Abstract
Myopia, a leading cause of distance vision impairment, is projected to affect half of the world’s population in 30 years. We analysed the relationship between certain demographic, environmental, and behavioural factors and myopia from a 2-year school-based, prospective trial conducted in Shanghai, China. This trial enrolled 6295 school-aged children at baseline and followed them up for 24 months. The relationship between abovementioned factors and myopia was examined and the role of sleep in childhood myopia development was highlighted. Our results suggest that ‘sleeping late’ is a risk factor for myopia prevalence at baseline (odds ratio [OR] = 1.55, p = 0.04), 2-year myopia incidence (odds ratio [OR] = 1.44, p = 0.02) and progression over 24 months (p = 0.005), after adjusting for residency area, age, gender, sleep duration, and time spent outdoors. The identification and consistency of results with late sleepers being a susceptible group to both myopia onset and progression suggests a complex relationship between circadian rhythm, indoor environment, habitual indoor activities and myopia development and progression. These results can offer new insights to future myopia aetiology studies as well as aid in decision-making of myopia prevention strategies.
Subject terms: Patient education, Risk factors, Refractive errors, Paediatric research, Epidemiology
Introduction
Myopia, commonly known as ‘short-sightedness’ or ‘near-sightedness’, is predicted to affect approximately five billion people worldwide by 20501. It is the most frequent cause of distance visual impairment in the world and results in enormous socio-economic burden2,3. Although there are many ways to correct the blurred distance vision caused by myopia, simple corrective strategies cannot halt the pathological changes that parallel continuous myopia progression. Such changes or complications can lead to irreversible visual impairment and, in severe cases, acquired blindness4. For example, myopic macular degeneration, one of the retinal abnormalities associated with myopia, has been identified as one of the leading causes of blindness and vision impairment in some parts of Asia5–7. Early onset of myopia in childhood will lead to extended courses of disease, which would not only result in a greater financial burden, but also produce higher risks for sight threatening complications2. Therefore, increase in the prevalence of myopia among children is particularly worrying.
Years of study has enabled the identification of a number of risk factors for myopia, such as family history of myopia, urban living environment, and a lack of outdoor exposure8–10. In recent years, a number of studies have investigated the relationship between myopia and sleep. Shorter sleep duration and poorer sleep quality was associated with greater myopic refractive error11,12. Altered light/dark or wake/sleep cycles were found to influence ocular growth patterns in animal studies13,14. Additionally, genetic factors regulating circadian rhythms were found to be involved in refractive error development in humans and retinal-specific knockouts of the clock gene were found to induce myopia in mice15,16. Furthermore, since the discovery of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) in the retina, their association with melatonin and therefore sleep/wake cycle regulation, the role of sleep has become more intriguing for myopia research17,18. However, most myopia studies to date that investigated the role of sleep focused solely on the duration of it and there is a lack of longitudinal studies exploring the ‘timing’ or circadian rhythm aspect of sleep. Henceforth, we present our findings on both the duration and the pattern of sleep and their associations with childhood myopia from a 2-year prospective, school-based study.
Method
Study design
This trial (the Shanghai Time Outside to Reduce Myopia trial) was approved by the Shanghai General Hospital Ethics Committee and adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02980445). Detailed information on the study design has been published previously19. A total of 6295 children (2949 girls, 46.8%), aged between 6 to 9 years (7.2 ± 0.7 years) at baseline, from 24 schools across eight districts in Shanghai, were enrolled. This was a school-based prospective trial, where schools were randomised to either the control group or the test group with different amount of outdoor activities as an intervention. Written informed consent was obtained from parents/carers of children participating in the study. Parents/carers consented to providing information on their child’s activity via a mobile phone based questionnaire. Children with systemic or ocular pathology or used any myopia control treatment were identified via a screening procedure at baseline and excluded for data analysis. Cycloplegic auto-refraction was performed at baseline and repeated annually. The cycloplegic agent employed in this procedure was 1% cyclopentolate (Cyclogyl; Alcon, Fort Worth, Texas, USA).
The questionnaire employed in this trial was administrated online via a mobile phone app to parents or carers at baseline and repeated four times per year (two times during school sessions and two times during holiday seasons). There were four sections in the questionnaire, containing questions on demographics (e.g. gender, date of birth, and name of school), family history of myopia, daily amount of various activities on a typical school day (e.g. how much time the child spent on outdoor activities), eye-using habits (e.g. reading distance) and myopia management experiences (if applicable). The questionnaire is included as "Appendix 1". This article focuses on environmental and behavioural factors, especially on those related to sleep patterns, to determine whether any relationship exists between sleep/wake-up time and myopia.
Definitions and classifications
Based on cycloplegic auto-refraction (KR-8900, Topcon, Tokyo, Japan) results, spherical equivalent (SE) was computed as sphere plus half cylinder. Myopia was defined SE ≤ − 0.5 dioptres (D); hyperopia was defined as SE ≥ + 0.75D; emmetropia was defined as SE > − 0.50D and < + 0.75D. Only right eyes were used for classifying refractive error. Incidence was defined as new myopic cases over 24 months among those who were not myopic at baseline. Progression was computed as a difference between 24-month and baseline SE.
For sleep pattern related questions, parents/carers were asked to recall and write down the specific time (in ‘hh:mm’ format) when the child went to sleep in the night and woke up in the morning on a usual school day.
Statistical analysis
Outcome variables for analyses were myopia at baseline, myopia incidence and progression by the 24-month visit. Questionnaire data from baseline, 12 and 24 months were used for the analysis. The baseline model used questionnaire data from the baseline visit. The incidence model used questionnaire data from 12 and 24 months visit. Progression model at 24 month used questionnaire data from 24 month visit. Associations of various demographics, environmental and behavioural factors with outcome variables were investigated. Chi-square and t-test were used for univariate analysis. Factors that were significant at the univariate level were included for multivariate testing. Age, gender, and area (urban/suburban) were included in all multivariate models as possible confounders. In addition, as participants of this trial were randomly assigned to three groups (a control group and two test groups with varying amount of outdoor time per day), the potential impact of study groups were also accounted for in multivariate models for incidence and progression. Myopia prevalence at baseline and incidence over 24 months were analysed using multiple logistic regression, while 24 month progression from baseline was analysed using general linear model. Backward elimination followed by forward entry was used as a method of model development. The final model included only factors that were significant at the 5% level of significance. Cluster based robust estimate of variance was employed for the incidence model that used 12 and 24 month visits. Interactions of main effects were assessed using Likelihood Ratio test and if significant, was further explored using subgroups of the interacting factor. All analyses were performed using Stata/IC 15.1 (StataCorp LLC, Texas 77845 USA).
Results
Study sample characteristics
At baseline, data were available for 6042 participants (mean age 7.36 ± 0.60 years; female 2835, 46.93%), amongst which there were 409 cases of myopia and the prevalence was 6.77% (409/6024). Detailed baseline sample descriptive data can be found in a previous publication19. After 2 years, at the 24-month visit, data were available for 5355 participants and myopes accounted for 27.64% (1480/5355) of the study sample. A total of 4982 children who were not myopic at baseline attended the 24-month visit, amongst them 1094 became myopic during this 24-months time period. The 2-year incidence of myopia was 21.92% (1094/4982).
Distribution of sleep-related variables
Prior to risk factor analyses, distributions of bedtime (time to sleep) and wake-up time of the cohort were examined. As shown in Figs. 1 and 2, distributions of both variables were not continuous but were clustered into distinct groups.
When bedtime was categorised into groups based on the observed distribution (Fig. 1), bedtime was observed to be linearly associated with baseline myopia prevalence as well as incidence at 24-months (Fig. 3). Therefore, subsequent analyses considered bedtime and wake-up time as categorical data.
We studied the sleep pattern of these children and found the average bedtime delayed from 21:04 at baseline to 21:17 at 24 months (One-way ANOVA, p < 0.001). Additionally, only 9.9% of children slept late (at 10 p.m. or after) at baseline, which, surprisingly, doubled to 20% by 24 months (Fig. 4). In contrast, the proportion of children who slept early (before 9 p.m.) decreased from 20.6 to 6.6% over this time period (Chi-square, p < 0.001).
Meanwhile, the average wake-up time of children advanced over time from 6:34 at baseline to 6:27 at 24 months (One-way ANOVA, p < 0.001). The percentage of children who woke up early (before 6:30) also increased by 1.47 times (Fig. 5, Chi-square, p < 0.001). As a result, their average sleep duration decreased from 9.49 to 9.17 h over this time period (One-way ANOVA, p < 0.001).
Sleep and baseline myopia
We compared myopes and non-myopes at baseline and investigated which demographic and/or behavioural factors were associated with myopia presented at baseline (Table 1). The univariate analysis shows that, apart from previously known risk factors, such as age, urban residency, family history of myopia and less outdoor activities, late bedtime (p = 0.009) and late wake-up time (p = 0.001) are also associated with baseline myopia.
Table 1.
Variables | Category | Non-myope | Myope | Unadjusted OR (95% CI) | p-value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % (row) | N | %(row) | ||||
Residency area | Sub-urban | 4537 | 93.93% | 293 | 6.07% | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 1096 | 90.43% | 116 | 9.57% | 1.64 (1.31, 2.05) | p < 0.001 | |
Age (years) | 6 | 1874 | 97.10% | 56 | 2.90% | 1 (Reference) | |
7 | 2747 | 92.09% | 236 | 7.91% | 2.87 (2.14, 3.87) | p < 0.001 | |
8 | 1012 | 89.64% | 117 | 10.36% | 3.87 (2.79, 5.37) | p < 0.001 | |
Gender | Male | 2964 | 92.45% | 242 | 7.55% | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 2668 | 94.11% | 167 | 5.89% | 0.77 (0.63, 0.94) | p = 0.01 | |
0 | 2443 | 96.29% | 94 | 3.71% | 1 (Reference) | ||
Myopic parent | 1 | 1883 | 91.90% | 166 | 8.10% | 2.29 (1.77, 2.97) | p < 0.001 |
2 | 1025 | 88.59% | 132 | 11.41% | 3.35 (2.54, 4.40) | p < 0.001 | |
Parent education level | Lower than undergrad | 3481 | 94.00% | 222 | 6.00% | 1 (Reference) | |
Undergrad | 1855 | 92.15% | 158 | 7.85% | 1.34 (1.08, 1.65) | p = 0.007 | |
Postgrad | 225 | 90.36% | 24 | 9.64% | 1.67 (1.07, 2.60) | p = 0.02 | |
Usual wake-up time | Before 6:30 a.m. | 1749 | 94.54% | 101 | 5.46% | 1 (Reference) | p = 0.24 |
6:30 to 6:59 a.m. | 2437 | 93.69% | 164 | 6.31% | 1.17 (0.90, 1.50) | ||
7 a.m. and after | 1445 | 90.94% | 144 | 9.06% | 1.73 (1.33, 2.25) | p < 0.001 | |
Usual bedtime | Before 9 p.m. | 1178 | 94.85% | 64 | 5.15% | 1 (Reference) | |
9 to 9:29 p.m. | 2894 | 93.26% | 209 | 6.74% | 1.33 (1.00, 1.77) | p = 0.05 | |
9:30 p.m. and after | 1552 | 91.94% | 136 | 8.06% | 1.61 (1.19, 2.19) | p = 0.002 | |
Sleep duration (h) | < 9.5 | 2315 | 92.90% | 177 | 7.10% | 1 (Reference) | |
≥ 9.5 and < 10 | 1846 | 93.37% | 131 | 6.63% | 0.93 (0.71, 1.17) | p = 0.53 | |
≥ 10 | 1463 | 93.54% | 101 | 6.46% | 0.90 (0.70, 1.16) | p = 0.43 | |
Reading time weekly total (h) | 16.25 ± 6.91 | 16.51 ± 7.43 | 1.01 (0.99, 1.02) | p = 0.45 | |||
Screen time weekly total (h) | 9.49 ± 0.54 | 9.47 ± 0.51 | 0.98 (0.95, 1.01) | p = 0.16 | |||
Outdoor time weekly total (h) | 7.26 ± 3.86 | 6.63 ± 3.83 | 0.96 (0.93, 0.98) | p = 0.002 |
When univariate significant factors were added into the multivariate model, the results (Table 2) show that sleeping late (late bedtime) is associated with higher odds of being myopic. The modelling steps indicated that sleeping late was a better predictor than waking up late. Although insignificant, sleep duration was retained in the model to ensure that the significance of bedtime was not affected by this factor. Even after taking sleep duration and weekly outdoor time into consideration, those who slept at 9:30 p.m. or later had 1.55-fold higher odds of being myopic at baseline compared to those who slept before 9 p.m. (p = 0.04). There was no significant interaction of bedtime with other main effects in the model (p > 0.20).
Table 2.
Variable | Adjusted OR (95% CI) |
Test statistic, p |
---|---|---|
Area: sub-urban | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 1.44 (1.12, 1.84) | z = 2.85, p = 0.004 |
Age (years): 6 years | 1 (Reference) | |
7 years | 3.02 (2.21, 4.14) | z = 6.91, p < 0.001 |
8 years | 4.08 (2.89, 5.75) | z = 8.00, p < 0.001 |
Gender: Male | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 0.77 (0.62, 0.95) | z = − 2.42, p = 0.02 |
Usual bedtime: Before 9 p.m. | 1 (Reference) | |
9 to 9:29 p.m. | 1.38 (0.99, 1.94) | z = 1.88, p = 0.06 |
9:30 p.m. and after | 1.55 (1.02, 2.34) | z = 2.06, p = 0.04 |
Sleep duration (h): < 9.5 h | 1 (Reference) | |
9.5 to 10 h | 1.07 (0.83, 1.39) | z = 0.53, p = 0.60 |
10 h and more | 1.23 (0.88, 1.72) | z = 1.22, p = 0.22 |
Outdoor weekly total (per h) | 0.96 (0.93, 0.98) | z = − 3.13, p = 0.002 |
Sleep and myopia incidence
Using a similar approach, we then examined the relationship between sleep and 2-year myopia incidence. Table 3 presents the association between each variable and new myopic cases that developed during this follow-up period. To sum up, sleeping late and waking up late, along with known risk factors, namely urban living environment, older age, family history of myopia and less outdoor time were found to be significantly associated with newly developed myopia over 24 months.
Table 3.
Variables | Category | Non-myope | New myope | Unadjusted OR (95% CI) | p-value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % (row) | N | % (row) | ||||
Residency area | Sub-urban | 6619 | 88.02% | 901 | 11.98% | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 1519 | 84.62% | 276 | 15.38% | 1.34 (1.14, 1.56) | p < 0.001 | |
Age (years) | 6 | 2775 | 89.46% | 327 | 10.54% | 1 (Reference) | |
7 | 3931 | 86.51% | 613 | 13.49% | 1.32 (1.14, 1.54) | p < 0.001 | |
8 | 1432 | 85.80% | 237 | 14.20% | 1.40 (1.16, 1.70) | p < 0.001 | |
Gender | Male | 4345 | 88.40% | 570 | 11.60% | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 3791 | 86.20% | 607 | 13.80% | 1.22 (1.07, 1.39) | p = 0.003 | |
Parental myopia | 0 | 3815 | 91.03% | 376 | 8.97% | 1 (Reference) | |
1 | 2688 | 86.63% | 415 | 13.37% | 1.57 (1.34, 1.83) | p < 0.001 | |
2 | 1213 | 78.16% | 339 | 21.84% | 2.84 (2.38, 3.37) | p < 0.001 | |
Parent education | Lower than undergrad | 5231 | 88.92% | 652 | 11.08% | 1 (Reference) | |
Undergrad | 2500 | 84.80% | 448 | 15.20% | 1.44 (1.25, 1.65) | p < 0.001 | |
Postgrad | 289 | 82.81% | 60 | 17.19% | 1.67 (1.21, 2.30) | p = 0.002 | |
Usual wake-up time | Before 6:30 a.m. | 3570 | 88.41% | 468 | 11.59% | 1 (Reference) | |
6:30 to 6:59 a.m. | 2909 | 86.73% | 445 | 13.27% | 1.17 (1.01, 1.35) | p = 0.03 | |
7 a.m. and after | 1656 | 86.25% | 264 | 13.75% | 1.22 (1.03, 1.44) | p = 0.02 | |
Usual bedtime | Before 9 p.m. | 737 | 89.99% | 82 | 10.01% | 1 (Reference) | |
9 to 9:29 p.m. | 4712 | 88.19% | 631 | 11.81% | 1.20 (0.94, 1.54) | p = 0.14 | |
9:30 p.m. and after | 2684 | 85.26% | 464 | 14.74% | 1.55 (1.21, 2.00) | p = 0.001 | |
Sleep duration (h) | < 9.5 | 5023 | 87.02% | 749 | 12.98% | 1 (Reference) | |
≥ 9.5 and < 10 | 2028 | 87.64% | 286 | 12.36% | 0.95 (0.82, 1.10) | p = 0.46 | |
≥ 10 | 1080 | 88.38% | 142 | 11.62% | 0.88 (0.73, 1.07) | p = 0.20 | |
Reading time weekly total | 16.58 ± 7.33 | 17.10 ± 7.47 | 1.01 (1.00, 1.02) | p = 0.03 | |||
Screen time weekly total | 6.77 ± 3.74 | 6.35 ± 3.74 | 0.97 (0.95, 0.99) | p = 0.001 | |||
Outdoor time weekly total | 8.45 ± 4.12 | 7.89 ± 4.01 | 0.97 (0.95, 0.98) | p < 0.001 |
After adjusting for residency area, age, and gender, children who slept at 9:30 p.m. or later had 1.4-fold higher odds of developing myopia (p = 0.02), compared to those slept early (before 9 p.m.) (Table 4). Similar to the baseline myopia model, time to sleep (i.e. bedtime) took precedence over time to wake up (i.e. wake-up time) in the multivariate model. Outdoor time is identified as a protective factor against myopia onset (OR = 0.97, p < 0.001) while longer sleep duration is not significant in the myopia onset model (p = 0.93). The interaction of bedtime with other main effects of the model was not significant (p > 0.26) except for residency area (p = 0.003), where the association of late bedtime with newly developed myopia over 24 months was significant in the sub-urban area, but was not significant in the urban area (p = 0.63), which comprised 20% of the study sample.
Table 4.
Variable | Adjusted OR (95% CI) | Test statistic, p |
---|---|---|
Area: Sub-urban | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 1.25 (1.06, 1.48) | z = 2.64, p = 0.008 |
Age (years): 6 years | 1 (Reference) | |
7 years | 1.33 (1.14, 1.55) | z = 3.59, p < 0.001 |
8 years | 1.40 (1.15, 1.70) | z = 3.4, p = 0.001 |
Gender: Male | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 1.23 (1.08, 1.41) | z = 3.07, p = 0.002 |
Usual bedtime: before 9 p.m. | 1 (Reference) | |
9 p.m. to 9:29 p.m. | 1.21 (0.91, 1.61) | z = 1.29, p = 0.20 |
9:30 p.m. and after | 1.45 (1.05, 2.00) | z = 2.27, p = 0.02 |
Sleep duration (h): < 9.5 h | 1 (Reference) | |
9.5 to 10 h | 1.02 (0.88, 1.20) | z = 0.3, p = 0.76 |
10 h and more | 1.04 (0.82, 1.32) | z = 0.32, p = 0.75 |
Outdoor weekly total (per h) | 0.97 (0.95, 0.99) | z = − 3.66, p < 0.001 |
*Odds ratios were adjusted for outdoor intervention.
Sleep and refractive error progression
At the 24-month visit, the mean myopic progression of 5305 participants over 2 years was − 0.92 ± 0.77 D for an average baseline age of 7.4 ± 0.6 years (range 6 to 9 years). Over 2 years 3.93%, 2.61%, and 1.50% of the children progressed by 1D, 1.5D, and 2D respectively. Univariate regression reveals significant associations between urban area, female gender, parental myopia, higher level parental education, late wake-up time, late bedtime, more reading, less screen time, and less outdoor time and more myopic progression (Table 5).
Table 5.
Variables | Category | Sample | 24-months Progression (± SD) | Beta Coef. (SE) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Residency area | Sub-urban | 4218 | − 0.89 ± 0.76 | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 1087 | − 1.03 ± 0.79 | − 0.14 (0.03) | p < 0.001 | |
Age (year) | 6 | 1663 | − 0.91 ± 0.78 | 1 (Reference) | |
7 | 2640 | − 0.91 ± 0.77 | 0.001 (0.02) | p = 0.98 | |
8 | 1002 | − 0.93 ± 0.76 | − 0.02 (0.03) | p = 0.50 | |
Gender | Male | 2818 | − 0.84 ± 0.75 | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 2486 | − 1.00 ± 0.79 | − 0.15 (0.02) | p < 0.001 | |
Parental myopia | 0 | 2268 | − 0.79 ± 0.70 | 1 (Reference) | |
1 | 1798 | − 0.94 ± 0.79 | − 0.15 (0.02) | p < 0.001 | |
2 | 980 | − 1.20 ± 0.83 | − 0.41 (0.03) | p < 0.001 | |
Parent education | Lower than undergrad | 3321 | − 0.85 ± 0.76 | 1 (Reference) | |
Undergrad | 1710 | − 1.04 ± 0.78 | − 0.19 (0.02) | p < 0.001 | |
Postgrad | 196 | − 1.05 ± 0.80 | − 0.21 (0.06) | p < 0.001 | |
Usual wake-up time | Before 6:30 a.m. | 2362 | − 0.85 ± 0.75 | 1 (Reference) | |
6:30 to 6:59 a.m. | 1900 | − 0.94 ± 0.78 | − 0.09 (0.02) | p < 0.001 | |
7 a.m. and after | 1039 | − 1.01 ± 0.80 | − 0.12 (0.03) | p < 0.001 | |
Usual bedtime | Before 9 p.m. | 359 | − 0.85 ± 0.81 | 1 (Reference) | |
9 to 9:29 p.m. | 2918 | − 0.87 ± 0.76 | − 0.02 (0.04) | p = 0.58 | |
9:30 p.m. and after | 2027 | − 0.98 ± 0.78 | − 0.13 (0.04) | p = 0.002 | |
Sleep duration (h) | < 9.5 | 3578 | − 0.92 ± 0.77 | 1 (Reference) | |
≥ 9.5 and < 10 | 1167 | − 0.90 ± 0.77 | 0.02 (0.03) | p = 0.55 | |
≥ 10 | 556 | − 0.93 ± 0.79 | − 0.01 (0.04) | p = 0.76 | |
Reading time weekly total in h | 5656 | − 0.005 (0.001) | p < 0.001 | ||
Screen time weekly total in h | 5512 | 0.02 (0.003) | p < 0.001 | ||
Outdoor time weekly total in h | 5559 | 0.01 (0.003) | p < 0.001 |
After adjusting for residency area, age, and gender, the multivariate general linear model in Table 6 presents strong evidence supporting the association of late bedtime with higher myopic progression (p < 0.001). Even after accounting for the effect of sleep duration and time spent outdoors, sleeping late was still a significant risk factor. Myopia progression in children who slept after 9:30 p.m. was 0.16 D greater on average compared to those who slept before 9 p.m., with the magnitude of progression increasing with later bedtime. Though one of the levels of sleep duration (> 10 h) appeared to have a detrimental effect, the multivariate model showed that the overall effect of longer sleep duration was not a significant factor (p = 0.13). Similarly, it was not significant in the univariate analysis (p > 0.5). There was no significant interaction of bedtime with other main effects in the model (p > 0.13).
Table 6.
Variable | Coef. (95% CI) | p-value |
---|---|---|
Area: Sub-urban | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | − 0.11 (− 0.17, − 0.06) | p < 0.001 |
Age (years): 6 years | 1 (Reference) | |
7 years | − 0.003 (− 0.05, −0.04) | p = 0.91 |
8 years | − 0.02 (− 0.08, − 0.04) | p = 0.46 |
Gender: Male | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | − 0.15 (− 0.19, − 0.11) | p < 0.001 |
Usual bedtime: Before 9 p.m. | 1 (Reference) | |
9 p.m. to 9:29 p.m. | − 0.06 (− 0.16, − 0.03) | p = 0.21 |
9:30 pm and after | − 0.16 (− 0.26, − 0.05) | p = 0.004 |
Sleep duration (hours): < 9.5 h | 1 (Reference) | |
9.5 to 10 h | − 0.02 (− 0.08, − 0.03) | p = 0.41 |
10 h and more | − 0.09 (− 0.17, − 0.02) | p = 0.045 |
Outdoor weekly total (per h) | 0.01 (0.004, 0.01) | p < 0.001 |
*Odds ratios were adjusted for outdoor intervention.
Sleep pattern and risk factors for late bedtime
In an effort to understand why some children were late sleepers, we then explored factors associated with sleeping late in this cohort. Remarkably, the known risk factors for myopia were also significantly associated with late bedtime. Results in Table 7 show that sleeping late is more prevalent in children who: reside in urban areas (p < 0.001); are older aged (p = 0.01 and p = 0.04); have myopic parents (p < 0.001); have higher parental education level (p < 0.001); wake up late (p < 0.001); spent more time on reading (p < 0.001) and on screen (p = 0.03) per week; and/or spend less time outdoors per week (p = 0.002).
Table 7.
Variables | Category | Slept before 9:30 p.m. | Slept at 9:30 p.m. and after | Unadjusted OR (95% CI) | p-value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | % (row) | N | % (row) | ||||
Residency area | Sub-urban | 3657 | 75.86% | 1164 | 24.14% | 1 (Reference) | |
Urban | 688 | 56.77% | 524 | 43.23% | 2.39 | p < 0.001 | |
Age (years) | 6 | 1432 | 74.31% | 495 | 25.69% | 1 (Reference) | |
7 | 2114 | 70.96% | 865 | 29.04% | 1.18 (1.04, 1.35) | p = 0.01 | |
8 | 799 | 70.90% | 328 | 29.10% | 1.19 (1.01, 1.40) | p = 0.04 | |
Gender | Male | 2311 | 72.17% | 891 | 27.83% | 1 (Reference) | |
Female | 2034 | 71.87% | 796 | 28.13% | 1.02 (0.91, 1.14) | p = 0.80 | |
Myopic parent | 0 | 1927 | 75.99% | 609 | 24.01% | 1 (Reference) | |
1 | 1428 | 69.90% | 615 | 30.10% | 1.36 (1.20, 1.56) | p < 0.001 | |
2 | 769 | 66.46% | 388 | 33.54% | 1.60 (1.37, 1.86) | p < 0.001 | |
Parent education | Lower than undergrad | 2823 | 76.36% | 874 | 23.64% | 1 (Reference) | |
Undergrad | 1322 | 65.77% | 688 | 34.23% | 1.68 (1.49, 1.89) | p < 0.001 | |
Postgrad | 140 | 56.22% | 109 | 43.78% | 2.51 (1.94, 3.27) | p < 0.001 | |
Usual wake-up time Sleep duration (hours) |
Before 6:30 a.m. | 1657 | 89.62% | 192 | 10.38% | 1 (Reference) | |
6:30 to 6:59 a.m. | 1862 | 71.70% | 735 | 28.30% | 3.41 (2.87, 4.05) | p < 0.001 | |
7 a.m. and after | 826 | 52.05% | 761 | 47.95% | 7.95 (6.65, 9.51) | p < 0.001 | |
< 9.5 | 1215 | 48.76% | 1277 | 51.24% | 1 (Reference) | ||
≥ 9.5 and < 10 | 1587 | 80.27% | 390 | 19.73% | 0.23 (0.20, 0.27) | p < 0.001 | |
≥ 10 h | 1543 | 98.66% | 21 | 1.34% | 0.01 (0.01, 0.02) | p < 0.001 | |
Reading time weekly total | 15.93 ± 6.79 | 17.14 ± 7.24 | 1.03 (1.02, 1.03) | p < 0.001 | |||
Screen time weekly total | 6.70 ± 3.80 | 6.94 ± 4.02 | 1.02 (1.00, 1.03) | p = 0.03 | |||
Outdoor time weekly total | 7.32 ± 3.85 | 6.97 ± 3.87 | 0.98 (0.96, 0.99) | p = 0.002 |
When comparing sleep patterns between weekdays and weekends, children were found to wake up by almost one hour later during weekends than during weekdays (mean difference − 0.80 h, 95% CI [− 0.82, − 0.77], p < 0.001) whereas they went to sleep at a similar time as week days (mean difference − 0.25 h, 95% CI [− 0.26, − 0.24], p < 0.001). This suggests that the time that the child went to sleep (i.e. bedtime) may be a more robust variable for sleep patterns in children, compared to wake-up time.
Discussion
Our results suggest that, in this age group of 6 to 10 years, sleeping late (after 9:30 p.m.) significantly increased the risk of myopia. Late bedtime was seen to be a consistent risk factor associated with higher prevalence of myopia at baseline, higher incidence of myopia over 2 years, and greater progression of refractive error over 2 years.
Although the current study sample is much larger, younger, and without high degrees of myopia, our findings do echo with a previous study published by Ayaki et al., where a group of 21 highly myopic teenagers (mean age 16.7 ± 2.4 years) were found to have a late bedtime compared to teenagers with either mild or no myopia12. The authors also found this group had the shortest sleep duration. In our study, sleep duration did not differ between myopes and non-myopes at baseline (9.49 vs. 9.47 h, p = 0.6) or at 24-month visit (9.16 vs. 9.18 h, p = 0.6). While our findings did not reveal any relationship between sleep duration and myopia, a number of previous studies have reported significant yet contradictory results. Some studies found sleeping less promotes myopia, whilst others found the opposite. For example, one study involving 3625 Korean teenagers (age range 12–19 years) identified an inverse relationship between sleep duration and the severity of myopia11. Subjects who had more than nine hours of sleep were 41% less likely to have myopia compared to those who slept less than 5 h per night, after adjusting for myopia related risk factors. Similarly, results from a study that enrolled 15,136 Chinese children (age range 6 to 18 years) indicated an increased risk for myopia (adjusted-OR = 3.37) amongst those who slept less than 7 h per night compared to those who slept more than 9 h per night20. In contrast, another study of 1902 Chinese children (mean age 9.80 ± 0.44 years) identified a higher risk for myopia amongst those who slept longer every night (OR = 1.02, 95% CI [1.01, 1.04]), after adjusting for age, gender, sleep disorder score, weekly near work and outdoor hours21. As previously iterated, our study demonstrated no evidence supporting a relationship between sleep duration and myopia, which is similar to the conclusion drawn by a Singaporean study on 376 infants, where they found no association between sleep duration at 12 months and myopia at 3 years22. A recent Chinese study also reported negative results for sleep duration and myopia progression, although the association became significant in girls after stratifying the sample by gender23. Moreover, unlike the Korean adolescent sample studied by Jee et al. or the Chinese children sample studied by Xu et al.11,20, children of the current sample had sufficient sleep for their age group (an average of 9.49 ± 0.54 h per night at baseline) according to consensus recommendations developed by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine, who recommends at least 9 h sleep every night for 6 to 9 years old24. This, perhaps, along with the relatively narrow age range of our sample (6 to 9 years compared to up to 19 years in other studies), are the reasons why sleep duration did not stand out as a significant factor here.
Our findings highlight the impact of sleeping late on myopia onset and progression, although the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. On the one hand, sleeping late could hint at more late-night myopigenic activities and more exposure to artificial lighting conditions of the child. In the evening, while staying in an indoor environment, a child is highly likely to spend more time on near-based activities, such as reading or on digital screens. The impact of excessive near work on myopia development has already been broadly studied25–27, although the ‘timing’ factor has never been discussed in those studies. This probably also contributed to the inconsistency findings on the relationship of near work and myopia26,28,29. The effect of artificial lighting is another frequently investigated yet still equivocal topic amongst myopia studies30–35. Due to inconsistent results seen in animal models and the multi-dimensional complexity of artificial lighting34, much more research is needed before optimal artificial lighting conditions can be identified, if at all, for myopia prevention. Additionally, children who read more and spent more time on screen and less time outdoors were found more likely to sleep late (Table 7), which echoes with previous discussion by Morgan et al. that increased education pressure is a risk factor for myopia36.
On the other hand, sleep-related ‘timing’ or ‘time-of-day’ variable, is in fact a circadian rhythm marker and interests have already been mounting around the relationship between circadian rhythms and myopia16,18,37,38. Sleep–wake cycle is perhaps the most frequently perceived example of circadian rhythms by human beings. As a consequence of involuntarily shifting between two ‘time zones’: one determined by our internal clock, the circadian rhythm, and the other governed by study, work or other social duties in modern societies, misalignment of biological and social time is almost universal39. Disturbance to the circadian clock can not only affect the academic performance of children40, but also cause several health problems41–43. In terms of the visual system, a number of animal studies have demonstrated the importance of regular rhythmicity of lighting conditions on normal ocular growth and emmetropisation13,14,44. For example, retinal-specific knockouts of the clock gene can induce myopia in mice16. In humans, circadian dysregulation has been reported for myopic subjects. Compared to non-myopes, myopes were found to have higher serum concentration of melatonin in the morning, shorter sleep duration, and poorer sleep quality11,12,45. Seasonal changes reported in myopia progression could also suggest an impact of seasonal variations of circadian rhythms on the development of myopia46. Evidence supporting a role of circadian rhythm in myopia development is further strengthened by the results from a recent meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies, where genetic factors regulating circadian rhythm are identified to also participate in the development of myopia and refractive error15. Finally, yet importantly, several ocular biometry parameters, such as intraocular pressure (IOP), choroidal thickness, and axial length, were found to exhibit diurnal rhythms in humans47,48. Nickla et al. identified in chicks a positive correlation between the phase difference in axial length and choroid thickness and ocular growth rate and proposed that such phase difference could be seen as a predictor for eye growth rate49. The author further suggested that myopia treatment could incorporate ‘timing’ as a factor in implementation in order to achieve the best possible outcome37.
Evidence for a complex relationship between circadian rhythm and myopia development is mounting, despite the fact that the underlying mechanisms are yet to be illuminated. Nevertheless, our results can confirm that sleeping late is closely associated with myopia, but additional research is needed to determine whether sleeping late makes children more prone to myopigenic activities under poor lighting conditions when they are supposed to be sleeping or more susceptible to abnormal eye growth due to circadian disturbance. These are just two of the many more questions for future myopia studies.
The strengths of the current study include the large sample size and a school-based design of the trial. The extensive support received from the local governmental and school personnel made it a logistically successful trial with good data collection. The mobile phone app-based questionnaire provided a convenient way to address the questionnaire and enabled timely data entry by the parents/carers, thus minimising data entry errors associated with transfer of data from paper-based questionnaires to digital forms. Furthermore, refractive error was determined by cycloplegic auto-refraction and therefore increasing the confidence in the refractive error data. Yet, there were a number of limitations. To begin with, data collected via questionnaires are subject to recall bias. Although a wearable device was used in this trial, it was not worn after 7 p.m. and therefore sleep-related information was not captured. Secondly, although the sample size was large, the trial sample was localised and derived from the metropolitan Shanghai with children sharing same ethnic and cultural background. This might make the findings of current study less generalisable to other populations. Finally, the age range of the current cohort was relatively narrow (6 to 9 years old at baseline). Therefore, further studies focusing on samples from other regions, wider age range and more diversity are desirable to determine if these results hold valid for the wider population.
Conclusion
This study has shown that sleeping late is a risk factor for higher prevalence, higher incidence, and greater progression of myopia in urban Chinese primary school children. The association between late bedtime and childhood myopia development may allude to a more complex relationship of indoor environment, activities, circadian rhythm and myopia, which needs to be further explored.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors thank the 24 primary schools involved in the projects and the time, efforts, and commitment of the participating children and their parents/carers. The authors also thank the support of Shanghai Eye Disease Prevention and Treatment Center, Shanghai Eye Hospital, Shanghai Municipal Health and Family Planning Committee; Shanghai Municipal Education Commission; Jingan, Huangpu, Baoshan, Pudong Jiading, Jinshan, Chongming and Fengxian district-level eye disease prevention and control branch centres; related community health service centres. The authors thank Dr. Arthur Back and Dr. Nina Tahhan for their valuable comments on the manuscript draft.
Author contributions
Conceptualization: P.R.S., T.J.N., X.N.L.; Data Curation: X.H., S.X., J.W.; Methodology: X.N.L., T.J.N., P.R.S.; Statistical Analysis: X.N.L., T.J.N.; Data Interpretation: X.N.L., T.J.N., P.R.S.; Writing – Original Draft: X.N.L.; Writing – Review & Editing: X.N.L., T.J.N., P.R.S.; Supervision: T.J.N., P.R.S.; Funding Acquisition: P.R.S., X.X.
Funding
This work was supported by Brien Holden Vision Institute, Discipline of Public Health -Eye health in Shanghai (Grant No. 15GWZK0601), Municipal Human Resources Development Program for Outstanding Young Talents in Medical and Health Sciences in Shanghai (Grant No. 2017YQ019), Three-year Action Program of Shanghai Municipality for Strengthening the Construction of the Public Health System (2011-2013) (Grant No. 2011-15), Three-year Action Program of Shanghai Municipality for Strengthening the Construction of the Public Health System (2015–2017) (Grant No. GWIV-13.2).
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Xiao Nicole Liu, Email: n.liu@bhvi.org.
Xun Xu, Email: Drxuxun@sjtu.edu.cn.
Padmaja R. Sankaridurg, Email: p.sankaridurg@bhvi.org
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-74348-7.
References
- 1.Holden BA, Fricke TR, Wilson DA, Jong M, Naidoo KS, Sankaridurg P, et al. Global prevalence of myopia and high myopia and temporal trends from 2000 through 2050. Ophthalmology. 2016;123(5):1036–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2016.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Holden B, Sankaridurg P, Smith E, Aller T, Jong M, He M. Myopia, an underrated global challenge to vision: where the current data takes us on myopia control. Eye (Lond) 2014;28(2):142–146. doi: 10.1038/eye.2013.256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Naidoo KS, Fricke TR, Frick KD, Jong M, Naduvilath TJ, Resnikoff S, et al. Potential lost productivity resulting from the global burden of myopia: systematic review, meta-analysis, and modeling. Ophthalmology. 2019;126(3):338–346. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2018.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Ikuno Y. Overview of the complications of high. Retina (Philadelphia, Pa) 2017;37(12):2347–2351. doi: 10.1097/IAE.0000000000001489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hsu WM, Cheng CY, Liu JH, Tsai SY, Chou P. Prevalence and causes of visual impairment in an elderly Chinese population in Taiwan: the Shihpai Eye Study. Ophthalmology. 2004;111(1):62–69. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2003.05.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Iwase A, Araie M, Tomidokoro A, Yamamoto T, Shimizu H, Kitazawa Y, et al. Prevalence and causes of low vision and blindness in a Japanese adult population: the Tajimi Study. Ophthalmology. 2006;113(8):1354–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2006.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wu L, Sun X, Zhou X, Weng C. Causes and 3-year-incidence of blindness in Jing-An District, Shanghai, China 2001–2009. BMC Ophthalmol. 2011;11:10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2415-11-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Morgan IG, Ohno-Matsui K, Saw SM. Myopia. The Lancet. 2012;379(9827):1739–1748. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60272-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Xiong SY, Sankaridurg P, Naduvilath T, Zang JJ, Zou HD, Zhu JF, et al. Time spent in outdoor activities in relation to myopia prevention and control: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Acta Ophthalmol. 2017;95(6):551–566. doi: 10.1111/aos.13403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Grzybowski A, Kanclerz P, Tsubota K, Lanca C, Saw SM. A review on the epidemiology of myopia in school children worldwide. BMC Ophthalmol. 2020;20(1):27. doi: 10.1186/s12886-019-1220-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Jee D, Morgan IG, Kim EC. Inverse relationship between sleep duration and myopia. Acta Ophthalmol. 2016;94(3):e204–e210. doi: 10.1111/aos.12776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ayaki M, Torii H, Tsubota K, Negishi K. Decreased sleep quality in high myopia children. Sci. Rep. UK. 2016;6:33902. doi: 10.1038/srep33902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lauber JK, Shutze JV, Mcginnis J. Effects of exposure to continuous light on the eye of the growing chick. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 1961;106:871–872. doi: 10.3181/00379727-106-26505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nickla DL, Totonelly K. Brief light exposure at night disrupts the circadian rhythms in eye growth and choroidal thickness in chicks. Exp. Eye Res. 2016;146:189–195. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2016.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hysi PG, Choquet H, Khawaja AP, Wojciechowski R, Tedja MS, Yin J, et al. Meta-analysis of 542,934 subjects of European ancestry identifies new genes and mechanisms predisposing to refractive error and myopia. Nat. Genet. 2020;52(4):401–407. doi: 10.1038/s41588-020-0599-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Stone RA, McGlinn AM, Chakraborty R, Lee DC, Yang V, Elmasri A, et al. Altered ocular parameters from circadian clock gene disruptions. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(6):e0217111. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Berson DM. Strange vision: ganglion cells as circadian photoreceptors. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(6):314–320. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ostrin LA. Ocular and systemic melatonin and the influence of light exposure. Clin. Exp. Optom. 2019;102(2):99–108. doi: 10.1111/cxo.12824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.He X, Sankaridurg P, Xiong S, Li W, Zhang B, Weng R, et al. Shanghai time outside to reduce myopia trial: design and baseline data. Clin. Exp. Ophthalmol. 2019;47(2):171–178. doi: 10.1111/ceo.13391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Xu X, Wang D, Xiao G, Yu K, Gong Y. Sleep less, myopia more. Theory Clin. Pract. Pediatr. 2017;1(1):11–17. doi: 10.25082/TCPP.2017.01.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zhou Z, Morgan IG, Chen Q, Jin L, He M, Congdon N. Disordered sleep and myopia risk among Chinese children. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(3):e0121796. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0121796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sensaki S, Sabanayagam C, Chua S, Htoon HM, Broekman BFP, Thiam DGY, et al. Sleep duration in infants was not associated with myopia at 3 years. Asia-Pac. J. Ophthalmol. 2018;7(2):102–108. doi: 10.22608/APO.2017390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wei SF, Li SM, Liu L, Li H, Kang MT, Sun YY, et al. Sleep duration, bedtime, and myopia progression in a 4-year follow-up of Chinese children: the Anyang childhood eye study. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2020;61(3):37. doi: 10.1167/iovs.61.3.37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Paruthi S, Brooks LJ, D'Ambrosio C, Hall WA, Kotagal S, Lloyd RM, et al. Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: a consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2016;12(6):785–786. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.5866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Saw SM, Chua WH, Hong CY, Wu HM, Chan WY, Chia KS, et al. Nearwork in early-onset myopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2002;43(2):332–339. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ip JM, Saw SM, Rose KA, Morgan IG, Kifley A, Wang JJ, et al. Role of near work in myopia: findings in a sample of Australian school children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2008;49(7):2903–2910. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Huang HM, Chang DS, Wu PC. The association between near work activities and myopia in children—a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(10):e0140419. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0140419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Saw SM, Shankar A, Tan SB, Taylor H, Tan DT, Stone RA, et al. A cohort study of incident myopia in Singaporean children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2006;47(5):1839–1844. doi: 10.1167/iovs.05-1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jones LA, Sinnott LT, Mutti DO, Mitchell GL, Moeschberger ML, Zadnik K. Parental history of myopia, sports and outdoor activities, and future myopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2007;48(8):3524–3532. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Quinn GE, Shin CH, Maguire MG, Stone RA. Myopia and ambient lighting at night. Nature. 1999;399(6732):113–114. doi: 10.1038/20094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Saw SM, Wu HM, Hong CY, Chua WH, Chia KS, Tan D. Myopia and night lighting in children in Singapore. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 2001;85(5):527–528. doi: 10.1136/bjo.85.5.527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Guggenheim JA, Hill C, Yam TF. Myopia, genetics, and ambient lighting at night in a UK sample. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 2003;87(5):580–582. doi: 10.1136/bjo.87.5.580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hung LF, Arumugam B, She ZH, Ostrin L, Smith EL. Narrow-band, long-wavelength lighting promotes hyperopia and retards vision-induced myopia in infant rhesus monkeys. Exp. Eye Res. 2018;176:147–160. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2018.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rucker F. Monochromatic and white light and the regulation of eye growth. Exp. Eye Res. 2019;184:172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2019.04.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rucker F, Henriksen M, Yanase T, Taylor C. The role of temporal contrast and blue light in emmetropization. Vis. Res. 2018;151:78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Morgan IG, French AN, Ashby RS, Guo X, Ding X, He M, et al. The epidemics of myopia: aetiology and prevention. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 2018;62:134–149. doi: 10.1016/j.preteyeres.2017.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Nickla DL. Ocular diurnal rhythms and eye growth regulation: where we are 50 years after Lauber. Exp. Eye Res. 2013;114:25–34. doi: 10.1016/j.exer.2012.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Chakraborty R, Ostrin LA, Nickla DL, Iuvone PM, Pardue MT, Stone RA. Circadian rhythms, refractive development, and myopia. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2018;38(3):217–245. doi: 10.1111/opo.12453. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T. Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol. Int. 2006;23(1–2):497–509. doi: 10.1080/07420520500545979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Phillips AJK, Clerx WM, O'Brien CS, Sano A, Barger LK, Picard RW, et al. Irregular sleep/wake patterns are associated with poorer academic performance and delayed circadian and sleep/wake timing. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):3216. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03171-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Qian J, Scheer F. Circadian system and glucose metabolism: implications for physiology and disease. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 2016;27(5):282–293. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2016.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Sleep, circadian rhythm and body weight: parallel developments. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2016;75(4):431–439. doi: 10.1017/S0029665116000227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Khaper N, Bailey CDC, Ghugre NR, Reitz C, Awosanmi Z, Waines R, et al. Implications of disturbances in circadian rhythms for cardiovascular health: a new frontier in free radical biology. Free Radic. Biol. Med. 2018;119:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2017.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gottlieb MD, Fugate-Wentzek LA, Wallman J. Different visual deprivations produce different ametropias and different eye shapes. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1987;28(8):1225–1235. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kearney S, O'Donoghue L, Pourshahidi LK, Cobice D, Saunders KJ. Myopes have significantly higher serum melatonin concentrations than non-myopes. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2017;37(5):557–567. doi: 10.1111/opo.12396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Donovan L, Sankaridurg P, Ho A, Chen X, Lin Z, Thomas V, et al. Myopia progression in Chinese children is slower in summer than in winter. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2012;89(8):1196–1202. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3182640996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Liu JH, Kripke DF, Twa MD, Gokhale PA, Jones EI, Park EH, et al. Twenty-four-hour pattern of intraocular pressure in young adults with moderate to severe myopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2002;43(7):2351–2355. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Burfield HJ, Carkeet A, Ostrin LA. Ocular and systemic diurnal rhythms in emmetropic and myopic adults. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2019;60(6):2237–2247. doi: 10.1167/iovs.19-26711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Nickla DL. The phase relationships between the diurnal rhythms in axial length and choroidal thickness and the association with ocular growth rate in chicks. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 2006;192(4):399–407. doi: 10.1007/s00359-005-0077-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding authors on reasonable request.