Skip to main content
BMC Geriatrics logoLink to BMC Geriatrics
. 2020 Oct 21;20:418. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01761-y

Train the brain with music (TBM): brain plasticity and cognitive benefits induced by musical training in elderly people in Germany and Switzerland, a study protocol for an RCT comparing musical instrumental practice to sensitization to music

Clara E James 1,2,, Eckart Altenmüller 3,4, Matthias Kliegel 2,5, Tillmann HC Krüger 4,6, Dimitri Van De Ville 7,8, Florian Worschech 3,4, Laura Abdili 1, Daniel S Scholz 3,4, Kristin Jünemann 4,6, Alexandra Hering 2,5, Frédéric Grouiller 9, Christopher Sinke 4,6, Damien Marie 1
PMCID: PMC7576734  PMID: 33087078

Abstract

Background

Recent data suggest that musical practice prevents age-related cognitive decline. But experimental evidence remains sparse and no concise information on the neurophysiological bases exists, although cognitive decline represents a major impediment to healthy aging. A challenge in the field of aging is developing training regimens that stimulate neuroplasticity and delay or reverse symptoms of cognitive and cerebral decline. To be successful, these regimens should be easily integrated in daily life and intrinsically motivating. This study combines for the first-time protocolled music practice in elderly with cutting-edge neuroimaging and behavioral approaches, comparing two types of musical education.

Methods

We conduct a two-site Hannover-Geneva randomized intervention study in altogether 155 retired healthy elderly (64–78) years, (63 in Geneva, 92 in Hannover), offering either piano instruction (experimental group) or musical listening awareness (control group). Over 12 months all participants receive weekly training for 1 hour, and exercise at home for ~ 30 min daily. Both groups study different music styles. Participants are tested at 4 time points (0, 6, and 12 months & post-training (18 months)) on cognitive and perceptual-motor aptitudes as well as via wide-ranging functional and structural neuroimaging and blood sampling.

Discussion

We aim to demonstrate positive transfer effects for faculties traditionally described to decline with age, particularly in the piano group: executive functions, working memory, processing speed, abstract thinking and fine motor skills. Benefits in both groups may show for verbal memory, hearing in noise and subjective well-being. In association with these behavioral benefits we anticipate functional and structural brain plasticity in temporal (medial and lateral), prefrontal and parietal areas and the basal ganglia.

We intend exhibiting for the first time that musical activities can provoke important societal impacts by diminishing cognitive and perceptual-motor decline supported by functional and structural brain plasticity.

Trial registration

The Ethikkomission of the Leibniz Universität Hannover approved the protocol on 14.08.17 (no. 3604–2017), the neuroimaging part and blood sampling was approved by the Hannover Medical School on 07.03.18. The full protocol was approved by the Commission cantonale d’éthique de la recherche de Genève (no. 2016–02224) on 27.02.18 and registered at clinicaltrials.gov on 17.09.18 (NCT03674931, no. 81185).

Keywords: Music induced brain and behavioral plasticity, Age-related cognitive decline, One-year music practice, Randomized controlled trial, Working memory, Executive functions, Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), Voxel based Morphometry (VBM), Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), Multivariate data-driven analyses

Background

In young people, musical instrumental training triggers functional and structural brain plasticity and may enhance general cognitive and perceptual-motor function, explained by the widely distributed brain areas involved in music performance that support many other cognitive and perceptual-motor skills [112]. Evidence accumulates that this also applies to healthy elderly, at least in the behavioral domain [1316].

We postulate showing advantages of music education for faculties generally acknowledged to decay during ordinary aging: executive functions, different types of memory, processing speed, language function, logical thinking, hearing in noise (auditory selective attention), manual dexterity, bimanual coordination, and to provoke functional and structural brain plasticity. Last but not least, we anticipate increased well-being. Optimal aging cannot be defined solely by objective factors such as mental performance and health, but also by subjective features such as quality of life [17]. Intriguingly, these objective and subjective factors appear to be closely related (for a review see [18]). The ultimate objective, if our study results confirm our hypotheses, is systematic implementation of government-funded musical practice facilities in elderly centers and nursing homes.

Age-related cognitive decline in healthy elderly

Whereas cross-sectional studies indicate a definite decrease for different cognitive functions from early adulthood onwards, longitudinal studies however only demonstrate marked deterioration for most functions after midlife. In the majority of healthy older adults, executive functions decline [19, 20], and its sub-processes such as working memory updating, inhibition and task-switching (cognitive flexibility) all gradually deteriorate with age [19]. Actually, decrease of working memory function, a fundamental component of general cognition, may explain several of the other higher order cognitive aging phenomena, like for instance deterioration of executive functions [21, 22]. In a lifespan perspective however [2325], development during aging results from the combination of decline of certain physiological and cognitive functions, with the lifelong prospect of evolving through learning and interventions.

Brain aging in healthy elderly

Unraveling brain processes that underlie cognitive decline symptoms, and developing training regimens that stimulate neuroprotection mechanisms to delay or even reverse those symptoms, may represent a key to potential remedies [19, 20]. Cognitive regression in healthy elderly people follows local structural and functional brain modifications such as reduction of prefrontal cortex and hippocampal gray matter (GM) and loss of anterior white matter integrity and functional connectivity [2629]. Global connectivity however, i.e. networks of long-range cortical fiber structural connections (white matter), and functional connections (as measured by resting state functional MRI (fMRI; functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging) carrying information flow dynamics throughout the whole brain [3032], critical for cognitive functioning, also becomes less performant in the aging brain.

Recently evidence accumulated that hippocampal function is vital to all types of relational memory, independently of the time span between learning and recollection [33, 34]. Atrophy of the hippocampus, in general occurring in late adulthood [19], provokes deterioration of working memory and long term memory, decrease of abstract thinking (fluid intelligence) [35] and progressively increased risk for dementia [36].

Cognitive activities and physical exercise -in addition to treatment of general medical conditions- may delay age-related atrophy in the hippocampus, or even expand its size [3638]. Musical practice can induce functional and structural plasticity in the anterior and middle part of the hippocampus, and these changes are accompanied by increased proficiency in musical tasks, working memory and fluid intelligence [4, 3941].

Countervailing age-related cognitive decline by means of interventions

Therapies based on real life experiences, like musical practice, dancing or playing chess, more naturally induce far-reaching transfer of learning as they are complex and variable compared to laboratory training [42, 43]. This illimited complexity allows progressive increase in difficulty, keeping learning challenging at all times. As these activities are pleasant for an average individual, they are intrinsically motivating and therefore easy to maintain over long periods of time, meanwhile increasing well-being.

Among a multitude of cognitive training research in the elderly, [21, 22, 4446], only two used piano or keyboard training [13, 14], over periods of 6 and 4 months, in musically untrained healthy elderly between 60 and 85 years. In one study [13] the piano group significantly improved in executive function and working memory, compared to a passive control group. Another more recent study [14] also showed improved executive function and enhanced well-being, and also a trend for motor advantages, as compared to a control group performing other leisure activities. In yet another study in healthy elderly (mean age 77 years) 15 weeks of drumming and singing improved verbal and visual memory as compared to literature training [16].

Musical training as a model for behavioral and cerebral plasticity

As musical performance relies on widely distributed brain areas involved in many other cognitive and perceptual-motor skills [6, 7, 4751], musical instrumental training provides a strong means of improving cognitive and perceptual-motor function provoking functional and structural brain changes. Most studies on brain and behavioral plasticity induced by musical practice concerned cross-sectional studies in young adults with differing levels of musical practice from childhood. Progressive music intrinsic and overall cognitive advantages in combination with widespread functional and structural brain plasticity occurred as a function of training intensity in auditory, limbic, perceptual-motor and higher order cognitive networks [1, 36, 9, 11, 40, 5258].

Brain areas most susceptible to age-related cognitive decay, in inferior frontal and temporal cortices and their interconnecting white matter tracts, are strongly involved in musical instrumental practice and share neural substrates with language functions [7, 9, 47, 53, 55, 59, 60], working memory [5, 7, 53, 55, 59, 60] and broad-spectrum cognitive function, including executive functions (see next sub-chapter) [35, 55, 61]. A study comparing elderly mono- and dizygotic twins, of whom only one practiced music, demonstrated that musical instrumental practice, controlling for sex, education-level and physical activity, reduced the probability of dementia and other age related cognitive impairments [62]. Overall, cognitive decline and dementia can be postponed by recent and past musical activities [63, 64]. Music-supported therapy also proved effective in restoration of motor skills in elderly suffering from decline of motor function following stroke [65, 66]. Increased gray matter in qualified musicians as compared to less proficient musicians or non-musicians could be consistently demonstrated in the inferior frontal cortex [1, 3, 55, 67], the hippocampus [4, 39, 40, 68], and the entorhinal cortex that projects to the hippocampus [69]. Common age-related shrinking of inferior frontal cortices appeared absent in older male orchestra musicians [67]. A recent review article acknowledges music training as a promising approach for age-related cognitive decline [70]. Strikingly, even people practicing music moderately and exclusively during their childhood, experienced advantages for neural timing in speech perception at an advanced age [71].

Far transfer effects in young and older people following music training

Far transfer effects for higher-order cognitive functioning following musical training occurred in adult musicians for verbal (auditory & visual modality) and tonal working memory [4, 6, 58], verbal long-term memory [72], executive control [73, 74], fluid intelligence [4, 75], visuo-spatial ability [55, 63] and attention [61]. Specifically in healthy elderly far transfer after musical training showed for executive function, attention, working memory, verbal and visual memory, sensorimotor function and subjective well-being [13, 14, 16]. Musical practice sharpens auditory attention and hearing in noise in children, young and elderly adults [61, 76, 77], both recurrent problems in normal aging. Perceptual-motor skills, that decline with advanced age, may also profit [14] and interact with higher order cognitive functions [78]. The triple auditory, visually and sensory feedback on the precision of motor performance yielded during musical practice may explain this observation.

Intervention studies provoking brain and behavioral changes in elderly and older adults

Not only were older adults (mean age 60 years) able to learn three-ball cascade juggling over 3 months, moreover their gray matter increased transiently in the left hippocampus, bilateral nucleus accumbens and associative visual areas, quite similarly as in young adults [79]. Four weeks of video action gaming in elderly (60 to 85 years) modified frontal task related electroencephalography) and changes persisted after a 6 months delay [80]. Aerobic training in elderly induced volume increase in the prefrontal cortex and anterior hippocampus after 6 to 12 months of training [36, 81], and white matter integrity improved in frontal, temporal and parietal tracts [29]. In mature adults (≈35–40 years of age), increase in volume of the posterior hippocampus occurred, after acquiring their taxi drivers license in London, thus learning by heart the map of the city [82]. Piano tuners, also trained in advanced adulthood, showed volume increase in the anterior hippocampus and frontal operculum [83]. Learning to read musical notation over 3 months induced specific effects on visuo-spatial skills and on the function of the fusiform gyrus and superior parietal areas in initially musically naïve adults [84, 85]. Five-week musical training of adult musical novices sufficed to induce functional audio-motor (temporal-frontal) coupling, thus brain plasticity [8688]. Specifically in elderly, 4 to 6 months piano training enhanced executive functions and working memory, compared to control groups [13, 14]. In another study involving healthy elderly, drumming and singing could improve verbal and visual memory [16]. Moreover, learning a new skill engages neural plasticity more strongly in older adults [89]. Also specifically in elderly, advantages of intensive new skill learning for memory function manifested [90].

We conclude that piano training in elderly musical novices may induce functional and morphological brain plasticity in the auditory and prefrontal cortex, hippocampus and elsewhere in the brain, as well as in long- and short-range white matter tracts and functional connectivity, together with concomitant cognitive and sensorimotor advantages, even at an advanced age. Intensive music listening and learning may already provoke a subset of these cognitive advantages.

Although decline of certain physiological and cognitive functions during aging is inevitable, brain and behavior remain plastic from cradle to grave. Therefore, lifelong education for seniors should become a significant policy focus in the framework of population ageing [79].

Methods / design

For the sake of clarity and brevity, a great deal of comprehensive information concerning Methods and Design, is provided in the Tables:

Table 1: Trial Registration Data Set (according to WHO guidelines), p. 6

Table 2: Prescreening procedure in order to verify inclusion/exclusion criteria, p. 8

Table 3: Documents: information & consent forms, agreements, p. 9

Table 4: Behavioral Test Battery (psychometric tests), p. 10

Table 5: (f)MRI measurements, p. 11

Table 6: Study procedure with timelines, p. 12

List of Abbreviations, p. 15

Table 1.

Trial Registration Data Set (according to WHO guidelines)

Data category Information32
Primary registry and trial identifying number ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT03674931
Date of registration in primary registry 17.09.2018
Secondary identifying numbers Study ID number: 81185
Source(s) of monetary or material support Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF no. 100019E-170,410) and Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG no.323965454)
Primary sponsor SNSF and DFG: Lead Agency grant (two-countries study)
Contact for public and scientific queries Prof. Clara E. James: clara.james@hesge.ch
Title Train the Brain With Music: Brain Plasticity and Cognitive Benefits Induced by Musical Practice in Elderly People in Germany and Switzerland (TBM)
Countries of recruitment Switzerland, Germany
Health condition(s) or problem(s) studied Age-related cognitive decline
Intervention(s) Experimental group: intensive piano learning over 1 year
Active control group: musical culture learning over 1 year (history and listening)
Key inclusion and exclusion criteria Ages eligible for study: 64 to 78 years Sexes eligible for study: both, Accepts healthy volunteers: only
Inclusion criteria: Healthy right-handed volunteers, between 64 and 78 years of age, native French/German speakers. No regular musical practice over the lifespan. Only retired individuals may participate.
Exclusion criteria: Impaired/not-corrected auditory or visual accuracy, neurological diseases in the present or the past, cardiovascular diseases, excessive hypertension, obesity, diabetes mellitus, beginning dementia, mild cognitive impairment, clinical depression
Study type Interventional
Allocation: randomized intervention model according to 4 stratification factors (age, gender, score at the COGTEL (global cognition test) and socioeconomic status). Groups are balanced for those factors. Single blind study (subject)
Primary purpose: prevention of cognitive and sensorimotor decline in retired elderly
Date of first enrolment May 2018
Target sample size 150
Recruitment status Completed
Primary outcome(s) Positive transfer effects from intensive piano training (intervention group) - as compared to listening and learning about music without practice (control group) - on age-related cognitive decline: for working memory, executive functions, speed of information processing, auditory selective attention (hearing in noise) and abstract thinking. Associated functional and structural brain plasticity may show in gray and white matter in temporal (medial and lateral), prefrontal and parietal areas. Less decline or increase of gray matter volume and activation change may show in the hippocampus, as well as improved functional networking in frontal areas during working memory tasks.
Key secondary outcomes Benefits of fine perceptual-motor skills may also manifest in the intervention group as compared to the control group associated with functional and structural brain plasticity in sensorimotor areas. Subjective quality of life, verbal memory and hearing in noise may improve in both groups, the latter two associated with functional and structural brain plasticity in auditory and frontal areas. Epigenetic alterations may be induced by piano learning in relation with functional and structural imaging parameters.

Research hypothesis

We intend exhibiting for the first time that musical activities, particularly musical instrumental practice, can countervail cognitive and perceptual-motor decline supported by functional and structural brain plasticity.

Aims and objectives of the study

Our long-term aim is to promote healthy mental aging in elderly and therefore independence, autonomy and well-being through musical activities, specifically musical instrumental training of a complex instrument.

The preparatory phase of our study consisted in developing distinct musical interventions specifically designed for musically naive elderly aiming to support cognitive and sensorimotor development. Our study objective is investigating whether piano practice (intervention group) over one full year, as compared to listening and learning about music without practice (active control group), can provoke multiple far transfer effects on working memory, executive functions, speed of information processing, auditory selective attention (hearing in noise), abstract thinking, manual dexterity and bimanual coordination, all of which are strongly involved in piano practice and most relevant for independence and autonomy in everyday life. In association with these behavioral benefits we anticipate functional and structural brain plasticity in temporal (medial and lateral), prefrontal and parietal areas and the basal ganglia. We foresee potential benefits in both groups for verbal memory, hearing in noise and well-being, with more moderate cerebral plasticity in prefrontal, auditory and emotion regulation areas.

The ultimate goal, if our study results confirm our hypotheses, is systematic implementation of government-funded musical practice facilities in activity centers for elderly and nursing homes.

Design

The study is a two-site Hannover-Geneva longitudinal randomized single blind intervention study in altogether ~ 155 retired healthy elderly (64–78) years, (63 in Geneva, 92 in Hannover, selected according to our inclusion/exclusion criteria out of ~ 500 applications), divided into 2 parallel groups, offering either piano training (intervention group) or musical sensitization training (active control group), both provided by professional musicians.

The active control group serves to control for music listening of similar styles, acquiring general knowledge, social interaction, attentional aspects, weekly instruction away from home, and daily homework.

All participants were informed before enrolling that the study aim is to compare two distinct music interventions and that both may have positive impact on cognitive functioning and brain plasticity (single blind procedure).

In order to prevent strong inhomogeneity between the groups, we use a blocked (“stratified”) randomized design, in order to prevent strong inhomogeneity between the groups at baseline. Stratification involved the factors age, gender, score at the COGTEL1 [95, 97] and socioeconomic status. After recruiting all participants, we applied a clustering procedure, separately at each site, to obtain the closest pairs of participants based on the stratification factors. We transformed the latter to z-scores and computed the Euclidian distance between participants, and finally, an algorithm attributed randomly 1 participant of each pair to each group. So, half of the participants ended up in the intervention group, and the other half in the control group at both sites.

Over 12 months participants receive weekly training for 1 hour (we aim at providing 40 courses over the 12 months, adapting to the holidays of the seniors), and also exercise at home for ~ 30 min per day. Participants committed not to miss more than maximum 8 courses on the full curriculum. Both groups study similar different music styles (classical music, world music, jazz, etc.). Subjects will be tested at 4 time points (0, 6, 12 months, and post-training at 18 months) on cognitive, perceptual and motor abilities (Behavioral Test Battery; Table 4), as well as via wide-ranging functional and structural neuroimaging assessments (Table 5).

Table 4.

Behavioral Battery (psychometric tests) (measured at all 4 time points: t0: baseline; t1: 6 months; t2: 12 months & t3: 18 months)

BASIC COGNITION
Processing speed & Attention Digit-symbol WAIS-IV, Wechsler, 2011 [98]
Short term memory Forward Digit Span WAIS IV, Wechsler, 2011
Backward Digit Span WAIS IV, Wechsler, 2011
EXECUTIVE CONTROL
- Shifting Number switch Zuber et al., 2016 [99]
Perceptual switch + Prospective Memory Zuber et al., 2016
- Inhibition GoDigit -NoGo Enge et al., 2014 [100]
Stop-Signal Task Enge et al., 2014
FLUID INTELLIGENCE
Logical reasoning Matrices test WAIS IV, Wechsler, 2011
KNOWLEDGE
Episodic memory Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Bean, 2011 [101]; Rey, 1964 [102]; Helmstaedter et al., 2001 [103]
MUSIC COGNITION
Musical aptitude

GOLD MSI (melody, rhythm)

Harmony test (“Pietri”)

Müllensiefen, 2014 [92]

Oechslin et al., 2013 [5]

AUDITIVE COGNITION
Speech-in-noise perception International Matrix Test (Oldenburg) Kollmeier et al., 2015 [104]
PERCEPTUAL MOTOR SKILL
Visuo-manual aptitude Purdue Pegboard Tiffin and Asher, 1948 [105]
Scale playing on keyboard Scale analysis Jabusch et al., 2004 [106]
OTHER
WHOQOL-BREFa Well-being WHO, 2014b
Physical Activity Scale for Elderly PASE (last two weeks activities) Washburn, 1993 [107]

aWorld Health Organization Quality of Life-BREF (BREF for abbreviated)

bhttps://www.who.int/mental_health/media/en/76.pdf

Table 5.

MRIa measurements (measured at all 4 time points: t0: baseline; t1: 6 months; t2: 12 months & t3: 18 months)

TYPE OF MRI MEASUREMENT / SCAN Main technical details of the Siemens protocols
Grey matter volume assessment [108] MP2RAGEb; duration: 8.22 min; voxel size: 1 mm isotropic; 176 slices; FOVc: 256x240x176 mm; TRd/TEe: 5000/2.98 ms; TI1f/TI2g: 700/2500 ms; flip angle 1/2: 4/5 degrees

Functional MRI

Tonal working memory task [109]

EPIh; duration: 20.00 min; voxel size: 2.5 mm isotropic; 54 slices; FOV: 210x210x135 mm; multiband accelerator factor: 3; TR/TE: 1350/31.6 ms

Resting state functional MRI

Allows to measure activity in the Default Mode Network (DMN) reflecting global functional connectivity of the brain [110]

EPI; duration: 10.31 min; voxel size: 2.5 mm isotropic; 54 slices; FOV: 210x210x135 mm; multiband accelerator factor: 3; TR/TE: 1350/31.6 ms

White matter assessment

Allows to compute i.a. Fractional Anisotropy for evaluating white matter integrity reflecting structural connectivity [111].

DTIi; duration 7.38 min; voxel size: 1.5 mm isotropic; 84 slices; FOV: 222x222x126 mm; multiband accelerator factor: 3; TR/TE: 5163/109.2 ms; b-values: 0/1500 s/mm2; 65 directions, anterior to posterior phase encoding direction followed by a reverse direction for distortion correction; b-values: 0/ s/mm2)
Blood perfusion assessment [112] ASLj; duration 6.10 min; voxel size: 2.5 × 2.5 × 3.0 mm; 36 slices; FOV: 220x220x129 mm; TR/TE: 3640/21.0 ms; flip angle: 90 degrees

a MRI: Magnetic Resonance Imaging

b MP2RAGE: Magnetization-Prepared 2 Rapid Acquisition Gradient Echoes

c FOV: Field Of View

d TR: Time to Repetition

e TE: Time to Echo

f TI1: Inversion Time 1

g TI2; Inversion Time 2

h EPI: Echo Planar Imaging

i DTI: Diffusion Tensor Imaging

j ASL: Arterial Spin Labeling

Our research is inspired by Bugos and colleagues [13], but uses an extended methodology: 1) an active (vs passive) control group also learning music, 2) longer training (12 vs 6 months), 3) a narrower age bracket (64–78 vs 60–85 years of age), 4) extensive inclusion and exclusion criteria (for instance no prior musical education for more than 6 months (vs 5 years), 5) no major health problems, 6) a greater number of participants (150 vs 31), thus increased statistical power, and 7) more comprehensive psychometric testing involving cognitive, auditory, musical, sensorimotor and well-being measures. In addition, our study includes 8) comprehensive demographic, cognitive reserve index, sport leisure activities, well-being and food frequency questionnaires, as well as 9) multimodal (f)MRI brain imaging. Then 10) we also apply blood sampling, in order to perform genetic and epigenetic analyses with respect to music training and learning. Finally, 11) the piano groups will be trained in dyads, as group interaction is supposed to be specifically effective for learning in older adults [113].

Setting

In Geneva (GE) all music courses and passage of the behavioral test battery take place at the Geneva School of Health Sciences (Haute Ecole de Santé de Genève (HEdS-GE) of the University of Applied Sciences and Arts Western Switzerland (HES-SO)). In Hannover (HA), Germany, music courses and passage of the behavioral test battery take place at the Hannover University of Music Drama and Media (HMTMH: Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover), and its Institute for Music Physiology and Musicians’ Medecine (IMMM: Institut für Musikphysiologie und Musikermedizin).

Professional musicians provide the music lessons, some of which are finishing their Master studies at the Geneva University of Music (Haute Ecole de Musique de Genève, HEM-GE) or at the HMTMH, but all already possess a Bachelor degree. Others are already established music/piano teachers and performers with a recently obtained Master’s degree. All music teachers possess several years of teaching experience prior to our study and were prepared to teach our specific population of elderly under the supervision of a team of music education specialists: Prof. Thomas Bolliger of the HEM-GE, and Prof. Wolfgang Zill and Prof. Andrea Welte in Hannover (HMTMH), who worked out a workflow together for both courses.

In Geneva, all brain imaging (MRI) is performed at the BBL, the Brain and Behavior Laboratory of the Geneva University. In Hannover all brain imaging takes place at the Hannover Medical school.

Registered nurses perform the blood sampling, in Geneva they are collaborators of the Geneva School of Health Sciences and in Hannover of the Hannover Medical School. Participants may refuse the blood sampling.

Participants

The following criteria were verified by means of initial phone calls, a comprehensive demographic questionnaire, questionnaires on musical activities, tests on major hearing impairment, tests on cognitive functioning and clinical depression. A comprehensive list of all (pre) screening tests, that ensured the criteria were met, can be found in Table 2.

Table 2.

Prescreening procedure in order to verify inclusion/exclusion criteria

PRESCREENINGa Action Data collection
First contact Telephone call; decision to proceed to Pre-screening Step 1 Age – gender – musical history – general health – native language – retirement – MRI compatibility – overall availability – contact information
PRE-SCREENING STEP 1
Demographic questionnaireb Send/receive by post In-house questionnaire, detailed information on languages; handedness; physical capability to come to the courses; health: hospitalizations (causes), locomotion, vision (correction), audition (hearing aids), height & weightc, sleep quality; cardiovascular & neurological diseases, hypertension, diabetes, arthrosis, arthritis, rheumatism, bone fractures, thyroid problems, memory problems, other health problems; consent yes/no to contact the family doctor in case of fortuitous findings; smoking habits over the lifespan; alcohol consumption over the lifespan; present medication; other drug use, remarks; sociodemographic information: age, gender, nationality, civil status, siblings, children, highest level of education (5 categories), main professional engagements, gross monthly household incomed (5 categories); MRI compatibility: claustrophobia, implants; Interest for the study: motivation
Food Frequency Questionnaire FFQe [91] Send/receive by post Detailed information on food consumption (quality and quantity) over the last 4 weeks
Music questionnaires Send/receive by post

Musical history (in-house questionnaire; GEMMI lab): amount and intensity of music listening and extracurricular music training: solfege/theory, instrumental training, choir singing, intracurricular music training in school

Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (GOLD MSI): self-report inventory and test battery for individual differences in musical sophistication (ability to engage with music) [92]

Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire [93] Send/receive by post Adapted to cover different parts of life (2–25 y.o., last 5 years and last year)
In-house questionnaire on stressful life events Send/receive by post Self-report inventory on stressful events that may influence cognitive functioning, covering 6 time periods (12–25 y.o., 25–55 y.o, 55–64 y.o.; last 10 years, 5 years and year; GEMMI lab)
Geriatric Depression Scalef [94] Send/receive by post Short version. Check for clinical depression
PRE-SCREENING STEP 2

First laboratory visit

- Basic clinical finger snap audition test

- Psychometric tests

i. Right-handedness

ii. COGTELh [95]

Verify /complete questionnaires of Step 1

Pass 3 psychometric tests

Check for major hearing impairmentg

Pass the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (revised) to check for right handedness [96]; Measure gross overall cognitive level and check for potential (mild) cognitive decline (COGTEL)

a Outliers (based on cut-off scores and or other exclusion criteria) for the tests of Pre-screening Step 1 & 2 are excluded from the study

b All questionnaires are written in French and German; their translation was thoroughly verified by two bilingual researchers

c Allows to compute the Body Mass Index (BMI), obese people are excluded (BMI > 30), as obesity impacts on brain (shrinkage) [133]

d Adapted to Switzerland, France and Germany

e Swiss Food Frequency Questionnaire, a semi-quantitative questionnaire existing in French and German, on 80 food items divided in 12 food groups

f Geriatric Depression Scale short version

g Refined information on hearing will be acquired during the Behavioral Battery, see Table 4 International Matrix Test

h The COGTEL is a test of global cognition applied by telephone or face-to-face (we applied face-to-face). This hybrid cognitive test, developed for older adults by the team of Prof. Matthias Kliegel, provides a global measure of cognition, based on several memory and executive function subtests

Inclusion Criteria: good overall health; between 64 and 78 years of age; native or fluent French or German speakers; right-handed (for brain organizational reasons [114]); no regular musical practice over the lifespan (< 6 months); retirement.

Exclusion criteria: impaired/not-corrected auditory or visual accuracy; neurological diseases in the present or the past; mild cognitive impairment or beginning dementia (assured via the COGTEL score); cardiovascular diseases; excessive hypertension; obesity; diabetes mellitus; clinical depression; MRI incompatibility (f.i. implants or claustrophobia). Concomitant musical training during the study is prohibited (see Trust Agreement; Table 3).

Table 3.

Documents: information & consent forms, agreements

TYPE OF DOCUMENT When What Action Who
Information on the study

During presentations; via flyers;

“participant information form” (distributed or sent by post or mail)

Objectives of the study; Information on the inclusion/exclusion criteria; information on procedures and timelines; announcement of randomization within two different groups of music education; information on measurements and their frequency (n = 4): MRI, behavioral testing and blood sampling; information of reimbursing of travel expenses; telephone numbers and mails of the research team Answer questions during presentations or by mail or telephone and during first lab visit Research team
Declaration of informed consent After inclusion in the study (following the 2 pre-screening steps see Table 2) Consent to voluntary participate to the study with the right to withdraw at any time; accept the use of data for scientific and educational purposes, with assurance that the data will remain anonymous; to yes/no accept blood sampling (NOT an exclusion criterion); who to inform in case of potential unexpected detection of anomalies. Read & Sign Participant & member research team
Trust agreement Before the courses start The participant declares aiming to perform homework at least 30 min per day, 5 times per week, not to miss more than maximum 8 courses (on 40 in total), and announce absences and holidays well in advance with reasons and not to engage in any supplementary musical education Read & Sign Participant & member research team
MRI safety questionnaire Before MRI measurements Questions assuring MRI compatibility Read & Sign Participant & MRI operator
Debriefing on the fMRI task After MRI measurements Answer questions on sound perception, comprehension of the task, rating of difficulty of different experimental conditions, used strategies to perform the task, remarks Read, answer questions Participant & member research team
Homework questionnaire 3, 6, 9 & 12 months after the beginning of the music interventions Time per day and per week spent on homework on average Read and -sign Participant

In Geneva, Professor Giovanni Frisoni of the Medical Faculty of the University of Geneva, neurologist, specialist in the field of dementia, agreed to consult us and to receive participants showing signs of risk of mild cognitive impairment. In addition, Dr. Philippe Schaller, medical doctor, Head of Cité Générations, specialist in chronic diseases, agreed to consult us concerning general health issues of our elderly population.

In Hannover, Prof Eckart Altenmüller (neurologist) and Prof. Tillmann Krüger (medical doctor specialized in geriatric medicine and psychiatry), investigators on the study, will cover these issues.

Different documents: information & consent forms and agreements, were read and or signed by our participants, they are listed in Table 3.

Consent to voluntary participate to the study involved: the right to withdraw at any time, accepting the use of the collected data for scientific and educational purposes –with the assurance that the data will remain anonymous-, to accept blood sampling or not -on a voluntary basis- and finally, whether to inform the participant or his or her treating physician in the event of unexpected potential detection of abnormalities.

Interventions

Music interventions

Intervention group: piano courses

Courses take place in dyads (2 participants together). Three Yamaha keybords P-45 B2 are set up in the classroom, with height adaptable piano stools. A Yamaha keybord P-45 B and a height adaptable piano stool are provided to each participant for use at home. During the courses the teacher sits in the middle, and in the beginning imitation and listening exercises are performed. Most of them are playful and allow the participants to familiarize themselves with the keyboard and adopt a correct and relaxed body posture. Clapping and singing or even walking within a certain rhythm are part of the lessons. Progressively music reading is introduced using a method specifically developed for our elderly population based on Jens Schlichting “Piano Prima Vista” (Internote GmbH Musikverlag 2013), and the Hall Leonard piano method for adults (ISBN: 9789043134378 & 9789043152037). Both methods exist in German and French. This material is enriched by different other pieces out of different textbooks (“A dozen a Day volume 1 (ISBN 9780711954311); Bastien Piano Basics - Piano Level 1 (ISBN-10: 0849752663; Manfred Schmitz Jugend-album für Klavier (ISBN: 978-3-932587-41-2), etc.), and sometimes by transcriptions of a favorite piece of one of the participants, arranged by the music teachers. Individual instruction is alternated with playing together, accompanied by the teachers. Sometimes one participant plays the right-hand part, while the other plays the left and vice versa. Homework includes practicing the exercises, the pieces and also occasionally composing music to perform during the course. A certain choice is left to the participants concerning the repertoire. Some improvisation is also introduced in association with basic notions of tonality and chords. The relationship between the teachers and the participants is informal.

Control group: musical culture3

Courses take place in groups of 4 to 6 individuals in order to create a stimulating environment. Lessons involve learning about the timbre and functioning of different musical instruments, music history, learning about composers, styles, etc. Presented music includes medieval, renaissance, classical, romantic and also modern classical repertoire, comprising orchestra and chamber music, opera, but also world music of all 4 continents and other styles (for instance music from the south Americas, jazz, pop, funk, etc.). During the highly interactive courses, the teacher explains the structure of the pieces, the role of the different instruments and voices and affective aspects of the pieces and develops a structured way of listening (“auditory analysis”) among the participants. During the lessons music related vocabulary may be written down, reassembling those terminologies in a notebook is part of the homework. Each week a voluntary participant presents music of his/her choice. The support material consists of extracts from textbooks, internet pages, documentaries, radio broadcasts, and presentations (f.i. “TED talks”, ted.com) and links for listening mainly of YouTube videos and audio material. Internet links and other materials for the upcoming course are provided in the week preceding the lessons. Homework involves resuming the course, listening to different musical pieces, reading texts and preparing presentations. Participants are continuously encouraged to express why he/she appreciates, or not, particular pieces or styles of music.

Measurements and procedures

Subjects will be tested at 4 time points (t0: baseline, t1: after 6 months, t2: after training completion (12 months), t3: 6 months after training completion (18 months)) on 1) cognitive, perceptual and sensorimotor abilities by means of a battery of psychometric tests (Table 4) as well as via 2) wide-ranging functional and structural neuroimaging data (Magnetic Resonance Imaging MRI; see Table 5 and 3) Blood sampling.

Psychometric testing

A complete list of the psychometric tests is provided in Table 4.

Test-retest effects4

Test-retest effects are in principle corrected for by the existence of a control group.

However, most tests are modified from time point to time point, by

  • Using different test material (stimuli) at each time point (COGTEL, Matrices test; Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Harmony test (“Pietri”); GOLD MSI, International Matrix Test)

  • Changing the order of the stimuli (Number Switch, Perceptual switch + Prospective Memory, Go-NoGo, Stop-Signal Task).

Only the tests: Digit-symbol and Digit Span forward and backward, and the sensorimotor tests Purdue Pegboard and Scale Analysis are repeated with an identical format at all time points. The questionnaire WHOQOL-BREF on subjective well-being is also repeated in identical format.

Finally, within time-points, the order of the tests is pseudo-randomized between individuals (as well as the lists of words in the International Matrix Tests).

(f)MRI measurements

Nota bene: the MRI measurements will not require any special preparation (such as having to drink or be injected with contrast materials or radioactive dye).

In Geneva the MRI measurements are performed on the imaging platform at the Brain and Behaviour Laboratory and benefit from support of the technical staff (BBL, http://bbl.unige.ch/), situated at the University Medical Centre of the University of Geneva, at close proximity to the HEdS-GE; with consent of the Director of the BBL, Patrik Vuilleumier, Full Professor at the Medical Faculty of the University of Geneva, neurologist and also Head of the Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology and Imaging of Cognition.

At both sites, 3 T Siemens scanners are used, however In Geneva acquisitions are performed on a 3 T whole body Siemens Trio system (Siemens TIM-TRIO, Erlangen, Germany) whereas In Hannover acquisitions are performed on a 3 T whole body Siemens Skyra system (Siemens MAGNETOM Skyra, Erlangen). At both sites, the scanners are equipped with an identical standard Siemens 32-channel head coil.

We adapted the sequences parameters, opting for a compromise between a good signal to noise ratio and a minimum amount of time. For the functional acquisitions, we adopted a multiband echo planar imaging fMRI with an accelerator factor of 3, allowing us to reduce the repetition time to 1.350 s which represents a good temporal resolution for fMRI (see Table 5). As a consequence of these choices, we could reduce the total duration of the MRI scanning to less than 1 hour, which is important given our elderly population.

The fact that 3 T Siemens scanners will be used at both sites already favors blending the data [115]. However, the challenge was to end up with matching MRI sequence parameters at both sites, allowing to pool the data with a minimum of post-hoc adaptations. We managed to end up with identical sequence parameters (see Table 5). Nevertheless, in all analyses, a “scanner” covariate will be included in order to disentangle scanner-effect from effects-of-interest.

A complete list of the different MRI measurements (n = 5) and main protocol parameters at both sites is provided in Table 5.

fMRI tonal working memory task

We use an in-scanner tonal working memory task (courtesy of Prof. Robert Zatorre and Dr. Philippe Albouy) [109]. We added a control condition to the original task that now comprises 3 experimental conditions, which can be used later on in different combinations for the fMRI contrasts in the analyses.

In all 3 conditions, 2 pitch patterns of 3 beeps (sinus tones, with a certain tone frequency or pitch) are played via MRI compatible earphones, with a visual number code displayed on the computer screen in between the 2 pitch patterns. In all conditions, the participant responds by pressing one of two buttons on a response pad (left vs. right).

In the simple condition, 2 pitch patterns with 3 different pitches are played, thus forming kind of a melody, the first always followed by the visual number code 123 that represents the order of the notes of this first pitch pattern (1 for the 1rst note, 2 for the 2nd note and 3 for the 3rd note). The task of the participant is thus to respond whether the second pattern was identical to the first. If the order of the second pitch pattern is indeed identical, he/she should respond “correct” (left button). If the second pitch pattern is different (different order of the notes), they should respond “incorrect” (right button).

In the complex condition the trial sequence is the same, but the visual number code is never 123. It can be 321, 312, 132, 231 or 213 (all combinations except 123) meaning that there will be a specific change in the order of the notes between the first and the second pitch pattern. The task of the participant is to determine, whether the visual number code, that represents the manipulation of the order of the notes of the first pitch pattern, corresponds to the order of the notes of the second pattern. For instance if the tonal pattern is reversed, i.e. the notes are played in reversed order in the second pattern, and the visual number code provides the numbers 321, he/she should respond “correct” (left button), if the order of the notes of second pitch pattern does not correspond to the visual number code, they should respond “incorrect” (right button).

In the control condition participants listen to a pitch pattern consisting of 3 beeps of identical frequency/pitch, followed by a visual number code of 3 identical numbers, either 111, 222 or 333. They then receive an instruction on the computer screen whether they have to push the right or the left button of the response pad.

Bloodsampling

In addition, we collect one blood sample (10 ml) of each consenting participant (until now most people consented), close to each MRI measurement (t0, t1, t2, t3), for analyzing genotypes (only once) and methylation status (all times) of specific neuronal plasticity and learning related parameters (e.g. brain derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), dopamine, sertonine, protein phosphatase 1, reelin and some others).

Procedures

A Flowchart of the study procedures with timelines is provided in Table 6.

Table 6.

Study procedure with timelines

RECRUITEMENT & PRE-screening
 Months Stage What Where Action Duration
 MONTH 1–3 Preparatory phase

Recruitment of participants (n = ~ 70 in GE; n = ~ 100 in HA, on a total of ~  500 applications) according to exclusion/exclusion criteria

Detailed preparation of Behavioral Battery (psychometric testing) &

(f)MRI protocols

Local journals/

elderly journals/

flyers/presentations

HEdS-GE2 & IMMM3

BBL5 / HMS6

Announcements (press & flyers)

Presentations1/1rst check by telephone

Creation of E-prime4 programs; preparation of paper and pencil tests;

Synchronization of protocols between MRI scanners in Geneva and Hannover

 Month 4 Piloting of (f)MRI protocols (N = 20) & Behavioral Battery (N = 10) on non-included elderly individuals BBL / HEdS-GE / HMS / IMMM Feasibility testing
Pre-screening Step 1 See Table 2 At home Send/receive (mail/post) 60 min.
 Month 5–6 Pre-screening Step 2 (N GE = 63, N HA = 92)

Check/completion of questionnaires of Step 1

Basic clinical audition test

Psychometric tests

i. Right-handedness

ii. COGTEL

HEdS-GE/ IMMM Come to HEdS-GE, IMMM 1x

20 min.

2 min.

5 min.

15 min.

After inclusion in the study
 Month 7–10

Recruitment and training of music teachers

Preparing of music education materials

t0/baseline

i. Behavioral Battery (psychometric testing)

ii. (f)MRI measurements

iii. Blood sampling (by registered nurse)

HEdS-GE / IMMM

BBL / HMS

HEdS-GE / IMMM

Come to HEdS-GE, IMMM 1x

Come to BBL, HMS 1x

Come to HEdS-GE, HMS 1x

2.5 h7

1.5 h

10 min.

 Month 10–22 Interventions

12 months of 1 h weekly music courses &

homework

HEdS-GE / IMMM

At home

HEdS-GE / IMMM

Practice daily at home

1 h/week

5 × 30 min./week

 MONTH 16 t1/ after 6 months of intervention

i. Behavioral Battery (psychometric testing)

ii. (f)MRI measurements

iii. Blood sampling

HEdS-GE / IMMM

BBL / HMS

HEdS-GE / HMS

Come to HEdS-GE, IMMS 1x

Come to BBL, HMS 1x

2.5 h

1.5 h

10 min.

 Month 22 t2/directly after intervention completion

i. Behavioral Battery (psychometric testing)

ii. COGTEL

iii. (f)MRI measurements

iv. Blood sampling

v. Food Frequency Questionnaire

vi. Cognitive reserve index1 (CRIq)8 [116]

HEdS-GE / IMMM

HEdS-GE / IMMM

BB / HMS

HEdS-GE / HMS

At home

HEdS-GE / IMMM

Come to HEdS-GE, IMMM 1x

(same session as Behavioral Battery)

Come to BBL, HMS 1x

Come to HEdS-GE, HMS 1x

Send (mail/post)

Come to HEdS-GE, IMMM 1x

2.5 h

15 min.

1.5 h

10 min.

30 min.

15 min.

Month 23–26 Cope with Delays, Analyses & Publications, Congress Attending
Month 27 t3/6 months after intervention completion

i. Behavioral Battery (psychometric testing)

ii. (f)MRI measurements

iii. Blood sampling

HEdS-GE / IMMM

BBL / HMS

HEdS-GE / HMS

Come to HEdS-GE, IMMM 1x

Come to BBL, HMS 1x

Come to HEdS-GE, HMS1x

2.5 h

1.5 h

10 min.

Month 28–36 Cope with Delays, Analyses & Publications, Congress Attending

1Presentations at Cité Séniors Genève (http://www.ville-geneve.ch/themes/social/seniors/cite-seniors/); at the HEdS-GE2 and in Hannover at the HMTMH (Hannover University of Music Drama and Media)

2 HEdS-GE Haute école de Genève, 47 av. de Champel 47, 1206 Genève

3 Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians’ Medecine, Hannover, part of the HMTMH

4 Presentation soft-ware (https://pstnet.com/products/e-prime/)

5 Brain and Behaviour Laboratory (BBL, http://bbl.unige.ch/) , University Medical Centre of the University of Geneva

6 Hannover Medical School

7 During the Behavioral Battery several short breaks are included

8 This test has been added posteriorly

Additional questionnaires

Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq)

The CRIq [116] has been added posthoc to the testing. It measures cognitive reserve built up over the lifespan. It will be added to the Behavioral Battery at t2 (12 months, see Table 6). Cognitive reserve is the resilience to neuropathological damage in older adults, as a consequence of life-long learning and experiences.

Food frequency questionnaire FFQ

The FFQ [91] is used during the pre-screening. It consists of a semi-quantitative questionnaire existing in French and German, on 80 food items divided in 12 food groups. We want to investigate the potential links between quality and quantity of food intake, learning, cognitive functioning, general health and brain function and structure.

Progress measures

In order to evaluate the progress in both groups following training, we gather the following measures:

Piano group
  • Evaluation at 3, 6 and 12 months after intervention onset by the teachers, and at 3 and 12 months after intervention onset by professional musicians that do not follow the courses otherwise. The participants are evaluated on 7 different 5-point Likert scales, concerning 1) technique, 2) rhythm, 3) expressivity, 4) text compliance / music reading, 5) ensemble playing, 6) motivation / homework and 7) general progress.

  • A simplified version of Beethoven’s Ode to joy, to be played with both hands on the piano, is recorded (MIDI) after a 2 week preparation at 3 months, at 12 months (end of training) and at 18 months (6 months after course completion).

  • Slow up-and-down playing of a scale of 5 notes at a pace of 76 beats per minutes indicated by a metronome, with 1 note per beat, c-d-e-f-g (so no position change) at the central octave of the piano is recorded (MIDI) at t0, t1, t2 and t3.

  • Fast up-and-down playing of a scale of 5 notes at 76 beats per minute, with 2 notes per beat, c-d-e-f-g (so no position change) at the central octave of the piano is recorded (MIDI) at t0, t1, t2 and t3.

  • The “Genie test”, where participants press the same note (central C) 5 times in crescendo, i.e. increasing the loudness note after note, and 4 times in decrescendo, decreasing the loudness note after note at t0, t1, t2 and t3 (no rhythm to follow)

Musical culture / control group
  • Evaluation at 3, 6 and 12 months after intervention onset by the teachers, and at 3 and 12 months after intervention onset by professional musicians that do not follow the courses otherwise. The participants are evaluated on 6 different 5-point Likert scales, concerning 1) group ambiance, 2) individual participation, 3) interaction (individual), 4) joy (individual), 5) motivation / homework (individual), 6) general progress (individual).

  • A multiple choice questionnaire involving musical knowledge and listening is passed at t0, t1, t2 and t3

  • Slow up-and-down playing of a scale of 5 notes at a pace of 76 beats per minutes indicated by a metronome, with 1 note per beat, c-d-e-f-g (so no position change) at the central octave of the piano is recorded (MIDI) at t0, t1, t2 and t3.

Both groups also complete a training amount questionnaire at 3, 6, 9 and 12 months (see Table 3 “Homework questionnaire”) of intervention, indicating how much time per day and per week each participant spent on homework on average over the last 3 months. These progress and training amount measures will be linked in the final analyses to other brain and behavioral measures.

Data monitoring and quality assurance

At each site data acquisition and quality is constantly monitored by experimenters for each participant (Damien Marie and Laura Abdili in Geneva, Florian Worschech, Kristin Jünemann and Christopher Sinke in Hannover). Progress meetings (by Skype) are organized regularly between both sites. A second quality check is performed on the server where all data are backed up (see Declarations, Availability of data and materials, p. 16) and analyzed by Damien Marie (see Declarations p. 16). A third checkup is performed by an independant Data Manager (scientific collaborator at PhD level) both at the the HEdS-GE and the HMTMH, after each data point.

We will report reasons for withdrawal of individuals for each randomization group. Exact information on missing data at each time point will be reported in all publications. Missing data will be processed in the analyses with cutting edge means (for instance using the regularized iterative Principal Component Analysis (PCA) algorithm developed by Josse, Husson and Pagès (2009) [117], implemented in the R package missMDA.

Statistical analyses

First, we will study cross-sectionally all relationships between behavior and brain data collected at baseline (data from t0) for all participants (1) based on a priori hypotheses using general linear models, correlations and regression analyses; (2) Using data-driven multivariate approaches that allow to reveal more intricate relationships between the data.

Brain data will mostly be analyzed using in-house script routines calling the last versions of the standard neuroimaging software pipelines (SPM12, FSL6, FreeSurfer, MRtrix etc.), all running on the same operating system on our server (Linux xfce4.12) to avoid any potential operating system bias [118].

Second, we will examine and compare the development in both experimental groups over time (t0 vs. t1 vs. t2 vs. t3) for the comprehensive set of behavioral and brain data using univariate and multivariate methods. Individuals’ intensity of training, progress, demographic characteristics and post-hoc appreciation of the courses will be taken into account (implemented as co-variates).

Analyzing separately behavioral data and several kinds of brain imagery data cross-sectionally or over time provides valuable information on these specific data. However, blending diverse behavioral and different kinds of brain data within data-driven multivariate analyses may unravel hidden “covert” relationships [119, 120]. For these advanced analyses we will be assisted by Prof. D. Van De Ville, computer scientist; expert in advanced (f)MRI data analyses).

Detailed information on multivariate analyses

In close collaboration with Prof. Van De Ville and his team of data scientists, we will perform final analyses on our data using multivariate data-driven techniques such as multimodal Independent Component Analysis (ICA) [119, 120] and Partial Least of Squares (PLS) [121, 122], which have both been used successfully on multimodal brain imagery data. These techniques allow to (1) significantly reduce the dimensionality of the data (as compared to a voxel-by-voxel analysis), reducing multiple comparison correction; (2) improve the interpretability by identifying brain regions or networks that can be more easily related to known processes. Two types of methods will be used:

1) Joint, fusion, and linked Independent Component Analysis (ICA) [123, 124] represent several blends of multimodal extensions of ICA that allow combining different types of data such as functional and structural neuroimaging data. Depending on the type of ICA, weaker or stronger relationships between the modalities are assumed. In addition, we will also decompose the fMRI data into functional brain networks using innovation-driven co-activation patterns (iCAPs), a recent approach of dynamic functional connectivity [125] that can quantify temporal interactions between different networks. The results of these analyses should reveal interactions between different types of information and consequently allow us to investigate differences between our groups [119, 123].

2) Partial Least of Squares (PLS) is another powerful technique that can identify components of multivariate relationships between imaging and behavioral data [122, 126].

Implementations of these approaches (1 & 2) are publicly available in addition to a number of in-house extensions that allow using them in the most flexible way, including machine-learning approaches [127, 128].

Power analyses

No standardized computational tools exist for the power analysis of longitudinal neuroimaging studies [129]. A true longitudinal design provides increased statistical power reducing the confounding effects of between-subject variability [130]. Foreseeing an important attrition of 25% in our elderly population, we recruited ~ 30 participants per experimental and per control group in Geneva, and ~ 45 individuals per group in Hannover, so a total of 150 participants. Anticipating around 25% of attrition, final analysis will thus be performed in principle on more than 112 participants, ~ 56 in the experimental and ~ 56 in the control groups.

In order to acquire sufficient statistical power to reliably detect a 2% difference in Gray Matter volume [36] between 2 groups at the whole brain level, a longitudinal study design requires on average 38 subjects, equally divided between the 2 groups [131]. Colcombe and colleagues [81] found reliable frontal GM and WM (White Matter) differences after only 6 months of aerobic training in elderly with much smaller sample sizes (n total = 59, divided evenly over an experimental and a control group) than in this study. To detect reductions in mean tract FA (Fractional Anisotropy, reflects fiber density, measure resulting from Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI)) of 2–20%, sample sizes between 7 and 28 subjects are recommended for cross-sectional analyses for an effect size of 5%, depending on the white matter tracts studied [132], but longitudinal studies are more powerful.

Longitudinal intervention studies with 150 participants are rare. All the cited studies involved smaller sample size compared to the current study. Therefore, we consider to have opted for a fair compromise between feasibility of the study and number of participants.

Outcomes of the study

Primary outcomes

We expect positive transfer effects from intensive piano training (intervention group) - as compared to listening and learning about music without practice (control group) - over one full year on age-related cognitive decline: for working memory, executive functions, speed of information processing, auditory selective attention (hearing in noise) and abstract thinking. Associated functional and structural brain plasticity may show in gray and white matter in temporal (medial and lateral), prefrontal and parietal areas. We specifically anticipate - in connection with these cognitive changes - less decline or increase of gray matter volume and activation in the hippocampus, and improved functional networking in frontal areas during working memory tasks.

Secondary outcomes

Benefits for fine perceptual-motor skills may also manifest in the intervention group as compared to the control group associated with functional and structural brain plasticity in sensorimotor areas.

Tertiary outcomes

Subjective quality of life, verbal memory and hearing in noise may improve in both groups, the latter two associated with functional and structural brain plasticity in auditory and frontal areas.

Quaternary outcomes

Specific results of epigenetic changes may be induced by piano learning as a specific form of exercise and show a relationship with functional and structural imaging parameters.

Control outcome

Higher scores for the musical tests (GOLD MSI for rhythm and melody, Pietri task for harmony (see Table 4)) in the piano group as compared to control group.

Broader impact

If this applied research study is successful, it may serve as a springboard for the development and implementation of targeted music practice interventions for different types of healthy elderly and patients in the context of population aging and lifespan development, prolonging autonomy, serving public health and thus reducing health costs.

Study specific risks/ inconveniences

Risks of (f)MRI scanning

  1. (f)MRI scans are non-invasive and yield few potential risks because they do not involve any radiation, only a strong magnetic field. A strictly protocolled MRI safety procedure will be applied in both labs (Hannover: Department of Neuroradiology of the Hannover Medical School, Geneva: Brain and Behaviour Laboratory of the University of Geneva), verifying for any metal objects outside and inside the body, as well as for special conditions (pacemaker, etc.).

  2. People not comfortable with (f)MRI scanning will be excluded from the experiment.

  3. Full information will be provided on the (f)MRI technique beforehand, group visits of the MRI room will allow participants to verify whether they can cope with the MRI environment.

Coverage of damages: insurance

Geneva:

Insurance « Responsabilité civile » de la Haute école de santé de Genève.

AXA Winterthur Police no. 4.626.347

Sum of insurance CHF 10′000’000.- Fixed sum per event, including bodily injury, property damage and insured expenses.

Hannover:

Insurance: Alte Leipziger, Police no 30–660–104-204 FD. Sum of Insurance Euro 3′000’000 Fixed sum per event, including bodily injury, property damage and insured expenses.

Discussion

This multi-site study required harmonization between the Geneva and Hannover site concerning all musical interventions, tests, measurements and questionnaires.

We managed (see Table 4) to create an identical test battery at both sites, by choosing tests existing in French and German, but also assured by translations performed by several bilingual members of the team.

The musical education programs were also composed in very similar ways, assured by continuous exchange between the team of musical education experts.

Concerning MRI protocols, the challenge was to end up with corresponding MRI sequence parameters at both sites, allowing to pool the data with a mimimum of post-hoc adaptations. As 3 T Siemens scanners are present at both sites, blending the data is possible [115]. We managed to end up with identical sequence parameters (see Table 5). Even so, in all analyses, we will add a “scanner” covariate in order to disentangle potential scanner-effect from effects-of-interest.

Although some slight timing differences exists between the Geneva and Hannover sites, the protocols are identical and follow the same chronological path (see Table 6).

In this study, it is impossible for the experimenters to remain ignorant of group membership.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Prof. Wolfgang Zill (Hannover), Prof. Thomas Bolliger (Geneva) and Prof. Andrea Welte (Hannover) for their important input to the composition of the music courses; we also thank all music teachers.

Then we thank Yamaha Germany and Switzerland for the generous provision of the electronic keyboards.

We also would like to thank thank Professor Giovanni Frisoni, Director of the Memory Center of the Geneva University Hospitals for his support and advice concerning the study protocol, PD Dr. med. Markus Gschwind, senior physician in neurology of the Kantonsspital Aarau, Switzerland for his consulting on potentially abnormal brain scans, and Dr. Andreas Posada for his help composing the scripts of the computerized behavioral tasks.

Finally we thank our Master students on the project, David Müller, Cécile Mueller, Pauline Berthouzoz, Nadia Bersier and Cyrille Stucker for their contribution.

Study status

The study status is “ongoing”. The interventions, that will last 12 months, started in the 3rd week of February 2019 for a certain number of participants, but for organizational reasons (recruitment, number of rooms & number of teachers available at the same time), approximately 30% of the participants enrolled up-to 7–8 months later and will finish interventions in the Fall of 2020. So only at that time point, t2 data collection will be completed. The delayed measures (t3) will follow after 6 months.

Abbreviations

ASL

Arterial Spin Labeling

BMI

Body Mass Index

BBL

Brain and Behaviour Laboratory

(f)MRI

(functional) Magnetic Resonance Imaging

CRIq

Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire

DFG

Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft

DTI

Diffusion Tension Imaging

EPI

Echo-Planar Imaging

FA

Fractional Anisotropy

FFQ

Food Frequency Questionnaire

FOV

Field Of View

GE

Geneva (Switzerland)

GEMMI Lab

Geneva Musical Minds Laboratory

COGTEL

Cognitive Telephone Screening Instrument

GM

Grey matter

HA

Hannover (Germany)

HES-SO

Haute Ecole Spécialisée de Suisse occidentale

HEdS-GE

Haute école de santé de Genève

HEM-GE

Haute Ecole de Musique de Genève

HEPIA

Haute Ecole du Paysage, d’Ingénieurie et d’Architecture

HMTMH

Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover

ICA

Independent Component Analysis

IMMM

Institut für Musikphysiologie und Musikermedizin

iCAPs

Innovation-Driven Co-Activation PatternS

ISBN

International Standard Book Number

MP2RAGE

Magnetization-Prepared 2 Rapid Acquisition Gradient Echoes

PLS

Partial Least of Squares

RAID1

Redundant Array of Independent Disks

SNSF

Swiss National Science Foundation

T

Tesla

t0

Baseline measurement

t1

6-months measurement

t2

12-months measurement

t3

6-months post-intervention measurement

TI1

First inversion time

TI2

Second inversion Time 2

TE

Time of Echo

TED

Technology, Entertainment and Design

TR

Time of Repetition

VBM

Voxel Based Morphometry

WHOQOL-BREF

World Health Organization Quality of Life-BREF questionnaire

WM

White matter

Authors’ contributions

CEJ wrote a first version of this manuscript, based on a Lead Agency (LA) grant proposal submitted to the DFG (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) and SNSF (Swiss National Science Foundation) , written and submitted by EA and CEJ as main investigators. Detailed input to the LA grant proposal was provided by MK, DVDV and TK. MK and TK are co-investigators, DVDV is scientific partner (SNSF) of the LA project. Postdoc DM importantly contributed to the writing of this manuscript and also to the finalization of the study protocol, concerning psychometric testing, MRI protocols and MRI scanner synchronization between Hannover and Geneva. The finalization of the study protocol was performed in collaboration with FG, FW, DSS, AH, LA & KJ, after the acceptation of the grant. All authors have read and approved the submitted manuscript.

Authors’ information

The combined expertise of the 4 investigators Prof. E. Altenmüller (HA, neurologist and professional musician, expert in the domain of rehabilitation after stroke through music and musicians’ diseases), Prof. C. James (GE, cognitive psychologist, neuroscientist and professional musician, expert in the domain of brain and behavioral plasticity following musical training in healthy individuals), Prof. M. Kliegel (GE, developmental psychologist, aging expert, especially in cognitive training in the elderly) and Prof. T. Krüger (HA, medical doctor, expert in mental health and brain imaging, specialized in geriatric medicine), is further supported by Prof. D. Van De Ville (GE, computer scientist; expert in advanced (f)MRI data analyses), Dr. C. Sinke (HA), Dr. Damien Marie (GE) (both neuroscientists and MRI specialists). The team of experts in musical pedagogy consists of Prof. T. Bolliger (GE), Prof. W. Zill (HA) and Prof. A Welte (HA). This team comprehensively covers all necessary knowledge and skills to optimally perform and disseminate the study.

Funding

German Research Foundation / Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG no. 323965454)

Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF no. 100019E-170410)

Role of the DFG and SNSF: both funding bodies are governmental/federal instances for high level research and apply external peer review by several independent experts. Annual detailed financial reports and a detailed final scientific report are required. These funding bodies played no role in the design of the study and will not have any role during its execution, analyses, interpretation of the data, or decision to submit results.

Yamaha Germany and Switzerland generously provided us with 90 electronic keyboards for the full duration of the study, garantying the research team entire academic freedom. This funding body played no role in the design of the study and will not have any role during its execution, analyses, interpretation of the data, or decision to submit results.

Availability of data and materials

As data collection is ongoing, measured GE and HA data are temporarily stored at a 18 To secured server hosted in a secured datacenter at the HES-SO Geneva (HEPIA: Haute Ecole du Paysage, d’Ingénieurie et d’Architecture), accessible to only a part of GE and HA team members via a double authentication VPN (first authentication through user password triggers the receipt of a second password by text message on the personal phone of the user) and encrypted ssh connection.

The server back-up system has two levels. The first level concerns the hardware storage that is set up in RAID1 (Redundant Array of Independent Disks), each data file is duplicated on two separate hard-drives to prevent losing data because of a hard-drive failure. The second level consists of an encrypted copy of all data in another third-party datacenter following research ethics guidelines (automatic process performed every 3 h)

The server will also be used for all analyses (Intel Xeon Gold 5115 2.4 GHz processor, 2 sockets * 10 cores * 2 Threads = 40 threads, 4*32Go RDIMM 2666MT/s Dual Rank126 Go of RAM)

As soon as the data collection will be completed, all data and all other documents will also be stored at YARETA, a FAIR digital solution for long-term preservation of research data for all Geneva Universities (https://yareta.unige.ch). The HEdS-GE already possesses an organizational unit on the YARETA platform.

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available at present due to the fact that 1) the data are only partially collected, and 2) will be published by our research team first, but they will be available in the future from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Measured data are anonymized, the code system for the anonymization of the participants is only preserved at the respective GE and HA Institutes, secured by unique passwords, on 1 computer and 1 hard drive at each site, only accessible to Eckart Altenmüller, Clara James, Damien Marie & Christopher Sinke (Postdoctoral fellows GE & HA), Florian Worschech (PhD student HA) and one other PhD student (HA).

Ethics approval and consent to participate

In Geneva, the full protocol including MRI was approved by the Commission cantonale d’éthique de la recherche de Genève (CCER; no. 2016–02224) on 22.05.2017 (protocol version 3), amendment no. 1 on 27.02.2018 (additional blood sampling; protocol version 4) and amendment no. 2 on 21.01.2020 (increased number of participants, completed measurements, information on data storage, up to date Timelines; protocol version 5). Protocol versions 4 & 5 conserved the CCER number 2016–02224.

In Hannover, the Ethikkomission of the Leibniz Universität Hannover (LUH) approved the whole protocol on 14.08.2017 (no. 3604–2017), the Hannover Medical School consecutively approved the MRI part separately on 29.08.2017 and additional blood sampling as an amendment on 07.03.2018, conserving the LUH number (3604–2017).

Retrospectively registered on 17.09.2018 (clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT03674931, study no. 81185), some participants were already enrolled.

Consent for publication

All participants signed a declaration of informed consent to participate in the study and accepted by their signature that the obtained data could be used for publication, under the condition that data will remain anonymous (see Table 3).

Competing interests

The authors declare that there are no competing interests.

Footnotes

1

Cognitive Telephone Screening Instrument. This cognitive screening instrument is a test of global cognition that can be applied by telephone or face-to-face (we applied face-to-face; see Table 2).

2

The keyboards were generously offered by Yamaha Germany and Yamaha Switzerland. Their generous gift included instruments for the classrooms and for each piano group participant to take home.

3

Culture musicale (GE; French nomenclature) / Musik erleben und verstehen (HA; German nomenclature)

4

Test-retest effects are learning effects between time points of data collection

Table 1 (p. 6) depicts the WHO (World Health Organization) Trial Registration Set, to serve as a brief structured summary of the study.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Gaser C, Schlaug G. Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. J Neurosci. 2003;23(27):9240–9245. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Herholz SC, Zatorre RJ. Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior, function, and structure. Neuron. 2012;76(3):486–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.James CE, Oechslin MS, Van De Ville D, Hauert CA, Descloux C, Lazeyras F. Musical training intensity yields opposite effects on grey matter density in cognitive versus sensorimotor networks. Brain Struct Funct. 2014;219(1):353–366. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0504-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Oechslin MS, Descloux C, Croquelois A, Chanal J, Van De Ville D, Lazeyras F, et al. Hippocampal volume predicts fluid intelligence in musically trained people. Hippocampus. 2013;23(7):552–558. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Oechslin MS, Van De Ville D, Lazeyras F, Hauert CA, James CE. Degree of musical expertise modulates higher order brain functioning. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23(9):2213–2224. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Schulze K, Mueller K, Koelsch S. Neural correlates of strategy use during auditory working memory in musicians and non-musicians. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33(1):189–196. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07470.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brown S, Martinez MJ, Parsons LM. Music and language side by side in the brain: a PET study of the generation of melodies and sentences. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;23(10):2791–2803. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04785.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Francois C, Grau-Sanchez J, Duarte E, Rodriguez-Fornells A. Musical training as an alternative and effective method for neuro-education and neuro-rehabilitation. Front Psychol. 2015;6:475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Oechslin MS, Gschwind M, James CE. Tracking training-related plasticity by combining fMRI and DTI: the right hemisphere ventral stream mediates musical syntax processing. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(4):1209–1218. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hyde KL, Lerch J, Norton A, Forgeard M, Winner E, Evans AC, et al. The effects of musical training on structural brain development: a longitudinal study. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1169:182–186. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04852.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.James CE, Oechslin MS, Michel CM, De Pretto M. Electrical neuroimaging of music processing reveals mid-latency changes with level of musical expertise. Front Neurosci. 2017;11(613):613. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.James CE, Zuber S, Dupuis-Lozeron E, Abdili L, Gervaise D, Kliegel M. Formal string instrument training in a class setting enhances cognitive and sensorimotor development of primary school children. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:567. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Bugos JA, Perlstein WM, McCrae CS, Brophy TS, Bedenbaugh PH. Individualized piano instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2007;11(4):464–471. doi: 10.1080/13607860601086504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Seinfeld S, Figueroa H, Ortiz-Gil J, Sanchez-Vives MV. Effects of music learning and piano practice on cognitive function, mood and quality of life in older adults. Front Psychol. 2013;4:810. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Verghese J, Lipton RB, Katz MJ, Hall CB, Derby CA, Kuslansky G, et al. Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. N Engl J Med. 2003;348(25):2508–2516. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa022252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Dege F, Kerkovius K. The effects of drumming on working memory in older adults. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2018. p. 242–50. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 17.Baltes PB, Baltes MM. Psychological perspectives on successful aging: the model of selective optimization with compensation. In: Baltes PB, Baltes MM, editors. Successful aging: perspectives from the behavioral sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990. pp. 1–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lindenberger U. Human cognitive aging: corriger la fortune? Science. 2014;346(6209):572–578. doi: 10.1126/science.1254403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Grady C. The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(7):491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Reuter-Lorenz PA, Park DC. Human neuroscience and the aging mind: a new look at old problems. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2010;65(4):405–415. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbq035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zinke K, Zeintl M, Rose NS, Putzmann J, Pydde A, Kliegel M. Working memory training and transfer in older adults: effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Dev Psychol. 2014;50(1):304–315. doi: 10.1037/a0032982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zinke K, Zeintl M, Eschen A, Herzog C, Kliegel M. Potentials and limits of plasticity induced by working memory training in old-old age. Gerontology. 2012;58(1):79–87. doi: 10.1159/000324240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Baltes PB, Reese HW, Lipsitt LP. Life-span developmental psychology. Annu Rev Psychol. 1980;31:65–110. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.000433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Willis SL, Schaie KW. Cognitive training and plasticity: theoretical perspective and methodological consequences. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2009;27(5):375–389. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2009-0527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Li SC. Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: the interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychol Bull. 2003;129(2):171–194. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hedden T, Gabrieli JD. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Raz N, Yang YQ, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Lindenberger U, Ghisletta P. White matter deterioration in 15 months: latent growth curve models in healthy adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(2):429 e421–429 e425. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Fennema-Notestine C, McEvoy LK, Hagler DJ, Holland D, et al. One-year brain atrophy evident in healthy aging. J Neurosci. 2009;29(48):15223–15231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3252-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Voss MW, Heo S, Prakash RS, Erickson KI, Alves H, Chaddock L, et al. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(11):2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wen W, Zhu W, He Y, Kochan NA, Reppermund S, Slavin MJ, et al. Discrete neuroanatomical networks are associated with specific cognitive abilities in old age. J Neurosci. 2011;31(4):1204–1212. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4085-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Grady CL, Protzner AB, Kovacevic N, Strother SC, Afshin-Pour B, Wojtowicz M, et al. A multivariate analysis of age-related differences in default mode and task-positive networks across multiple cognitive domains. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20(6):1432–1447. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zonneveld HI, Pruim RH, Bos D, Vrooman HA, Muetzel RL, Hofman A, et al. Patterns of functional connectivity in an aging population: the Rotterdam study. NeuroImage. 2019;189:432–444. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bergmann HC, Rijpkema M, Fernandez G, Kessels RP. Distinct neural correlates of associative working memory and long-term memory encoding in the medial temporal lobe. NeuroImage. 2012;63(2):989–997. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.James C, Morand S, Barcellona-Lehmann S, Michel CM, Schnider A. Neural transition from short- to long-term memory and the medial temporal lobe: a human evoked-potential study. Hippocampus. 2009;19(4):371–378. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Reuben A, Brickman AM, Muraskin J, Steffener J, Stern Y. Hippocampal atrophy relates to fluid intelligence decline in the elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS. 2011;17(1):56–61. doi: 10.1017/S135561771000127X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Fotuhi M, Do D, Jack C. Modifiable factors that alter the size of the hippocampus with ageing. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8(4):189–202. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Woodard JL, Sugarman MA, Nielson KA, Smith JC, Seidenberg M, Durgerian S, et al. Lifestyle and genetic contributions to cognitive decline and hippocampal structure and function in healthy aging. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9(4):436–446. doi: 10.2174/156720512800492477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Groussard M, La Joie R, Rauchs G, Landeau B, Chetelat G, Viader F, Desgranges B, Eustache F, Platel H. When music and long-term memory interact: effects of musical expertise on functional and structural plasticity in the hippocampus. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):1–8. 10.1371/journal.pone.0013225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 40.Herdener M, Esposito F, di Salle F, Boller C, Hilti CC, Habermeyer B, et al. Musical training induces functional plasticity in human hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2010;30(4):1377–1384. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4513-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.James CE, Britz J, Vuilleumier P, Hauert CA, Michel CM. Early neuronal responses in right limbic structures mediate harmony incongruity processing in musical experts. NeuroImage. 2008;42(4):1597–1608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Green CS, Bavelier D. Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychol Aging. 2008;23(4):692–701. doi: 10.1037/a0014345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Green CS, Strobach T, Schubert T. On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychol Res. 2014;78(6):756–72. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288(18):2271–2281. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.18.2271. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Engvig A, Fjell AM, Westlye LT, Moberget T, Sundseth O, Larsen VA, et al. Effects of memory training on cortical thickness in the elderly. NeuroImage. 2010;52(4):1667–1676. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kirchhoff BA, Anderson BA, Barch DM, Jacoby LL. Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22(4):788–799. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.James CE. Music and language processing share behavioral and cerebral features. Front Psychol. 2012;3:52. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Flaugnacco E, Lopez L, Terribili C, Montico M, Zoia S, Schon D. Music training increases phonological awareness and Reading skills in developmental dyslexia: a randomized control trial. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0138715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Francois C, Schon D. Musical expertise boosts implicit learning of both musical and linguistic structures. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(10):2357–2365. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Turker S, Reiterer SM, Seither-Preisler A, Schneider P. "when music speaks": auditory cortex morphology as a neuroanatomical marker of language aptitude and musicality. Front Psychol. 2017;8:2096. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Seither-Preisler A, Parncutt R, Schneider P. Size and synchronization of auditory cortex promotes musical, literacy, and attentional skills in children. J Neurosci. 2014;34(33):10937–10949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5315-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.James CE, Michel CM, Britz J, Vuilleumier P, Hauert CA. Rhythm evokes action: early processing of metric deviances in expressive music by experts and laymen revealed by ERP source imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(12):2751–2767. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Oechslin MS, Imfeld A, Loenneker T, Meyer M, Jancke L. The plasticity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus as a function of musical expertise: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2009;3:76. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.076.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Schneider P, Scherg M, Dosch HG, Specht HJ, Gutschalk A, Rupp A. Morphology of Heschl's gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(7):688–694. doi: 10.1038/nn871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sluming V, Brooks J, Howard M, Downes JJ, Roberts N. Broca's area supports enhanced visuospatial cognition in orchestral musicians. J Neurosci. 2007;27(14):3799–3806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0147-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Imfeld A, Oechslin MS, Meyer M, Loenneker T, Jancke L. White matter plasticity in the corticospinal tract of musicians: a diffusion tensor imaging study. NeuroImage. 2009;46(3):600–607. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bengtsson SL, Nagy Z, Skare S, Forsman L, Forssberg H, Ullen F. Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(9):1148–1150. doi: 10.1038/nn1516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Schulze K, Zysset S, Mueller K, Friederici AD, Koelsch S. Neuroarchitecture of verbal and tonal working memory in nonmusicians and musicians. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(5):771–783. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Tillmann B, Koelsch S, Escoffier N, Bigand E, Lalitte P, Friederici AD, et al. Cognitive priming in sung and instrumental music: activation of inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2006;31(4):1771–1782. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wan CY, Schlaug G. Music making as a tool for promoting brain plasticity across the life span. Neuroscientist. 2010;16(5):566–577. doi: 10.1177/1073858410377805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Strait DL, Kraus N. Can you hear me now? Musical training shapes functional brain networks for selective auditory attention and hearing speech in noise. Front Psychol. 2011;2:113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 62.Balbag MA, Pedersen NL, Gatz M. Playing a musical instrument as a protective factor against dementia and cognitive impairment: a population-based twin study. Int J Alzheimers Dis. 2014;2014:836748. doi: 10.1155/2014/836748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hanna-Pladdy B, Gajewski B. Recent and past musical activity predicts cognitive aging variability: direct comparison with general lifestyle activities. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6:198. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Hanna-Pladdy B, MacKay A. The relation between instrumental musical activity and cognitive aging. Neuropsychology. 2011;25(3):378–386. doi: 10.1037/a0021895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Altenmüller E, James CE: The impact of music interventions on motor rehabilitation following stroke in elderly. In: Music and the Aging Brain. edn.: Elsevier; 2020:407–432. 10.1016/B978-0-12-817422-7.00016-X.
  • 66.Grau-Sanchez J, Munte TF, Altenmuller E, Duarte E, Rodriguez-Fornells A. Potential benefits of music playing in stroke upper limb motor rehabilitation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;112:585–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sluming V, Barrick T, Howard M, Cezayirli E, Mayes A, Roberts N. Voxel-based morphometry reveals increased gray matter density in Broca's area in male symphony orchestra musicians. NeuroImage. 2002;17(3):1613–1622. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Groussard M, Viader F, Landeau B, Desgranges B, Eustache F, Platel H. The effects of musical practice on structural plasticity: the dynamics of grey matter changes. Brain Cogn. 2014;90:174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Gartner H, Minnerop M, Pieperhoff P, Schleicher A, Zilles K, Altenmuller E, et al. Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Front Psychol. 2013;4:636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Klimova B, Valis M, Kuca K. Cognitive decline in normal aging and its prevention: a review on non-pharmacological lifestyle strategies. Clin Interv Aging. 2017;12:903–910. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S132963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.White-Schwoch T, Woodruff Carr K, Anderson S, Strait DL, Kraus N. Older adults benefit from music training early in life: biological evidence for long-term training-driven plasticity. J Neurosci. 2013;33(45):17667–17674. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2560-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Chan AS, Ho YC, Cheung MC. Music training improves verbal memory. Nature. 1998;396(6707):128. doi: 10.1038/24075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bialystok E, Depape AM. Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2009;35(2):565–574. doi: 10.1037/a0012735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Zuk J, Benjamin C, Kenyon A, Gaab N. Behavioral and neural correlates of executive functioning in musicians and non-musicians. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e99868. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Meyer J, Oguz PG, Moore KS. Superior fluid cognition in trained musicians. Psychol Music. 2018;0305735618808089.
  • 76.Parbery-Clark A, Skoe E, Kraus N. Musical experience limits the degradative effects of background noise on the neural processing of sound. J Neurosci. 2009;29(45):14100–14107. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3256-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Slater J, Skoe E, Strait DL, O'Connell S, Thompson E, Kraus N. Music training improves speech-in-noise perception: longitudinal evidence from a community-based music program. Behav Brain Res. 2015;291:244–252. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bugos JA. The effects of bimanual coordination in music interventions on executive functions in aging adults. Front Integr Neurosci. 2019;13:68. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Boyke J, Driemeyer J, Gaser C, Buchel C, May A. Training-induced brain structure changes in the elderly. J Neurosci. 2008;28(28):7031–7035. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0742-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji F, Janowich J, et al. Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature. 2013;501(7465):97–101. doi: 10.1038/nature12486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61(11):1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Woollett K, Maguire EA. Acquiring "the knowledge" of London's layout drives structural brain changes. Current Biol. 2011;21(24):2109–2114. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Teki S, Kumar S, von Kriegstein K, Stewart L, Lyness CR, Moore BC, et al. Navigating the auditory scene: an expert role for the hippocampus. J Neurosci. 2012;32(35):12251–12257. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0082-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Stewart L, Henson R, Kampe K, Walsh V, Turner R, Frith U. Brain changes after learning to read and play music. NeuroImage. 2003;20(1):71–83. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00248-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Stewart L. A neurocognitive approach to music reading. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2005;1060:377–386. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bangert M, Altenmuller EO. Mapping perception to action in piano practice: a longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neurosci. 2003;4:26. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-4-26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Lappe C, Herholz SC, Trainor LJ, Pantev C. Cortical plasticity induced by short-term unimodal and multimodal musical training. J Neurosci. 2008;28(39):9632–9639. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2254-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Lahav A, Saltzman E, Schlaug G. Action representation of sound: audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. J Neurosci. 2007;27(2):308–314. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4822-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Greenwood PM, Parasuraman R. Neuronal and cognitive plasticity: a neurocognitive framework for ameliorating cognitive aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2010;2:150. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Park DC, Lodi-Smith J, Drew L, Haber S, Hebrank A, Bischof GN, et al. The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: the synapse project. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(1):103–112. doi: 10.1177/0956797613499592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.de Mestral C, Marques-Vidal P, Gaspoz JM, Theler JM, Guessous I. Independent association between socioeconomic indicators and macro- and micro-nutrient intake in Switzerland. PLoS One. 2017;12(4):e0174578. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174578. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Mullensiefen D, Gingras B, Musil J, Stewart L. The musicality of non-musicians: an index for assessing musical sophistication in the general population. PLoS One. 2014;9(2):e89642. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Amireault S, Godin G. The Godin-Shephard leisure-time physical activity questionnaire: validity evidence supporting its use for classifying healthy adults into active and insufficiently active categories. Percept Mot Skills. 2015;120(2):604–622. doi: 10.2466/03.27.PMS.120v19x7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Sheikh JI, Yesavage JA. Geriatric depression scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontologist: The Journal of Aging and Mental Health. 1986;5(1–2):165–173. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Breitling LP, Wolf M, Muller H, Raum E, Kliegel M, Brenner H. Large-scale application of a telephone-based test of cognitive functioning in older adults. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2010;30(4):309–316. doi: 10.1159/000319896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Ihle A, Gouveia ER, Gouveia BR, Kliegel M. The cognitive telephone screening instrument (COGTEL): a brief, reliable, and valid tool for capturing Interindividual differences in cognitive functioning in epidemiological and aging studies. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2017;7(3):339–345. doi: 10.1159/000479680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale: fourth edition (WAIS-IV). [assessment instrument] San Antonio: Pearson; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Zuber S, Kliegel M, Ihle A. An individual difference perspective on focal versus nonfocal prospective memory. Mem Cognit. 2016;44(8):1192–1203. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0628-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Enge S, Behnke A, Fleischhauer M, Kuttler L, Kliegel M, Strobel A. No evidence for true training and transfer effects after inhibitory control training in young healthy adults. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2014;40(4):987–1001. doi: 10.1037/a0036165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Bean J. Rey auditory verbal learning test, Rey AVLT. Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2011:2174–75.
  • 102.Rey A. L’examen clinique en psychologie [the clinical psychological examination] Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S. VMLT Verbaler Lern und Merkfähigkeitstest. Göttingen: Beltz Test GmbH; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Kollmeier B, Warzybok A, Hochmuth S, Zokoll MA, Uslar V, Brand T, et al. The multilingual matrix test: principles, applications, and comparison across languages: a review. Int J Audiol. 2015;54(sup2):3–16. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2015.1020971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Tiffin J, Asher EJ. The Purdue pegboard: norms and studies of reliability and validity. J Appl Psychol. 1948;32:234–247. doi: 10.1037/h0061266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Jabusch HC, Vauth H, Altenmuller E. Quantification of focal dystonia in pianists using scale analysis. Mov Disord. 2004;19(2):171–180. doi: 10.1002/mds.10671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Washburn RA, Smith KW, Jette AM, Janney CA. The physical activity scale for the elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46(2):153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Marques JP, Gruetter R. New developments and applications of the MP2RAGE sequence--focusing the contrast and high spatial resolution R1 mapping. PLoS One. 2013;8(7):e69294. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Malinovitch T, Albouy P, Ahissar M, Zatorre RJ. Practicing an auditory working memory task recruits lower-level auditory areas in a task-specific manner. In: CogSci: 20017;3773.
  • 110.Leonardi N, Richiardi J, Gschwind M, Simioni S, Annoni JM, Schluep M, et al. Principal components of functional connectivity: a new approach to study dynamic brain connectivity during rest. NeuroImage. 2013;83:937–950. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Bosch B, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Rami L, Sala-Llonch R, Junque C, Sole-Padulles C, et al. Multiple DTI index analysis in normal aging, amnestic MCI and AD. Relationship with neuropsychological performance. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(1):61–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Haller S, Zaharchuk G, Thomas DL, Lovblad KO, Barkhof F, Golay X. Arterial spin labeling perfusion of the brain: emerging clinical applications. Radiology. 2016;281(2):337–356. doi: 10.1148/radiol.2016150789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L. Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging. 1992;7(2):242–251. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.7.2.242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Isaacs KL, Barr WB, Nelson PK, Devinsky O. Degree of handedness and cerebral dominance. Neurology. 2006;66(12):1855–1858. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000219623.28769.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Stonnington CM, Tan G, Kloppel S, Chu C, Draganski B, Jack CR, Jr, et al. Interpreting scan data acquired from multiple scanners: a study with Alzheimer's disease. NeuroImage. 2008;39(3):1180–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Nucci M, Mapelli D, Mondini S. Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2012;24(3):218–226. doi: 10.3275/7800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Josse J, Husson F, Pagès J. Gestion des données manquantes en analyse en composantes principales. J de la Société Française de Statistique. 2009;150(2):28–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Glatard T, Lewis LB, Ferreira da Silva R, Adalat R, Beck N, Lepage C, Rioux P, Rousseau M-E, Sherif T, Deelman E, et al. Reproducibility of neuroimaging analyses across operating systems. Front Neuroinforma. 2015;9(12):1–14. 10.3389/fninf.2015.00012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 119.Calhoun VD, Adali T, Giuliani NR, Pekar JJ, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GD. Method for multimodal analysis of independent source differences in schizophrenia: combining gray matter structural and auditory oddball functional data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27(1):47–62. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Sui J, He H, Yu Q, Chen J, Rogers J, Pearlson GD, et al. Combination of resting state fMRI, DTI, and sMRI data to discriminate schizophrenia by N-way MCCA + jICA. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:235. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Grady C, Charlton RA, Yu H, Alain C. Age differences in fMRI adaptation for sound identity and location. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;5:1–12. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 122.Krishnan A, Williams LJ, McIntosh AR, Abdi H. Partial least squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: a tutorial and review. NeuroImage. 2011;56(2):455–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Groves AR, Beckmann CF, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Linked independent component analysis for multimodal data fusion. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):2198–2217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Levin-Schwartz Y, Calhoun VD, Adali T. Information sciences and systems (CISS), 2014 48th annual conference on: 19–21 march 2014 2014. 2014. Data-driven fusion of EEG, functional and structural MRI: a comparison of two models; pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Preti MG, Bolton TA, Van De Ville D. The dynamic functional connectome: state-of-the-art and perspectives. Neuroimage. 2017;160:41–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Salami A, Eriksson J, Nyberg L. Opposing effects of aging on large-scale brain systems for memory encoding and cognitive control. J Neurosci. 2012;32(31):10749–10757. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0278-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Haller S, Lovblad KO, Giannakopoulos P, Van De Ville D. Multivariate pattern recognition for diagnosis and prognosis in clinical neuroimaging: state of the art, current challenges and future trends. Brain Topogr. 2014;27(3):329–337. doi: 10.1007/s10548-014-0360-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Richiardi J, Achard S, Bunke H, Ville DVD. Machine learning with brain graphs: predictive modeling approaches for functional imaging in systems neuroscience. IEEE Signal Process Mag. 2013;30(3):58–70. doi: 10.1109/MSP.2012.2233865. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Bernal-Rusiel JL, Reuter M, Greve DN, Fischl B, Sabuncu MR. Spatiotemporal linear mixed effects modeling for the mass-univariate analysis of longitudinal neuroimage data. NeuroImage. 2013;81:358–370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Thompson WK, Hallmayer J, O'Hara R. Design considerations for characterizing psychiatric trajectories across the lifespan: application to effects of APOE-epsilon4 on cerebral cortical thickness in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(9):894–903. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111690. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Steen RG, Hamer RM, Lieberman JA. Measuring brain volume by MR imaging: impact of measurement precision and natural variation on sample size requirements. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28(6):1119–1125. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A0537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Heiervang E, Behrens TE, Mackay CE, Robson MD, Johansen-Berg H. Between session reproducibility and between subject variability of diffusion MR and tractography measures. NeuroImage. 2006;33(3):867–877. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Hamer M, Batty GD. Association of body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio with brain structure: UK biobank study. Neurology. 2019;92(6):e594–600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

As data collection is ongoing, measured GE and HA data are temporarily stored at a 18 To secured server hosted in a secured datacenter at the HES-SO Geneva (HEPIA: Haute Ecole du Paysage, d’Ingénieurie et d’Architecture), accessible to only a part of GE and HA team members via a double authentication VPN (first authentication through user password triggers the receipt of a second password by text message on the personal phone of the user) and encrypted ssh connection.

The server back-up system has two levels. The first level concerns the hardware storage that is set up in RAID1 (Redundant Array of Independent Disks), each data file is duplicated on two separate hard-drives to prevent losing data because of a hard-drive failure. The second level consists of an encrypted copy of all data in another third-party datacenter following research ethics guidelines (automatic process performed every 3 h)

The server will also be used for all analyses (Intel Xeon Gold 5115 2.4 GHz processor, 2 sockets * 10 cores * 2 Threads = 40 threads, 4*32Go RDIMM 2666MT/s Dual Rank126 Go of RAM)

As soon as the data collection will be completed, all data and all other documents will also be stored at YARETA, a FAIR digital solution for long-term preservation of research data for all Geneva Universities (https://yareta.unige.ch). The HEdS-GE already possesses an organizational unit on the YARETA platform.

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available at present due to the fact that 1) the data are only partially collected, and 2) will be published by our research team first, but they will be available in the future from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Measured data are anonymized, the code system for the anonymization of the participants is only preserved at the respective GE and HA Institutes, secured by unique passwords, on 1 computer and 1 hard drive at each site, only accessible to Eckart Altenmüller, Clara James, Damien Marie & Christopher Sinke (Postdoctoral fellows GE & HA), Florian Worschech (PhD student HA) and one other PhD student (HA).


Articles from BMC Geriatrics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES