Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2020 Oct 22;10:18039. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-75015-7

Innate connectivity patterns drive the development of the visual word form area

Jin Li 1,, David E Osher 1, Heather A Hansen 1, Zeynep M Saygin 1,
PMCID: PMC7582172  PMID: 33093478

Abstract

What determines the functional organization of cortex? One hypothesis is that innate connectivity patterns, either structural or functional connectivity, set up a scaffold upon which functional specialization can later take place. We tested this hypothesis by asking whether the visual word form area (VWFA), an experience-driven region, was already functionally connected to proto language networks in neonates scanned within one week of birth. Using the data from the Human Connectone Project (HCP) and the Developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP), we calculated intrinsic functional connectivity during resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and found that neonates showed similar functional connectivity patterns to adults. We observed that (1) language regions connected more strongly with the putative VWFA than other adjacent ventral visual regions that also show foveal bias, and (2) the VWFA connected more strongly with frontotemporal language regions than with regions adjacent to these language regions. These data suggest that the location of the VWFA is earmarked at birth due to its connectivity with the language network, providing evidence that innate connectivity instructs the later refinement of cortex.

Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Visual system

Introduction

Decades of research suggest that the adult brain is composed of patches of cortex that are specialized for unique mental functions. To what extent is the functional organization of the human brain innate? Recent advances in developmental neuroimaging have made it possible to start to answer this question. For example, a previous study showed category-selective responses in high-level visual cortex for faces and scenes in infants1, although the origin of the earliest face response is still under debate (e.g., proto-organization for visual features, innate face-template, or social origin24). Further, research in congenitally blind individuals suggests that cortical selectivity for high-level visual categories may not require visual experience5. In addition to the early emergence of visual processing, a previous study also found a neural precursor of language processing in infants6. Specifically, they found brain activity in left superior temporal and angular gyri to human speech in 3-month-old infants. These studies support the protomap hypothesis, which suggests that early genetic instructions give rise to the mature functional areas of cortex. However, the mechanisms that drive this early functional specialization remains ambiguous.

One possibility is that in the case of visual areas, pre-existing retinotopic biases may predispose a region to become selective to foveal or peripheral stimuli (Retinotopic/Eccentricity Hypothesis)711. However, this hypothesis is unlikely to fully explain the dissociation between face and word representations in visual cortex; faces and words are both foveal stimuli1113 but are represented in different cortical areas. The Connectivity Hypothesis (which is not mutually exclusive from the Retinotopic Hypothesis) proposes that the specialization of a given brain region is largely shaped by how it connects and communicates with the rest of the brain. Previous work showed that structural connectivity (via diffusion imaging) as well as functional connectivity (via resting-state scans) can predict task-based selectivity across the brain14,15. Further, Barttfeld et al. found a lateral-to-mesial organization in ventral visual cortex in newborns, suggesting that functional connectivity present at birth may constrain the subsequent functional specialization of visual areas16. This work suggests that connectivity is tightly intertwined with functional selectivity, and that perhaps early connectivity patterns may earmark the location of functionally selective cortices. Can the visual word form area (VWFA), which responds strongly to visual words or letter strings in literate individuals17,18 be differentiated from the adjacent fusiform face area (FFA) by its connections to high-level cortex like the frontotemporal language network?

In adults, the VWFA connects with perisylvian language cortex, differentiating it from adjacent visual cortex19; other studies also found that white matter fibers that originated from the VWFA pass through fascicles that may be critical for language processing20,21. In children, a longitudinal study found that connectivity patterns in pre-literate 5-year-olds predicted the location of the VWFA in each child at age 8 after they learned to read, and differentiated it from the adjacent FFA22. The connectivity patterns that predicted the VWFA included putative language areas, suggesting that connectivity to these regions may earmark the future location of the VWFA, and also set up a scaffold upon which future functional specialization can take place. However, while the 5-year-olds could not read (and at that age, lacked neural selectivity to letters or letter-like stimuli), they still would have had years of visual experience with letters and words. Is the putative VWFA already connected differently and set up to be differentiated from adjacent visual regions, even at birth with no visual experience with words and little visual experience at all?

Here, we tested this proto-organization of the VWFA in both the adult and newborn brain by analyzing resting state data from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) and the Developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP). Based on the Connectivity Hypothesis, we hypothesized that although the VWFA is highly experience-dependent, it is already ‘prewired’ to be selective for visual words by communicating with proto language regions at birth. By examining neonates who were scanned within one week of birth, we asked (1) Do language regions show stronger functional connectivity (FC) with the putative VWFA than with other high-level visual areas like face, scene, and object areas? and (2) Does the VWFA show stronger FC with language regions than with adjacent frontotemporal regions like the multiple-demand (MD) network, speech regions, and primary auditory cortex (A1)?

Results

We examined whether the putative VWFA showed privileged connections with language regions even at birth. Because neonates cannot participate in task-based fMRI experiments, and because they do not yet have a VWFA, we overlaid functional parcels from previous studies and atlases2225 to the neonates and adults in this study. Although we cannot define functional regions individually, the functional parcels provide the anchor space within which functionally specific regions may later develop. Thus, these specific regions will likely develop somewhere within these parcels in each individual neonate. Where they develop within the parcel will vary from person to person, and these parcels capture that variability. As an empirical check, we applied these parcels to an independent group of adults (different individuals than adults involved in the analyses below; see “Methods”) who performed the fMRI localizer tasks for all of the mental domains examined in the present study (see Supplementary Results 1 and 2 and Supplementary Figs. S1 and S2). We successfully identified subject-specific functional regions (fROIs) within these parcels; these fROIs demonstrated functionally selective responses for the conditions of interest in independent fMRI runs (Supplementary Results 1 and 2 and Supplementary Figs. S1 and S2). These results support the functional relevance and specificity of these parcels and the spatial variability of functional specialization across subjects which is captured by these parcels.

Note that all the analyses were performed on the volume and voxel-wise results were projected to the surface for a more comprehensive visual presentation of data.

The selectivity of VWFA-language connections compared with other visual areas

First, we asked: do language regions selectively connect to the expected site of the VWFA, compared with other adjacent high-level visual regions? We compared the functional connectivity (FC) of language regions to the VWFA vs. to other high-level visual areas in the ventral stream, specifically in regions in the vicinity of the VWFA, including face selective regions (Fusiform Face Area, FFA; Occipital Face Area, OFA), scene selective region (Parahippocampal Place Area; PPA), and object selective regions (Lateral Occipital, LO; Posterior Fusiform Sulcus, PFS) (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

FC between language regions (seed) and high-level visual regions (targets). Seed, language (yellow); targets, VWFA (purple), faces (blue), scenes (olive), objects (light green). Parcels were projected to the template brain and visualized with Freeview from FreeSurfer (v6.0.0). (a) Mean FC between language regions and high-level visual regions in ventral visual stream. Connectivity values were Fisher z transformed. Individual data points (n = 40 for each age group) were shown for each category. Error bars denote s.e.m. Horizontal bars reflect significant post hoc paired t-tests p < 0.05, corrected. (b) FC fingerprint of language regions. Connectivity values were mean-centered and averaged within each of the four categories to plot the relative patterns for the adult (n = 40) and neonate groups (n = 40). (c) FC pattern dissimilarity for within and between groups (n = 40 for each age group). Euclidean distance between each individual and others either from the same group or different group was calculated. n.s. non-significant.

We first performed a complete 2-way mixed design ANOVA with age group (neonate, adult) as the between-group variable and target (VWFA, faces, scenes, objects) as the within-group variable. We found significant main effects for both target and age group (target, (F(3,312) = 24.47, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.19, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0.11, 0.26]; age group, F(1,312) = 14.07, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.04, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0.01, 0.09]; and a significant interaction (F(3,312) = 4.92, p = 0.002, partial η2 = 0.05, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0.01, 0.09]). Similar results were observed after accounting for size (Supplementary Results 3). Post-hoc t-tests revealed that in adults, language regions showed significantly higher FC with the VWFA than they did with faces (t(39) = 7.58, p < 0.001), Cohen’s d = 1.20, corrected; 95% CI [0.11, 0.19]), scenes (t(39) = 9.39, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.48, corrected; 95% CI [0.16, 0.25]) and objects (t(39) = 7.84, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.24, correct; 95% CI [0.09, 0.16]) (Fig. 1b). The neonates showed a similar pattern, where connectivity between language regions and the VWFA was significantly higher than connectivity of language regions to face (t(39) = 6.28, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.99, corrected; 95% CI [0.09, 0.18]) and scene (t(78) = 3.90, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.62, corrected, 95% CI [0.04, 0.14]) regions, but we found no statistically significant evidence for a difference between language regions’ connectivity to the VWFA vs. object regions in neonates (t(39) = 0.55, p = 0.59, Cohen’s d = 0.09; 95% CI [− 0.03, 0.06]) (Fig. 1a). The difference of language connectivity to the VWFA vs. Object regions was significantly larger in adults compared to neonates (t(78) = 4.20, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.94, 95% CI [0.06, 0.17]). The same results were found when we intersected the functional parcels used here with meta-analysis maps generated from Neurosynth (https://neurosynth.org/; see Supplementary Results 5 and Supplementary Fig. S7), suggesting that the results are largely applicable to other ways of defining functional brain regions and with narrower definitions of the functional parcels.

An exploratory analysis revealed that the VWFA was more connected than object regions to the more canonical aspects of the language network, the language parcel that likely encompasses Broca’s and the language parcel that like encompasses Wernicke’s areas (“Methods”) in neonates as well as in adults (neonates: Broca: t(39) = 3.06, p = 0.004, Cohen’s d = 0.48, corrected; 95% CI [0.03, 0.15]; Wernicke: t(39) = 3.23, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 0.51, corrected; 95% CI [0.04, 0.16]; adults: Broca: t(39) = 7.21, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.14, corrected; 95% CI [0.21, 0.38]; Wernicke: t(39) = 3.13, p = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 0.50, corrected; 95% CI [0.03, 0.15]).

To further compare connectivity patterns between groups, we next looked at the connectivity fingerprints of language regions to visual cortex in neonates and adults (Fig. 1b). Here we plot the relative connectivity of language regions to each of the four target categories (VWFA, face, scene, object regions) as compared to the mean of all four categories. We found that neonates had a very similar shape of the connectivity fingerprints as adults, suggesting similar FC patterns between groups. We statistically quantify the similarity of FC patterns between adults and neonates using Euclidean distance (as a measure of similarity) of the 4-dimensional FC pattern between participants. No statistically significant evidence for a difference between the within-group similarity and between-group similarity was found (within-adults vs. within neonates: t(78) = − 0.72, p = 0.47, Cohen’s d = − 0.16; 95% CI [− 0.07, 0.03]; within-adults vs. neonates-adults: t(78) =  − 1.68, p = 0.10, Cohen’s d = − 0.37; 95% CI [− 0.11, 0.01]; within-neonates vs. neonates-adults: t(78) =  − 0.85, p = 0.40, Cohen’s d = − 0.19; 95% CI [− 0.10, 0.04]) (Fig. 1c; see “Methods” for more details).

These results indicate that neonates show an overall similar FC pattern as adults, with the highest connectivity between language regions and the VWFA. Interestingly, neonates show similar connectivity between language-VWFA and language-object regions for the language network as a whole, but show dissociations in VWFA vs. object connectivity to the more canonical aspects of the language network, suggesting that further developmental refinement of connectivity does occur, especially to specific aspects of the language circuit.

Functional connectivity at a voxel-wise level in ventral temporal cortex

Next, we applied a voxel-wise approach to analyze the functional connectivity profiles of language regions at a more fine-grain level. This analysis allowed us to examine spatial specificity of language-VWFA connectivity, and also addressed potential biases (e.g., size) of using predefined functional regions, and thus serves as a complement to the parcel-wise analyses. We used language regions as the seed and we looked within ventral temporal cortex (VTC) for voxels that connected most with these language regions. Figure 2a shows the connectivity of VTC voxels to language regions in representative neonates and adults (see Supplementary Fig. S3 for average results). Consistent with the previous parcel-wise analysis, the voxels that have highest connectivity to language regions were mostly located in the lateral portion of VTC, which is within the putative VWFA location. To quantitatively identify which functional regions these voxels belonged to, we parametrically increased the connectivity threshold from the median to the top 95th percentile of FC across VTC, and calculated the number of voxels within the VTC that were connected to language regions; we then quantified how many of these voxels belonged in each functional region using Dice coefficient (“Methods”). We found that voxels that were connected to language regions were always located in the expected VWFA, vs. all other functional regions in the vicinity; this result was significant for all thresholds (Fig. 2b).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Voxel-wise analyses within the ventral temporal cortex (VTC) using language regions as the seed. (a) Heatmaps for voxels with connectivity to language regions in representative neonates and adults, thresholded at z(r) greater than 0.1 (p < 0.001). Results were project to individuals’ surfaces and visualized with Freeview from FreeSurfer (v6.0.0). (b) Parametrically increasing the threshold of FC from the median to the 95th percentile within VTC, we quantified how many of these voxels belonged in each functional region using Dice coefficient. Averaged FC (Fisher’s z transformed) across neonates (n = 40; 50th: z(r) = 0.25, p < 0.001; 95th: z(r) = 0.52, p < 0.001; Average FC across adults (n = 40; 50th: z(r) = 0.22, p < 0.001; 95th: z(r) = 0.45, p < 0.001). Error bars denote s.e.m across participants. *Significant paired t-test (VWFA vs. average of other functional regions, p < 0.05, corrected).

Overall, the parcel-based and voxel-wise results indicate that the cortical tissue that may later develop sensitivity to visual words has patterns of connectivity that are relatively adult-like in the neonatal brain, suggesting that it may be earmarked for function due to its preferential connectivity with language regions at birth. However, we also found differences between neonates and adults, especially with respect to object cortex, suggesting that there exist changes in this connectivity scaffold that likely result due to experience with literacy.

Functional connectivity between the putative VWFA and language regions

Next, we asked, does the VWFA connect more to language regions vs. regions in the vicinity of language areas? We calculated FC between the VWFA (seed region) and the language, MD, speech, and A1 regions (target regions) (Fig. 3). We first performed a complete 2-way mixed design ANOVA with age group (neonate, adult) as the between-group variable and target (language, MD, speech, A1) as the within-group variable to examine VWFA’s connectivity. We found both that main effect of age and target were significant (age, F(1,312) = 9.29, p = 0.002, partial η2 = 0.03, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0, 0,07]; target, F(3,312) = 24.45, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.19, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0.11, 0.26]), and the interaction was also significant (F(3,312) = 3.90, p = 0.009, partial η2 = 0.04, 95% CI of partial η2 = [0, 0.08]). Similar results were observed after accounting for size (Supplementary Results 3). Post-hoc t-tests revealed that in both adults and neonates the putative VWFA was more connected with language regions than with the other regions (Adults, MD: t(39) = 5.72, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.90, corrected; 95% CI [0.10, 0.21]; Speech: t(39) = 6.48, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.02, corrected; 95% CI [0.14, 0.26]; A1: t(39) = 9.32, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.47, corrected; 95% CI [0.23, 0.36]; Neonates, MD: t(39) = 8.47, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.34, corrected; 95% CI [0.11, 0.18]; Speech: t(39) = 4.79, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.76, corrected; 95% CI [0.06, 0.14]; A1: t(78) = 5.63, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.89, corrected; 95% CI [0.09, 0.19]) (Fig. 3a).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

FC between VWFA (seed) and non-visual regions (targets). Seed, VWFA (purple); targets, language (yellow), speech (light purple), A1 (orange), MD (green). Parcels were projected to the template brain and visualized with Freeview from FreeSurfer (v6.0.0). (a) Mean FC between VWFA and regions in temporal and frontal cortices. Connectivity values were Fisher z transformed. Error bars denote s.e.m. Individual data points (n = 40 for each age group) were shown for each category. Horizontal bars reflect significant post hoc paired t-tests p < 0.05, corrected. (b) FC fingerprint of VWFA. Connectivity values were mean-centered and averaged within each of the four categories to plot the relative patterns for the adult (n = 40) and neonate (n = 40) groups. (c) FC pattern dissimilarity for within and between groups (n = 40 for each age group). Euclidean distance between each individual and others either from the same group or different group. n.s. non-significant.

To further compare FC patterns between groups, we next plotted the connectivity fingerprint of the VWFA in neonates and adults, and observed similar fingerprint shapes between the two groups (Fig. 3b). Quantitative analyses of the similarity of FC profiles also confirmed this observation: no statistically significant evidence for a difference between the within-group similarity and between-group similarity was found (within-adults vs. within neonates: t(78) = − 0.55, p = 0.58, Cohen’s d = -0.12; 95% CI [− 0.08, 0.05]; within-adults vs. neonates-adults: t(78) =  − 1.29, p = 0.20, Cohen’s d = − 0.29; 95% CI [− 0.11, 0.02]; within-neonates vs. neonates-adults: t(78) = -0.66, p = 0.51, Cohen’s d = − 0.15; 95% CI [− 0.11, 0.05]) (Fig. 3c; see “Methods” for details).

To control for potential distance confounds and to further hone in on more spatially specific FC, we split up the target regions into frontal and temporal cortices and examined VWFA connectivity to language regions vs. adjacent functional regions in frontal and temporal cortex separately via planned t-tests. In both adults and neonates, the VWFA showed significantly higher FC to temporal language regions than adjacent speech (adults: t(39) = 5.48, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.87, corrected; 95% CI [0.09, 0.20]; neonates: t(39) = 6.22, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.98, corrected; 95% CI [0.08, 0.16]) and A1(adults: t(39) = 7.16, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.13, corrected; 95% CI [0.17, 0.31]; neonates: t(39) = 6.01, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.95, corrected; 95% CI [0.11, 0.22]) (Supplementary Fig. S4a). Next, we compared the VWFA’s connectivity to frontal language regions vs. adjacent multiple-demand (MD) regions. In both adults and neonates, the VWFA showed significantly higher FC to frontal language regions as compared to adjacent MD regions (adults: t(39) = 9.71, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.54, corrected; 95% CI [0.23, 0.35]; neonates: t(39) = 4.51, p < 0.001, Cohen’s = 0.71, corrected; 95% CI [0.05, 0.13]) (Supplementary Fig. S4b). The same results were found with Neurosynth-overlapped regions (Supplementary Fig. S7).

Altogether, these results are consistent with previous adult studies by showing that the VWFA has higher FC to language related regions than to adjacent regions; here we also find that the neonatal VWFA has similar patterns of FC to language regions as the adult VWFA.

Functional connectivity at a voxel-wise level in frontal and temporal cortices

We also performed voxel-wise parametric analyses for the frontal and temporal cortex using the VWFA as the seed. Consistent with the parcel-based analysis, we found that the voxels connected to the VWFA were most likely located in the expected language regions in temporal and frontal cortex in both neonates and adults (Supplementary Fig. S5a,b). Heatmaps that illustrate the connectivity of frontotemporal voxels to the VWFA in representative neonates and adults are provided in Supplementary Fig. S5c, and a volumetric presentation of average VWFA FC to the whole-brain in adults and neonates is also provided (Supplementary Fig. S6). These results indicate that voxels connected to the VWFA are located within putative frontal and temporal language regions in neonates and adults alike.

Discussion

A mosaic-like functional organization is consistently found in the adult brain. However, the driving factor of this functional organization and its variation across individuals remains unclear. The Connectivity Hypothesis proposes that the future function of a given brain area is largely shaped by how this region connects with the rest of the brain. Classic studies of ‘rewired’ ferrets showed that the cortical region that would have developed into A1 took on many of the properties of V1 after retinal input was rerouted to that location, showing in animal models that connectivity precedes function2630. Here, we tested the Connectivity Hypothesis in human neonates and specifically for a high-level visual function that is uniquely human. We asked: is the putative VWFA already pre-wired at birth to develop differential functional specialization from its neighbors?

The VWFA serves as a good model to study the emergence of functionally selective regions since this region is highly experience-dependent. We first found higher connectivity of language regions with the VWFA than with adjacent regions in visual cortex, and we further replicated previous FC findings in adults31, showing higher connectivity of the VWFA with language regions that might be involved in different aspects of language processing (i.e., lexico-semantic processing, syntactic processing, structural processing) than with adjacent regions in frontotemporal cortex. Importantly, we also found that this region may be earmarked to become selective to visual words by showing preferential connectivity with language regions. This research provides the earliest possible evidence in humans that the cortical tissue that will likely later develop sensitivity to visual words has a connectivity pattern at birth that makes it a fertile ground for such development—even before any exposure to words.

The organization of visual cortex, including high-level cortex, is largely biased by retinotopy1113. This retinotopic organization is present very early in development, as evidenced by previous work, a recent study found that infant macaques, much like adults, showed a proto-organization for retinotopy throughout the visual system32. It is possible that early genetic instructions and underlying molecular/cytoarchitectonic determine the retinotopic preferences of neurons within these visual regions, including in high-level regions. Indeed, it has been posited that the VWFA starts out as part of the face network, and becomes increasingly selective to words and less selective to faces in the left hemisphere as literacy is acquired18,33. This hypothesis is attractive because the perception of both faces and words require high-spatial frequency information that is represented foveally. Thus, with a retinotopic bias/connectivity from lower-level visual regions, it may be possible to first differentiate face regions from scene regions (foveal vs. peripheral bias) early in development (if not at birth), and then face from word regions after literacy is gained, perhaps through differential connections with fronto-temporal language regions. However, retinotopic organization or connectivity to early retinotopic cortex alone cannot explain the early differentiation of the VWFA from face regions, as we found here. We propose that in addition to its predisposition to foveal stimuli, the location of the future VWFA also depends on its innate connectivity with language regions even at birth.

Numerous open questions remain. First, what are the changes that occur with reading? In other words, if the VWFA is not selective to words in neonates, how does it become word-selective with literacy? Our results also suggest that there are certain differences between neonates and adults, such as overall differences in connectivity magnitude, strengthening of specific connections (i.e., increased connectivity between VWFA and frontal language regions in adults), and refinement of connections (i.e., decreased connectivity between VWFA and speech, A1 in adults). For example, our parcel-based analyses show that object regions also have high FC to language regions in neonates whereas in adults they do not; on a finer grain, our parametric analyses show that the putative VWFA is in fact differentiated from object regions in their connectivity to language cortex. Further, we found that the VWFA is also differentiated from object cortex by their connections to a narrower definition of language regions (i.e. putative Broca’s and Wernicke’s areas). These findings suggest that while the VWFA is already differentiated from face, scene, and to some extent object regions at birth, there is likely further refinement to fully differentiate orthographic representations from other objects in visual cortex, in line with e.g. Augustin et al.34 and Kubota et al.35. Experience with spoken and written language will likely strengthen connections with specific aspects of the language circuit and further differentiate this region’s function from its neighbors as an individual gains literacy.

Another question that remains unanswered is how the connectivity patterns themselves arose prenatally and evolutionarily. It is likely that a complex mechanism of intrinsic properties of cortical regions and early signaling mechanisms set up these large-scale connections. The VWFA may simply be in a privileged location, due to a myriad of mechanisms including cellular properties and intrinsic circuitry, in addition to large-scale connectivity that facilitates its later selectivity. Future studies combining animal models with studies in other human populations, e.g. premature human infants, may help further elucidate the evolution of these mechanisms. Moreover, the present study focused on functional connectivity, which raises another interesting question about whether there exists innate structural connectivity between the putative VWFA and language regions at birth and what its developmental trajectory looks like. A recent study observed white matter (i.e., arcuate fasciculus) alterations in 18 months infants with familiar risk of developmental dyslexia36. A potential future direction in this line of research is to explore the role of white matter maturation properties (e.g., fiber density and myelination) in prolonged language development, and examine how their interplay with functional connectivity and experience. Other open avenues of future research include looking at effective connectivity to try to tease apart the directionality of connectivity (which would need to be verified with animal models) as well as graph theoretical approaches37 to show similarities or differences in network structure between neonates and adults.

Further, there are different ways to define the VWFA which may capture different representational levels of word processing. For example, several studies using words vs. scrambled words found a region in the posterior occipital temporal sulcus (OTS)38 that might involve visual feature extraction39. The VWFA parcel used in the present study was defined with the contrast of words vs. line-drawing objects, thus defining voxels that distinguish orthographic lexicon from non-visual semantics that may be also involved in general object processing. While this parcel covers the site of the classical VWFA40 in the middle OTS, it also extends anteriorly. Our supplementary data shows that the VWFA is more consistently observed in this more anterior area across subjects, and this anterior region is also found in previous studies that used the same contrast35,41. However, to make the current results more comparable to other definitions of the VWFA, we also restricted the VWFA to the most posterior part (VWFA-p) and found that the results still hold (see Supplementary Results). Previous studies also found a more general posterior-to-anterior gradient in the VTC that showed increased sensitivity from letters to real words42 and distinctive responses even for different representations of letters43. While beyond the scope of this current work, it remains an open question whether regions that play different roles in word recognition (e.g., processing letters, sublexical properties in pseudowords, orthographic lexicon in real words)44 have different FC to the language network.

Finally, it remains unknown how laterality arises in the human brain. The questions and hypotheses of this study pertain to the canonical location of the VWFA within the context of the ventral visual stream (i.e. with respect to other category-selective regions), rather than the laterality bias of the VWFA vs. e.g. the FFA. Consistent with previous studies in adults that compared VWFA connectivity to adjacent regions19,31,45, we restricted our analyses to the left hemisphere in order to eliminate potential confounds of cross-hemispheric differences. The development of functional specificity in right vs. left visual cortex remains an open question. A longitudinal study that can functionally define these regions in children (after they can participate in a task-based fMRI scan) can answer these questions and more, as discussed in the limitations below.

There exist certain limitations in the current study. We found evidence in favor of the Connectivity Hypothesis; stronger causal evidence would involve experimental manipulations of connectivity patterns to test if functional specialization changes as a consequence of these connectivity changes. However, this type of study would be invasive in newborn humans; here we attempt to leverage experience-dependent domains and a study of neonates to test the Connectivity Hypothesis but acknowledge the limitations of causal inferences that can be drawn from noninvasive studies. Further, a challenge of studying the functional organization of the neonatal brain is that there is no adequate way to localize functional responses using fMRI in neonates. Here we used functional parcels from previous studies and overlaid these parcels onto both adult and neonate brains. These parcels likely encompass the functional regions in individual subjects which offer better functional relevance than anatomical landmarks, but as a consequence, also likely overestimate the size of the functional regions. To further explore spatial specificity, we chose adjacent functional parcels as comparisons, explored smaller subsets of the language parcels, and performed voxel-wise analyses on individual subject data without predefined functional regions. However, the present results are still limited by the functional parcels as well as current registration and image processing methods in neonates; better registration methods such as surface-based registration to an adult template are currently unavailable in neonates but will likely improve the results and inferences drawn from these studies. Additionally, future studies may consider new approaches to localize functional responses in young infants or longitudinal studies, e.g., Saygin et al.18, to define each of these functional regions in individual subjects and further test the specificity of the current findings. Finally, we tested the Connectivity Hypothesis for the VWFA specifically. The findings suggest that connectivity-based scaffolding may be a general driving mechanism for the functional organization of human cortex, but the generality of this hypothesis for other mental domains remains to be tested.

Methods

Participants

Neonates

We used the initial release of the Developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) neonatal data (https://www.developingconnectome.org)46. Neonates were recruited and imaged at the Evelina Neonatal Imaging Centre, London. Informed parental consent was obtained for imaging and data release, and the study was approved by the UK Health Research Authority. All 40 neonates of the initial release were included in functional connectivity analysis and were born and imaged at term age (15 female, mean gestational age at birth = 38.99 weeks, gestational age range at scan = 37–44 weeks). All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

Adults

Adult data were obtained from the Human Connectome Project (HCP), WU-Minn HCP 1200 Subjects Data Release (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult)47. All participants were scanned at Washington University in St. Louis (WashU) and informed consent forms were obtained from all adult participants involved in the study. All experimental protocols were approved by the WashU institutional review board. 40 adults were included in functional connectivity analysis (15 female, age range = 22–36 years old). These adult participants were motion and sex matched to the neonates. Specifically, for each neonatal participant we matched with an adult from the HCP dataset with the same sex who showed the most similar motion parameter (i.e., framewise displacement, FD) with the k-nearest neighbors’ approach. By doing this, we are able to match the sex ratio and no evidence for a statistically difference was found for head motion between groups (t(78) = 0.77, p = 0.45, Cohen's d = 0.17, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.01]). All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

For the Supplementary analyses, an independent group of adults (N = 14, mean age = 27.0 ± 0.87, 6F; all right-handed) were recruited from the greater Boston area as part of a previous study conducted at MIT. All participants were screened for history of mental illness, gave written informed consent, and were compensated at $30 per hour. The study was approved by the Massachusetts Institute of Technology ethics committee.

Data acquisition

Neonates

Imaging was carried out on 3 T Philips Achieva (running modified R3.2.2 software) using a dedicated neonatal imaging system which included a neonatal 32 channel phased array head coil48. All neonates were scanned in natural sleep; previous studies have shown that the resting-state FC remains consistent while awake and asleep, as well as while under anesthesia49,50.

Resting-state fMRI

High temporal resolution fMRI developed for neonates using multiband (MB) 9 × accelerated echo-planar imaging was collected (TE/TR = 38/392 ms, voxel size = 2.15 × 2.15 × 2.15 mm3). The duration of resting-state fMRI scanning was approximately 15 min and consisted of 2300 volumes for each run. No in-plane acceleration or partial Fourier was used. Single-band reference scans were also acquired with bandwidth matched readout, along with additional spin-echo acquisitions with both AP/PA fold-over encoding directions.

Anatomical MRI

High-resolution T2-weighted and inversion recovery T1-weighted multi-slice fast spin-echo images were acquired with in-plane resolution 0.8 × 0.8 mm2 and 1.6 mm slices overlapped by 0.8 mm (T2-weighted: TE/TR = 156/12000 ms; T1 weighted: TE/TR/TI = 8.7/4795/1740 ms).

Adults

All the scans of WU-Minn HCP 1200 Subjects Data Release was carried out using a customized 3 T Connectome Scanner adapted from a Siemens Skyra (Siemens AG, Erlanger, Germany) with 32-channel Siemens receive head coil and a “body” transmission coil designed by Siemens specifically for the smaller space available using the special gradients for the WU-Minn and MGH-UCLA Connectome scanners.

Resting-state fMRI

Participants were scanned using the Gradient-echo EPI sequence (TE/TR = 33.1/720 ms, flip angle = 52°, number of slices = 72, voxel size = 2 × 2 × 2 mm3). The duration of resting-state fMRI scanning was approximately 15 min and consisted of 1200 volumes for each run. All participants accomplished two resting-state fMRI sessions. Within each session, there were two phases encoding in a right-to-left (RL) direction in one run and phase encoding in a left-to-right (LR) direction in the other run. In current analysis, we used the LR phase encoding from the first session. Participants were instructed to open their eyes with relaxed fixation on a projected bright cross-hair on a dark background.

Anatomical MRI

High-resolution T2-weighted and T1-weighted images were acquired with isotropic voxel resolution of 0.7mm3 (T2-weighted 3D T2-SPACE scan: TE/TR = 565/3200 ms; T1-weighted 3D MPRAGE: TE/TR/TI = 2.14/2400/1000 ms).

Preprocessing

Structural data preprocessing

The dHCP data were released as preprocessed data; they used the dHCP structural minimal preprocessing pipeline46, briefly: bias correction, brain extraction using BET from FSL, and segmentation of the T2w volume using DRAW-EM algorithm51 which were developed for neonatal brain segmentation. Gray and white matter masks were obtained from segmentations using DRAW-EM algorithm provided by dHCP. The HCP data were released as preprocessed data; they used the HCP structural preprocessing pipeline52, briefly: gradient distortion correction, brain extraction, a bias field correction, and registration between the T2-weighted scan and T1-weighted scan. Each individual brain was also aligned to common MNI152 template (with 0.7 mm isotropic resolution). Then, the FreeSurfer pipeline (based on FreeSurfer 5.3.0-HCP) was performed to segment the volume into predefined structures and surface reconstruction.

Functional data preprocessing

The pre-processed functional data released by the dHCP had already undergone basic pre-processing steps (for details see Fitzgibbon et al.53): distortion-correction, motion correction, 2-stage registration of the MB-EPI functional image to T2 structural image and also generated a combined transform from MB-EPI to 40-week T2 template, and ICA denoising using ICA-FIX54. The data released by the HCP had already undergone basic pre-processing steps (for details see Glasser et al.52): removed spatial distortions, corrected for motion, registered the fMRI data to both structural and MNI152 template, reduced the bias field, and ICA denoising using ICA-FIX54. The HCP data were registered to each individual's native space using the transformation supplied by the HCP and the following steps were performed on both the HCP and dHCP data: applied smoothing (Gaussian filter with the FWHM = 3 mm) within the all gray matter, and band-pass filter at 0.009–0.08 Hz. As a further denoising step, we used aCompCor55 to regress out signals from white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) to control physiological noise like respiration and heartbeat as well as non-neuronal contributions to the resting state signal. All the FC analyses were performed in native functional space.

Defining the functional parcels

The parcels used here were originally created from probabilistic maps of functional activation across independent groups of participants, and are generated such that they encapsulate most individuals’ functional regions, via the group-constrained subject-specific method (GSS)23. Contrary to traditional group-based methods (e.g., random-effects analyses) or using anatomical approximations or Talairach coordinates based on meta analyses, the GSS approach takes individual variability of functional responses (size, shape, and location) into account, providing the anchor space for functionally specialized regions that activate systematically across individuals. The present study especially benefits from this approach due to the study of nonverbal neonates. Additionally, these GSS studies were chosen particularly because the tasks and fMRI contrasts that were used to define the functional regions of interest offer better controls for the domains of interest. All parcels are available online or via contacting the corresponding author of the cited publications.

All parcels were mapped to the FreeSurfer CVS average-35 MNI152 brain (if they were not already publicly provided in that space) and were subsequently registered to each individual’s brain (see below). Language regions were released by Fedorenko et al. and were defined by Sentences vs. pronounceable non-word sentences23 thus controlling for prosody, low-level auditory features, and speaker identify, and are found to respond similarly to auditory and visual versions of the stimuli56,57. Temporal regions included: AntTemp, anterior temporal lobe; MidAntTemp, middle-anterior temporal lobe; MidPostTemp, middle-posterior temporal lobe; PostTemp, posterior temporal lobe; and AngG, angular gyrus. Frontal regions included: IFG, interior frontal gyrus; and IFGorb, orbital IFG. To get a narrower definition of language regions, we selected the IFG language parcel for Broca’s area and the MidAntTemp language parcel for Wernike’s area. The speech region was from Basilakos et al.58 and the region we used was in superior temporal gyrus, which was shown to be sensitive to the phonemic structure of human speech rather than low-level auditory properties or task-difficulty. A1 was anatomically defined as Heschl’s gyrus (superior and transverse temporal cortex from the FreeSurfer Desikan-Killiany parcellation59 in CVS average-35 MNI152 space). Multiple-demand (MD) parcels located in left frontal cortex were obtained from Fedorenko et al.24, showing activation to hard vs. easy conditions of working memory tasks24,57,60. These parcels were in MFGorb, orbital part of the middle frontal gyrus; Insula; IFGop, opercular part of the inferior frontal gyrus; SMA, supplementary motor area; and ACC, anterior/mid cingulate cortex. The VWFA, located in left occipitotemporal cortex, was created from Words vs. line drawings of Objects, from Saygin et al.18. The other high-level visual parcels were derived from Julian et al.25, and were based on responses to dynamic movie clips61 and activation for the contrast of interest. FFA and OFA located in the fusiform and occipital cortex respectively were identified with faces > objects contrasts6264 ; scene selective PPA was identified with scenes > objects contrast65 and was located in the parahippocampus; object selective LO and PFS were defined with objects > scrambled objects contrasts66 and located in the lateral occipital and posterior fusiform sulcus respectively. Because both VWFA and language are largely left lateralized33,67 , our study includes left hemisphere seeds and targets only, as was the case with previous studies of these regions19,31 as well as a recent study which also looked at VWFA connectivity using the adult HCP dataset45.

All functional parcels were placed in the template CVS average-35 MNI152 space, and were overlaid onto each individual’s native anatomical brain using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs version 2.1.0; https://stnava.github.io/ANTs)6870 for both adults and neonates. For registration between modalities (i.e., anatomical to native functional image for neonates), we used nearest neighbor interpolation with Freesurfer’s mri_vol2vol function (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/mri_vol2vol). To ensure no voxel belonged to more than one functional parcel, we assigned any intersecting voxels of two functional parcels to the one with smaller size as a way to compensate size differences (e.g., Brissenden et al.71). Additionally, voxels within white matter and cerebellum were also removed. In total, we used 20 non-overlapping functional parcels from eight categories in the present study.

Calculating functional connectivity

The mean timecourse of each functional parcel was computed from the preprocessed resting state images, and FC was calculated with Pearson’s correlation between the mean timecourse of each seed parcel and each target parcel. To generate normally distributed values, each FC value was Fisher z-transformed.

FC fingerprint plots

First, we calculated the average FC from the seed to each of the target categories. Then we subtracted the mean FC across all categories from each of the averaged FC. Thus, the value in the fingerprint plots indicates how the seed connects to the targets compared to the mean connectivity of the seed to all categories (mean-centering) across subjects in each group. We further quantified the similarity of FC patterns between adults and neonates. Specifically, for each participant, the Euclidean distance was calculated between the 4-dimensional FC pattern of the seed (i.e., VWFA or language regions) and the average FC pattern of others either from the same group or the different group. This measured how similar each participant was to others.

Voxel-wise FC analysis in the ventral temporal cortex (VTC) and frontotemporal cortex

We performed a voxel-wise analysis across VTC to get a finer characterization of the connectivity pattern with language regions. We defined the VTC from the Desikan-Killiany parcellation59, including the fusiform and inferior temporal labels, in FreeSurfer CVS average-35 MNI152 space, which were registered to each individual’s anatomy and masked with the gray matter image for each individual subject (as provided by the HCP and dHCP datasets). FC was computed between the mean timecourse of the language regions and the timecourse of each VTC voxel. Without predefining any functional parcels within the VTC, this analysis allowed us to characterize where the voxels with highest connectivity were located within the VTC. To quantify this, we performed a parametric analysis where we increased the threshold of FCs across all VTC voxels from the 50th percentile (median) to the 95th and calculated the overlap of these voxels with each of VTC regions with Dice coefficient. Specifically, each percentile determines the threshold for binarizing the connectivity data and overlap is calculated using Dice coefficient for each subject. For example, for the 50th percentile threshold, Dice coefficient was calculated by 2 * (A AND B) / A OR B, where set A are the voxels in VTC that are connected to language regions above the 50th percentile and set B are the voxels within the VWFA. We used Matlab to calculate percentiles and Dice coefficient. The same analysis was performed for frontal cortex and temporal cortex separately, and frontal and temporal cortex were again defined with Desikan-Killiany parcellation in the CVS average-35 MNI152 space and masked to only include gray matter within each subject’s individual space). Temporal cortex analyses were restricted to the more superior regions to prevent overlap with the VTC analysis. Individual subject results were projected to the surface of each subject using the surfaces provided by the dHCP and HCP with trilinear interpolation, which takes the average across the surface normal.

Statistics

2-way mixed design ANOVA were used to test our main focus. Age group (adults, neonates) was the between-subject variable and target (i.e., different target categories) was our within-subject variable (i.e., repeated-measures), and thus there was no experimental group randomization or blinding in the present study. Paired t-tests were conducted for within group comparisons and two-tailed t-tests for across-group comparisons. The 95% confidence interval of the mean FC true population difference was also reported for each post hoc t-test. Benjamini & Hochberg/Yekutieli false discovery rate control (FDR)72 was used for multiple comparisons correction. Each post hoc t-test was corrected for the total number of paired-wise comparisons for each analysis. Data distribution was assumed to be normal, but this was not formally tested.

Supplementary information

Acknowledgements

Adult data for the main Results were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. Neonate data were provided by the developing Human Connectome Project, KCL-Imperial-Oxford Consortium funded by the European Research Council under the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP/2007-2013) / ERC Grant Agreement no. [319456]. We are grateful to the families who generously supported this work. Analyses were completed using the Ohio Supercomputer (https://www.osc.edu). We would like to thank members of the Saygin Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Lab for feedback and comments and for Drs. Fedorenko and Kanwisher for help with the localizers for the Supplementary Results. Funding: This research was partly funded by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation (to Z.M.S) and Supplementary Results data were partly funded by NICHD/NIH grant F32HD079169 to Z.M.S. and MINT foundation grant to Kanwisher.

Author contributions

J.L.: conceptualization, formal analysis, methodology, writing-original draft & editing; D.E.O. and H.A.H.: methodology, writing-review; Z.M.S conceptualization, supervision, writing-review & editing.

Data availability

The data used in this study are publicly available. All relevant accession codes are publicly available in HCP (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult) and dHCP (https://www.developingconnectome.org).

Code availability

The code that supports the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Jin Li, Email: li.9361@osu.edu.

Zeynep M. Saygin, Email: saygin.3@osu.edu

Supplementary information

is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-75015-7.

References

  • 1.Deen B, et al. Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants. Nat. Commun. 2017;8:13995. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Arcaro MJ, Schade PF, Livingstone MS. Universal mechanisms and the development of the face network: What you see is what you get. Sci: Annu. Rev. Vis; 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Farroni T, et al. Newborns' preference for face-relevant stimuli: Effects of contrast polarity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2005;102:17245–17250. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502205102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Powell LJ, Kosakowski HL, Saxe R. Social origins of cortical face areas. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018;22:752–763. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.van den Hurk J, Van Baelen M, de Beeck HPO. Development of visual category selectivity in ventral visual cortex does not require visual experience. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2017;114:E4501–E4510. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612862114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L. Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science. 2002;298:2013–2015. doi: 10.1126/science.1077066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Srihasam K, Vincent JL, Livingstone MS. Novel domain formation reveals proto-architecture in inferotemporal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:1776. doi: 10.1038/nn.3855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Grill-Spector K, Kourtzi Z, Kanwisher N. The lateral occipital complex and its role in object recognition. Vis. Res. 2001;41:1409–1422. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00073-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dehaene S, Cohen L. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dehaene S, Cohen L. Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron. 2007;56:384–398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hasson U, Levy I, Behrmann M, Hendler T, Malach R. Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron. 2002;34:479–490. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00662-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Malach R, Levy I, Hasson U. The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002;6:176–184. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01870-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gomez, J., Barnett, M. & Grill-Spector, K.J.N.h.b. Extensive childhood experience with Pokémon suggests eccentricity drives organization of visual cortex. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 611–624 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 14.Osher DE, et al. Structural connectivity fingerprints predict cortical selectivity for multiple visual categories across cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;26:1668–1683. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Saygin ZM, et al. Anatomical connectivity patterns predict face selectivity in the fusiform gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:321. doi: 10.1038/nn.3001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Barttfeld P, et al. A lateral-to-mesial organization of human ventral visual cortex at birth. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:3107–3119. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1676-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Baker CI, et al. Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2007;104:9087–9092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703300104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dehaene S, et al. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science. 2010;330:1359–1364. doi: 10.1126/science.1194140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bouhali F, et al. Anatomical connections of the visual word form area. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:15402–15414. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4918-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Yeatman JD, Rauschecker AM, Wandell BA. Anatomy of the visual word form area: adjacent cortical circuits and long-range white matter connections. Brain Lang. 2013;125:146–155. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Epelbaum S, et al. Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: new diffusion imaging evidence for an old concept. Cortex. 2008;44:962–974. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Saygin ZM, et al. Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1250. doi: 10.1038/nn.4354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fedorenko E, Hsieh P-J, Nieto-Castañón A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Kanwisher N. New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1177–1194. doi: 10.1152/jn.00032.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fedorenko E, Duncan J, Kanwisher N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2013;110:16616–16621. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1315235110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Julian JB, Fedorenko E, Webster J, Kanwisher N. An algorithmic method for functionally defining regions of interest in the ventral visual pathway. Neuroimage. 2012;60:2357–2364. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sur M, Garraghty PE, Roe AW. Experimentally induced visual projections into auditory thalamus and cortex. Science. 1988;242:1437–1441. doi: 10.1126/science.2462279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Roe AW, Pallas SL, Hahm J-O, Sur M. A map of visual space induced in primary auditory cortex. Science. 1990;250:818–820. doi: 10.1126/science.2237432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Roe AW, Pallas SL, Kwon YH, Sur M. Visual projections routed to the auditory pathway in ferrets: receptive fields of visual neurons in primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 1992;12:3651–3664. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-09-03651.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sharma J, Angelucci A, Sur M. Induction of visual orientation modules in auditory cortex. Nature. 2000;404:841. doi: 10.1038/35009043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Horng S, et al. Differential gene expression in the developing lateral geniculate nucleus and medial geniculate nucleus reveals novel roles for Zic4 and Foxp2 in visual and auditory pathway development. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:13672–13683. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2127-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stevens WD, Kravitz DJ, Peng CS, Tessler MH, Martin A. Privileged functional connectivity between the visual word form area and the language system. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:5288–5297. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0138-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Arcaro MJ, Livingstone MS. A hierarchical, retinotopic proto-organization of the primate visual system at birth. Elife. 2017;6:e26196. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dundas EM, Plaut DC, Behrmann M. The joint development of hemispheric lateralization for words and faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2013;142:348–358. doi: 10.1037/a0029503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Augustine E, Jones SS, Smith LB, Longfield E. Relations among early object recognition skills: objects and letters. J. Cognit. Dev. 2015;16:221–235. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.815620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kubota, E.C., Joo, S.J., Huber, E. & Yeatman, J.D. Word selectivity in high-level visual cortex and reading skill. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 36.Langer N, et al. White matter alterations in infants at risk for developmental dyslexia. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:1027–1036. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Arslan, S., Parisot, S. & Rueckert, D. Joint spectral decomposition for the parcellation of the human cerebral cortex using resting-state fMRI. in International Conference on Information Processing in Medical Imaging 85–97 (Springer, 2015). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.White AL, Palmer J, Boynton GM, Yeatman JD. Parallel spatial channels converge at a bottleneck in anterior word-selective cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2019;116:10087–10096. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1822137116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lerma-Usabiaga G, Carreiras M, Paz-Alonso PM. Converging evidence for functional and structural segregation within the left ventral occipitotemporal cortex in reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2018;115:E9981–E9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803003115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Vogel AC, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The left occipitotemporal cortex does not show preferential activity for words. Cereb. Cortex. 2012;22:2715–2732. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Grill-Spector K, Weiner KS. The functional architecture of the ventral temporal cortex and its role in categorization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2014;15:536–548. doi: 10.1038/nrn3747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Vinckier F, et al. Hierarchical coding of letter strings in the ventral stream: dissecting the inner organization of the visual word-form system. Neuron. 2007;55:143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Rothlein D, Rapp B. The similarity structure of distributed neural responses reveals the multiple representations of letters. Neuroimage. 2014;89:331–344. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Glezer LS, Jiang X, Riesenhuber M. Evidence for highly selective neuronal tuning to whole words in the “visual word form area”. Neuron. 2009;62:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Chen L, et al. The visual word form area (VWFA) is part of both language and attention circuitry. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07882-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Makropoulos A, et al. The developing human connectome project: A minimal processing pipeline for neonatal cortical surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 2018;173:88–112. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Van Essen DC, et al. The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. Neuroimage. 2013;80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hughes EJ, et al. A dedicated neonatal brain imaging system. Magn Reson Med. 2017;78:794–804. doi: 10.1002/mrm.26462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Liu X, Yanagawa T, Leopold DA, Fujii N, Duyn JH. Robust long-range coordination of spontaneous neural activity in waking, sleep and anesthesia. Cereb. Cortex. 2015;25:2929–2938. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Larson-Prior LJ, et al. Cortical network functional connectivity in the descent to sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106:4489–4494. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900924106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Makropoulos A, et al. Automatic whole brain MRI segmentation of the developing neonatal brain. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2014;33:1818–1831. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2014.2322280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fitzgibbon, S.P., et al. The developing Human Connectome Project (dHCP) automated resting-state functional processing framework for newborn infants. BioRxiv. 766030 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 54.Salimi-Khorshidi G, et al. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. Neuroimage. 2014;90:449–468. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Behzadi Y, Restom K, Liau J, Liu TT. A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage. 2007;37(1):90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Scott TL, Gallée J, Fedorenko E. A new fun and robust version of an fMRI localizer for the frontotemporal language system. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;8:167–176. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2016.1201466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Fedorenko E. The role of domain-general cognitive control in language comprehension. Front. Psychol. 2014;5:335. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Basilakos A, Smith KG, Fillmore P, Fridriksson J, Fedorenko EJCC. Functional characterization of the human speech articulation network. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28:1816–1830. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Desikan RS, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Blank I, Kanwisher N, Fedorenko E. A functional dissociation between language and multiple-demand systems revealed in patterns of BOLD signal fluctuations. J. Neurophysiol. 2014;112:1105–1118. doi: 10.1152/jn.00884.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Pitcher D, Dilks DD, Saxe RR, Triantafyllou C, Kanwisher N. Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective cortical regions. Neuroimage. 2011;56:2356–2363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.McCarthy G, Puce A, Gore JC, Allison T. Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997;9:605–610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Gauthier I, et al. The fusiform “face area” is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2000;12:495–504. doi: 10.1162/089892900562165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–4311. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Epstein R, Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature. 1998;392:598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Grill-Spector K, et al. Differential processing of objects under various viewing conditions in the human lateral occipital complex. Neuron. 1999;24:187–203. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80832-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Frost JA, et al. Language processing is strongly left lateralized in both sexes: Evidence from functional MRI. Brain. 1999;122:199–208. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.2.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wang H, Yushkevich P. Multi-atlas segmentation with joint label fusion and corrective learning—an open source implementation. Front. Neuroinform. 2013;7:27. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2013.00027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Menze BH, et al. The multimodal brain tumor image segmentation benchmark (BRATS) IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2014;34:1993–2024. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2014.2377694. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Avants BB, et al. The Insight ToolKit image registration framework. Front. Neuroinform. 2014;8:44. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Brissenden JA, Levin EJ, Osher DE, Halko MA, Somers DC. Functional evidence for a cerebellar node of the dorsal attention network. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:6083–6096. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0344-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Benjamini Y, Yekutieli D. False discovery rate: adjusted multiple confidence intervals for selected parameters. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2005;100:71–81. doi: 10.1198/016214504000001907. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data used in this study are publicly available. All relevant accession codes are publicly available in HCP (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult) and dHCP (https://www.developingconnectome.org).

The code that supports the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES