Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Perspect Psychol Sci. 2020 Aug 10;15(5):1178–1186. doi: 10.1177/1745691620927702

Parents’ Role in Addressing Children’s Racial Bias: The Case of Speculation Without Evidence

Katharine E Scott 1, Kristin Shutts 1, Patricia G Devine 1
PMCID: PMC7568821  NIHMSID: NIHMS1636172  PMID: 32777191

Abstract

Children begin displaying racial biases early in development, leading many who author popular press articles to generate suggestions for preventing and decreasing such biases. One common theme in the popular press is that parents should play a role in addressing children’s biases. In the present article, we analyze the current recommendations parents receive about addressing their children’s biases and consider their utility. We conclude that the evidence base supporting the effectiveness of parental intervention to reduce children’s biases is scant. Next, we offer suggestions for how to construct a useful evidence base from which good recommendations could be drawn. In so doing, we issue a call to action for researchers to conduct research that will yield empirically-supported, specific, shareable suggestions for parents who are seeking advice about how to engage with their children in this important domain. We additionally suggest that researchers become actively involved in the dissemination of the research findings so as to improve the lives of those who receive and express biases.

Keywords: Children, Prejudice Interventions, Race, Intergroup Relations, Parenting


Racial biases emerge in childhood (Bigler & Liben, 2007; Levy & Killen, 2008) and are particularly pervasive among White U.S. children. White children favor their own group over other racial groups (e.g., African Americans) on laboratory measures of social preferences as well as on resource allocation tasks by 4 to 5 years of age (e.g., Baron & Banaji, 2006; Engelmann et al., 2018; Kowalski, 2003; Renno & Shutts, 2015; Shutts et al., 2013). By age 6, children also begin applying stereotypes to racial groups, indicating, for instance, that unfamiliar African Americans will underperform academically (Pauker et al., 2010).

Race-based biases are noticed by, and have detrimental effects on, children of color. For example, by 10–12 years of age, 67% of African American children report having been insulted by a peer because of their race (Simons et al., 2002). Further, a growing body of research reveals that experiences with racial discrimination from peers negatively impact the academic achievement, mental health, and physical health of children of color (Marcelo & Yates, 2019; Trent et al., 2019; Wong et al., 2003). Beyond effects on those who are the targets of bias, racial biases can also have negative consequences for those who are the perpetrators of bias. Adults who are more racially biased experience more distress, anxiety, and impaired cognitive functioning in interracial interactions (Devine et al., 1991; Plant & Devine, 2003; Richeson & Shelton, 2003). Children who are racially biased may limit their interactions with racial outgroup members, thereby missing opportunities to develop friendships or to become culturally competent.

Against this backdrop, numerous articles have appeared in the popular press in recent years offering advice to parents—most often White parents—for how to reduce their children’s racial biases. Generally, we share the intuitions of popular press writers about the positive role parents could play in addressing race with their children. Although there are many sources of influence on the development of children’s racial biases (e.g., media, teachers, peers), parents are in a unique position to monitor their children’s behavior across diverse situations and over years. In addition, White adults have values that guide how they think about and react to their own biases (Plant & Devine, 1998) and White parents extend these values to their children (Scott et al., 2019).

After carefully evaluating the types of advice offered to White parents for how to address race with their children, however, we are not sanguine that the advice being given to parents will help them address racial biases in their children. Our pessimism comes from the fact that the recommendations typically found in popular press articles are not derived from empirical evidence; indeed there is little research to support how White parents should engage with their children about race. Absent a solid evidentiary base, it is unknown whether the recommendations would yield the hoped-for beneficial effects, no effects, or counter-intended effects.

In what follows, we first review the recommendations that are offered to White parents for addressing race with their children. Next, we elaborate our critique of the recommendations. Finally, we discuss the type of research needed for the field to be in a position to provide useful and effective suggestions to parents—suggestions that could and should be communicated in the popular press.

Popular Suggestions for Parents

To determine the types of suggestions that are frequently offered to parents for addressing race with their children, we conducted a Google search. For the search (done on May 9th, 2019), we specified that articles: 1) must contain the words “race” and “children” somewhere in the title or article; and 2) could contain any of the following words and phrases in the title or article: “racism,” “child,” “parent,” “racist,” “bias,” “prejudice,” “prevent,” “eliminate,” “strategy,” “what to do,” or “reduce.” We chose terms we thought parents might search for when thinking about how to address race with their children as well as terms we thought would locate the types of articles parents would come across when reading parenting blogs or the newspaper. We intentionally omitted words that align with a particular strategy (e.g., “talk”) to avoid biasing the types of articles returned towards a particular strategy.

Our search yielded hundreds of results and we selected and coded the top 50 popular press articles (i.e., not peer-reviewed journal articles) that were relevant to the topic of addressing children’s racial biases. We limited our review to 50 articles for substantive and practical reasons. First, by the time we reached 50 articles, the articles started to become less specifically targeted towards parents and less relevant to how parents should address race with their children. Second, we believed parents themselves would be unlikely to engage with more than the top 50 articles. The articles were coded independently by the first author and a trained research assistant. Reliability between the coders was high (interrater percent agreement = 86.63%, κ = 0.73). The first author examined all coding discrepancies and made the final coding determination. The coding of each article and links to the articles can be found on OSF.

Before describing the advice offered to parents, we note that 66% of the articles either explicitly stated that they were for White parents or suggested that they were about addressing White children’s biases (see Table 1). None of the articles returned in the search solely focused on parents and children of color1. The focus on White parents in these articles likely stems from research showing that White children display more substantial racial biases than children of color early in development (Dunham et al., 2007; Dunham et al., 2013). Furthermore, whereas parents of color engage in frequent conversations with their children about race (for a review, see Hughes et al., 2006), White parents are generally reluctant to talk about race with their children, which may suggest that White parents would especially need guidance about how to address issues of race and racial biases. Given the focus on White parents and children in the popular press articles, we center primarily on suggestions given to and research on White parents and children in the present article. From this point forward, when we discuss parents, we will be referring to White parents unless otherwise noted.

Table 1.

Suggestions for Parents to Address Children’s Racial Bias from the Popular Press

Suggestion All Articles
(N = 50)
Articles that are
Likely or
Definitely for
White Parents (N
= 35)
Articles that are
Definitely for White
Parents (N = 19)
Have conversations about race 98.00% 94.74% 96.97%
Read children’s books about race or featuring diverse characters 70.00% 47.37% 60.61%
Encourage children to embrace diversity 42.00% 26.32% 21.21%
Become aware of parents’ personal biases 38.00% 31.58% 42.42%
Model non-biased and inclusive behavior 34.00% 31.58% 27.27%
Facilitate intergroup contact for children 32.00% 36.84% 36.36%

Note. Percentages of articles containing each suggestion: Each cell indicates the number of articles making the suggestion divided by the total number of articles. The “All Articles” column considers all 50 articles. The next column considers just articles that were coded as having White parents as the intended audience (either White parents were the implied intended audience based on the article’s content, or the article stated that its target audience was White parents). The final column considers only articles that were explicitly addressed to White parents. Some articles contained more than one suggestion; for this reason, sums of the columns exceed 100%.

Across the popular press articles, one suggestion appeared in nearly every article: Parents should have conversations about race with their children in order to address their children’s racial biases (see Table 1). For the remainder of this piece, we focus on the suggestion to discuss race with children given its high prevalence across articles. This suggestion likely stems from the logic that if parents do not talk about race and their children are biased, then talking with children about race may be necessary to prevent or reduce biases. Another potential motivation for this suggestion derives from the literature on colorblindness (i.e., the tendency to ignore or downplay the relevance of race) with adults: Many studies have established that White adults’ racial colorblindness is detrimental to intergroup relations (for a review, see Plaut et al., 2018). For example, White people’s colorblindness leads White people to be seen as more biased by people of color (Apfelbaum et al., 2008), increases social distance between people of different races (Madera & Hebl, 2013), and decreases minority group members’ psychological engagement with work (Plaut et al., 2009).

Although it would be desirable to prevent or reduce biases in children before the biases become deeply entrenched, it is not clear that parent-led conversations in this domain will lead to beneficial outcomes. Indeed, reviewing the popular press articles suggesting that parents should talk about race brought a major concern into focus. In light of the frequency of the recommendation for parents to talk with their children about race, we are struck by the paucity of systematic research evaluating the actual impact of such conversations.

Lack of Evidentiary Base in Popular Advice

To our knowledge, there is no empirical evidence supporting the efficacy of parent-child conversations for reducing children’s racial biases. There is research showing that White parents generally adopt a colorblind approach in conversations about race with their children (e.g., Pahlke et al., 2012). However, reviewing Google Scholar and PsycInfo returned no empirical articles evaluating the actual impact of parent-child conversations about race when they do occur2. Furthermore, in a recent review on the development of intergroup biases, Skinner and Meltzoff (2019) concluded that the articles they reviewed on parental socialization used exclusively correlational approaches to evaluate relations between parents’ and children’s racial biases; no articles evaluated causal influences of parental-racial socialization. In another review focused on intervention research, Aboud and colleagues (2012) did not find any intervention research evaluating parental engagement with their children on issues surrounding race. In sum, current searches and previous reviews highlight the complete absence of evidence and illuminate a lacuna in the literature.

Some of the conversation topics and approaches recommended in popular press articles (see Table 2) certainly make contact with existing empirical research and therefore read as potentially advisable. The recommendation to use “fair” and “unfair” language is linked to research showing children are averse to inequality (Shaw & Olson, 2012); the recommendation to teach about the history of race relations is linked to research on effective school curricula (Hughes et al., 2007); and the recommendation to tackle race head-on (e.g., call out racism in the environment, name race) are likely linked to research on colorblindness reviewed in the previous section. Critically, however, none of these recommendations are based on empirical research in which parents talked to their children about race.

Table 2.

Recommended Content for Conversations about Race

Recommendation Percentage of Articles
Making Recommendation
Use “fair” and “unfair” language to discuss racial biases 42.86%
Call out racism in the environment (e.g., on television) 40.82%
Do not shush or shut down children’s questions about race 38.78%
Name race (e.g., label skin color when reading books) 30.61%
Teach about the history of race relations 26.53%
Ask children questions about what they think about race 22.45%
Tell children that everyone is equal 12.24%

Note. Percentages reflect the proportion of articles (among those that suggested talking to children about race) that made each particular recommendation.

Consider, for example, the recommendation that parents should address the history of race relations when they have conversations with their children about race. It is indeed true that one study (Hughes et al., 2007) showed that elementary school-age White children who experienced lessons presented by a trained teacher addressing the history of race relations in the U.S. had more positive views of African American individuals compared to those in a control group. However, it is not at all clear whether children would be similarly affected by a history of race relations that a typical parent, who likely lacks the type of training and expertise on the topic possessed by a teacher, could implement at home. We suspect many parents would struggle to articulate the history of race relations in this country. Even parents who are knowledgeable about the history of race relations would likely need some guidance on which issues are developmentally appropriate, when to address current events, and how to tailor conversations for a child. More broadly, research carried out by experimenters in controlled laboratories and lessons implemented by skilled professionals may or may not be relevant to the question of whether and how parents should talk to their children about race. And, this research does not clearly identify the tools parents would need to implement the recommendations in their homes.

A number of scholars focused on intergroup biases have discouraged making suggestions for interventions that have not been rigorously evaluated (Cox & Devine, 2019; Legault et al., 2011; Moss-Racusin et al., 2014; Paluck, 2012). Though recommendations could yield beneficial effects, it is also possible that they do nothing at all. Recommendations could also backfire and inadvertently lead to greater rather than less intergroup bias in children. So serious are these concerns that in a recent review of diversity trainings, Levy Paluck went so far as to say that “field experiments should be considered an ethical imperative, on the level of rigorous testing of medical interventions, to ensure that interventions do not create a backlash or otherwise harmful effects” (p. 185, 2012). We concur with this assessment.

Indeed, the adult prejudice-reduction literature provides a clear cautionary tale against promoting the implementation of bias-reduction or diversity-training strategies that have not been carefully evaluated. Rooted in concerns about the problematic nature of racial biases, in recent years, businesses introduced diversity trainings across their organizations. Dobbin and Kalev (2015) evaluated the impact of diversity training in a variety of business settings and found that organizations that had such trainings were not more diverse overall than companies that did not have diversity trainings and, additionally, minority employees left the organizations that had diversity trainings at higher rates than their majority counterparts (see Bezrukova et al., 2016; Kalev et al., 2006; and Pendry et al., 2007 for additional reviews). Further, Pendry and colleagues (2007) noted that people often respond to diversity trainings with feelings of threat, anger, and defensiveness.

In sum, to date, the primary suggestion provided to White parents for addressing race and racial biases with their children lacks the empirical backing to be broadly implemented3. As illustrated in the examples reviewed above, recommending an untested suggestion is ill-advised at best and irresponsible at worst. Telling parents to have conversations with their children about race to reduce bias, in the absence of evidence that doing so is effective, is akin to providing unevaluated diversity trainings in organizations, if not more concerning given that negative effects could be perpetuated throughout children’s lifespan.

Future Directions for Scientists

Thus far in this piece, we have argued that the advice offered to parents in popular press articles focused on addressing race with their children lacks a sufficient evidentiary base. In this section, we turn to considering what would be needed to create sufficient evidence from which good recommendations could be drawn—in particular, recommendations that could plausibly lead to beneficial outcomes. Then, we consider how to effectively disseminate research findings and recommendations to parents.

A primary recommendation for creating a useful evidentiary base for parent-led bias interventions is to conduct research that includes parents and their children. The reason for this recommendation is straightforward: Studying parents and their children is the only way to understand what parents can do (e.g., what support they need, how they interpret and implement suggestions) and what effects parents’ actions can have on children. To our knowledge, no existing research has successfully engaged both parents and children in interventions to reduce children’s racial biases.

In addition to actually including parents, consideration of the existing recommendations in the popular press led us to recognize the importance of conducting research that yields specific recommendations. Specific recommendations are critical to ensure effective usage, avoid misinterpretation of recommendations, and promote parents’ self-efficacy in addressing race with their children. Recommendations should clearly detail the people for whom they are effective, the circumstances that promote their success, and the particular content that has beneficial outcomes. For example, which recommendations should be implemented at what ages? What exactly should parents say or do to realize the recommendations? Should parents enact recommendations in particular contexts or under specific circumstances? To evaluate these boundary conditions, research studies must test particular recommendations across ages, racial backgrounds, time, and situations.

Creating recommendations with precision is particularly critical given that race is a topic that fosters considerable anxiety for White adults and often leads to avoidance of issues surrounding race or interracial interactions (for a review, see Shelton et al., 2006). If parents are anxious about race-related conversations, they are likely to withdraw from or have difficulty with such conversations—especially in the absence of concrete guidance about what to say and do and when to have such conversations. Indeed, the literature supports the notion that even though White parents generally believe it is important to discuss race with their children, they typically avoid such discussions altogether (Pahlke et al., 2012; Vittrup, 2018). Moreover, if parents do engage in race-related conversations but exhibit considerable anxiety, their children may detect their discomfort potentially leading to more negative outcomes. When White preschool children view interracial interactions in which a White adult is displaying nonverbal discomfort, children’s attitudes towards Black people can become less positive (Castelli et al., 2008). Adults’ interracial anxiety is reduced when they are given specific, structured recommendations about what to do and say (Avery et al., 2009), suggesting the benefit of conducting research that yields concrete, structured recommendations for engaging with their children about race.

Beyond including parents in research that evaluates specific recommendations for intervention, how else might the field build a useful evidentiary base? To provide some guidance, we turn to the approach Devine and colleagues followed when building and testing an intervention for adults to reduce their own biases (“Prejudice Habit-Breaking Intervention”; PHBI; Devine et al., 2012; see Cox & Devine, 2019 for a fuller characterization of the approach). We note that our aim here is not to be prescriptive or to advocate for a particular theory or articulate a particular research program. Rather, our goal is to encourage research that will build a solid evidence base by providing a general roadmap regarding how we as a field could first think about, and then act on, the idea of involving parents in bias intervention work with children.

When creating an intervention to address adults’ racial biases, Devine and colleagues (2012) began by evaluating what the research literature suggested about the nature of adults’ prejudice and how prejudice might be attenuated. In doing so, they turned to a well-developed model (Devine, 1989) that conceptualized prejudice as a type of habit. They then considered the necessary components for breaking a habit—including awareness, concern, access to tools to reduce the habit, and sustained motivation. With this analysis in mind, Devine and colleagues designed an intervention that addressed each component in turn. The intervention revealed how people could be unwittingly complicit in the perpetuation of discrimination and educated people about the consequences of bias. Then the intervention taught participants strategies that had previously been shown to be effective in short-term laboratory-based intervention studies and that could be applied to facilitate their long-term goal of reducing biases. Each component of the intervention was crafted with clear intentions and the tools provided concrete, specific behaviors or activities for adults to enact to overcome their biases. In order to evaluate the efficacy of their intervention, the researchers implemented multiple randomized controlled studies with different samples (e.g., college students; university professors) in both laboratory and field settings. Further, they assessed a wide range of outcomes (e.g., self-report and behavioral) over time (from two weeks to two years). The PHBI was the first and remains the only intervention that has been shown experimentally to produce long-term reductions in biases (Carnes et al., 2015; Devine et al., 2012; Devine et al., 2017; Forscher et al., 2017).

Our recommendation is not (necessarily) to apply the PHBI directly to the parent-child context, but rather to encourage researchers to follow a similar trajectory in mapping out their research agenda. Drawing lessons from the PHBI, researchers could similarly draw on the extensive body of theoretical and empirical work focused on the development of children’s racial biases. This literature could inform an analysis of how prejudice is developed in childhood, as well as how it might be attenuated. Of particular relevance in the parent-child domain are well-articulated theories of how biases are socialized (e.g., Bigler & Liben, 2007; Hughes et al., 2006; Perry et al., 2019). Further, there are examples of bias intervention strategies that have demonstrated efficacy in short-term laboratory studies with children (e.g., studies showing that perspective taking, intergroup contact, and individuation can reduce children’s racial biases; Houlette et al., 2004; Qian et al., 2017; Stathi et al., 2014). Overall, the results of such studies likely informed some of the recommendations listed in Table 1, but could and should be integrated into research with parents and their children.

Empirical research testing the effects of different strategies parents might implement in the service of addressing race with their children is long overdue. As in the PHBI, it will be critical that researchers focus on results from randomized controlled studies with longitudinal assessments. Such studies should be conducted in both laboratory and field settings. Laboratory studies will reveal strategies that parents can feasibly implement when they are provided with guidance and structure; field research will be necessary to evaluate whether laboratory-based findings generalize to circumstances where everyday distractions and pressures are present. In an iterative fashion, should there be obstacles or barriers outside the laboratory, the interventions could be tweaked to enhance their efficacy in everyday settings. Further, careful attention should be paid to the types of outcomes that will provide evidence that particular recommendations are actually effective: who are the targets for whom recommendations are seeking to improve outcomes (White children? children of color?), what should be measured (social preferences on a friendship choice task in the lab? climate in classrooms?), and how do the measures researchers select operationalize relevant constructs? Creating a nuanced understanding of effective intervention techniques and conducting more inclusive research can build an evidence base from which to draw specific suggestions for parents to address children’s biases.

Dissemination of Research

Although conducting and publishing carefully conducted research on parents and their children with specific practical suggestions is necessary, it is not likely to be sufficient to achieve the intended impact. Most parents do not read psychology journals in order to glean parenting advice. This reality raises the question of whose responsibility it is to bring the work into the public realm and how to disseminate research in a way that accurately reflects the science and is useable by the target consumers (in this case, parents). There are a variety of productive avenues that researchers can pursue to ensure that their work will have the greatest potential for the intended beneficial effects. Here, we outline a few possible approaches that could enhance the successful and accurate dissemination of research.

One suggestion is for researchers to work with journals to provide summaries of research articles for parents that present core findings in lay terms. Such articles should clearly delineate for whom different strategies will be effective, when parents should utilize various strategies, and should provide step-by-step guidance for the implementation of strategies, as informed by rigorous research studies. However, we acknowledge that many scientists do not have interest in writing for popular audiences or do not have skills to share their work in the popular realm. In such cases, researchers could provide assistance to journalists to write accurate and sufficiently nuanced articles. Alternatively, by working with community partners, researchers could train others to serve as the disseminators of science.

Beyond writing for the popular press, scientists in the field of bias reduction could create and moderate websites directed towards the public to review research in accessible yet specific ways. Such websites could provide parents with the background and resources to effectively address issues of race with their children. Although we raise a few potential suggestions for how to effectively disseminate research, we invite scientists to carefully and creatively consider methods for reaching the target populations. Ensuring the successful dissemination of science after the rigorous evaluation of methods will increase the impact beyond the scientific community and help bridge the all-too-frequent divide between research and practice.

Conclusion

Until research is conducted to evaluate how parents should address race with their children, the field (and parents) are in a difficult position. It is ill-advised to provide guidance absent the type of evidence we have outlined as necessary throughout this paper. However, parents with young children may miss opportunities to address race with their children if they postpone engaging with race-related topics until the research exists. Given this dilemma, we propose that researchers highlight laboratory studies that provide hints about what may be effective for reducing children’s racial biases. In doing so, however, it is essential that researchers are cautious and honest about the state of the science, clearly noting when recommendations are extending beyond the existing evidence base and working to avoid the prevalent tendency to come across as authoritative absent evidence.

In sum, we hope that this paper serves as a call to action for researchers in this field to conduct intervention research in the service of producing empirically-supported, specific, shareable suggestions for parents who are seeking advice about how to productively address race with their children. As we move forward, we gain optimism about the potential for parental engagement to address children’s biases from two sources: First, in other domains, parents can change how children act when parents are given concrete suggestions—in particular, suggestions derived from theory, that are specific, and that have been rigorously evaluated (e.g., see Brody et al., 2016 on parenting interventions to reduce risky parenting and improve children’s health; see Harackiewicz et al., 2012 on parenting interventions to improve children’s educational outcomes). Second, there is a great deal of interest in and energy devoted to addressing children’s racial biases: Our internet search generated hundreds of articles and blog posts, and these articles and posts have been read by thousands of people. Our hope is that we can come together as a field to capitalize on that interest and provide effective, evidence-based suggestions going forward.

Supplementary Material

Supporting Information

Acknowledgments

We thank Seth Pollak and Kristina Olson for comments on previous versions of this paper. We also thank Nicole Huth for her help coding articles presented in this paper.

Preparation of this article was supported by a Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation Professorship awarded to the last author, MIRA 1 R35 GM127043-01, and a core grant to the Waisman Center from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (U54 HD090256).

Footnotes

1

Articles that discuss how parents of color should address children’s personal experiences of discrimination, preparation for bias, racial pride, and self-esteem are also present in the popular press but did not appear with our search terms and are outside of the scope of the present piece.

2

One study (Vittrup & Holden, 2011) randomly assigned White parents to show children race-related videos and discuss race with their children. However, only 10% of parents actually engaged in in-depth conversations (about 5 parents). Thus, this research does not provide evidence about the effects of parent-child conversations about race.

3

We note that the other suggestions for parents that are described in Table 1 also do not have a sufficient evidentiary base to suggest that they may be valuable in reducing children’s racial biases. The literature provides no clear evidence that any of the provided suggestions, when implemented by parents, reduce children’s racial biases.

References

  1. Aboud FE, Tredoux C, Tropp LR, Brown CS, Niens U, & Noor NM (2012). Interventions to reduce prejudice and enhance inclusion and respect for ethnic differences in early childhood: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 32(4), 307–336. 10.1016/j.dr.2012.05.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apfelbaum EP, Sommers SR, & Norton MI (2008). Seeing race and seeming racist? Evaluating strategic colorblindness in social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 918–932. 10.1037/a0011990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Avery DR, Richeson JA, Hebl MR, & Ambady N (2009). It does not have to be uncomfortable: The role of behavioral scripts in Black–White interracial interactions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1382–1393. 10.1037/a0016208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baron AS, & Banaji MR (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(1), 53–58. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bezrukova K, Spell CS, Perry JL, & Jehn KA (2016). A meta-analytical integration of over 40 years of research on diversity training evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 1222–1274. 10.1037/bul0000067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bigler RS, & Liben LS (2007). Developmental Intergroup Theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brody GH, Yu T, Chen E, Beach SRH, & Miller GE (2016). Family-centered prevention ameliorates the longitudinal association between risky family processes and epigenetic aging. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(5), 566–574. 10.1111/jcpp.12495 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Carnes M, Devine PG, Baier Manwell L, Byars-Winston A, Fine E, Ford CE, Forscher P, Isaac C, Kaatz A, Magua W, Palta M, & Sheridan J (2015). The effect of an intervention to break the gender bias habit for faculty at one institution: A cluster randomized, controlled trial. Academic Medicine, 90(2), 221–230. 10.1097/ACM.0000000000000552 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Castelli L, De Dea C, & Nesdale D (2008). Learning social attitudes: Children’s sensitivity to the nonverbal behaviors of adult models during interracial interactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 1504–1513. 10.1177/0146167208322769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cox WTL, & Devine PG (2019). The prejudice habit-breaking intervention: An empowerment-based confrontation approach In Mallett R & Monteith MJ (Eds.), Confronting Prejudice and Discrimination: The Science of Changing Minds and Behaviors (pp. 249–274). Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  11. Devine PG (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Attitudes and Social Cognition, 56(1), 5–18. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Devine PG, Forscher PS, Austin AJ, & Cox WTL (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1267–1278. 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Devine PG, Forscher PS, Cox WTL, Kaatz A, Sheridan J, & Carnes M (2017). A gender bias habit-breaking intervention led to increased hiring of female faculty in STEMM departments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 211–215. 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Devine PG, Monteith MJ, Zuwerink JR, & Elliot AJ (1991). Prejudice with and without compunction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(6), 817–830. 10.1037/0022-3514.60.6.817 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dobbin F, & Kalev A (2015). Why firms need diversity managers and task forces. In How global migration changes the workforce diversity equation (pp. 170–198). [Google Scholar]
  16. Dunham Y, Baron AS, & Banaji MR (2007). Children and social groups: A developmental analysis of implicit consistency in Hispanic Americans. Self and Identity, 6(2–3), 238–255. 10.1080/15298860601115344 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dunham Y, Chen EE, & Banaji MR (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental invariance and early enculturation. Psychological Science, 24(6), 860–868. 10.1177/0956797612463081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Engelmann JM, Herrmann E, & Tomasello M (2018). Concern for group reputation increases prosociality in young children. Psychological Science, 29(2), 181–190. 10.1177/0956797617733830 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Forscher PS, Mitamura C, Dix EL, Cox WTL, & Devine PG (2017). Breaking the prejudice habit: Mechanisms, timecourse, and longevity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 133–146. 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harackiewicz JM, Rozek CS, Hulleman CS, & Hyde JS (2012). Helping parents to motivate adolescents in mathematics and science: An experimental test of a utility-value intervention. Psychological Science, 23(8), 899–906. 10.1177/0956797611435530 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Houlette MA, Gaertner SL, Johnson KM, Banker BS, Riek BM, & Dovidio JF (2004). Developing a more inclusive social identity: An elementary school intervention. Journal of Social Issues, 60(1), 35–55. 10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00098.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hughes D, Rodriguez J, Smith EP, Johnson DJ, Stevenson HC, & Spicer P (2006). Parents’ ethnic-racial socialization practices: A review of research and directions for future study. Developmental Psychology, 42(5), 747–770. 10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Hughes JM, Bigler RS, & Levy SR (2007). Consequences of learning about historical racism among European American and African American children. Child Development, 78(6), 1689–1705. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01096.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kalev A, Dobbin F, & Kelly E (2006). Best practices or best guesses? Assessing the efficacy of corporate affirmative action and diversity policies. American Sociological Review, 71(4), 589–617. 10.1177/000312240607100404 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kowalski K (2003). The emergence of ethnic and racial attitudes in preschool-aged children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143(6), 677–690. 10.1080/00224540309600424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Legault L, Gutsell JN, & Inzlicht M (2011). Ironic effects of antiprejudice messages: How motivational interventions can reduce (but also increase) prejudice. Psychological Science, 22(12), 1472–1477. 10.1177/0956797611427918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Levy SR, & Killen M (Eds.). (2008). Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Madera JM, & Hebl MR (2013). “Don’t stigmatize”: The ironic effects of equal opportunity guidelines in interviews. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(1), 123–130. 10.1080/01973533.2012.746601 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Marcelo AK, & Yates TM (2019). Young children’s ethnic–racial identity moderates the impact of early discrimination experiences on child behavior problems. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 25(2), 253–265. 10.1037/cdp0000220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Moss-Racusin CA, van der Toorn J, Dovidio JF, Brescoll VL, Graham MJ, & Handelsman J (2014). Scientific diversity interventions. Science, 343(6171), 615–616. 10.1126/science.1245936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Pahlke E, Bigler RS, & Suizzo M-A (2012). Relations between colorblind socialization and children’s racial bias: Evidence from European American mothers and their preschool children. Child Development, 83(4), 1164–1179. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01770.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Paluck EL (2012). Interventions aimed at the reduction of prejudice and conflict In Tropp LR (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Intergroup Conflict (pp. 179–192). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  33. Pauker K, Ambady N, & Apfelbaum EP (2010). Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development: Racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81(6), 1799–1813. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01511.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Pendry LF, Driscoll DM, & Field SCT (2007). Diversity training: Putting theory into practice. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 80(1), 27–50. 10.1348/096317906X118397 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Perry SP, Skinner AL, & Abaied JL (2019). Bias awareness predicts color conscious racial socialization methods among White parents. Journal of Social Issues, josi.12348. 10.1111/josi.12348 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Plant EA, & Devine PG (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 811–832. 10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Plant EA, & Devine PG (2003). The antecedents and implications of interracial anxiety. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(6), 790–801. 10.1177/0146167203029006011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Plaut VC, Thomas KM, & Goren MJ (2009). Is multiculturalism or color blindness better for minorities? Psychological Science, 20(4), 444–446. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02318.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Plaut VC, Thomas KM, Hurd K, & Romano CA (2018). Do color blindness and multiculturalism remedy or foster discrimination and racism? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(3), 200–206. 10.1177/0963721418766068 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Qian MK, Quinn PC, Heyman GD, Pascalis O, Fu G, & Lee K (2017). Perceptual individuation training (but not mere exposure) reduces implicit racial bias in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 845–859. 10.1037/dev0000290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Renno MP, & Shutts K (2015). Children’s social category-based giving and its correlates: Expectations and preferences. Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 533–543. 10.1037/a0038819 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Richeson JA, & Shelton JN (2003). When prejudice does not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function. Psychological Science, 14(3), 287–290. 10.1111/1467-9280.03437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Scott KE, Shutts K, & Devine PG (2019). Parents’ expectations for and reactions to children’s racial biases. Child Development. 10.1111/cdev.13231 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Shaw A, & Olson KR (2012). Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 382–395. 10.1037/a0025907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shelton JN, Richeson JA, & Vorauer JD (2006). Threatened identities and interethnic interactions. European Review of Social Psychology, 17(1), 321–358. 10.1080/10463280601095240 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shutts K, Roben CKP, & Spelke ES (2013). Children’s use of social categories in thinking about people and social relationships. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(1), 35–62. 10.1080/15248372.2011.638686 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Simons RL, Murry V, Mcloyd V, Lin K-H, Cutrona C, & Conger RD (2002). Discrimination, crime, ethnic identity, and parenting as correlates of depressive symptoms among African American children: A multilevel analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 14(02). 10.1017/S0954579402002109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Skinner AL, & Meltzoff AN (2019). Childhood experiences and intergroup biases among children. Social Issues and Policy Review, 13(1), 211–240. 10.1111/sipr.12054 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Stathi S, Cameron L, Hartley B, & Bradford S (2014). Imagined contact as a prejudice-reduction intervention in schools: The underlying role of similarity and attitudes: Imagined contact in schools. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(8), 536–546. 10.1111/jasp.12245 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  50. Vittrup B (2018). Color blind or color conscious? White American mothers’ approaches to racial socialization. Journal of Family Issues, 39(3), 668–692. 10.1177/0192513X16676858 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Wong CA, Eccles JS, & Sameroff A (2003). The influence of ethnic discrimination and ethnic identification on African American adolescents’ school and socioemotional adjustment. Journal of Personality, 71(6), 1197–1232. 10.1111/1467-6494.7106012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting Information

RESOURCES