Skip to main content
Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine logoLink to Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine
. 2020 Sep 15;113(10):389–393. doi: 10.1177/0141076820945272

A price tag on clinical empathy? Factors influencing its cost-effectiveness

J Howick 1,, S Mittoo 2, L Abel 3, J Halpern 4, SW Mercer 5
PMCID: PMC7575288  PMID: 32930031

Introduction

Good communication is a pillar of medical professionalism.1 As such, it is required independently of its economic cost. Yet, faced with scarce resources, it is useful to know the extent to which devoting resources to ensure or enhance high-quality communication is cost-effective. The benefits of enhancing one particular type of communication, namely empathic positive communication, have been studied in a number of randomised trials.2 These trials suggest that compared with ‘usual care’, empathic positive communication can reduce pain, improve patient satisfaction and increase quality of life. Observational studies suggest that empathic care has benefits ranging from reducing mortality3 and practitioner burnout4 to increasing safety.5 On the other hand, these very same studies reveal that additional time is required to undertake empathy training and to treat patients in the clinic; both of these can be costly. Unfortunately, with a few notable exceptions,6 empathy’s cost-effectiveness has not been rigorously evaluated. A definitive answer to whether practitioner empathy is cost-effective requires sufficiently powered trials or decision models that measure relevant outcomes, and this is starting to happen.6 In this overview, we consider the factors that might be weighed in future trials of empathy’s cost-effectiveness (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Factors that potentially influence the cost-effectiveness of empathy.

Factors likely to improve cost-effectiveness Factors likely to worsen cost-effectiveness
Direct benefits from randomised trials Reduced pain1517 Direct costs Additional time often required by practitioners2
Improved quality of life6,7
Improved patient satisfaction16,1820
Direct benefits from observational studies Reduced mortality by 50% among diabetic patients3
Improved wellbeing6,21
Reduced hospital duration of stay and readmission rates22
Indirect benefits Improved diagnosis11 Indirect costs Training empathy7,15
Improved medication adherence13
Reduced hospital admissions and psychiatric hospitalisation
Reduced patient stress.23 Lowered stress, in turn, may reduce pain, depression and anxiety and even lower the risk of heart disease12
Improved trust24 and subsequently medication adherence13
Benefits to practitioners Reduced practitioner burnout4
Reduced medico-legal risks25

Benefits of empathy (likely to favourably influence cost-effectiveness)

We are aware of two trials that measured the impact of enhanced empathic care on quality of life. The first measured participant-assessed quality of life, using the 34-question Irritable Bowel Syndrome Quality of Life score.7 In a sample of 262 patients, empathic care seemed to improve quality of life by a small, statistically significant amount (standardised mean difference 0.43 [95% confidence interval 0.13–0.73]). In another trial with 152 patients, a complex intervention which included empathy (as well as longer consultations, continuity of care and additional support to treat multimorbid patients) found that the quality of life was improved. In this study, quality of life measured over the 12-month period was higher in the intervention group (p = 0.002), and the intervention was highly cost-effective over the 12-month period. Modelling suggested that cost-effectiveness would continue.6

Pain (which enhanced empathy can reduce) strongly influences the quality of life measures commonly used to evaluate cost-effectiveness. Three trials (1067 patients) within a systematic review of empathic care found a small, non-statistically significant benefit (standardised mean difference −0.05 [95% confidence interval 0.32–0.22]).2

We found four trials (including 955 patients) reporting satisfaction as an outcome which suggested that this was improved by a small amount (standardised mean difference 0.26 [95% confidence interval 0.02–0.54]).2

Observational studies report (among other things) that enhanced empathy reduces mortality by 50% among diabetic patients,3 reduces symptom burden and improves wellbeing,6 increases patient enablement,8 increases patient safety,5 improves self-efficacy and adherence9 and reduces practitioner burnout.4

The mechanisms by which enhanced empathic care produces beneficial results are currently somewhat speculative. First, empathy can be helpful to make a correct diagnosis, as without it, patients may not share details of symptoms, especially embarrassing ones. They are also more likely to remain engaged.10 By contrast, doctors perceived to be unfriendly are less likely to get enough information from patients to make the right diagnoses or prescribe the right treatment. One study even showed that unempathic doctors could cause harm by scaring patients away from medical care when they need it.11 Next, an empathic doctor will help put a patient at ease and reduce their stress; lowered stress, in turn, may reduce pain, depression, anxiety and even lower the risk of heart disease.12 Being positive (which is part of empathic care) also activates the patient’s endogenous opioid system, further reducing pain. In addition, empathy seems to facilitate trust10 and subsequently medication adherence.13 Adherence, in turn, is linked to better clinical outcomes, with one study showing that up to 62% of patients were more likely to adhere to treatment based on the quality of physician communication.14 Anecdotally, those of us who are practitioners (SM, SWM and JoH) find that the benefits of enhanced empathic care could reduce hospital admissions and psychiatric hospitalisation. Patients seem more likely to visit empathic practitioners who can prevent more serious events like hospital admissions and also reduce fears about issues such as medication shortages and medical tests.

Costs of empathy

The main way in which enhanced empathic care would increase healthcare costs is the potential additional time often required by practitioners to treat patients.2 However, the extent to which enhanced empathy requires more time is not clear. Some trials of enhanced empathy do not increase the consultation time by focusing on non-verbal communication.19

Another cost is the time and money spent training practitioners to enhance empathy. Some trials that trained practitioners to enhance their empathy required a team of professionals, video consultations and two days.15 However, trials that had much less (4 hours) training did not result in smaller patient benefits when compared with trials involving doctors that had more extensive training.2 While this training period amounts to a one-off cost whose marginal cost approaches zero, it nonetheless has the potential to be substantial.

Other considerations for measuring cost-effectiveness

Better communication (which includes empathic communication) reduces medico-legal risks, which represents a further benefit.25 Some worry that a potential cost is increased practitioner burnout caused by the alleged increased emotional labour caused by empathic care. The evidence in this area, however, is mixed, with a growing consensus that therapeutic empathy reduces practitioner burnout and increases job satisfaction.4 Few things are likely to be more costly than losing physicians due to burnout and having to replace them. These long-term human resource outcomes are rarely included in cost-effectiveness analyses but may prove relevant in the evaluation of empathic interventions.

Technology and the future of empathy’s cost-effectiveness

The costs in training and additional time spent in empathic care will be subject to change as technology and artificial intelligence becomes increasingly involved in healthcare.26 While it is unlikely that humanoid robots will be able to express empathy as well as real humans, the opportunity cost of training artificially intelligent robots (should they ever move out of the laboratory27) will be close to zero. They have to be trained in some way, so choosing to train them to be more empathic does not seem to represent an additional cost. Likewise, the growth of digital online learning (catalysed radically during the COVID-19 era) has the potential to reduce the marginal cost of empathy training, as online courses are highly scalable.

Limitations to measuring cost-effectiveness

Measures of empathy’s cost-effectiveness will be as accurate as the underlying studies measuring its effects. The main problems with randomised trials of empathy are difficulty in blinding practitioners and subsequent contamination. Practitioners who are randomised to the control group of a trial of enhanced empathy know which group they are in, and their knowledge could affect the results. For example, the fact that they are aware that their empathy will be measured could lead them to be more empathic than they would usually be, thus decreasing the apparent benefits of empathy training. With that in mind, it is important to take the observational evidence into account, because contamination is not a confounder of observational studies.

To be sure, the results of observational studies have their own biases. A particularly salient potential problem with observational studies is selection bias. Patients who are healthier to begin with are more likely to be more sociable, less angry, easier to empathise with and more likely to do better over time than those who are less healthy to begin with. Still, the fact that all the observational studies reveal a large effect with a consistent direction is suggestive of a real effect.

The next main problem with trials of empathy is definition and measurement. One study identifyed 36 distinct ways to measure empathy within healthcare.28 In spite of this, there is an emerging consensus that within healthcare, clinical empathy has three components: understanding; demonstrating understanding; and therapeutic action.29 Relatedly, the two scales of clinical empathy that are most widely used – the Consultation and Relational Empathy (CARE) measure30 and the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy (JSE)31 – are compatible with the definition of clinical empathy.

Discussion and recommendations

Beyond being a professional requirement, enhanced empathy has been proven in randomised clinical trials to serve two of the most important goals of medicine – decreasing pain and improving quality of life, by a small amount. Observational studies consistently show that empathy improves adherence to treatment, which is an important determinant of the cost-effectiveness of healthcare. Enhanced empathic care may also increase healthcare costs with resources required to encourage empathic care and additional time spent with patients. Our analysis found that there appear to be more potential benefits than costs.

Ultimately, whether empathy is cost-effective will depend on how robust all of its effects are, relative to all of its costs, over time. This can only be measured in comprehensive systematic cost-effectiveness analyses that take the factors listed here into account. Such analyses can weigh the factors listed in this paper. We recommend that a decision analytic model be used to evaluate potential cost-effectiveness using existing evidence, informing the design of future interventions and trials.

Acknowledgements

None.

Declarations

Competing interests

None declared.

Funding

LA was funded by the Wellcome Trust (grant number 217826/Z/19/Z).

Guarantor

JH.

Contributorship

JeH wrote the initial draft, which SM extensively revised. LA, SWM and JoH edited and added to the manuscript. All authors revised subsequent drafts and approved the final manuscript.

Ethics approval

Not required.

Provenance

Not commissioned; peer-reviewed by John Abramson and Francisco Javier Carrera-Hueso.

ORCID iD

J Howick https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0280-7206

References

  • 1.World Medical Association. World Medical Association International Code of Medical Ethics. Ferney-Voltaire: WMA, 1949.
  • 2.Howick J, Moscrop A, Mebius A, et al. Effects of empathic and positive communication in healthcare consultations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J R Soc Med 2018; 111: 240–252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dambha-Miller H, Feldman AL, Kinmoth AL, Griffin SJ. Association between primary care practitioner empathy and risk of cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality among patients with type 2 diabetes: a population-based prospective cohort study. Ann Fam Med 2019; 17: 311–318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Sturzu L, Lala A, Bisch M, Guitter M, Dobre D, Schwan R. Empathy and burnout – a cross-sectional study among mental healthcare providers in France. J Med Life 2019; 12: 21–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Doyle C, Lennox L, Bell D. A systematic review of evidence on the links between patient experience and clinical safety and effectiveness. BMJ Open 2013; 3: E001570–E001570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mercer SW, Fitzpatrick B, Guthrie B, et al. The CARE Plus study – a whole-system intervention to improve quality of life of primary care patients with multimorbidity in areas of high socioeconomic deprivation: exploratory cluster randomised controlled trial and cost-utility analysis. BMC Med 2016; 14: 88–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, et al. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ 2008; 336: 999–1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mercer SW, Jani BD, Maxwell M, Wong SY, Watt GC. Patient enablement requires physician empathy: a cross-sectional study of general practice consultations in areas of high and low socioeconomic deprivation in Scotland. BMC Fam Pract 2012; 13: 6–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Flickinger TE, Saha S, Roter D, et al. Clinician empathy is associated with differences in patient-clinician communication behaviors and higher medication self-efficacy in HIV care. Patient Educ Couns 2016; 99: 220–226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Halpern J. Gathering the patient’s story and clinical empathy. Perm J 2012; 16: 52–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Butler CC, Pill R, Stott NC. Qualitative study of patients' perceptions of doctors' advice to quit smoking: implications for opportunistic health promotion. BMJ 1998; 316: 1878–1881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Benson H. The Relaxation Response, New York: Morrow, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Riess H. The science of empathy. J Patient Exp 2017; 4: 74–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zolnierek KB, Dimatteo MR. Physician communication and patient adherence to treatment: a meta-analysis. Med Care 2009; 47: 826–834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chassany O, Boureau F, Liard F, et al. Effects of training on general practitioners’ management of pain in osteoarthritis: a randomized multicenter study. J Rheumatol 2006; 33: 1827–1834. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Vangronsveld KL, Linton SJ. The effect of validating and invalidating communication on satisfaction, pain and affect in nurses suffering from low back pain during a semi-structured interview. Eur J Pain 2012; 16: 239–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.White P, Bishop FL, Prescott P, Scott C, Little P, Lewith G. Practice, practitioner, or placebo? A multifactorial, mixed-methods randomized controlled trial of acupuncture. Pain 2012; 153: 455–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fujimori M, Shirai Y, Asai M, et al. Effect of communication skills training program for oncologists based on patient preferences for communication when receiving bad news: a randomized controlled trial. J Clin Oncol 2014; 32: 2166–2172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Little P, White P, Kelly J, et al. Randomised controlled trial of a brief intervention targeting predominantly non-verbal communication in general practice consultations. Br J Gen Pract 2015; 65: e351–e356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Soltner C, Giquello JA, Monrigal-Martin C, et al. Continuous care and empathic anaesthesiologist attitude in the preoperative period: impact on patient anxiety and satisfaction. Br J Anaesth 2011; 106: 680–686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mercer SW, Higgins M, Bikker AM, et al. General practitioners’ empathy and health outcomes: a prospective observational study of consultations in areas of high and low deprivation. Ann Fam Med 2016; 14: 117–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Trzeciak S, Gaughan JP, Bosire J, et al. Association between medicare summary star ratings for patient experience and clinical outcomes in US hospitals. J Patient Exp 2016; 3: 6–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Weiss R, Vittinghoff E, Fang MC, et al. Associations of physician empathy with patient anxiety and ratings of communication in hospital admission encounters. J Hosp Med 2017; 12: 805–810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Halpern J. Clinical empathy in medical care. In: Decety J. (ed). Empathy: From Bench to Bedside, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Moore PJ, Adler NE, Robertson PA. Medical malpractice: the effect of doctor-patient relations on medical patient perceptions and malpractice intentions. West J Med 2000; 173: 244–250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Pealing L, Tempest HV, Howick J, et al. Technology: a help or hindrance to empathic healthcare? J R Soc Med 2018; 111: 390–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Giannopulu I, Terada K, Watanabe T. Emotional empathy as a mechanism of synchronisation in child-robot interaction. Front Psychol 2018; 9: 1852–1852. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hemmerdinger JM, Stoddart SDR, Lilford RJ. A systematic review of tests of empathy in medicine. BMC Med Educ 2007; 7: 24–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Howick J, Bizzari V, Dambha-Miller H, et al. Therapeutic empathy: what it is and what it isn’t. J R Soc Med 2018; 111: 233–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mercer SW, Maxwell M, Heaney D, et al. The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Fam Pract 2004; 21: 699–705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hojat M, Gonnella JS, Nasca TJ, et al. The Jefferson Scale of physician empathy: further psychometric data and differences by gender and specialty at item level. Acad Med 2002; 77: S58–S60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine are provided here courtesy of Royal Society of Medicine Press

RESOURCES