Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2019 Jun 27;14(5):1865–1875. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00128-1

Perivascular spaces in the centrum semiovale at the beginning of the 8th decade of life: effect on cognition and associations with mineral deposition

Maria del C Valdés Hernández 1,2,3,4,, Lucia Ballerini 1,2,3, Andreas Glatz 1, Susana Muñoz Maniega 1,2,3, Alan J Gow 3,4, Mark E Bastin 1,3, John M Starr 3,5, Ian J Deary 3,6, Joanna M Wardlaw 1,2,7,3
PMCID: PMC7572330  PMID: 31250262

Abstract

Brain iron deposits (IDs) are indicative of microvessel dysfunction which may predispose to small vessel disease (SVD) brain damage and worsen cognition later in life. Visible perivascular spaces in the centrum semiovale (CSO-PVS) are SVD features linked with microvessel dysfunction. We examined possible associations of CSO-PVS volume and count with brain IDs and cognitive abilities in 700 community-dwelling individuals from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 who underwent detailed cognitive testing and multimodal brain MRI at mean age 72.7 years. Brain IDs were assessed automatically followed by manual editing. PVS were automatically assessed in the centrum semiovale and deep corona radiata supraventricular. General factors of overall cognitive function (g), processing speed (g-speed) and memory (g-memory) were used in the analyses. Median (IQR) volumes of IDs and CSO-PVS expressed as a percentage of intracranial volume were 0.0021 (0.011) and 0.22 (0.13)% respectively. Median count of CSO-PVS was 410 (IQR = 201). Total volumes of CSO-PVS and ID, adjusted for head size, were correlated (Spearman ρ = 0.13, p < 0.001). CSO-PVS volume, despite being correlated with all three cognitive measures, was only associated with g-memory (B = -114.5, SE = 48.35, p = 0.018) in general linear models, adjusting for age, sex, vascular risk factors, childhood intelligence and white matter hyperintensity volume. The interaction of CSO-PVS count with diabetes (B = -0.0019, SE = 0.00093, p = 0.041) and volume with age (B = 1.57, SE = 0.67, p = 0.019) were also associated with g-memory. Linear regression models did not replicate these associations. Therefore, it does not seem that CSO-PVS burden is directly associated with general cognitive ability in older age.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1007/s11682-019-00128-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Iron deposition, Brain, Perivascular spaces, Virchow-Robin spaces, MRI, Cognition, Ageing

Introduction

It is known that minerals accumulate in several brain regions and cell types (Ward et al. 2014). Macro-aggregation of minerals with para−/ferromagnetic properties is distinguishable on T2*-weighted gradient echo and susceptibility-weighted (SWI) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2012a). The mineral most commonly found in these aggregates is iron. It mainly accumulates gradually with age in areas associated with motor activity (Rouault 2013) (i.e. corpus striatus, substantia nigra and brainstem) via dysfunctional brain regulatory mechanisms (McCarthy and Kosman 2014), or as a residual from very small chronic haemorrhages, namely microbleeds, in abnormal blood vessels (i.e. capillaries) throughout the brain (Martinez-Ramirez et al. 2014). The iron deposits (IDs) coming from these two sources can aggregate with other minerals forming “calcified” clusters (Ramonet et al. 2002; Valdes Hernandez et al. 2016; Valdes Hernandez et al. 2014). Several MRI studies have reported associations of these three forms of mineral accumulations (Yates et al. 2014) with cognitive decline (Valdés Hernández et al. 2015; Sullivan et al. 2009), neurodegeneration (Graham et al. 2000; Thompson et al. 2001; Ke and Qian 2003), and perivascular spaces (PVS) in the white matter (Charidimou et al. 2014).

As IDs reflect the presence of mineral deposition in the perforating vessel walls, they likely indicate some degree of microvessel dysfunction which may predispose to small vessel disease (SVD) brain damage later in life. IDs have been related to the drop in intelligence (IQ) between childhood and old age: those having more IDs when older, having the worse drop in IQ (Penke et al. 2012). Other indicators of low cognitive performance in youth (e.g. years of education) have also been associated with an increase in the risk of SVD imaging markers in later life (Backhouse et al. 2017). Visible perivascular spaces (PVS) are also a sign of microvessel dysfunction including increased blood brain barrier leakage (Brown et al. 2018). They also play an essential role in maintaining brain fluid balance and are thought to be part of the pathogenic pathway to the development of SVD and its associated brain damage. Moreover, serum markers of cerebral amyloid angiopathy in individuals with abundant lobal microbleeds have been associated with number of microbleeds and burden of PVS specifically in the centrum semiovale (CSO-PVS) (Charidimou et al. 2014; Ishikawa et al. 2018), prompting researchers to suggest that prominent CSO-PVS are a marker of amyloid deposition and hence related to microbleed formation (Ishikawa et al. 2018). The role of PVS in cognition in the healthy elderly in relation to declining cognition is, however, less clear. Some studies have suggested that PVS in the basal ganglia (Passiak et al. 2019; MacLullich et al. 2004) and hippocampus (Jimenez-Balado et al. 2018) are associated with cognitive decline while two meta-analyses (Hilal et al. 2018. Francis et al. 2019) have found inconsistent results. The role of, specifically, CSO-PVS in cognitive decline or impairment in adulthood has been studied in the context of small vessel disease (Benjamin et al. 2018; Yao et al. 2014), stroke/transient ischaemic attack (Hurford et al. 2014), hypertension (Uiterwijk et al. 2014), and epidemiology (i.e. population from specific geographic regions, e.g. Pomerania, Dijon, Hong Kong, Singapore) (Hilal et al. 2018) with conflicting results. Given the societal impact of dementia, the putative association between burden of PVS in different brain regions and dementia and/or dementia risk has been also explored (Francis et al. 2019; Debette et al. 2019) with inconclusive results. To the best of our knowledge the current literature lacks information of computational measures of CSO-PVS burden in community-dwelling older individuals in relation to specific cognitive domains. It is not known, therefore, whether CSO-PVS can (or not) be considered a determinant of non-pathological cognitive ageing.

Brain IDs and CSO-PVS have been separately associated with the burden of white matter hyperintensities (WMH), mainly thought to be of vascular origin, and whose progression has been linked to endothelial (Poggesi et al. 2015) and blood-brain barrier permeability dysfunction (Wardlaw et al. 2013b), related to the formation of both iron deposits and PVS burden (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2016). However, evidence of association between increase number of PVS and WMH is inconsistent and more data are needed (Francis et al. 2019). Moreover, hypercholesterolaemia, hypertension, diabetes, previous strokes and presence of cardiovascular disease have been related previously to the presence of CSO-PVS (Wardlaw et al. 2013a; Francis et al. 2019) and IDs (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2015).

In a large narrow-age cohort of community-dwelling septuagenarian, we investigate whether volumes of PVS and IDs are related to each other, and whether CSO-PVS volume and count are associated with poorer cognition than that expected for a given premorbid IQ. By seeking to answer these research questions, we also explore the role of WMH and vascular risk factors in CSO-PVS burden and how these in turn might affect cognition; seeking information that could explain the link between early life factors and greater SVD burden in later life. We hypothesise that CSO-PVS and brain ID are associated and that both, independently, have associations with cognitive function in generally healthy older people. From this cohort, previous research has reported independent associations between WMH, IDs and cognitive function (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2013; Penke et al. 2012; Valdés Hernández et al. 2015), and with other related factors (e.g. nutritional (Valdes-Hernandez et al. 2014), or vascular risk (Aribisala et al. 2014a; Valdés Hernández et al. 2015)); the mediating role of WMH on the associations between IDs and cognition (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2016), and the association of PVS, WMH and markers of inflammation (Aribisala et al. 2014b) (http://www.lothianbirthcohort.ed.ac.uk/).

Materials and methods

We analysed structural brain MRI and cognitive data from 701 community-dwelling individuals from the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 at mean age 72.7 years (SD 0.7, range 71.1 to 74.2), from whom written informed consent was obtained under protocols approved by the Lothian (REC 07/MRE00/58) and Scottish Multicentre (MREC/01/0/56) Research Ethics Committees. From the MRI, brain IDs were assessed automatically (Glatz et al. 2015) in a pipeline that clusters the bimodal distribution of T1W and T2*W intensities in the basal ganglia and compares this distribution with that of normal appearing tissue; an adaptive threshold is then applied to obtain a robust estimate of the volume of T2*W hypointensities that might correspond to brain IDs. WMH volume was assessed semi-automatically (Hernandez et al. 2010) using a multispectral colour-fusion segmentation scheme, and CSO-PVS volume and count were obtained fully automatically (Ballerini et al. 2018) after thresholding the output from a multidimensional filter that enhances vessel-like structures (Fig. 1). All MRI data, and the automatic and semi-automatic methods used here are publicly available (see Online Methods for details). The cognitive variables analysed were general fluid intelligence (g), general information processing speed (g-speed), and general memory (g-memory), and intelligence (IQ) at age 11. They were generated using principal component analysis from batteries of well-validated cognitive tests (Penke et al. 2012). Age in days, biological sex, and self-reported vascular risk factors (VRF) (i.e. hypertension, hypercholesterolaemia, diabetes and history of cardiovascular disease and stroke) were also used in the analyses. We used MATLAB R2014a to perform linear regression and general linear models (GLMs). Variables and interaction terms for the GLMs were selected based on plausibility from published analyses. All volumetric imaging measurements used in these models were adjusted by head size. For the linear regression models, a combined VRF variable was obtained by summing all self-reported VRF by coding present = 1, not present =0. GLM and regression models used a sample size of 540, which is the number of individuals with MRI datasets that provided accurate CSO-PVS measures (i.e. volume and count). IBM SPSS Statistics (release 21.0.0.0, 2012) was used to calculate bootstrapped bivariate parametric and non-parametric cross-correlations between the imaging and cognitive variables, and generate the descriptive statistics for all variables involved in the analyses. For the correlations, cases were excluded pairwise from the 701 individuals that comprised our initial sample, and the imaging variables were also adjusted by head size. Parametric correlations between raw ID volumes and cognition, raw WMH and ID volumes, and raw WMH volumes and cognition were not calculated as they have been reported previously (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2015; Valdes Hernandez et al. 2013). All results are given with a precision of two significant decimal places. Full details of the sample, cognitive data used, MRI acquisition and processing can be found in the Online Methods.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic representation of the automatic (light blue) and semi-automatic (light yellow) image processing pipelines. Normal-appearing white matter (WM) masks are obtained from combining T1- and T2-weighted images (T1W and T2W respectively), whilst white matter hyperintensity (WMH) masks are obtained from the FLAIR and T2*-weighted colour combination. Brain iron deposits are obtained from thresholding the outliers of the bimodal lower cluster of the T1- and T2*-weighted images (T1W and T2*W respectively). Perivascular spaces (PVS) in the centrum semiovale (CSO) are obtained from thresholding the T2W image after enhancing the tubular-like structures in the region of interest

Results

Sample characteristics

The descriptive statistics of the imaging, self-reported vascular risk factors and cognitive variables involved in the analyses are given in Table 1. CSO-PVS were successfully automatically assessed in 540 individuals, with numbers of PVS ranging from 41 to 847, which represent volumes from 0.24 ml to 8.28 ml (mean 3.29 ml, 0.22% of the ICV). These correspond with neuroradiological ratings in the Potter-Wardlaw scale (Potter et al. 2015) from 1 to 4 (median 2). IDs and WMH in this sample have been characterised in detail previously (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2013; Valdes Hernandez et al. 2012b). Briefly, IDs were assessed in 672/701 MRI datasets, who had the sequences required for assessing mineral deposition. From these, IDs were present in 490 participants. The median total volume of IDs in the sample (i.e. n = 672) was 0.04 ml (0.003% of the ICV), and the median load in those individuals who had them was 0.10 ml (0.007% of the ICV) (IQR 0.25 ml), ranging from 0.0020 ml to 3.22 ml. The WMH median volume in this sample was 7.7 ml (0.53% of the ICV) (IQR = 13.33 ml) (Table 1). Given the known association between biological sex and brain ID burden (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2015) and reports that considered sex differences, men may have greater PVS volume than women, particularly in the white matter (Ramirez et al. 2015); thus descriptive statistics are given for each biological sex group. However, only the median and distribution of PVS count significantly differed between men (median: 440, IQR: 229) and women (median: 375, IQR: 174) in this sample (p = 0.002 and p < 0.001 respectively). The number of men and women in the sample was balanced. The number of men with diabetes and history of cardiovascular disease was approximately double the number of women with these VRFs. This biological sex difference was not observed for hypertension, hypercholesterolaemia and previous strokes.

Table 1.

Imaging volumetric measures, vascular risk factors and cognitive variables considered in the analyses. Values reported are mean (standard deviation) unless indicated otherwise

Parameter n Men (n = 373) Women (n = 328) Total (n = 701)
g factor 696 −0.020 (1.074) 0.054 (0.93) 0.014 (1.01)
g-speed 686 −0.047 (1.064) 0.058 (0.96) 0.0031 (1.02)
g-memory 681 −0.094 (1.073) 0.11 (0.95) 0.0025 (1.02)
Age 11 IQ 663 98.86 (16.52) 102.73 (13.73) 100.68 (15.39)
MMSE 700 28.56 (1.59) 28.98 (1.16) 28.76 (1.42)
Total ID vol (ml) 672 0.046 (0.20) 0.036 (0.18) 0.040 (0.20)
CSO-PVS vol (ml) 540 3.41 (1.56) 3.14 (1.33) 3.29 (1.46)
WMH vol (ml) 673 7.87 (13.22) 7.38 (13.83) 7.70 (13.33)
Hypertension (n(%)) 701 184 (26.25) 157 (22.35) 341 (48.6)
Diabetes (n(%)) 701 53 (7.56) 24 (3.44) 77 (11)
Hypercholesterolaemia (n(%)) 701 156 (22.25) 134 (19.25) 291 (41.5)
CVD (n(%)) 701 124 (17.69) 67 (9.51) 191 (27.2)
Stroke (n(%)) 700 69 (9.86) 55 (7.84) 124 (17.7)

Legend: n: valid sample size, WMH: white matter hyperintensities, ID: iron deposition, CSO-PVS: perivascular spaces in the centrum semiovale and corona radiata supraventricular, CVD: history of cardiovascular disease, (): median (interquartile range) values as variables were not normally distributed

Bivariate relationships

The cross-correlation matrix showing the bootstrapped bivariate associations among the cognitive ability measures and brain variables adjusted for head size is shown in Table 2. The percentage of total ID volume in ICV was negatively correlated with general cognition (Spearman ρ = −0.090, p = 0.049). CSO-PVS volume (also adjusted for head size) was negatively correlated with general cognition (Spearman ρ = −0.095, p = 0.036), g-memory (Spearman ρ = −0.10, p = 0.029) and g-speed (Spearman ρ = −0.11, p = 0.017). However, CSO-PVS count was not related with the cognitive variables. The percentage of CSO-PVS volume in ICV and PVS count were correlated with the other two imaging measurements adjusted by head size: ID volumes (Spearman ρ = 0.13 and 0.14 p < 0.001) and WMH volume (Spearman ρ = 0.48 and 0.19 respectively, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2, Table 2). The percentages of WMH and ID volumes in ICV were not correlated.

Table 2.

Bivariate pairwise cross-correlations between the cognitive and imaging variables evaluated. Spearman (ρ) values are given in the triangle below the main diagonal and Pearson’s (r) values are given in the upper triangle above the main diagonal. The significance level is indicated as follows: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001. All results are given with a precision of two significant decimal places

g factor g-speed g-memory Age 11 IQ %Total ID vol. in ICV (†) %CSO-PVS volume in ICV CSO-PVS count %WMH vol. in ICV (†)
g factor 1 0.76** 0.70** 0.60** −0.079 −0.11* −0.036 −0.18**
g-speed 0.74** 1 0.50** 0.42** −0.066 −0.12** −0.041 −0.20**
g-memory 0.68** 0.46** 1 0.55** −0.071 −0.11* −0.082 −0.14**
Age 11 IQ 0.56** 0.39** 0.54** 1 −0.0060 −0.057 −0.045 −0.048
%Total ID vol. in ICV (†) −0.090* −0.077 −0.087 −0.016 1 0.052 0.066 0.022
%CSO-PVS volume in ICV −0.095* −0.11* −0.10* −0.067 0.13** 1 0.82** 0.43**
CSO-PVS count −0.011 −0.033 −0.060 −0.033 0.14** 0.83** 1 0.088
%WMH volume in ICV (†) −0.18** −0.25** −0.14** −0.058 0.039 0.49** 0.19** 1

Legend: (†) Not normally distributed data

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

3D representation of the distribution of total iron deposition, white matter hyperintensity and centrum semiovale perivascular spaces volumes adjusted by intracranial volume (ICV) in the sample (left) and axial T2-weighted slice showing the perivascular spaces (red), white matter hyperintensities (green) and their overlap (yellow)

General linear models and linear regression models

Association between CSO-PVS measures and total ID volume

GLM revealed that the percentage of CSO-PVS volume in ICV was not associated with the percentage of total ID volume in ICV but with the load of WMH (B = 0.051 p < 0.01). The same GLM, but using instead CSO-PVS count, yielded similar results with the association with WMH volume (B = 22.85 p < 0.01). However, in both models, CSO-PVS measures were associated with interaction factors between the percentage of ID volume in ICV, various VRF, and the percentage of WMH volume in ICV (Table 3). WMH were observed around CSO-PVS in many cases (Fig. 2).

Table 3.

General linear models

Model no. Outcome Predictors (in addition to age and biological sex) Interaction terms
name name B SE p value name B SE p value
1 %CSO-PVS vol. in ICV % ID vol. in ICV 0.12 0.31 0.69 ID vol · WMH vol −0.39 0.15 0.011
% WMH vol. in ICV 0.051 0.0052 1.11e-20 ID vol · hypertension 1.083 0.36 0.0029
hypertension −0.0065 0.0092 0.48 ID vol · hypercholest. −0.59 0.32 0.065
diabetes 0.0061 0.013 0.64 ID vol · hist of CVD −0.88 0.37 0.019
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.0037 0.0094 0.70 ID vol · prev stroke 0.77 0.38 0.042
History of CVD 0.011 0.010 0.25
Previous stroke −0.017 0.012 0.15
2 CSO-PVS count % ID vol. in ICV 15.69 518.43 0.97 ID vol · WMH vol −371.46 256.13 0.15
% WMH vol. in ICV 22.85 8.61 0.0082 ID vol · hypertension 1709.10 600.60 0.0046
hypertension −20.085 15.33 0.19 ID vol · hypercholest. −1047.30 532.46 0.049
diabetes 28.23 21.77 0.19 ID vol · hist of CVD −1052.90 621.21 0.091
Hypercholesterolaemia 2.51 15.67 0.87 ID vol · prev stroke 1323.70 628.74 0.036
History of CVD 17.37 16.62 0.30
Previous stroke −39.77 19.93 0.046
3 g-factor % CSO-PVS vol. in ICV −83.65 45.22 0.065 CSO-PVS vol · age 1.15 0.62 0.065
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.15 0.052 0.0037 CSO-PVS vol · hist of CVD −0.32 0.98 0.74
hypertension −0.19 0.091 0.034 CSO-PVS vol · diabetes −1.33 1.37 0.33
diabetes 0.14 0.35 0.68
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.012 0.094 0.90
History of CVD 0.0043 0.24 0.98
Previous stroke −0.13 0.11 0.26
4 g-factor CSO-PVS count −0.044 0.029 0.13 CSO-PVS count · age 0.00061 0.00041 0.13
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.16 0.048 0.00086 CSO-PVS count · hist of CVD −0.00029 0.00065 0.66
hypertension −0.19 0.091 0.033 CSO-PVS count · diabetes −0.0012 0.00087 0.15
diabetes 0.39 0.41 0.35
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.020 0.094 0.83
History of CVD 0.063 0.30 0.83
Previous stroke −0.12 0.11 0.30
5 g-speed % CSO-PVS vol. in ICV −56.35 45.80 0.22 CSO-PVS vol · age 0.77 0.63 0.22
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.18 0.053 0.00087 CSO-PVS vol · hist of CVD −0.14 0.99 0.89
hypertension −0.13 0.092 0.14 CSO-PVS vol · diabetes −1.75 1.38 0.21
diabetes 0.15 0.35 0.66
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.11 0.095 0.25
History of CVD −0.019 0.25 0.94
Previous stroke −0.081 0.11 0.48
6 g-speed CSO-PVS count −0.037 0.030 0.22 CSO-PVS count · age 0.00051 0.00041 0.22
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.19 0.048 7.27e-05 CSO-PVS count · hist of CVD 0.00061 0.00066 0.36
hypertension −0.13 0.092 0.15 CSO-PVS count · diabetes −0.0014 0.00088 0.12
diabetes 0.34 0.42 0.42
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.10 0.095 0.29
History of CVD −0.31 0.30 0.31
Previous stroke −0.063 0.11 0.58
7 g-memory % CSO-PVS vol. in ICV −114.46 48.35 0.018 CSO-PVS vol · age 1.57 0.67 0.019
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.13 0.056 0.020 CSO-PVS vol · hist of CVD 0.15 1.045 0.88
hypertension 0.00035 0.097 0.99 CSO-PVS vol · diabetes −1.96 1.46 0.18
diabetes 0.16 0.37 0.67
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.064 0.10 0.52
History of CVD −0.0095 0.26 0.97
Previous stroke −0.16 0.12 0.20
8 g-memory CSO-PVS count −0.030 0.031 0.34 CSO-PVS count · age 0.00040 0.00043 0.35
% WMH vol. in ICV −0.14 0.051 0.0048 CSO-PVS count · hist of CVD 0.00053 0.00070 0.45
hypertension 0.0022 0.097 0.98 CSO-PVS count · diabetes −0.0019 0.00093 0.041
diabetes 0.54 0.44 0.22
Hypercholesterolaemia −0.073 0.10 0.47
History of CVD −0.18 0.32 0.56
Previous stroke −0.13 0.12 0.29

Association between CSO-PVS measures and cognitive indicators

As per GLMs, the percentage of CSO-PVS volume in ICV and its interaction with age were associated with g-memory. The interaction of CSO-PVS count with diabetes was also associated with g-memory (Table 3). For each cognitive measure as an outcome variable, two linear regression models that used the CSO-PVS volume adjusted by ICV as the dependent variable, together with age, biological sex, percentage of WMH volume in ICV and the combined VRF variable as covariates, were used to re-evaluate the putative associations between CSO-PVS volumes and cognitive indicators. One of these models included age 11 IQ as covariate and the other did not. Linear regression models could not confirm that the CSO-PVS volume as percentage in ICV was associated with any of the three cognitive measures analysed. From these models, the percentage of WMH volume in ICV was the imaging parameter negatively associated with all cognitive variables, with strength and significance levels similar to those reported previously using structural equation models (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2013). The same re-evaluation was done using, instead, as independent variable, the number of CSO-PVS counted yielding similar results.

Discussion

CSO-PVS volume, despite being correlated with the three cognitive domains, was only weakly associated with g-memory in this sample after accounting for age, biological sex, white matter hyperintensity burden and VRFs in a model that also considered the interactions of CSO-PVS burden with age, cardiovascular risk factors and diabetes. From these models, the interaction of the number of CSO-PVS with diabetes and the volume of CSO-PVS with age were also associated with g-memory. However, these associations could not be reproduced on linear regression models, which did not account for the interactions previously mentioned but evaluated the possible influence of childhood intelligence. Thus, we can conclude that in our cohort 1) the burden of CSO-PVS has no direct association with general cognition at older age, and 2) the possible effect that the CSO-PVS burden could have in general memory at older age could be moderated by the presence (or not) of diabetes and/or by age.

The first conclusion is in-line with those reported by other studies that have explored the possibility of an association of PVS burden with cognition in older people. A previous study in a healthy ageing sample (100 men) reported a correlation of PVS visual ratings in the basal ganglia and centrum semiovale with specific cognitive tests: non-verbal reasoning and general visuospatial ability (MacLullich et al. 2004). A population-based study on 1778 non-demented participants from 65 to 80 years of age with a 4 year follow-up, concluded that baseline PVS assessments were not associated with baseline cognitive performance (Zhu et al. 2010). Although the same study found that higher rating scores of PVS in the basal ganglia were associated with cognitive decline after 4 years, this was not the case for PVS scores in the white matter (i.e. CSO). Studies have found associations between higher PVS scores in the basal ganglia and poorer general cognition (Huijts et al. 2014; Hurford et al. 2014), but not between the latter and either total or CSO PVS (i.e. visual scores, volumes or count) (Benjamin et al. 2018; Molad et al. 2017). A meta-analysis of the literature (Francis et al. 2019) only found one study (Uiterwijk et al. 2014) that reported an association between CSO-PVS and cognition, but with borderline significance. Systematic literature reviews have not found enough evidence to conclude on the possible association between CSO-PVS and dementia or dementia risk (Francis et al. 2019; Debette et al. 2019). Our results add to the hypotheses that basal ganglia and CSO might have different underlying small vessel arteriopathies to which PVS are functionally linked (Hurford et al. 2014). However, the variability in the associations reported between PVS and cognition could be partly due to studies having used PVS visual rating scores, and the relatively lower sensitivity or perhaps higher heterogeneity of the visual rating scales applied. Robust computational methods to quantitatively assess PVS are emerging (Feldman et al. 2018; Dubost et al. 2019) in addition to the method applied in this work (Ballerini et al. 2018). Cross-validation of their results in different cohorts through a coordinated international effort is now needed.

The second conclusion adds to the current knowledge on the possible effects that the presence of CSO-PVS could have on general memory in older age and opens an avenue of research on mechanisms underlying the progression of CSO-PVS. A large study of 1818 stroke- and dementia-free participants, which assessed PVS specifically in the hippocampus, a brain structure known to be associated with memory, failed to find any association between the burden of PVS in this structure and baseline cognitive performance or incident dementia in the 8-year follow-up (Yao et al. 2014). However, this study did not assess PVS in the centrum semiovale and neither the possible moderator role of diabetes, present only in 8% of the sample. Moreover, it did not evaluate a general memory score, but only visual memory. Risk factors for the presence of PVS differ in various brain regions (Zhang et al. 2014). The burden of WMH, known to have a strong negative association with cognition, has been cited as the main risk factor for CSO-PVS (Zhang et al. 2014). In our sample, the total volume of WMH was the only covariate consistently associated with both CSO-PVS and the three cognitive measures, an association that was explored in detail previously (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2013), and which has also been reported in patients with cerebral small vessel disease (SVD) (Benjamin et al. 2018; Molad et al. 2017). As well as increasing age, Type 2 diabetes is known to be associated with higher WMH burden (Tamura and Araki 2015). The inter-relations between WMH burden, CSO-PVS volume and count, age, impaired glucose and insulin transfer mechanisms and memory, although have been independently explored, warrant further research.

Our hypothesis that CSO-PVS would be associated with ID was partly prompted by studies that found associations between serum markers of cerebral amyloid angiopathy, number of microbleeds and CSO-PVS burden (Charidimou et al. 2014; Ishikawa et al. 2018). A study on equal-sized samples of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients and patients diagnosed with vascular cognitive impairment reported CSO-PVS being associated with AD and not with amyloid burden - a pathological finding characteristic of AD - (Banerjee et al. 2017). We obtained statistically significant non-parametric correlations between CSO-PVS and total ID loads, but subsequent evaluations showed no direct association between these imaging markers. Interestingly, the number of CSO-PVS was associated with the interaction between ID volume adjusted by head size and hypercholesterolaemia. In our cohort, IDs in the corpus striatum, not brain microbleeds, determine the brain ID burden (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2015), and they have been found associated with hypercholesterolaemia (Valdes Hernandez et al. 2015).

As the actual volume of iron accumulation in tissues cannot be accurately determined using structural MRI techniques (Valdés Hernández et al. 2015), our analyses are based on volumetric measurements that, although accurate, reflect the effect that iron particles in brain tissue have on the MR signal. This is partly affected by the susceptibility of the metal/metalloid particles influenced by their aggregation, proportion and interaction with the underlying tissue among other factors (Glatz et al. 2013) and merits more research. Due to the low incidence of microbleeds in this cohort we did not analyse them separately. Replication of our analyses on cohorts with higher prevalence of this type of iron deposition is, therefore, necessary. Another limitation is that it was possible to obtain valid quantitative measures of CSO-PVS burden in only 77% of the available datasets, mainly due to motion artefacts. Although the fully automatic nature of these PVS measures makes them robust against inter−/intra-observer variations, the analyses were based on data from around 30% fewer individuals than the recruited sample. Also, given that we analysed a year-of-birth cohort, we could not fully explore the influence of age. However, the association between increasing age and higher PVS burden in both: basal ganglia and CSO, is well reported in the present literature (Francis et al. 2019; Hilal et al. 2018; Debette et al. 2019).

However, despite these limitations, this paper adds to the literature investigating the role that CSO-PVS play on cognition and their association with brain ID in a large cohort representative of “healthy ageing”, the latter being explored here for the first time. Amongst its strengths are the use of state-of-the-art quantitative methods to assess CSO-PVS, IDs and WMH volumes. Our study has both scientific and practical values, adding information related to the putative mechanisms underlying cognition, which could, in turn, help designing interventions aimed at successful cognitive ageing. Our sample is representative of the ID distribution, CSO-PVS and WMH load of community-dwelling septuagenarian Caucasian individuals, being our study relevant for epidemiological and ageing studies.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1 (28.2KB, docx)

(DOCX 28 kb)

Acknowledgements

The LBC1936 Study was funded by Age UK and the UK Medical Research Council (http://www.disconnectedmind.ed.ac.uk/) (including the Sidney De Haan Award for Vascular Dementia). Funds were also received from The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1). Funding from the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC) and Medical Research Council (MRC) are also gratefully acknowledged. MCVH is funded by Row Fogo Charitable Trust (Grant No. BROD.FID3668413), MCVH and JMW received funds from the European Union Horizon 2020 [PHC-03-15, project No 666881, ‘SVDs@Target’] and the Fondation Leducq Network for the Study of Perivascular Spaces in Small Vessel Disease [ref no. 16 CVD 05] who funded LB.

Sponsor’s role

The sponsors did not participate in the design, methods, subject recruitment, data collections, analysis or preparation of this manuscript.

Author’s contributions

Maria del C. Valdés Hernández: conception and design, statistical analysis, software development, data generation, analysis and interpretation, writing the manuscript.

Lucia Ballerini: software development, image analysis, generation of imaging data, revision and final approval of the manuscript.

Andreas Glatz: image analysis, software development and final approval of the manuscript.

Susana Muñoz Maniega: image analysis and final approval of the manuscript.

Alan J. Gow: cognitive testing, analysis and generation of the cognitive data, revision and final approval of the manuscript.

Mark E. Bastin: MRI protocol design and quality assurance, revision and final approval of the manuscript.

John M. Starr: analysis and interpretation of data, revision and final approval of the manuscript.

Ian J. Deary and Joanna M Wardlaw: study conception and design, data analysis and interpretation, revision and final approval of the manuscript.

In addition, we thank the LBC1936 participants, the LBC1936 research team including Catherine Murray for her work on recruitment, in the cognitive testing, analysis and generation of the cognitive and imaging data, nurses at the Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Facility, radiographers and other staff at the Brain Research Imaging Centre (http://www.sbirc.ed.ac.uk/): a SINAPSE collaboration Centre.

Funding

This study was funded by Age UK (grant Disconnected Mind to The Lothian Birth Cohort Study 1936), The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (Grant No. MR/K026992/1), Row Fogo Charitable Trust (Grant No. BROD.FID3668413), the European Union Horizon 2020 (Grant No. PHC-03-15, project No 666881, ‘SVDs@Target’), and the Fondation Leducq Network for the Study of Perivascular Spaces in Small Vessel Disease (Grant no. 16 CVD 05).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Author MCVH is funded by Row Fogo Charitable Trust (Grant No. BROD.FID3668413), and has received funding from the European Union Horizon 2020 (Grant No. PHC-03-15, project No 666881, ‘SVDs@Target’) and the Fondation Leducq Network for the Study of Perivascular Spaces in Small Vessel Disease (Grant no. 16 CVD 05),

Author JMW received the following research grants that relate to this study: Row Fogo Charitable Trust (Grant No. BROD.FID3668413), the European Union Horizon 2020 (Grant No. PHC-03-15, project No 666881, ‘SVDs@Target’), and the Fondation Leducq Network for the Study of Perivascular Spaces in Small Vessel Disease (Grant no. 16 CVD 05),

Author LB is funded by Fondation Leducq Network for the Study of Perivascular Spaces in Small Vessel Disease (Grant no. 16 CVD 05),

Author IJD received the following research grants that relate to this study: Age UK (grant Disconnected Mind to The Lothian Birth Cohort Study 1936), The University of Edinburgh Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology (Grant No. MR/K026992/1),

Author SMM is funded by Age UK (grant Disconnected Mind to The Lothian Birth Cohort Study 1936),

Author AJG declares no conflict of interest,

Author AG declares no conflict of interest,

Author MEB declares no conflict of interest,

Author JMS declares no conflict of interest..

Ethical approval

This study protocol is part of the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936 Study protocols, all which were approved by the Lothian (REC 07/MRE00/58) and Scottish Multicentre (MREC/01/0/56) Research Ethics Committees, in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Aribisala BS, Morris Z, Eadie E, Thomas A, Gow A, Valdes Hernandez MC, Royle NA, Bastin ME, Starr J, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Blood pressure, internal carotid artery flow parameters and age-related white matter hyperintensities. Hypertension. 2014;63:1011–1018. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.113.02735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aribisala BS, Wiseman S, Morris Z, Valdes-Hernandez MC, Royle NA, Munoz Maniega S, Gow AJ, Corley J, Bastin ME, Starr J, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Circulating inflammatory markers are associated with MR visible perivascular spaces but not directly with white matter hyperintensities. Stroke. 2014;45:605–607. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.004059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Backhouse EV, McHutchison CA, Cvoro V, Shenkin SD, Wardlaw JM. Early life risk factors for cerebrovascular disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurology. 2017;88:976–984. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ballerini L, Lovreglio R, Valdés Hernández MC, Ramirez J, MacIntosh B, Black S, Wardlaw JM. Perivascular spaces segmentation in brain MRI using optimal 3D filtering. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:2132. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-19781-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Banerjee G, Kim HJ, Fox Z, Jager HR, Wilson D, Charidimou A, Na HK, Na DL, Seo SW, Werring DJ. MRI-visible perivascular space location is associated with Alzheimer's disease independently of amyloid burden. Brain. 2017;140:1107–1116. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Benjamin P, Trippier S, Lawrence AJ, Lambert C, Zeestraten E, Williams OA, Patel B, Morris RG, Barrick TR, MacKinnon AD, Markus HS. Lacunar infarcts, but not perivascular spaces, are predictors of cognitive decline in cerebral small-vessel disease. Stroke. 2018;49:586–593. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.117.017526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Brown R, Benveniste H, Black SE, Charpak S, Dichgans M, Joutel A, Nedergaard M, Smith KJ, Zlokovic BV, Wardlaw JM. Understanding the role of the perivascular space in cerebral small vessel disease. Cardiovascular Research. 2018;114:1462–1473. doi: 10.1093/cvr/cvy113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Charidimou A, Jaunmuktane Z, Baron JC, Burnell M, Variet P, Peeters A, Xuereb J, Jager R, Brandner S, Werring DJ. White matter perivascular spaces. An MRI marker in pathology-proven cerebral amyloid angiopathy? Neurology. 2014;82:57–62. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000438225.02729.04. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Debette S, Schilling S, Duperron MG, Larsson SC, Markus HS. Clinical significance of magnetic resonance imaging markers of vascular brain injury: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurology. 2019;76(1):81–94. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2018.3122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dubost F, Yilmaz P, Adams H, Bortsova G, Ikram MA, Niessen W, Vernooij M, de Bruijne M. Enlarged perivascular spaces in brain MRI: Automated quantification in four regions. Neuroimage. 2019;185:534–544. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Feldman RE, Rutland JW, Fields MC, Marcuse LV, Pawha PS, Delman BN, Balchandani P. Quantfication of perivascular spaces at 7T: A potential MRI biomarker for epilepsy. Seizure. 2018;54:11–18. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2017.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Francis, F., Ballerini, L., & Wardlaw, J. M. (2019). Perivascular spaces and their associations with risk factors, clinical disorders and neuroimaging features: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Stroke, 174749301983032. 10.1177/1747493019830321 (Epub ahead of print. [DOI] [PubMed]
  13. Glatz A, Valdes Hernandez MC, Kiker AJ, Bastin ME, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Characterization of multifocal T2*-weighted MRI hypointensities in the basal ganglia of elderly, community-dwelling subjects. Neuroimage. 2013;82C:470–480. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Glatz A, Bastin ME, Kiker AJ, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM, Valdes Hernandez MC. Automated segmentation of multifocal basal ganglia T2*-weighted MRI hypointensities. Neuroimage. 2015;105:332–346. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Graham JM, Paley MN, Grunewald RA, Hoggard N, Griffiths PD. Brain iron deposition in Parkinson's disease imaged using the PRIME magnetic resonance sequence. Brain. 2000;123:2423–2431. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.12.2423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hernandez MC, Ferguson KJ, Chappell FM, Wardlaw JM. New multispectral MRI data fusion technique for white matter lesion segmentation: Method and comparison with thresholding in FLAIR images. European Radiology. 2010;20:1684–1691. doi: 10.1007/s00330-010-1718-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hilal S, Tan CS, Adams HHH, Habes M, Mok V, Venketasubramanian N, Hofer E, Ikram MK, Abrigo J, Vernooij MW, Chen C, Hosten N, Volzke H, Grabe HJ, Schmidt R, Ikram MA. Enlarged perivascular spaces and cognition. A meta-analysis of 5 population-based studies. Neurology. 2018;91(9):e832–e842. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000006079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Huijts M, Duits A, Staals J, Kroon AA, de Leuw PW, van Oostenbrugge RJ. Basal ganglia enlarged perivascular spaces are linked to cognitive function in patients with cerebral small vessel disease. Current Neurovascular Research. 2014;11:136–141. doi: 10.2174/1567202611666140310102248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hurford R, Charidimou A, Fox Z, Cipolotti L, Jager R, Werring DJ. MRI-visible perivascular spaces: Relationship to cognition and small vessel disease MRI markers in ischaemic stroke and TIA. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2014;85:522–525. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-305815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ishikawa M, Yamada S, Yamamoto K. Dilated perivascular spaces in the centrum Semiovale begin to develop in middle age. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2018;61:1919–1626. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jimenez-Balado J, Riba-Llena I, Garde E, Valor M, Gutiérrez B, Pujadas F, Delgado P. Prevalence of hippocampal enlarged perivascular spaces in a sample of patients with hypertension and their relation with vascular risk factors and cognitive function. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2018;89:651–656. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-316724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ke Y, Qian ZM. Iron misregulation in the brain: A primary cause of neurodegenerative disorders. Lancet Neurology. 2003;2:246–253. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(03)00353-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. MacLullich AM, Wardlaw JM, Ferguson KJ, Starr JM, Seckl JR, Deary IJ. Enlarged perivascular spaces are associated with cognitive function in healthy elderly men. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2004;75:1519–1523. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.030858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Martinez-Ramirez S, Greenberg SM, Viswanathan A. Cerebral microbleeds: Overview and implications in cognitive impairment. Alzheimer's Research & Therapy. 2014;6:33. doi: 10.1186/alzrt263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. McCarthy RC, Kosman DJ. Glial cell ceruloplasmin and hepcidin differentially regulate iron efflux from brain microvascular endothelial cells. PLoS One. 2014;9:e89003. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Molad J, Kliper E, Korczyn AD, Ben Assayag E, Ben Bashat D, Shenhar-Tsarfaty S, Aizenstein O, Shopin L, Bornstein NM, Auriel E. Only white matter Hyperintensities predicts post-stroke cognitive performances among cerebral small vessel disease markers: Results from the TABASCO study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2017;56:1293–1299. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Passiak BS, Liu D, Kresge HA, Cambronero FE, Pechman KR, Osborn KE, Gifford KA, Hohman TJ, Schrag MS, Davis LT, Jefferson AL. Perivascular spaces contribute to cognition beyond other small vessel disease markers. Neurology. 2019;92(12):e1309–e1321. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Penke L, Valdes Hernandez MC, Muñoz Maniega S, Gow AJ, Murray C, Starr JM, Bastin ME, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Brain iron deposits are associated with general cognitive ability and cognitive aging. Neurobiology of Aging. 2012;33:510–551. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Poggesi, A., Pasi, M., Pescini, F., & Pantoni LInzitari, D. (2015). Circulating biologic markers of endothelial dysfunction in cerebral small vessel disease: A review. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism E pub ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  30. Potter GM, Chappell FM, Morris Z, Wardlaw JM. Cerebral perivascular spaces visible on magnetic resonance imaging: Development of a qualitative rating scale and its observer reliability. Cerebrovascular Diseases. 2015;39:224–231. doi: 10.1159/000375153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Ramirez J, Berezuk C, McNeely AA, Scott CJM, Gao F, Black SE. Visible Virchow-Robin spaces on magnetic resonance imaging of Alzheimer's disease patients and Normal elderly from the Sunnybrook dementia study. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2015;43:415–424. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ramonet D, Pugliese M, Rodriguez MJ, de Yebra L, Andrade C, Adroer R, Ribalta T, Mascort J, Mahy N. Calcium precipitation in acute and chronic brain diseases. Journal of Physiology-Paris. 2002;96:307–312. doi: 10.1016/s0928-4257(02)00020-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Rouault TA. Iron metabolism in the CNS: Implications for neurodegenerative diseases. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2013;14:551–564. doi: 10.1038/nrn3453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sullivan EV, Adalsteinsson E, Rohlfing T, Pfefferbaum A. Relevance of iron deposition in deep gray matter brain structures to cognitive and motor performance in healthy elderly men and women: Exploratory findings. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2009;3:167–175. doi: 10.1007/s11682-008-9059-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tamura Y, Araki A. Diabetes mellitus and white matter hyperintensity. Geriatrics & Gerontology International. 2015;15:34–42. doi: 10.1111/ggi.12666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Thompson KJ, Shoham S, Connor JR. Iron and neurodegenerative disorders. Brain Research Bulletin. 2001;55:155–164. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00510-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Uiterwijk R, Huijts M, Staals J. Subjective cognitive failures in patients with hypertension are related to cognitive performance and cerebral microbleeds. Hypertension. 2014;64:653–657. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.114.03621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Valdes Hernandez MC, Maconick LC, Tan EMJ, Wardlaw JM. Identification of mineral deposits in the brain on radiological images: A systematic review. European Radiology. 2012;22:2371–2381. doi: 10.1007/s00330-012-2494-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Valdes Hernandez MC, Morris Z, Dickie DA, Royle NA, Munoz Maniega S, Aribisala BS, Bastin ME, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Close correlation between quantitative and qualitative assessments of white matter lesions. Neuroepidemiology. 2012;40:13–22. doi: 10.1159/000341859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Valdes Hernandez M, Booth T, Murray C, Gow A, Penke L, Morris Z, Munoz Maniega S, Royle N, Aribisala B, Bastin M, Starr J, Deary I, Wardlaw J. Brain white matter damage in aging and cognitive ability in youth and older age. Neurobiology of Aging. 2013;34:2740–2747. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Valdes Hernandez MC, Glatz A, Kiker AJ, Dickie DA, Aribisala BS, Royle NA, Munoz Maniega S, Bastin ME, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Differentiation of calcified regions and iron deposits in the ageing brain on conventional structural MR images. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 2014;40:324–333. doi: 10.1002/jmri.24348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Valdés Hernández MC, Ritchie S, Glatz A, Allerhand M, Munoz Maniega S, Gow AJ, Royle NA, Bastin ME, Starr JM, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Brain iron deposits and lifespan cognitive ability. AGE. 2015;37:100. doi: 10.1007/s11357-015-9837-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Valdes Hernandez MC, Ritchie S, Glatz A, Allerhand M, Mu+¦oz Maniega S, Gow AJ, Royle NA, Bastin ME, Starr JM, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Brain iron deposits and lifespan cognitive ability. Age (Dordr) 2015;37:100. doi: 10.1007/s11357-015-9837-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Valdes Hernandez MC, Allerhand M, Glatz A, Clayson L, Muñoz Maniega S, Gow AJ, Royle NA, Bastin ME, Starr J, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Do white matter hyperintensities mediate the association between brain iron deposition and cognitive abilities in older people? European Journal of Neurology. 2016;23:1202–1209. doi: 10.1111/ene.13006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Valdes-Hernandez MC, Allan J, Glatz A, Kyle J, Corley J, Brett CE, Munoz Maniega S, Royle NA, Bastin ME, Starr JM, Deary IJ, Wardlaw JM. Exploratory analysis of dietary intake and brain iron accumulation detected using magnetic resonance imaging in older individuals: The Lothian birth cohort 1936. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging. 2014;19:64–69. doi: 10.1007/s12603-014-0523-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ward RJ, Zucca FA, Duyn JH, Crichton RR, Zecca L. The role of iron in brain ageing and neurodegenerative disorders. Lancet Neurology. 2014;13:1045–1060. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70117-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wardlaw JM, Doubal FN, Valdes-Hernandez MC, Wang X, Chappell FM, Shuler K, Armitage P, Carpenter T, Dennis MS. Blood-brain barrier permeability and long term clinical and imaging outcomes in cerebral small vessel disease. Stroke. 2013;44:525–527. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.112.669994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Wardlaw JM, Smith C, Dichgans M. Mechanisms of sporadic cerebral small vessel disease: Insights from neuroimaging. Lancet Neurology. 2013;12:483–497. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70060-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Yao M, Zhu Y, Soumaré A, Dufoil C, Mazoyer B, Tzourio C, Chabriat H. Hippocampal perivascular spaces are related to aging and blood pressure but not to cognition. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35:2118–2125. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Yates PA, Villemagne VL, Ellis KA, Desmond PM, Masters CL, Rowe CC. Cerebral microbleeds: A review of clinical, genetic, and neuroimaging associations. Frontiers in Neurology. 2014;4:205. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Zhang C, Chen Q, Wang Y, Zhao X, Wang C, Liu L, Pu Y, Zou X, Du W, Pan Y, Li Z, Jing J, Wang Don behalf of the Chinese Intracranial Atherosclerosis (CICAS) Study Group Risk factors of dilated Virchow-Robin spaces are different in various brain regions. PLoS One. 2014;9:e105505. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0105505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Zhu YC, Dufouil C, Soumare A, Mazoyer B, Chabriat H, Tzourio C. High degree of dilated Virchow-Robin spaces on MRI is associated with increased risk of dementia. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease. 2010;22:663–672. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

ESM 1 (28.2KB, docx)

(DOCX 28 kb)


Articles from Brain Imaging and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES