Skip to main content
International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health logoLink to International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health
. 2020 Sep 29;17(19):7143. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17197143

Racial Differences in Perceived Food Swamp and Food Desert Exposure and Disparities in Self-Reported Dietary Habits

Kristen Cooksey Stowers 1,2,*, Qianxia Jiang 3, Abiodun T Atoloye 1, Sean Lucan 4, Kim Gans 3
PMCID: PMC7579470  PMID: 33003573

Abstract

Both food swamps and food deserts have been associated with racial, ethnic, and socioeconomic disparities in obesity rates. Little is known about how the distribution of food deserts and food swamps relate to disparities in self-reported dietary habits, and health status, particularly for historically marginalized groups. In a national U.S. sample of 4305 online survey participants (age 18+), multinomial logistic regression analyses were used to assess by race and ethnicity the likelihood of living in a food swamp or food desert area. Predicted probabilities of self-reported dietary habits, health status, and weight status were calculated using the fitted values from ordinal or multinomial logistic regression models adjusted for relevant covariates. Results showed that non-Hispanic, Black participants (N = 954) were most likely to report living in a food swamp. In the full and White subsamples (N = 2912), the perception of residing in a food swamp/desert was associated with less-healthful self-reported dietary habits overall. For non-Hispanic Blacks, regression results also showed that residents of perceived food swamp areas (OR = 0.66, p < 0.01, 95% CI (0.51, 0.86)) had a lower diet quality than those not lving in a food swamp/food desert area. Black communities in particular may be at risk for environment-linked diet-related health inequities. These findings suggest that an individual’s perceptions of food swamp and food desert exposure may be related to diet habits among adults.

Keywords: neighborhood environment, food swamps, food deserts, diet quality

1. Introduction

Poor diet is one of the top contributors to obesity and many chronic diseases in the United States [1]. Dietary habits may help explain some socioeconomic and racial/ethnic inequities in health [2,3,4]. Though the overall diet quality of US adults has modestly improved compared to the past decades, socioeconomic and racial/ethnic disparities persist [3,5]. For example, a national study found that non-Hispanic Blacks had worse diet quality measured by Healthy Eating Index (HEI) compared to non-Hispanic Whites [3].

Neighborhood food environments play an important role in individuals’ diet quality, weight status, and overall health [6]. “Food desert,” areas where there is little access to healthful food, have long been identified as one possible driver of the obesity epidemic in the US [7,8]. However, recent studies are increasingly shifting their focus on the culpability of “food swamps” (i.e., areas where unhealthy food retailers inundate healthier alternatives) [9]. Previous studies suggest that food swamps are strong predictors of disparities in obesity prevalence among adults [10]. Prior research also points to strong linkages between race and ethnicity, exposure to food swamps and other inequitable food environments, and dietary disparities [10,11,12,13,14,15,16].

Neighborhood food environments are commonly measured by “objective” measures such as geographic information system (GIS)-based measurements and store audits, but accounting for perceptions (e.g., perceived availability and accessibility of food or food stores measures) is arguably more important [17]. Though policies attempting to promote healthy eating have mainly focused on improving geographic proximity to the locations of healthy food outlets, the importance of using self-reported individual perceptions of the neighborhood food environment has been demonstrated [18,19,20]. Perceived accessibility to different food outlets may reflect individuals’ shopping behaviors, dietary behaviors, and nutritional status as well or better than objective measures of proximity [21,22,23,24,25].

Recent studies utilizing self-reported food environment measures found significant associations between healthy food access and diet quality [26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34]. However, little is known about how perceived food environment relates to self-reported dietary intake and related health, particularly for racial and ethnic minority groups in the US. This study sought to understand how perceptions of food store availability and accessibility are related to reported diet, weight status, and general health. In particular, the study explores whether living in a perceived food desert and food swamp was more likely to be reported by lower-income or racial and ethnic minority individuals, and if such perceptions are related to lower-quality diets, higher weight, and worse reported health.

2. Materials and Methods

The current study is based on an online research sample. Study participants came from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), a crowdsourcing website that is widely used to obtain high-quality data rapidly and inexpensively [35,36,37,38]. This study utilizes data from 30 survey questions that were distributed on Qualtrics, assessing (1) food store access, (2) dietary habits, (3) perceived weight status, (4) perceived health status, and (5) demographics. The survey also included the following red herring question to identify whether or not participants fully read and engaged in the survey: “How closely do you read survey questions? To show that you’re paying attention, please click the ‘Not closely at all’ option as your answer”. The survey was in English only.

Prior to launching the survey, cognitive interviews were conducted with 10 volunteers to test all survey questions with an emphasis on pretesting the food store access and dietary habits questions. The think-aloud cognitive interview approach [39] was utilized to ensure that individuals were interpreting the questions as intended and that survey questions were measuring the constructs as intended (i.e., content validity). Each interview lasted 45–60 min. Question-wording and overall flow were modified based on the cognitive interviews.

The original national sample of U.S. adults consisted of 6357 participants. Drawing from previous research on enhancing data quality in online survey research by screening for inattentive respondents and “speedy” completion times, participants were excluded if they wrongly answered “red herring” questions [40,41,42] (609 participants excluded). Relatedly, participants were also excluded if their response time was less than 6.5 min (1443 participants excluded based on the quartile), indicating that they did not answer the questions carefully [41,42]. The final sample consisted of 4305 participants. In line with other studies paying “MTurkers” 0.50 to 1.00 USD to complete a survey task, [43,44,45,46] we paid 0.70 USD to each MTurk online survey participant. The Institutional Review Boards of Duke University approved all study procedures and materials. Respondents signed an online consent form to indicate that they agreed to participate in the study.

2.1. Measures

2.1.1. Outcome Variables

We have three outcome measures including self-reported dietary quality, weight status, and health status. We used the questionnaire from the Information Resources, Inc. (IRI, Chicago, IL, USA) MedProfiler Health and Wellness Survey which is completed by a nationally representative sample of Nielsen panelists on an annual basis and collects data on topics such as eating habits, health opinions, health concerns, and medical conditions [47]. The survey measures participants’ diet quality by asking them how frequently they eat dairy, five servings of fruits/vegetables each day, dessert or indulgent snacks, whole grains, sugar-sweetened beverages, organic foods, at a fast-food restaurant, at a full-service/sit-down restaurant, and how often they cook at home. The frequency that participants engage in each dietary habit was measured on a six-point scale ranging from never, rarely, sometimes, often, most of the time, and always. Combining these individual categories of dietary habits, we created a diet quality composite score ranging from −12 to 27 that deducted 1–6 points for unhealthy dietary habits (i.e., eating dessert, eating fast food, and drinking sugar-sweetened beverages), and added 1–6 points for the remaining variables. Finally, we collapsed categories based on the distribution of diet quality composite score to create a high (12 to 27, 31.0%), medium (7 to 11, 36.0%), and a low category (−12 to 6, 31.3%). Self-reported weight status was measured in a question asking participants how they described their weight by choosing from the following responses: underweight, about right, slightly overweight, and very overweight. To assess self-reported health quality, we used a question asking participants to rate their health quality as one of five different levels including poor, fair, good, very good, and excellent.

2.1.2. Sociodemographic Characteristics

Demographic characteristics were measured in the survey and included gender; annual household income (lower income (<50k USD) vs. higher income (>50k USD)); race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, non-Hispanic Asian, non-Hispanic other, and Hispanic); education level (high school or less, associate’s degree and some college, bachelor’s degree or higher); current family structure (single without children, single with children, married without children, married with children, life partner without children, and life partner with children); geographic area (living in rural, suburban, or urban area); car ownership (own a car or someone in my house owns a car, do not own a car); region of the United States (Midwest, Northeast, Southeast, Southwest, and West); and age.

2.1.3. Perceived Food Swamp and Food Desert Exposure

To measure perceived food swamp exposure, participants were asked about the number of different food stores they have within one mile from their home (for those living in urban or suburban areas) or five miles (if residing in a rural area) [48]. Food stores included grocery stores, supercenter/club stores, farmer’s markets, full-service/sit-down restaurants, convenience/corner stores, fast-food/limited service establishments, and gas stations with foods. We then created a modified Retail Food Environment Index (mRFEI) score to measure self-reported food swamp exposure based on the following equation: mRFEI = ((grocery store + farmer’s market + full service/sit-down restaurant)/total stores). If the mRFEI score = 0, it was designated as a perceived food desert area; if 0 < the mRFEI score < the median (0.368), it was designated as a perceived food swamp area; otherwise, it was defined a nonfood swamp and food desert area [11,49].

2.2. Statistical Approach

Multinomial logistic regression models were employed to assess the likelihood of living in a perceived food swamp area or food desert area (measured with mRFEI). The reference group included individuals not living in a food swamp/desert area. To assess racial and ethnic differences relative to non-Hispanic Whites and between each racial and ethnic minority group, separate multinomial regression models were run, including the following reference groups: non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, non-Hispanic Asian, non-Hispanic Other, and Hispanic. The estimates from the multinomial regression tests were then used to compute racial and ethnic differences in the relative risk (i.e., probability) of living in a food swamp/desert area.

To examine the relationship between the food environment and the three main outcome variables in both the full sample and three subsamples (i.e., non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, and Hispanic), we first checked to see if the ordinal logistic regression parallel line assumption held. The ordinal logistic regression model was then chosen to assess the predicted probabilities of perceived diet quality and health quality. However, since the assumption of parallel trend test was violated for perceived weight status, multinomial logistic regression models were chosen for this particular outcome variable. For all regression models, the independent variable is perceived food swamp and food desert exposure. Covariates included family income, race/ethnicity, education level, current family structure, geographic area, car ownership, gender, region of the country, and age. For all three main outcome variables, analyses were also stratified by race and ethnicity. Further, for models including the full sample of respondents, we included race and income interaction terms. We ran all statistical analyses using SPSS version 24 [50].

3. Results

The final sample consisted of 4305 participants with 38.0% male, 67.6% non-Hispanic White, 22.2% non-Hispanic Black, 2.0% non-Hispanic Asian, 3.8% Hispanic, 33.6% single, and 30.8% low income. Participants were on average 41.3 years old and about half (48.8%) of them lived in a suburban area. Based on mRFEI measures, 40.7% of study participants reported a retail environment consistent with living in a food swamp, and 6.5% reported a retail environment consistent with living in a food desert. More than half of study participants (58.4%) owned a car or someone in the house owned a car. 41.3% of respondents had a bachelor’s degree or more. See Table 1.

Table 1.

Participant demographics (N = 4305).

Variables Mean (SD) or N (%)
Sociodemographic Variables
Age 41.3 (14.3)
Gender
Male 1634 (38%)
Female 2666 (61.9%)
Household income
Lower (annual household income <50k) 2109 (49.0%)
Higher 2177 (50.7%)
Education
High school or less 861 (20.0%)
Associate’s degree or some college 1657 (38.5%)
Bachelor’s degree or higher 1777 (41.3%)
Race/Ethnicity
Non-Hispanic White 2912 (67.6%)
Non-Hispanic Black 954 (22.2%)
Non-Hispanic Asian 84 (2.0%)
Non-Hispanic other 173 (4.0%)
Hispanic 1 162 (3.8%)
Current family structure
Single without children 1050 (24.4%)
Single with children 391 (9.1%)
Married without children 309 (7.2%)
Married with children 865 (20.1%)
Life partner without children 169 (3.9%)
Life partner with children 100 (2.3%)
Vehicle ownership
Own a car or someone in my house owns a car 2513 (58.4%)
Urban/suburban/rural area
Urban 1239 (28.8%)
Suburban 2102 (48.8%)
Rural 964 (22.4%)
Region
Midwest 969 (22.5%)
Northeast 855 (19.9%)
Southeast 1313 (30.5%)
Southwest 409 (9.5%)
West 758 (17.6%)
Neighborhood Food Environment
Food desert/swamp area 2
Living in a food desert area
Living in a food swamp area
279 (6.5%)
1751 (40.7%)
Not living in a food desert/swamp area 2039 (47.4%)
Outcome Variables
Diet quality 3
Low 1335 (31.0%)
Medium 1549 (36.0%)
High 1347 (31.3%)
Perceived Health quality
Poor 202 (4.7%)
Fair 974 (22.6%)
Good 1695 (39.4%)
Very good 1029 (23.9%)
Excellent 401 (9.3%)
Perceived Weight status
Slightly underweight 317 (7.4%)
About right 1582 (36.7%)
Slightly overweight 1813 (42.1%)
Very overweight 591 (13.7%)

1 Hispanic White: 114 (2.7%); Hispanic Black: 12 (0.2%); Hispanic Asian: 4 (0.1%); Hispanic other: 29 (0.7%). 2 If 0 < the mRFEI score < the median (0.368), it was designated as a perceived food swamp area; if the mRFEI score = 0, it was designated as a perceived food desert area; otherwise, it was defined not living in a food swamp/food desert area. 3 Diet quality was measured on a 6-point scale. We collapsed categories based on the distribution of diet quality score to create a high, medium, and low category.

Regarding outcome variables, 72.6% of respondents stated that they had good health quality or above. More than half (55.8%) perceived themselves as slightly or very overweight. For diet quality, we calculated that 31.0% had low diet quality, 36.0% medium diet quality, and 31.3% high diet quality (see Table 1).

Refer to Table 2 for the likelihood that participants perceived living in a food swamp or food desert (measured using mRFEI) by race and ethnicity. The results of multinomial logistic regression models showed that compared to non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks were 38% more likely to perceive living in a food swamp (RR = 1.38, p < 0.001); non-Hispanic Asians were less likely to perceive living in a food swamp than Asians (RR = 0.70, p < 0.05). Compared to non-Hispanic Blacks, non-Hispanic Asians were less likely to perceive living in both food swamps (RR = 0.53, p < 0.001) and food deserts (RR = 0.13, p < 0.05); and Hispanics were less likely to perceive living in a food swamp (RR = 0.8, p < 0.05). Compared to non-Hispanic Asians, non-Hispanic Others were more likely to perceive living in both food swamps (RR = 1.59, p < 0.05) and food deserts (RR = 7.52, p < 0.05). Last, Hispanics were more likely to perceive living in a food swamp than non-Hispanic Others (RR = 1.50, p < 0.05). As a robustness check, we also ran multinomial logistic regression models of the likelihood of residing in a food swamp/desert by including food swamp measures based on the average (vs. median) mRFEI and found similar results. The only differences were that the likelihood of residing in a food swamp for non-Hispanic Asian vs. White became statistically insignificant, plus the following likelihood of food desert status results became statistically insignificant: non-Hispanic Asian vs. Black and non-Hispanic Asian vs. non-Hispanic Other.

Table 2.

Summary of multinomial logistic regression models predicting the likelihood of living in a perceived food swamp or food desert (measured using Modified Retail Food Environment Index (mRFEI)), by race and ethnicity.

Race/Ethnicity Food Swamp Food Desert
Predictors OR 95% CI RR 5 OR 95% CI RR 5
lower higher lower higher
Non-Hispanic Black 1 1.71 *** 1.46 2.00 1.38 *** 1.13 0.82 1.54 1.11
Non-Hispanic Asian 1 0.57 * 0.35 0.92 0.70 * 0.13 * 0.02 0.94 0.14
Non-Hispanic Other 1 1.20 0.87 1.67 1.11 1.10 0.59 2.06 1.09
Hispanic 1 1.09 0.78 1.52 1.05 0.64 0.29 1.41 0.67
Non-Hispanic Asian 2 0.33 *** 0.20 0.54 0.53 *** 0.12 * 0.02 0.85 0.13 *
Non-Hispanic Other 2 0.70 * 0.50 0.99 0.85 0.98 0.50 1.91 0.98
Hispanic 2 0.64 * 0.45 −0.91 0.80 * 0.57 0.25 1.30 0.61
Non-Hispanic Other3 2.13 * 1.20 3.78 1.59 * 8.51 * 1.08 67.31 7.52 *
Hispanic 3 1.92 * 1.08 3.43 1.50 * 4.97 0.59 41.47 4.64
Hispanic 4 0.52 * 0.29 0.93 0.94 0.20 0.02 1.68 0.62

1 Relative risk with non-Hispanic White as the reference group. 2 Relative risk with non-Hispanic Black as the reference group. 3 Relative risk with non-Hispanic Asian as the reference group. 4 Relative risk with non-Hispanic Other as the reference group. 5 We compared the risks of living in a food swamp area or food desert area among different race groups by computing relative risk ratios. Relative Risk is calculated by dividing the probability of an event occurring for group 1 divided by the probability of an event occurring for group 2. Abbreviations: OR, odds ratios; CI, confidence interval; RR, relative risk ratios; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

See Table 3 for the total sample, ordinal logistic regression results showed that residents of food deserts (OR = 0.74, p < 0.05, 95% CI (0.58, 0.94)) and food swamps (OR = 0.75, p < 0.001, 95% CI (0.66, 0.84)) were significantly more likely to have a lower diet quality score than those not living in a food swamp/food desert area. For non-Hispanic Blacks (N = 954), regression results showed that residents of food swamp areas (OR = 0.66, p < 0.01, 95% CI (0.51, 0.86)) had a lower diet quality than those of Black Americans not living in a food swamp/food desert. There were no statistically significant differences in the relationship between residing in a food swamp or food desert and diet quality in the Hispanic subsample, or perceived health quality and weight status by food swamp/desert residential status in the total sample or any subsample. Therefore, we did not include the results in our paper. We included the regression models with full covariates in Appendix A.

Table 3.

Summary of logistic regression models predicting diet quality by residing in a food swamp or food desert (measured using Modified Retail Food Environment Index (mRFEI)), stratified by race.

Independent Variables/Covariates All Non-Hispanic White (N = 2912) Non-Hispanic Black (N = 954) Hispanic (N = 162)
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper
Residing in food swamp 1 0.75 *** 0.66 0.84 0.75 *** 0.64 0.87 0.66 ** 0.51 0.86 1.53 0.79 2.96
Residing in food desert 0.74 * 0.58 0.94 0.75 * 0.56 0.99 0.81 0.48 1.39 0.25 0.05 1.27
Lower income (vs. higher income) 0.86 * 0.74 0.99 0.85 * 0.73 0.99 0.90 0.68 1.18 1.51 0.78 2.93
Male 0.77 *** 0.69 0.87 0.76 ** 0.66 0.88 0.83 0.63 1.09 0.54 0.28 1.06
Age 1.01 *** 1.01 1.01 1.01 * 1.00 1.01 1.02 *** 1.01 1.03 1.02 0.99 1.05
Single without children 2 0.57 *** 0.42 0.78 0.58 ** 0.40 0.82 0.61 0.27 1.38 0.22 0.04 1.24
Single with children 0.89 0.60 1.32 0.95 0.56 1.61 0.84 0.43 1.65 0.68 0.08 6.02
Married with children 0.81 0.53 1.23 0.84 0.47 1.48 0.77 0.38 1.58 1.11 0.09 14.09
Life partner without children 1.28 0.82 1.98 1.3 0.75 2.30 1.28 0.53 3.14 2.45 0.23 25.81
Life partner with children 1.11 0.74 1.67 1.19 0.70 2.02 0.92 0.45 1.87 0.40 0.04 4.25

1 Reference group is not living in a food swamps area; 2 Reference group is married without children. Abbreviations: OR, odds ratios; CI, confidence interval; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

4. Discussion

Our study is among the first to explore the role of perceived food swamp and food desert exposure on self-reported dietary habits, health quality, and weight status in a racially and ethnically diverse, national sample of US adults. Overall, participants living in perceived food swamps and food deserts were more likely to report lower diet quality, but this did not translate to statistically significant results for self-reported weight or health status. Further, we found racial and ethnic disparities in the likelihood of individuals residing in a perceived food swamp or food desert. In addition, results from the current study reveal that the relationship between perceived food swamp or food desert exposure and dietary quality vary among non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks, and Hispanic populations.

4.1. Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Perceived Food Swamp/Desert Status

According to the results of multinomial regression models stratified by race and ethnicity, non-Hispanic Black Americans were more likely to report living in a food swamp than adults identifying as non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Asian, or Hispanic. Though few studies have assessed the likelihood of residing in a food swamp by race and ethnicity to date [51], national and local studies across the US found that residents of minority neighborhoods are more likely to categorize their neighborhood as a food desert (i.e., poor access to supermarkets, chain grocery stores, and healthful food products) [24,52,53,54,55]. For example, previous research has revealed that residents living in predominantly Black and Hispanic neighborhoods tend to have lower access to chain supermarkets or large grocery stores, and higher access to fast-food outlets when compared to non-Hispanic, White counterparts [52]. Prior studies also suggest that these inequities in the built, food retail environment can be partially attributed to racial residential segregation [56,57]. The current study builds upon this research by going beyond counts of grocery stores and fast-food establishments to utilize a relative measure of the different types of stores (i.e., mRFEI) within a defined boundary. The findings of this study align with previous research indicating that predominantly Black neighborhoods are less likely to have equal access to healthy and unhealthy food retailers [58]. Another study found that more convenience stores were located near secondary schools in predominantly racial/ethnic-minority neighborhoods compared to predominantly White census tracts [59]. Building upon previous studies suggesting that Black and Hispanics are more likely to live in food deserts [13,56], we found that Black Americans were also more likely to perceive living in a food desert than Asians. Notably, relative to earlier studies, we used perceived food desert measures instead of objective measures of accessibility to food outlets. Still, our findings warrant future efforts to address racial and ethnic disparities in the risk of living in a food desert or food swamp.

4.2. Perceived Food Swamp/Desert Status and Diet

Consistent with previous studies using objective neighborhood environment measures [10,60], we found that residents of perceived food deserts and swamps had poorer diets relative to those not living in a food swamp/food desert areas in the full sample. This finding suggests that an individual’s perceptions of food swamp and food desert exposure may be an important correlate of diet quality. However, there is limited evidence supporting the elimination of food deserts as a strategy to address poor diet quality [60]. A recent study found that opening a supermarket in a food desert did not result in people buying healthier food [61]. Recent studies of neighborhood food environment are shifting their focus to the culpability of food swamps [9,51], which may be a stronger predictor of inequities in diet and obesity than food deserts [10,11].

4.3. Racial and Ethnic Differences in the Relationship between Perceived Food Swamp/Desert Status and Diet

Though to the authors’ knowledge, few studies have assessed the relationship between living in a food swamp or desert area [62] and diet quality across race/ethnicity groups, studies have indicated that residents of predominantly racial and ethnic minority neighborhoods are most likely to be affected by poor access to healthful food and tend to have lower diet quality [52,53,54,55]. In the Black subsample in our study, food swamp residents reported having a lower diet quality than those not living in a food swamp/food desert area. This finding aligns with previous studies that have consistently shown that neighborhoods with higher proportions of Black residents have fewer supermarkets and more convenience stores and fast-food restaurants [52,56,63]. However, in the Hispanic subsample, we did not find any statistically significant differences in the relationship between food swamps or food deserts and dietary health, weight status, or health outcomes.

There were no statistically significant differences in self-reported health quality and weight status by food swamp residential status in our study. Though there is limited research assessing the relationship between the food environment and perceived health, evidence has suggested diet quality has a strong impact on health outcomes [1,64,65,66,67]. Previous studies assessing the association between the neighborhood environment and weight status were inconsistent [6,17,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80]. Similar to our study, several studies did not find a statistically significant relationship between neighborhood food environment and obesity rates [71,72,73,74]. However, other studies have shown that neighborhood food environment factors, such as food desert and food swamp status, were significantly associated with individuals’ measured weight status [10,11]. For example, Chen et al. [7] used MedProfiler questions about dietary behaviors (e.g., frequency of consuming high-fiber, high-protein, low-carbohydrate, low-fat, low-salt, and low-sugar diets) and found, even after controlling for home food environment factors, food desert status was associated with obesity [67]. The different findings may be due to error inherent in self-reported measures of weight status and diet quality, and the varied ways that researchers used in measuring the built, food environment [67].

4.4. Limitations

The current analysis uses self-reported measures of food store exposure, which may be subject to various measurement errors, such as recall bias or socially desirable responses. The present study also uses a self-reported measure of dietary habits. Although we utilized the previously used and validated IRI MedProfiler measure of diet, the current study would have been strengthened by the ability to collect observational data or more comprehensive self-reports (i.e., 24-h dietary recall or Food Frequency Questionnaire) [68]. In addition, the relatively small sample size of Hispanic participants and the absence of acculturation and country of origin limit our understanding of Hispanic ethnicity as a moderator of the relationship between food swamps and diet, and may explain why we did not find statistically significant results in this subsample. Future surveys should be in Spanish as well as English to increase participation among individuals that identify as Hispanic. Further, the current study was only able to examine correlational relationships as the underlying dataset is cross-sectional. Future work should pursue longitudinal designs and natural experiments to explore causality.

5. Conclusions

The racial differences in food environment exposure and its relationship with dietary quality identified in this study are consistent with previous evidence pointing to the existence and impacts of racial residential segregation. It also highlights the importance of policies, planning, and development, plus other structural changes that will provide equal opportunity for improved diet quality among both White and racial and ethnic minority populations. Furthermore, the findings from our study suggest that an individual’s perceptions of food swamp and food desert exposure may be associated with dietary habits among adults in the United States. Although research is showing a decline in racial segregation [69], more policy efforts that address the disparities in access to unhealthy versus healthy food retail outlets across neighborhoods are needed to limit the negative impacts on nutrition and health-related outcomes.

Future research should incorporate how well objective and subjective measures of food swamp exposure align, and which are stronger predictors of inequities in health behaviors or outcomes. Future research should also engage communities to identify objective environmental conditions for intervention targets. Further, additional research is needed to identify best practices for maximizing resident engagement and inclusivity in the context of municipal planning and policy development efforts. The application of these best practices may increase the likelihood that future efforts to develop interventions targeting objective conditions in the local food environment will account for and integrate the perceptions and experiences of individuals that live there. If people perceive the remediation of food desert/swamp conditions, they might experience actual improvement in their weight and health. Still, additional research is needed to understand the effects of racial and ethnic disparities in neighborhood food swamp exposure as it will provide relevant information for decision/policymakers to make informed policy changes.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge [redacted for review] for funding the project and for providing feedback on the study design.

Appendix A

Table A1.

Summary of logistic regression models predicting diet quality by residing in a food swamp or food desert (measured using mRFEI), stratified by race. ** FULL MODEL **.

Independent Variables/Covariates All Non-Hispanic White (N = 2912) Non-Hispanic Black (N = 954) Hispanic (N = 162)
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper Lower Upper
Residing in Food swamp 1 0.75 *** 0.66 0.84 0.75 *** 0.64 0.87 0.66 ** 0.51 0.86 1.53 0.79 2.96
Residing in Food desert 0.74 * 0.58 0.94 0.75 * 0.56 0.99 0.81 0.48 1.39 0.25 0.05 1.27
Lower income (vs. higher income) 0.86 * 0.74 0.99 0.85 * 0.73 0.99 0.90 0.68 1.18 1.51 0.78 2.93
Non-Hispanic Black 2 0.66 * 0.53 0.82 - - - - - - - - -
Non-Hispanic Asian 0.83 0.49 1.41 - - - - - - - - -
Non-Hispanic Other 1.51 0.98 2.30
Hispanic 0.86 0.56 1.31 - - - - - - - - -
High school or less 3 0.45 *** 0.39 0.53 0.45 *** 0.37 0.54 0.58 ** 0.38 0.87 0.46 0.17 1.29
Associate’s degree and some college 0.75 *** 0.65 0.85 0.76 ** 0.64 0.89 0.74 * 0.56 0.97 0.97 0.48 1.98
Single without children 4 0.89 0.60 1.32 0.95 0.56 1.61 0.84 0.43 1.65 0.68 0.08 6.02
Single with children 0.81 0.53 1.23 0.84 0.47 1.48 0.77 0.38 1.58 1.11 0.09 14.09
Married with children 1.28 0.82 1.98 1.3 0.75 2.30 1.28 0.53 3.14 2.45 0.23 25.81
Life partner without children 1.11 0.74 1.67 1.19 0.70 2.02 0.92 0.45 1.87 0.40 0.04 4.25
Life partner with children 1.08 0.67 1.74 1.15 0.63 2.10 0.81 0.32 2.09 1.30 0.08 21.15
Own a car or someone in my house owns a car (vs. do not own a car) 1.16 0.92 1.45 1.29 0.93 1.79 0.97 0.68 1.37 3.42 0.61 19.22
Male (vs. female) 0.77 *** 0.69 0.87 0.76 ** 0.66 0.88 0.83 0.63 1.09 0.54 0.28 1.06
Midwest 5 0.72 ** 0.60 0.87 0.73** 0.59 0.90 0.73 0.47 1.14 0.621 0.20 1.96
Northeast 0.91 0.75 1.09 0.93 0.74 1.17 0.71 0.47 1.09 1.23 0.46 3.34
Southeast 0.72 *** 0.60 0.85 0.73 ** 0.59 0.90 0.58 ** 0.39 0.86 1.51 0.64 3.55
Southwest 0.80 0.64 1.00 0.88 0.67 1.17 0.52 * 0.29 0.90 0.66 0.25 1.74
Urban 6 0.89 0.75 1.05 0.98 0.80 1.21 0.70 0.46 1.06 0.64 0.24 1.70
Suburban 0.95 0.82 1.10 0.91 0.77 1.07 1.04 0.68 1.56 0.62 0.23 1.67
Age 1.01 *** 1.01 1.01 1.01 * 1.00 1.01 1.02 *** 1.01 1.03 1.02 0.99 1.05
Low income* Black/ 0.19 0.01 2.87 - - - - - - - - -
Low income * Asian 1.12 0.84 1.48 - - - - - - - - -
Low income * Other 1.29 0.56 2.97 - - - - - - - - -
Low income * Hispanic 0.72 0.40 1.29 - - - - - - - - -

1 Reference group is not living in food swamp/desert areas. 2 Reference group is Non-Hispanic White. 3 Reference group is Bachelor’s degree or higher. 4 Reference group is married without children. 5 Reference group is West. 6 Reference group is rural. 7 Reference group is high income * White. Outcome variable diet quality has three mutually exclusive categories: low, medium, high. Health quality has five mutually exclusive categories: poor, fair, good, very good, excellent. Weight status has four mutually exclusive categories: slightly underweight, about right, slightly overweight, very overweight. Abbreviations: OR, odds ratios; CI, confidence interval; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Author Contributions

K.C.S. led all phases of the study, including conceptualization and manuscript preparation. Q.J. contributed to the statistical analysis and original draft preparation. A.A., S.L., and K.G. provided input on data analyses and presentation, and reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Duke University Sanford School of Public Policy.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Lim S.S., Vos T., Flaxman A.D., Danaei G., Shibuya K., Adair-Rohani H., Aryee M. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990-2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2012;380:2224–2260. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61766-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.James W.P.T., Nelson M., Ralph A., Leather S. Socioeconomic determinants of health: The contribution of nutrition to inequalities in health. Br. Med. J. 1997;314:1545–1549. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7093.1545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wang Y., Chen X. How Much of Racial/Ethnic Disparities in Dietary Intakes, Exercise, and Weight Status Can Be Explained by Nutrition- and Health-Related Psychosocial Factors and Socioeconomic Status among US Adults? J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011;111:1904–1911. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2011.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fang Zhang F., Liu J., Rehm C.D., Wilde P., Mande J.R., Mozaffarian D. Trends and Disparities in Diet Quality Among US Adults by Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program Participation Status. JAMA Netw. Open. 2018;1:e180237. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.0237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rehm C.D., Peñalvo J.L., Afshin A., Mozaffarian D. Dietary intake among US Adults, 1999–2012. JAMA J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2016;315:2542–2553. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.7491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gordon-Larsen P. Food Availability/Convenience and Obesity. Adv. Nutr. 2014;5:809–817. doi: 10.3945/an.114.007070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Morland K., Diez Roux A.V., Wing S. Supermarkets, other food stores, and obesity: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2006;30:333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.LeDoux T.F., Vojnovic I. Going outside the neighborhood: The shopping patterns and adaptations of disadvantaged consumers living in the lower eastside neighborhoods of Detroit, Michigan. Health Place. 2013;19:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2012.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rose D., Bodor J., Swalm C., Rice J., Farley T., Hutchinson P. Deserts in New Orleans? Illustrations of Urban. Food Acces and Implications for Policy. [(accessed on 1 April 2020)];2008 Available online: http://medcontent.metapress.com/index/A65RM03P4874243N.pdf.
  • 10.Hager E.R., Cockerham A., O’Reilly N., Harrington D., Harding J., Hurley K.M., Black M.M. Food swamps and food deserts in Baltimore City, MD, USA: Associations with dietary behaviours among urban adolescent girls. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:2598–2607. doi: 10.1017/S1368980016002123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cooksey-Stowers K., Schwartz M.B., Brownell K.D., Cooksey-Stowers K., Schwartz M.B., Brownell K.D. Food swamps predict obesity rates better than food deserts in the United States. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2017;14:1366. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14111366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Baker E.A., Schootman M., Barnidge E., Kelly C. The role of race and poverty in access to foods that enable individuals to adhere to dietary guidelines. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2006;3:A76. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Walker R.E., Keane C.R., Burke J.G. Disparities and access to healthy food in the United States: A review of food deserts literature. Health Place. 2010;16:876–884. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sanchez-Vaznaugh E.V., Weverka A., Matsuzaki M., Sánchez B.N. Changes in Fast Food Outlet Availability Near Schools: Unequal Patterns by Income, Race/Ethnicity, and Urbanicity. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2019;57:338–345. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2019.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sweeney G., Rogers C., Hoy C., Clark J.K., Usher K., Holley K., Spees C. Alternative Agrifood Projects in Communities of Color: A Civic Engagement Perspective. J. Agric. Food Syst. Community Dev. 2015;5:1–7. doi: 10.5304/jafscd.2015.054.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Carlson C., Abrams E., Reid E., Williamson K. A Tale of Two Cities: Mapping and Analyzing the Food Environment of Raleigh and Durham, North Carolina. [(accessed on 31 August 2020)];2018 Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/324150276.
  • 17.Caspi C.E., Sorensen G., Subramanian S.V., Kawachi I. The local food environment and diet: A systematic review. Health Place. 2012;18:1172–1187. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2012.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ma X., Liese A.D., Bell B.A., Martini L., Hibbert J., Draper C., Jones S.J. Perceived and geographic food access and food security status among households with children. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19:2781–2788. doi: 10.1017/S1368980016000859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Usher K. Valuing All Knowledges Through an Expanded Definition of Access. J. Agric. Food Syst Community Dev. 2015;5:1–6. doi: 10.5304/jafscd.2015.054.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chen X., Kwan M.P. Contextual uncertainties, human mobility, and perceived food environment: The uncertain geographic context problem in food access research. Am. J. Public Health. 2015;105:1734–1737. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McGuirt J.T., Pitts S.B.J., Gustafson A. Association between spatial access to food outlets, frequency of grocery shopping, and objectively-assessed and self-reported fruit and vegetable consumption. Nutrients. 2018;10:1974. doi: 10.3390/nu10121974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Alber J.M., Green S.H., Glanz K. Perceived and Observed Food Environments, Eating Behaviors, and BMI. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2018;54:423–429. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Moore L.V., Diez Roux A.V., Brines S. Comparing perception-based and geographic information system (GIS)-based characterizations of the local food environment. J. Urban. Health. 2008;85:206–216. doi: 10.1007/s11524-008-9259-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dhillon J., Diaz Rios L.K., Aldaz K.J., De La Cruz N., Vu E., Asad Asghar S., Ortiz R.M. We don’t have a lot of healthy options: Food environment perceptions of first-year, minority college students attending a food desert campus. Nutrients. 2019;11:816. doi: 10.3390/nu11040816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Barnes T.L., Lenk K., Caspi C.E., Erickson D.J., Laska M.N. Perceptions of a Healthier Neighborhood Food Environment Linked to Greater Fruit and Vegetable Purchases at Small and Non-Traditional Food Stores. J. Hunger Environ. Nutr. 2019;14:741–761. doi: 10.1080/19320248.2018.1549518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Moore L.V., Diez Roux A.V., Nettleton J.A., Jacobs D.R. Associations of the local food environment with diet quality—A comparison of assessments based on surveys and geographic information systems. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2008;167:917–924. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sharkey J.R., Johnson C.M., Dean W.R. Food access and perceptions of the community and household food environment as correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among rural seniors. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:32. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dulin Keita A., Risica P.M., Drenner K.L., Adams I., Gorham G., Gans K.M. Feasibility and acceptability of an early childhood obesity prevention intervention: Results from the healthy homes, healthy families pilot study. J. Obes. 2014;2014:378501. doi: 10.1155/2014/378501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Green S.H., Glanz K. Development of the Perceived Nutrition Environment Measures Survey. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015;49:50–61. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2015.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Casagrande S.S., Franco M., Gittelsohn J., Zonderman A.B., Evans M.K., Kuczmarski M.F., Gary-Webb T.L. Healthy food availability and the association with BMI in Baltimore, Maryland. Public Health Nutr. 2010;14:1001–1007. doi: 10.1017/S1368980010003812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Holsten J.E., Deatrick J.A., Kumanyika S., Pinto-Martin J., Compher C.W. Children’s food choice process in the home environment. A qualitative descriptive study. Appetite. 2012;58:64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gustafson A., Lewis S., Perkins S., Wilson C., Buckner E., Vail A. Neighbourhood and consumer food environment is associated with dietary intake among Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP) participants in Fayette County, Kentucky. Public Health Nutr. 2013;16:1229–1237. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013000505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Franco M., Diez Roux A.V., Glass T.A., Caballero B., Brancati F.L. Neighborhood Characteristics and Availability of Healthy Foods in Baltimore. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2008;35:561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kumar S., Quinn S.C., Kriska A.M., Thomas S.B. “Food is directed to the area”: African Americans’ perceptions of the neighborhood nutrition environment in Pittsburgh. Health Place. 2011;17:370–378. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2010.11.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Peer E., Vosgerau J., Acquisti A. Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behav. Res. Methods. 2014;46:1023–1031. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0434-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sheehan K.B. Crowdsourcing research: Data collection with Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Commun. Monogr. 2018;85:140–156. doi: 10.1080/03637751.2017.1342043. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Buhrmester M., Kwang T., Gosling S.D. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Perspect Psychol. Sci. 2011;6:3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Chandler J., Rosenzweig C., Moss A.J., Robinson J., Litman L. Online panels in social science research: Expanding sampling methods beyond Mechanical Turk. Behav. Res. Methods. 2019;51:2022–2038. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01273-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Willis G.B., Artino A.R. What Do Our Respondents Think We’re Asking? Using Cognitive Interviewing to Improve Medical Education Surveys. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 2013;5:353–356. doi: 10.4300/JGME-D-13-00154.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jones M.S., House L.A., Gao Z. Respondent Screening and Revealed Preference Axioms: Testing Quarantining Methods for Enhanced Data Quality in Web Panel Surveys. Public Opin. Q. 2015;79:687–709. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfv015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Callegaro M., Yang Y., Bhola D.S., Dillman D.A., Chin T.-Y. Response Latency as an Indicator of Optimizing in Online Questionnaires. Bull. Sociol. Methodol. Méthodol. Sociol. 2009;103:5–25. doi: 10.1177/075910630910300103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Revilla M., Ochoa C. What are the Links in a Web Survey Among Response Time, Quality, and Auto-Evaluation of the Efforts Done? Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2015;33:97–114. doi: 10.1177/0894439314531214. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Keith M.G., Tay L., Harms P.D. Systems perspective of amazon mechanical turk for organizational research: Review and recommendations. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:1359. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Amazon Mechanical Turk Pricing. [(accessed on 23 June 2020)]; Available online: https://requester.mturk.com/pricing.
  • 45.Huang J.L., Bowling N.A., Liu M., Li Y. Detecting Insufficient Effort Responding with an Infrequency Scale: Evaluating Validity and Participant Reactions. J. Bus. Psychol. 2015 doi: 10.1007/s10869-014-9357-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Necka E.A., Cacioppo S., Norman G.J., Cacioppo J.T. Measuring the Prevalence of Problematic Respondent Behaviors among MTurk, Campus, and Community Participants. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0157732. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.IRI SymphonyIRI MedProfiler Health and Wellness SurveyTM Identify Opportunities for Health and Wellness Related Marketing. [(accessed on 28 September 2020)];2011 Available online: http://www.symphonyiri.com.
  • 48.Ver Ploeg M., Breneman V., Farrigan T., Hamrick K., Hopkins D., Kaufman P., Kinnison K. Access to Affordable and Nutritious Food: Measuring and Understanding Food Deserts and Their Consequences. [(accessed on 28 September 2020)];2009 Available online: https://www.ers.usda.gov/webdocs/publications/42711/12716_ap036_1_.pdf.
  • 49.Prevention C for DC and Census Tract Level State Maps of the Modified Retail Food Environment Index (MRFEI). Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. [(accessed on 15 April 2020)];2013 Available online: http://www.cdc.gov/obesity/downloads/2_16_mrfei_data_table.xls.
  • 50.IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. IBM Corp. Armonk; New York, NY, USA: 2013. Version 22.0. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Lucan S.C., Maroko A.R., Patel A.N., Gjonbalaj I., Elbel B., Schechter C.B. Healthful and less-healthful foods and drinks from storefront and non-storefront businesses: Implications for “food deserts”, “food swamps” and food-source disparities. Public Health Nutr. 2020;23:1428–1439. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019004427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Kumanyika S.K., Whitt-Glover M.C., Haire-Joshu D. What works for obesity prevention and treatment in black Americans? Research directions. Obes. Rev. 2014;15:204–212. doi: 10.1111/obr.12213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Bodor J.N., Rice J.C., Farley T.A., Swalm C.M., Rose D. Disparities in food access: Does aggregate availability of key foods from other stores offset the relative lack of supermarkets in African-American neighborhoods? Prev. Med. (Baltim.) 2010;51:63–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Dunn R.A., Sharkey J.R., Horel S. The effect of fast-food availability on fast-food consumption and obesity among rural residents: An analysis by race/ethnicity. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2012;10:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2011.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Fleischhacker S.E., Evenson K.R., Rodriguez D.A., Ammerman A.S. A systematic review of fast food access studies. Obes Rev. 2011;12:e460–e471. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00715.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bower K.M., Thorpe R.J., Rohde C., Gaskin D.J. The intersection of neighborhood racial segregation, poverty, and urbanicity and its impact on food store availability in the United States. Prev. Med. 2014;58:33–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Winkler M.R., Lenk K.M., Caspi C.E., Erickson D.J., Harnack L., Laska M.N. Variation in the food environment of small and non-traditional stores across racial segregation and corporate status. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22:1624–1634. doi: 10.1017/S1368980019000132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Li M., Ashuri B. Neighborhood racial composition, neighborhood wealth, and the surrounding food environment in Fulton County, GA. Appl. Geogr. 2018;97:119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2018.06.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Sturm R. Disparities in the food environment surrounding US middle and high schools. Public Health. 2008;122:681–690. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2007.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Block J.P., Subramanian S.V. Moving Beyond “Food Deserts”: Reorienting United States Policies to Reduce Disparities in Diet Quality. PLoS Med. 2015;12:e1001914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Allcott H., Diamond R., Dubé J.P., Handbury J., Rahkovsky I., Schnell M. Food deserts and the causes of nutritional inequality. Q. J. Econ. 2019;134:1793–1844. doi: 10.1093/qje/qjz015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Gray M.S., Lakkur S., Howard V.J., Pearson K., Shikany J.M., Safford M., Judd S.E. The Association between Residence in a Food Desert Census Tract and Adherence to Dietary Patterns in the REGARDS Cohort. Food Public Health. 2018;8:79–85. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.James P., Arcaya M.C., Parker D.M., Tucker-Seeley R.D., Subramanian S.V. Do minority and poor neighborhoods have higher access to fast-food restaurants in the United States? Health Place. 2014;29:10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2014.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Milte C.M., Thorpe M.G., Crawford D., Ball K., McNaughton S.A. Associations of diet quality with health-related quality of life in older Australian men and women. Exp. Gerontol. 2015;64:8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2015.01.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Parsons T.J., Papachristou E., Atkins J.L., Papacosta O., Ash S., Lennon L.T., Wannamethee S.G. Healthier diet quality and dietary patterns are associated with lower risk of mobility limitation in older men. Eur. J. Nutr. 2019;58:2335–2343. doi: 10.1007/s00394-018-1786-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bolton K.A., Jacka F., Allender S., Kremer P., Gibbs L., Waters E., de Silva A. The association between self-reported diet quality and health-related quality of life in rural and urban Australian adolescents. Aust. J. Rural Health. 2016;24:317–325. doi: 10.1111/ajr.12275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Chen D., Jaenicke E.C., Volpe R.J. Food environments and obesity: Household diet expenditure versus food deserts. Am. J. Public Health. 2016;106:881–888. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Powell L.M., Auld M.C., Chaloupka F.J., O’Malley P.M., Johnston L.D. Associations Between Access to Food Stores and Adolescent Body Mass Index. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2007;33(Suppl. 4):S301–S307. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Courtemanche C., Carden A. Supersizing supercenters? The impact of Walmart Supercenters on body mass index and obesity. J. Urban. Econ. 2011;69:165–181. doi: 10.1016/j.jue.2010.09.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Dubowitz T., Ghosh-Dastidar M., Eibner C., Slaughter M.E., Fernandes M., Whitsel E.A., Michael Y.L. The women’s health initiative: The food environment, neighborhood socioeconomic status, BMI, and blood pressure. Obesity. 2012;20:862–871. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Jeffery R.W., Baxter J., McGuire M., Linde J. Are fast food restaurants an environmental risk factor for obesity? Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2006;3:2. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-3-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Burdette H.L., Whitaker R.C. Neighborhood playgrounds, fast food restaurants, and crime: Relationships to overweight in low-income preschool children. Prev. Med. (Baltim.) 2004;38:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Sturm R., Datar A. Body mass index in elementary school children, metropolitan area food prices and food outlet density. Public Health. 2005;119:1059–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2005.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Ford P.B., Dzewaltowski D.A. Limited supermarket availability is not associated with obesity risk among participants in the Kansas WIC program. Obesity. 2010;18:1944–1951. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Drewnowski A., Aggarwal A., Hurvitz P.M., Monsivais P., Moudon A.V. Obesity and supermarket access: Proximity or price? Am. J. Public Health. 2012;102:e74–e80. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Aggarwal A., Cook A.J., Jiao J., Seguin R.A., Vernez Moudon A., Hurvitz P.M., Drewnowski A. Access to supermarkets and fruit and vegetable consumption. Am. J. Public Health. 2014;104:917–923. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hattori A., An R., Sturm R. Neighborhood food outlets, diet, and obesity among california adults, 2007 and 2009. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2013;10:E35. doi: 10.5888/pcd10.120123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Boone-Heinonen J., Gordon-Larsen P., Kiefe C.I., Shikany J.M., Lewis C.E., Popkin B.M. Fast food restaurants and food stores—Longitudinal associations with diet in young to middle-aged adults: The CARDIA study. Arch. Intern. Med. 2011;171:1162–1170. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Prince S.A., Kristjansson E.A., Russell K., Billette J.M., Sawada M.C., Ali A., Prud’homme D. Relationships between neighborhoods, physical activity, and obesity: A multilevel analysis of a large Canadian city. Obesity. 2012;20:2093–2100. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Morland K.B., Evenson K.R. Obesity prevalence and the local food environment. Health Place. 2009;15:491–495. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES