin types (Fig. 3A–D, Supplementary Figs 3 and 4). We detected relatively few filopodia and no differences between WT an Opa1+/− (0.49 ± 0.14 and 0.26 ± 0.08 spines/10 µm, respectively). The relative density was calculated for each type of spine in different parts of the dendritic tree (Fig. 3E–G). Opa1+/− mutants, both sexes, displayed a 24% reduction (P < 0.001; T = 4.27; df = 70; η2 = 0.21) in overall spine density on CA1 apical dendrites, Fig. 3E (9.6 spines/10 µm ± 0.41 in WT animals and 7.36 spines/10 µm ± 0.34 in Opa1+/−). It was noted that both proximal and distant apical dendrites were affected [P < 0.01; T = 3.02; df = 34; η2 = 0.21 for proximal, P = 0.0089 (two-tailed Mann–Whitney: Z = −1.830; η2 = 0.09) for distal]]. Proximal dendrites mostly lost stubby synapses (27.8% reduction from 2.579 ± 0.20 in WT to 1.86 ± 0.21 spines/10 µm in Opa1+/−, P = 0.017; T = 2.48; df = 34; η2 = 0.15), while distal dendrites mostly lost mushroom spines [21.4% reduction from 2.948 ± 0.27 in WT to 2.316 ± 0.23 in Opa1+/−, P = 0.049 (two-tailed Mann−Whitney: Z = −0.825; η2 = 0.02]. We have noticed some gender-related variation in spine loss and conducted multivariant ANOVA analyses to investigate all relevant factor (gender, genotype and spine types). We were able to confirm some of the differences; however, our data were too few to conduct full analyses for all type of spines. The genotype [P < 0.001, F(3,272) = 10.4, df = 3, 3% of total variance] and spine type [P < 0.001, F(3,272)=232.2, df = 3, 67% of total variance] and their interaction [P = 0.009, F(9,272) = 6.7, df = 3, 2% of total variance] were largely defining the differences observed. Post hoc comparison showed that for mushroom and stubby spines, there was not any gender-related differences in spine number (Fig. 3H and I). By contrast, we observed a female only reduction in long thin spines in Opa1+/− mice compared to WT (37.8% reductions, P = 0.0086, η2 = 0.18), from 4.63 ± 0.56 to 2.88 ± 0.31. Overall, female mutants had almost 30% of spines compare to WT females (P < 0.01; η2 = 0.31) reduction from 9.94 ± 0.60 spines/10 µm to 7.01 ± 0.44 spines/10 µm.
Synaptic proteins in hippocampal region
Quantitative RT-PCR was used to assess changes in transcript levels of Opa1, Dlg4 (discs large homolog 4; encoding for PSD95 protein) and Syn (encoding for synaptophysin protein) in the brain and retinas of WT and mutant mice at the age of 6–9 month prior to any detectable signs of neurodegeneration. Synaptophysin (Syn gene) and PSD95 (Dl4 gene) are two synaptic proteins that are abundant in various types of tissue and dynamically change in ageing and disease. Their combination has been used in several recent studies both in animal and human tissue (Chen et al., 2018; Liu et al., 2018) as a reliable synaptic marker (pre-synaptic and post-synaptic terminals are both labelled) to evaluate quantity and strength of synapses.We confirmed a two-fold reduction in Opa1 transcript expression in the brain and retina in Opa1 mutants (F-ratio 18.12; P < 0.01; effect size η2 = 0.40 in retina and η2 = 0.46 in brain) Fig. 4A but no reduction in the expression of transcripts encoding for synaptic proteins PSD95 (F-ratio 2.07; P = 0.124) and synaptophysin (F-ratio 1.15; P = 0.345). Analyses of the variability of gene expression in individual samples (Fig. 4B and C) indicate that for both synaptic proteins, the levels of gene expression were relatively stable in WT animals, but less so in the mutants. Our data indicate that WT animals showed a broader range of Opa1 gene expression with less variability compared to mutants.
Western blot
In addition to previously mentioned synaptic proteins, we assessed microtubular stabilizing Tau protein levels. Tau presents in synapses and is abundant in axons. Correlating changes in Tau with changes in synaptic proteins allows distinguishing different scenarios of synaptic restructuring. For example, i) reduction in synaptophysin with no changes in other proteins would indicate reduced efficacy of pre-synaptic terminal; ii) reduction in synaptophysin with significant reduction in Tau would signal axonal degeneration and iii) PSD95 reduction in combination with modest decline in Tau would signal reduced size or postsynaptic terminals. We tested 12 biological samples with 4–6 individual repeats for 6 different proteins [mitochondrial OPA1 protein, post-synaptic protein PSD95, pre-synaptic protein synaptophysin, stabilize microtubules protein Tau, loading proteins β-actin (microfilaments) and voltage-dependent anion channel (a multi-functional mitochondrial protein)]. An example of a gel (1 out of 6) containing 13 samples is shown in Fig. 5A and B and Supplementary Fig. 5. The β-actin was found to be invariant to Opa1 levels (Supplementary Fig. 6) and was used as a normalization protein. As expected, OPA1 levels were reduced in Opa1+/− by ∼50%, P < 0.001 (Fig. 5C). We detected a reduction in PSD95 (P = 0.0015) and a statistically borderline reduction in Tau (P = 0.058) proteins, but not Synaptophysin (P = 0.20). Since OPA1 protein levels in individual samples showed large variability, we used it to correlate the levels of synaptic proteins with the levels of OPA1 (Fig. 5D–F). Regression analysis indicated that levels of PSD95 and synaptophysin (but not Tau) proteins were correlated with available OPA1 quantity.
In conclusion, we demonstrate that the mitochondrial fusion protein, OPA1, is essential for maintaining structural integrity of the neuronal dendritic and synaptic connections in the hippocampus. Consistent with dendritic changes in the hippocampus, learning and memory were also vulnerable to OPA1 deficiency, highlighting the importance of mitochondrial integrity in these processes.
Discussion
Our behavioural data indicate previously unreported subtle learning and memory deficits in aged Opa1+/− mice. As Opa1+/− mice are visually compromised, we had a limited choice of suitable cognitive tests and were not able to determine the exact phenotype of cognitive impairment (Dere et al. 2005, 2007; Denninger, 2018). Ageing causes several well-documented behavioural changes including the decreased drive to explore. However, the validity of working memory as assessed by T-maze and object recognition tasks is well preserved with age (Ingram, 1988; Collier and Coleman, 1991; Fahlström et al., 2011). In our experiments, mutant animals showed not only hesitation to explore but also clear deficits in working memory tasks. Deteriorating vision is a hallmark of the Opa1+/− phenotype, in both male and female mice and we did not find any gender specific differences neither in magnitude nor in time course of vision loss. In contrast, working memory was affected differentially. A study by Brown and Wong (2007) suggested that mice with visual deficits were only disadvantaged when tested in highly visual tasks (like the Morris water maze), but had some additional advantages when tested in non-visual task (such as conditional order preference task). Moreover, another study by O’Leary et al. (2011), conducted in the dry maze, suggested that mice primarily use a non-visual search strategy, and learning and memory performance in a simple dry maze was not correlated with measures of visual ability. Indeed, similar memory tests (Wolf et al., 2016) are routinely used to demonstrate cognitive impairment in ageing models of various neurodegenerative diseases that also have documented visual deficits. We could not rule out that multiple brain regions were affected (Lisman et al., 2017), because the behavioural strategy clearly contributed to the observed behavioural differences (Molas et al., 2017). We focused our anatomical studies on the output hippocampal region CA1, since both T-maze and NOR tasks are hippocampus dependent (Tulving et al., 1972).
In the hippocampus, synaptic proteins (especially PSD95) were downregulated, resulting in Opa1-depended remodelling at the postsynaptic site. Anatomical analysis of CA1 hippocampal region revealed shorter dendrites with reduced branching in female Opa1+/− mice. The spine density was reduced in both sexes, with proximal dendrites losing mostly stubby spines, and distant dendrites losing mostly mushroom spines. In addition, the aged female mice had more long thin spines compare to males, and these spines were all lost as a result of the mutation. Spine shape can reflect either different inputs or strength and stability of the synapse (Bourne and Harris, 2008). Synaptic changes were consistent with the observed cognitive deficits in Opa1+/− mice.
Mitochondrial dysfunction has emerged as an underlying feature of many neurodegenerative diseases. Mitochondrial fusion protein OPA1 is depleted in dendrites in hippocampal neurons in Alzheimer’s disease (Wang et al., 2009). In the present study, we demonstrate that genetically encoded lower levels of OPA1 protein cause subtle anatomical changes in the CA1 hippocampal region (and possibly in other brain regions), at the level of spines and synapses. These changes are detrimental to learning and memory and resemble early structural changes found in Alzheimer’s animal models (Androuin et al., 2018). In patients carrying OPA1 mutations and having clinical signs of autosomal dominant optic atrophy, memory functions have not been systematically reported. However, cognitive deficits have been reported for OPA1 mutations leading to Behr syndrome and Parkinson-like symptoms (Carelli et al., 2015; Kleffner et al., 2015; Rubegni et al. 2017).
Pyramidal neurons in the hippocampal CA1 region express OPA1 protein in both their soma and dendrites (Bette et al., 2005). The observed reduction in the number of mature spines could have arisen either from reduced activity of pre-synaptic cells or/and from the failure of post-synaptic cells to maintain large spines and synapses. We cannot rule out the possibility that hippocampal deficits partially reflect sensory (visual) deprivation, as synaptic pruning in the retina of Opa1+/− mice was demonstrated by Williams et al. (2012) alongside with visual acuity deficits (Davies et al., 2007; Barnard et al., 2011). Homeostatic mechanisms routinely reshape dendrites and synapses based on ongoing activity. In vivo imaging of dendritic spines has shown a high degree of motility (Lendvai et al., 2000). Changes in spines in adults are associated with long-term potentiation/depression (Engert and Bonhoeffer, 1999). Nevertheless, remodelling of spines and synapses may have undesired functional effects, including the disruption of networks that underpin memory (Mendez et al., 2018). Von Bohlen Und Halbach et al. (2006) found that subtle changes in CA1 pyramidal cells, such as a decrease in spine numbers and length, correlate with behavioural deficits in learning tasks. Similar subtle changes are associated with neurological disorders such as schizophrenia and Down’s syndrome (reviewed in Fiala et al., 2002), stress (Galea et al., 1997), depression (reviewed in Qiao et al., 2016) and anxiety (reviewed in Leuner and Shors, 2013).
Patient studies suggest that women are more at risk of developing dementia (Corfield, 2017) and their symptoms are more severe. We have previously reported a reduction in brain ATP levels in aged Opa1+/− mice (Smith et al., 2016) that was more prominent for females. In the current study, female Opa1+/− mice were both slightly worse in behavioural tasks and displayed more changes in CA1 region. The shortening of CA1 pyramidal cell dendrites in female mice alone could account for poor T-test behavioural results (the test relies on the detection of spatial novelty). The distant apical dendrites of CA1 neurons receive direct input from the entorhinal cortex that may help to discriminate between different instances of memories for the same location (Remondes and Schuman, 2004; Zemla and Basu, 2017). In addition, a large number of long spines in aged female mice could indicate a general weakening of synaptic connections and hence weaker memory traces. A thin spine with a small head is likely to transduce a weaker signal than a stubby spine with a large head or a mushroom spine that can retain prolonged active status (Ebrahimi and Okabe, 2014). These differences could result from endocrine dysregulation. since hormones, especially oestrogen, affect dendritic arborization and spine density (Garrett and Wellman, 2009; Farrell et al., 2015) and ovariectomized females display a drastic decline in spine density in the CA1 region (Gould et al., 1990; reviewed in Frick et al., 2015; Molly et al., 2018). Oestrogen is known (Li et al., 2004) to facilitate the spine-maturation process that supports hippocampal-dependent memory on a time scale of a few hours (Jacome et al., 2016). Indeed, both males and females demonstrate improved behavioural score in memory tests when administrated female hormones (Frye et al., 2005).
In the hippocampus, most excitatory synapses terminate on dendritic spines. Spines vary in size, with volumes that are proportional to the area of the post-synaptic density (PSD). PSD95 is a multi-domain postsynaptic scaffolding protein that clusters glutamate receptors and largely determines the size and strength of synapses. Although spines and their synapses can persist for months in vivo, synaptic proteins last only a few hours. Synapse stability requires a constant turnover of synaptic proteins with individual PSDs competing for a limited quantity of protein. Mature spines are more stable and can retain PSD95 for up to 5 h compared to median just under 2 h (Gray et al., 2006). Our gene expression data suggest possible modulation of synaptic genes by Opa1. In control animals, the expression of genes encoding for synaptic proteins was stable, while expression of Opa1 varied. The opposite was true in mutants; the Opa1 was reduced but stable, while synaptic genes expression varied, adding to biological variability in the quantity of synaptic proteins. In addition, further age-related reduction in OPA1 protein caused a persistent deficit in the availability of PSD95 and thus increased competition between individual synapses. Cognitive impairment in mitochondrial diseases (reviewed by Finsterer, 2012; Picarda and McEwen, 2014; Khacho et al., 2017) is increasingly recognized and diagnosed as mitochondrial cognitive dysfunction (MCD). Variability in the onset and severity of inherited diseases (including dementias) often means that the same family mutation can generate different clinical histories and prognoses.
From the clinical point of view, testing memory function in patients harbouring OPA1 mutations could uncover subtle deficits that would facilitate genetic diagnosis and establish new disease phenotypes. Further work is required to determine the influence of, natural variation in OPA1 levels on cognitive function in healthy ageing.
Supplementary material
Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Nick White and VSBL, Cardiff University for help with confocal imaging, Anthony Hayes and Mark Issacs at Bioimaging Research Hub at Cardiff School of Biosciences for assistance and use of Imaris funded by Cardiff University's Research Infrastructure Fund. We also thank Peter Giles and Central Bioscience Services, Cardiff University for help with qRT-PCR and statistical analyses of gene expression data, and Thomas J. Freeman for help with meta-analyses of behavioural data.
Funding
This study was funded by Medical Research Council Research Grant G0700949 (M.V.); Tweadie Bequest (J.E.M.); Vetenskapsrådet (2018-02124; P.A.W.). Pete Williams is supported by Karolinska Institutet in the form of a Board of Research Faculty Funded Career Position and by St. Erik Eye Hospital philanthropic donations; Dementia Research Institute, Cardiff University, Postgraduate Studentship (R.J.B.).
Declaration
All procedures were performed in accordance with the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and European Commission Directive 2010/63/EU.
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
Glossary
- Actin (actin) =
β-actin isoform of multi-functional protein that form microfilameints
- ANOVA =
analysis of variance, a collection of statistical models and their associated estimation procedures to analyse the differences among group means in a sample
- ARVO =
The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology
- ATP =
adenosine triphosphate, the energy-carrying molecule that can release energy very quickly
- CA1 =
The region in the hippocampal circuit, from which a major output pathway goes to the entorhinal cortex
- Ct (in qPCR) =
cycle threshold
- DI =
discrimination index
- DiOlistics =
cell labelling technique
- DNA =
deoxyribonucleic acid
- EC =
European Commission
- EMPReSS =
European Mouse Phenotyping Resource for Standardized Screens
- HET =
heterozygote, Opa1 +/− genotype, has only one copy of Opa1 gene, in contrast to wild type that has two copies of Opa1 gene
- NOR =
novel objects recognition, behavioural test of memory
- OKN =
optokinetic response, a reflex to track a moving object with eyes or body
- OPA1 =
optic atrophy 1 (OPA1 human gene, Opa1 mouse gene, OPA1, associated protein)
- PSD95 =
post-synaptic density protein 95, a postsynaptic scaffolding protein in excitatory neurons
- qPCR =
quantitative polymerase chain reaction
- RI =
recognition index
- RIPA buffer =
radioimmunoprecipitation assay buffer
- RNA =
ribonucleic acid
- RT-PCR =
reverse transcription polymerase chain reaction, a laboratory technique combining reverse transcription of RNA into DNA and amplification of specific DNA targets
- RQ (in qPCR) =
relative quantification = 2-ΔΔCt
- SDS =
sodium dodecyl sulphate
- SPSS =
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, IBM SPSS® is a statistical software
- Tau =
tau protein, maintains stability of microtubules in axons, abundant in neurons
- TB, TN, TF =
time spent at the base of the maze/testing arena, and exploring novel and familiar environment/object, respectively
- T-maze =
T-shaped maze, used for behavioural test of memory
- VDAC =
voltage-dependent anion channel, or mitochondrial porin, membrane protein located on the outer mitochondrial membrane
- WT =
wild type, Opa1 +/+ genotype, has two copies of Opa1 gene
References
- Akkerman S, Blokland A, Reneerkens O, van Goethem NP, Bollen E, Gijselaers HJ, et al. Object recognition testing: methodological considerations on exploration and discrimination measures. Behav Brain Res 2012; 232: 335–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alexander C, Votruba M, Pesch UE, Thiselton DL, Mayer S, Moore A, et al. OPA1, encoding a dynamin-related GTPase, is mutated in autosomal dominant optic atrophy linked to chromosome 3q28. Nat Genet 2000; 26: 211–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Androuin A, Potier B, Nägerl UV, Cattaert D, Danglot L, Thierry M, et al. Evidence for altered dendritic spine compartmentalization in Alzheimer's disease and functional effects in a mouse model. Acta Neuropathol 2018; 135: 839–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cogn Process 2012; 13: 93–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atamna H, Tenore A, Lui F, Dhahbi JM. Organ reserve, excess metabolic capacity, and aging. Biogerontology 2018; 19: 171–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayzenshtat I, Jackson J, Yuste R. Orientation tuning depends on spatial frequency in mouse visual cortex. eNeuro 2016; 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnard AR, Charbel Issa P, Perganta G, Williams PA, Davies VJ, Sekaran S, et al. Specific deficits in visual electrophysiology in a mouse model of dominant optic atrophy. Exp Eye Res 2011; 93: 771–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basu S, Saha PK, Roszkowska M, Magnowska M, Baczynska E, Das N, et al. Quantitative 3-D morphometric analysis of individual dendritic spines. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 3545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bette S, Schlaszus H, Wissinger B, Meyermann R, Mittelbronn M. OPA1, associated with autosomal dominant optic atrophy, is widely expressed in the human brain. Acta Neuropathol 2005; 109: 393–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Binley KE Ng WS Barde YA Song B Morgan JE.. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor prevents dendritic retraction of adult mouse retinal ganglion cells. Eur J Neurosci 2016; 44: 2028–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bocca C, Kouassi Nzoughet J, Leruez S, Amati-Bonneau P, Ferré M, Kane MS, et al. Plasma metabolomic signature involving purine metabolism in human optic atrophy 1 (OPA1)-related disorders. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2018; 59: 185–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourne JN, Harris KM. Balancing Structure and Function at Hippocampal Dendritic Spines. Annu Rev Neurosci 2008; 31: 47–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown RE Wong AA.. The influence of visual ability on learning and memory performance in 13 strains of mice. Learn Mem 2007; 14: 134–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai Q, Tammineni P. Mitochondrial aspects of synaptic dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease. JAD 2017; 57: 1087–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli V, Musumeci O, Caporali L, Zanna C, La Morgia C, Del Dotto V, et al. Syndromic parkinsonism and dementia associated with OPA1 missense mutations. Ann Neurol 2015; 78: 21–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carelli V, La Morgia C, Sadun AA. Mitochondrial dysfunction in optic neuropathies: animal models and therapeutic options. Curr Opin Neurol 2013; 26: 52–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan DC. Mitochondria: dynamic organelles in disease, aging, and development. Cell 2006; 125: 1241–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chao de la Barca JM, Arrázola MS, Bocca C, Arnauné-Pelloquin L, Iuliano O, Tcherkez G, et al. The metabolomic signature of Opa1 deficiency in rat primary cortical neurons shows aspartate/glutamate depletion and phospholipids remodeling. Sci Rep 2019; 9: 6107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chauhan A, Vera J, Wolkenhauer O. The systems biology of mitochondrial fission and fusion and implications for disease and aging. Biogerontology 2014; 15: 1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen J, Niu Q, Xia T, Zhou G, Li P, Zhao Q, et al. ERK1/2-mediated disruption of BDNF-TrkB signaling causes synaptic impairment contributing to fluoride-induced developmental neurotoxicity. Toxicology 2018; 410: 222–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collier TJ Coleman PD.. Divergence of biological and chronological aging: Evidence from rodent studies. Neurobiol Aging 1991; 12: 685–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colligris P, Perez de Lara MJ, Colligris B, Pintor J. Ocular manifestations of Alzheimer's and other neurodegenerative diseases: the prospect of the eye as a tool for the early diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. J Ophthalmol 2018; 2018: 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corfield S. Women and dementia: a global challenge. Global Alzheimer’s & Dementia Action Alliance/Alzheimer’s Society report, 2017.
- Davies VJ, Hollins AJ, Piechota MJ, Yip W, Davies JR, White KE, et al. Opa1 deficiency in a mouse model of autosomal dominant optic atrophy impairs mitochondrial morphology, optic nerve structure and visual function. Hum Mol Genet 2007; 16: 1307–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Dotto V, Fogazza M, Carelli V, Rugolo M, Zanna C. Eight human OPA1 isoforms, long and short: what are they for? Biochim Biophys Acta Bioenerg 2018; 1859: 263–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delettre C, Lenaers G, Griffoin JM, Gigarel N, Lorenzo C, Belenguer P, et al. Nuclear gene OPA1, encoding a mitochondrial dynamin-related protein, is mutated in dominant optic atrophy. Nat Genet 2000; 26: 207–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denninger JK, Smith BM, Kirby ED. Novel object recognition and object location behavioral testing in mice on a budget. J Vis Exp 2018; 141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. Integrated memory for objects, places, and temporal order: evidence for episodic-like memory in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2005; 84: 214–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dere E, Huston JP, De Souza Silva MA. The pharmacology, neuroanatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2007; 31: 673–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudchenko PA. An overview of the tasks used to test working memory in rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004; 28: 699–709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebrahimi S, Okabe S. Structural dynamics of dendritic spines: molecular composition, geometry and functional regulation. Biochim Biophys Acta 2014; 1838: 2391–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engert F, Bonhoeffer T. Dendritic spine changes associated with hippocampal long-term synaptic plasticity. Nature 1999; 399: 66–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ennaceur A, Delacour J. A new one-trial test for neurobiological studies of memory in rats. 1. Behavioral data. Behav Brain Res 1988; 31: 47–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erpapazoglou Z, Mouton-Liger F, Corti O. From dysfunctional endoplasmic reticulum-mitochondria coupling to neurodegeneration. Neurochem Int 2017; 109: 171–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahlström A Yu Q Ulfhake B.. Behavioral changes in aging female C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol Aging 2011; 32: 1868–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrell MR, Gruene TM, Shansky RM. The influence of stress and gonadal hormones on neuronal structure and function. Horm. Behav 2015; 76: 118–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fiala JC, Spacek J, Harris KM. Dendritic spine pathology: Cause or consequence of neurological disorders? Brain Res Rev 2002; 39: 29–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fieni F, Lee SB, Jan YN, Kirichok Y. Activity of the mitochondrial calcium uniporter varies greatly between tissues. Nat Commun 2012; 3: 1317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finsterer J. Cognitive dysfunction in mitochondrial disorders. Acta Neurol Scand 2012; 126: 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fogarty MJ, Hammond LA, Kanjhan R, Bellingham MC, Noakes PG. A method for the three-dimensional reconstruction of Neurobiotin™-filled neurons and the location of their synaptic inputs. Front Neural Circuits 2013; 7: 153. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foster Olive MD, Franco AP, Gipson CD. Diolistic labeling and analysis of dendritic spines. Methods Mol Biol 2018; 1727: 179–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frick KM, Kim J, Tuscher JJ, Fortress AM. Sex steroid hormones matter for. Learn Mem 2015; 22: 472–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frye CA, Rhodes ME, Dudek B. Estradiol to aged female or male mice improves learning in inhibitory avoidance and water maze tasks. Brain Res 2005; 1036: 101–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea LA, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Deak T, Spencer RL, Dhabhar FS. Sex differences in dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons in response to chronic restraint stress. Neuroscience 1997; 81: 689–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett JE, Wellman CL. Chronic stress effects on dendritic morphology in medial prefrontal cortex: sex differences and estrogen dependence. Neuroscience 2009; 162: 195–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griparic L, van der Wel NN, Orozco IJ, Peters PJ, van der Bliek AM. Loss of the intermembrane space protein Mgm1/OPA1 induces swelling and localized constrictions along the lengths of mitochondria. J Biol Chem 2004; 279: 18792–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gould E, Woolley S, Frankfurt M, McEwen BS. Gonadal steroids regulate dendritic spine density in hippocampal pyramidal cells in adulthood. J Neurosci 1990; 10: 1286–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray NW, Weimer RM, Bureau I, Svoboda K. Redistribution of Synaptic PSD-95 in the. PLoS Biol 2006; 4: e370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacome LF, Barateli K, Buitrago D, Lema F, Frankfurt M, Luine VN. Gonadal hormones rapidly enhance spatial memory and increase hippocampal spine density in male rats. Endocrinology 2016; 157: 1357–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Javaid FZ, Brenton J, Guo L, Cordeiro MF. Visual and ocular manifestations of Alzheimer's disease and their use as biomarkers for diagnosis and progression. Front Neurol 2016; 7: 55. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeong Y, Huh N, Lee J, Yun I, Lee JW, Lee I, et al. Role of the hippocampal CA1 region in incremental value learning. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 9870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haass-Koffler CL, Naeemuddin M, Bartlett SE. An analytical tool that quantifies cellular morphology changes from three-dimensional fluorescence images. J Vis Exp 2012; e4233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ham M, Han J, Osann K, Smith M, Kimonis V. Meta-analysis of genotype-phenotype analysis of OPA1 mutations in autosomal dominant optic atrophy. Mitochondrion 2019; 46: 262–8. S1567-7249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedskog L, Pinho CM, Filadi R, Rönnbäck A, Hertwig L, Wiehager B, et al. Modulation of the endoplasmic reticulum–mitochondria interface in Alzheimer’s disease and related models. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013; 110: 7916–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyer MM, Phillips LL, Neigh GN. Sex differences in synaptic plasticity. Hormones and beyond. Front Mol Neurosci 2018; 11: 266. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2018.00266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingram DK. Complex maze learning in rodents as a model of age-related memory impairment. Neurobiol Aging 1988; 9: 475–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khacho M, Clark A, Svoboda DS, MacLaurin JG, Lagace DC, Park DC, et al. Mitochondrial dysfunction underlies cognitive defects as a result of neural stem cell depletion and impaired neurogenesis. Hum Mol Genet 2017; 26: 3327–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleffner I, Wessling C, Gess B, Korsukewitz C, Allkemper T, Schirmacher A, et al. Behr syndrome with homozygous C19ORF12 mutation. J Neurol Sci 2015; 357: 115–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krols M, van Isterdael G, Asselbergh B, Kremer A, Lippens S, Timmerman V, et al. Mitochondria-associated membranes as hubs for neurodegeneration. Acta Neuropathol 2016; 131: 505–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kushnareva Y, Seong Y, Andreyev AY, Kuwana T, Kiosses WB, Votruba M, et al. Mitochondrial dysfunction in an Opa1(Q285STOP) mouse model of dominant optic atrophy results from Opa1 haploinsufficiency. Cell Death Dis 2016; 7: e2309–e2309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leinonen H, Tanila H. Vision in laboratory rodents-Tools to measure it and implications for behavioral research. Behav Brain Res 2018; 352: 172–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lendvai B, Stern EA, Chen B, Svoboda K, Lendvai B, et al. Experience-dependent plasticity of dendritic spines in the developing rat barrel cortex in vivo. Nature 2000; 404: 876–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leuner B, Shors TJ. Stress, anxiety, and dendritic spines: what are the connections? Neuroscience 2013; 251: 108–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Z, Okamoto K, Hayashi Y, Sheng M. The importance of dendritic mitochondria in the morphogenesis and plasticity of spines and synapses. Cell 2004; 119: 873–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C, Brake WG, Romeo RD, Dunlop JC, Gordon M, Buzescu R, et al. Estrogen alters hippocampal dendritic spine shape and enhances synaptic protein immunoreactivity and spatial memory in female mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2004; 101: 2185–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao C, Ashley N, Diot A, Morten K, Phadwal K, Williams A, et al. Dysregulated mitophagy and mitochondrial organization in optic atrophy due to OPA1 mutations. Neurology 2017; 88: 131–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lisman J, Buzsáki G, Eichenbaum H, Nadel L, Ranganath C, Redish AD. Viewpoints: how the hippocampus contributes to memory, navigation, and cognition. Nat Neurosci 2017; 20: 1434–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Zhang Y, Zheng X, Fang T, Yang X, Luo X, et al. Galantamine improves cognition, hippocampal inflammation, and synaptic plasticity impairments induced by lipopolysaccharide in mice. J Neuroinflammation 2018; 15: 112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendez P, Stefanelli T, Flores CE, Muller D, Lüscher C. Homeostatic plasticity in the hippocampus facilitates memory extinction. Cell Rep 2018; 22: 1451–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molas S, Zhao-Shea R, Liu L, DeGroot SR, Gardner PD, Tapper AR. A circuit-based mechanism underlying familiarity signaling and the preference for novelty. Nat Neurosci 2017; 20: 1260–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niell CM, Stryker MP. Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci 2008; 28: 7520–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary TP Savoie V Brown RE.. Learning, memory and search strategies of inbred mouse strains with different visual abilities in the Barnes maze. Behav Brain Res 2011; 216: 531–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira AMM, Hawk JD, Abel T, Havekes R. Post-training reversible inactivation of the hippocampus enhances novel object recognition memory. Learn Mem 2010; 17: 155–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiao H, Li M, Xu C, Chen H, An S, Ma X. Dendritic spines in depression: what we learned from animal models. Neural Plast 2016; 2016: 1–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Picard M, McEwen BS. Mitochondria impact brain function and cognition. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014; 111: 7–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickles S, Vigié P, Youle RJ. Mitophagy and quality control mechanisms in mitochondrial maintenance. Curr Biol 2018; 28: R170–R185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rattan SI. Biology of ageing: principles, challenges, and perspectives. Rom J Morphol Embryol 2015; 56: 1251–3. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Remondes M, Schuman EM. Role for a cortical input to hippocampal area CA1 in the consolidation of a long-term memory. Nature 2004; 431: 699–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rich PR. The molecular machinery of Keilin's respiratory chain. Biochem Soc Trans 2003; 31: 1095–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubegni A, Pisano T, Bacci G, Tessa A, Battini R, Procopio E, et al. Leigh-like neuroimaging features associated with new biallelic mutations in OPA1. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2017; 21: 671–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarzi E, Angebault C, Seveno M, Gueguen N, Chaix B, Bielicki G, et al. The human OPA1delTTAG mutation induces premature age-related systemic neurodegeneration in mouse. Brain 2012; 135: 3599–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schon EA, Area-Gomez E. Mitochondria-associated ER membranes in Alzheimer disease. Mol Cell Neurosci 2013; 55: 26–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sholl DA. Dendritic organization in the neurons of the visual and motor cortices of the cat. J Anat 1953; 87: 387–406. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith TG, Seto S, Ganne P, Votruba M. A randomized, placebo-controlled trial of the benzoquinone idebenone in a mouse model of OPA1-related dominant optic atrophy reveals a limited therapeutic effect on retinal ganglion cell dendropathy and visual function. Neuroscience 2016; 319: 92–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skulachev VP. Mitochondrial filaments and clusters as intracellular power-transmitting cables. Trends Biochem Sci 2001; 26: 23–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staffend NA, Meise RL. DiOlistic labeling in fixed brain slices: phenotype, morphology, and dendritic spines. Curr Protoc Neurosci 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tezze C, Romanello V, Desbats MA, Fadini GP, Albiero M, Favaro G, et al. Age-associated loss of OPA1 in muscle impacts muscle mass, metabolic homeostasis, systemic inflammation, and epithelial senescence. Cell Metab 2017; 25: 1374–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thaung C, Arnold K, Jackson IJ, Coffey P. Presence of visual head tracking differentiates normal sighted from retinal degenerate mice. J Neurosci Lett 2002; 325: 21–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E, Donaldson W, Bower GH. Organization of memory. New York: Academic Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Von Bohlen Und Halbach O Zacher C Gass P Unsicker K.. Age-related alterations in hippocampal spines and deficiencies in spatial memory in mice. J Neurosci Res 2006; 83: 525–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Von Bohlen, Halbach O, Zacher C, Gass P, Unsicker K. Age-related alterations in hippocampal spines and deficiencies in spatial memory in mice. J Neurosci Res 2006; 83: 525–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wahl D, Solon-Biet SM, Wang QP, Wali JA, Pulpitel T, Clark X, et al. Comparing the effects of low-protein and high-carbohydrate diets and caloric restriction on brain aging in mice. Cell Rep 2018; 25: 2234–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang X, Su B, Lee HG, Li X, Perry G, Smith MA, et al. Impaired balance of mitochondrial fission and fusion in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 9090–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams PA Tribble JR Pepper KW Cross SD Morgan BP Morgan JE, et al. . Inhibition of the classical pathway of the complement cascade prevents early dendritic and synaptic degeneration in glaucoma. Mol Neurodegener 2016; 11: 26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams PA Howell GR Barbay JM Braine CE Sousa GL John SWM, et al. . Retinal ganglion cell dendritic atrophy in DBA/2J glaucoma. PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e72282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams PA, Morgan JE, Votruba M. Opa1 deficiency in a mouse model of dominant optic atrophy leads to retinal ganglion cell dendropathy. Brain 2010; 133: 2942–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams PA, Morgan JE, Votruba M. Mouse models of dominant optic atrophy: what do they tell us about the pathophysiology of visual loss? Vis Res 2011; 51: 229–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams PA, Piechota M, von Ruhland C, Taylor E, Morgan JE, Votruba M. Opa1 is essential for retinal ganglion cell synaptic architecture and connectivity. Brain 2012; 135: 493–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf A Bauer B Abner EL Ashkenazy-Frolinger T Hartz AMS.. A comprehensive behavioral test battery to assess learning and memory in 129S6/Tg2576 Mice. PLoS ONE 2016; 11: e0147733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu Y, Herman P, Rothman DL, Agarwal D, Hyder FJ. Evaluating the gray and white matter energy budgets of human brain function. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 2018; 38: 1339–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu-Wai-Man P, Davies VJ, Piechota MJ, Cree LM, Votruba M, Chinnery PF. Secondary mtDNA defects do not cause optic nerve dysfunction in a mouse model of dominant optic atrophy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2009; 50: 4561–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu-Wai-Man P, Votruba M, Burté F, La Morgia C, Barboni P, Carelli V. A neurodegenerative perspective on mitochondrial optic neuropathies. Acta Neuropathol 2016; 132: 789–806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zemla R, Basu J. Hippocampal function in rodents. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2017; 43: 187–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Original and derived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the authors on request.