Abstract
The neurotransmitter serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) has been linked to multiple aspects of cognition. For example, in rodents, discrimination and reversal learning are altered by experimentally induced changes in brain serotonin levels, and reduced expression of the 5-HT2B receptor subtype in mice and humans is associated with decreased serotonergic tone and increased behavioral impulsivity. Serotonin modulates cognitive flexibility as well as fear and anxiety, but the specific contributions of 5-HT2B receptors to these behaviors is unknown. The current study assessed mice with partial Htr2b deletion for performance on a touchscreen-based pairwise visual discrimination and reversal learning task followed by a test of cued fear learning. Male Htr2b heterozygous mice (+/−) and littermate controls (+/+) were trained to discriminate between two visual stimuli presented on a touch-sensitive screen, one which predicted delivery of a 14-mg food pellet and the other which was not rewarded. Once discrimination performance criterion was attained, the stimulus-reward contingencies were reversed. Htr2b +/− mice were faster to reach discrimination criterion than +/+ controls, and made fewer errors. Htr2b +/− mice were also slower to make responses and collect rewards. Conversely, measures of reversal learning were not different between genotypes. Pavlovian cued fear conditioning was also normal in Htr2b +/−mice. These data demonstrate a selective improvement in touchscreen-based discrimination learning in mice with partial deletion of the 5-HT2B receptor, and provide further insight into the role of the 5-HT2B receptor in cognition.
Keywords: serotonin, behavioral flexibility, touchscreen, fear, discrimination, learning
Introduction
The neurotransmitter serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine, 5-HT) has been suggested to mediate cognitive processes that contribute to flexible behavior [1,2]. Across species, behavioral flexibility is commonly assessed using assays of discrimination and reversal learning, where animals first learn to discriminate between distinct stimuli, and then must reverse this learned contingency. Serotonin depletion has been shown to facilitate the acquisition of a conditional visual discrimination in rodents [3,4], an effect that appears to be mediated by faster correct responding and reduced proactive interference from preceding trials [4,5].
In line with these observations, increasing serotonin levels with monoamine oxidase inhibitors impairs learning of a spatial discrimination [6,7]. Further, manipulations that increase serotonergic tone, such as genetic deletion of the serotonin transporter (SERT) and chronic fluoxetine treatment, reduce errors made during reversal learning [8]. Reversal learning is also impaired by depletion of cortical, but not striatal, serotonin as rodents fail to inhibit responses toward a previously rewarded stimulus [9–11]. Thus, these findings suggest that serotonin slows discrimination learning while facilitating flexibility during reversal. These effects may be mediated by multiple processes, including serotonergic control of response inhibition, reward sensitivity, negative feedback sensitivity, or general outcome tracking [2].
Serotonin acts on a diverse class of G-protein coupled and ligand-gated receptors. Although many of these receptors have been shown to regulate behavioral flexibility [12–16], relatively little is known regarding the contribution of 5-HT2B receptors to this process. 5-HT2B receptors are G-protein coupled receptors and are found in several brain regions involved in cognitive functions, including the raphe nuclei, hypothalamus, amygdala, hippocampus, septum, and cerebellum [17–19]. Interestingly, somatodendritic stimulation of G-q coupled 5-HT2B receptors in the dorsal raphe nuclei increases serotonergic tone [19–21], suggesting a variety of mechanisms through which this receptor subtype could modulate behavioral flexibility.
Prior studies have found that HTR2B knockout, produced by a heterozygous stop codon in this gene, is associated with higher rates of human impulsive behavior as well as psychosis and early-onset schizophrenia [22,23]. Various other HTR2B polymorphisms have been associated with a constellation of neurobehavioral dysfunctions, including drug abuse, aggression, and impulsive behavioral profiles [24–26]. In mice, constitutive deletion of the Htr2b gene leads to a number of behavioral alterations similar to those associated with the human HTR2B variants, including increased impulsivity, hyper-responsivity to novelty, deficient sensorimotor gating, social impairment and abnormal performance on various measures of learning, including fear learning [22,27]. Whether Htr2b contributes to flexible reward learning has not yet been investigated. Here, we sought to extend the existing literature on serotonin’s role in behavioral flexibility and fear learning by assessing the performance of mice heterozygous for a Htr2b null mutation on a touchscreen-based pairwise visual discrimination and reversal learning task and a test of cued fear learning.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Male Htr2b heterozygous (+/−) mice (n = 7) and wild-type littermate controls (Htr2b +/+, n = 9) were bred from founder mice (+/+ bred with +/−) received from the Mutant Mouse Resource & Research Centers Depository (MGI: 3604521). This line was developed by Deltagen using 129P2/OlaHsd ES cells that were backcrossed to produce a congenic C57BL/6J knockout mouse line. We have previously demonstrated robust performance on touchscreen-based tasks in the C57BL/6J strain [28,29]. All mice were bred at the National Institute on Drug Abuse Intramural Research Program (Baltimore, Maryland) and transferred to the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse Intramural Research Program (Rockville, MD) for testing. Mice averaged 190 days of age (range 140–260 days) at the time of testing and genotypes were matched for age and free-feeding weight at the start of testing. Mice were housed in groups of 2–4 for the duration of the experiment on a 12-h light-dark cycle and tested during the light phase of the cycle. All experimental procedures were performed in accordance with the Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory Animals [30] and approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse Intramural Research Program.
Discrimination and reversal testing
Apparatus and pre-training.
Prior to training, all mice were food restricted (85% of free-feeding weight) to motivate responding for the food reward. Mice underwent visual discrimination and reversal learning training using the Bussey-Saksida Touch Screen System (model 80614, Lafayette Instruments, Lafayette, IN, USA), as previously described [28,31]. A 14-mg food pellet served as the reward (#F05684, BioServ, Frenchtown, NJ, USA).
Discrimination.
Two novel 6.5 cm2 stimuli (‘fan’ and ‘marbles’) were presented simultaneously in a spatially randomized manner. For all mice, responses at the ‘fan’ stimulus were reinforced continuously (100% probability of reward) while responses at the ‘marble’ stimulus (=‘errors’) produced no food reward and a 15-s ‘timeout’ period during which the house light was turned off and reinforcers could not be earned. The designation of the correct stimulus was not counterbalanced across mice as we have found the fan and marble stimuli to be preferred equally by C57BL/6J male mice (e.g., Radke et al., 2019). Each error was followed by a correction trial (“correction”) in which the two stimuli were presented in the same spatial configuration. The next trial proper could not begin until a correct response was made on a correction trial. Mice were given the opportunity to complete 30 trials (excluding correction trials) per daily 60-min session until they reached a performance criterion of ≥85% correct responses on two consecutive sessions.
Reversal.
Here, the designation of stimuli as correct versus incorrect was reversed for each mouse such that the correct stimulus was ‘marble’ and the incorrect stimulus was ‘fan.’ Mice underwent daily reversal training sessions (30 trials/day) until a criterion of ≥85% correct responding (excluding correction trials) over two consecutive sessions was met.
Cued fear conditioning
Four weeks after the completion of reversal learning, mice underwent fear conditioning and testing as described previously [32,33]. Following 180 s of acclimation to the chamber, mice received three pairings of the conditioned stimulus (CS) (30-s, 80-dB white noise cue) and unconditioned stimulus (US) (a co-terminating, 2-s, 0.6-mA scrambled footshock) with an inter trial interval (ITI) of 60 to 90 s. 24-h later fear learning was tested in a new context. Following a 180-s acclimation period, the CS was presented four times for 30 s (5-s ITI).
Statistical analysis
For discrimination and reversal learning, dependent measures included sessions to criterion, total trials, percent correct responses (=100*(correct choices/total choice)), total errors, total correction trials (=corrections), and latency to choice (correct and incorrect), and latency to collect reward. To examine choice strategy, individual trials during each discrimination and reversal session were categorized as wins (=correct choice) or losses (=incorrect choice) [28]. Choice on trials following each win or loss were then classified either as a win-stay (correct following correct), or a lose-shift (correct following incorrect). The percentage of win-stay trials was calculated as follows: (# of win-stay trials / total # of wins) * 100. The percentage of lose-shift trials was calculated as follows: (# of lose-shift trials / total # of losses) * 100. Data from discrimination and reversal learning were analyzed by comparing genotypes with t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons with the Holm-Sidak method and adjusted p-values are reported. Sessions to criterion was analyzed by comparing survival curves with a Log-rank test.
In the fear conditioning task, % freezing was calculated (=[amount of time freezing/total time of baseline or CS] × 100). Due to a technical problem, data from three mice (two +/+ and one +/−) were lost from the fear conditioning session. Data were analyzed with RM two-way ANOVA with genotype and trial as factors and post hoc Dunnett’s test. All data were analyzed in GraphPad Prism (v8) with an alpha level of 0.05. Data are expressed as means ± standard error of the mean in all figures and tables.
Results
Htr2b +/− mice showed facilitated discrimination learning
Htr2b +/− mice reached discrimination criterion in fewer sessions than +/+ controls, as evidenced by a difference in the session-to-criterion survival curves in the genotypes (Log-rank test, χ2(1, n = 16) = 5.683, P = 0.017) (Figure 1A) and the number of sessions (t14 = 2.502, P = 0.050). Htr2b +/+ mice averaged 12.56 ± 1.08 sessions on discrimination and +/− mice averaged 8.43 ± 1.25. The total number of trials (t14 = 3.021, P = 0.036) and errors (t14 = 2.759, P = 0.045) completed were also lower in Htr2b +/− mice relative to +/+ controls (Figure 1B). Correction trials did not differ between the genotypes (t14 = 1.671, P = 0.117) (Figure 1B). The average number of trials, errors, and corrections completed per session were not different between genotypes (P > 0.05 for all). Htr2b +/− mice were slower to collect rewards (t14 = 2.547, P = 0.023) and make correct (t14 = 4.928, P < 0.001) and incorrect (t14 = 5.457, P < 0.001) responses (Figure 1C). Lastly, the percent of win-stay and lose-shift trials completed during discrimination were not affected by genotype (Figure 1D).
Figure 1: Htr2b +/− mice showed facilitated discrimination learning.
(A) Survival analysis of sessions to discrimination criterion suggests improved performance in Htr2b +/− mice. (B) Total trials, errors, and correction trials completed during all discrimination sessions in control +/+ mice (light gray) and +/− (red) mice. (C) Latency to collect rewards and make correct and incorrect responses during all discrimination sessions. (D) Percent of win-stay and lose-shift trials for discrimination. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 versus Htr2b +/+ controls.
Htr2b +/− mice show normal reversal learning
There were no genotype differences in session-to-criterion survival curves (Log-rank test, χ2(1, n = 16) = 0.012, P = 0.913) (Figure 2A) or sessions to reach reversal criterion (t14 = 0.382, P = 0.961). Htr2b +/+ mice averaged 15.22 ± 3.95 sessions on reversal and +/− mice averaged 13.43 ± 1.45. Total trials (t14 = 0.293, P = 0.961), errors (t14 = 0.447, P = 0.961) and correction trials (t14 = 0.679, P = 0.942) were also not different between the genotypes (Figure 2B). The average latency to make an incorrect response was higher in Htr2b +/− mice than +/+ controls (t14 = 2.740, P = 0.047) but though also trending higher, the latency to make a correct response (t14 = 2.213, P = 0.086) and collect the reward (t14 = 1.858, P = 0.086) was not significantly higher in the Htr2b +/− mice (Figure 2C). The percent of win-stay and lose-shift trials completed during reversal did not differ by genotype (Figure 2D).
Figure 2: Htr2b +/− mice show normal reversal learning.
(A) Survival analysis of sessions to reversal criterion suggests Htr2b +/− mice perform similar to controls. (B) Total trials, errors, and correction trials completed during all reversal sessions in control +/+ mice (light gray) and +/−(red) mice. (C) Latency to collect rewards and make correct and incorrect responses during all reversal sessions. (D) Percent of win-stay and lose-shift trials for reversal. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, versus Htr2b +/+ controls.
Htr2b +/− mice show normal conditioned fear learning
Mice acquired fear learning (main effect of trial: F3,36 = 17.82, P < 0.001) but there were no differences between Htr2b +/− and control mice (main effect of genotype: F1,12 = 0.255, P = 0.623; trial x genotype interaction: F3,36 = 0.424, P = 0.737) (Figure 3A). When data were collapsed across genotype, freezing during the second (P < 0.001) and third (P < 0.001) trials were significantly higher than at baseline (post-hoc Dunnett’s test).
Figure 3: Htr2b +/−mice show normal conditioned fear learning.
(A) Acquisition of fear was similar between genotypes, as evidence by freezing following three CS presentations. (B) Freezing was also similar between genotypes during a test of fear learning. Data expressed as mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 versus baseline (BL).
Mice of the different genotypes froze at similar levels during a test of fear learning (main effect of genotype: F1,15 = 0.25, P = 0.823; trial x genotype interaction: F4,60 = 0.530, P = 0.714) (Figure 3B). There was a significant main effect of trial (F4,60 = 5.924, P < 0.001). When data were collapsed across genotype, post-hoc comparisons (Dunnett’s test) revealed that freezing was significantly higher than baseline during the first (P < 0.001), second (P = 0.001), and third (P = 0.009) trials (trial 4: P = 0.072).
Discussion
The main finding of the current study was that male Htr2b +/− mice exhibit improved learning on a visual discrimination task. Htr2b +/− mice required fewer sessions, trials, and errors to reach the discrimination criterion, as compared to Htr2b +/+ control mice. This phenotype was specific to discrimination, as subsequent reversal learning was normal in Htr2b +/−mice. Pavlovian fear conditioning was also normal in these mice.
The mechanisms underlying the facilitation of discrimination learning in Htr2b +/− mice are unclear. One possible explanation for the enhanced visual discrimination observed here in Htr2b +/− mice is that partial loss of the 5-HT2B receptor may indirectly decrease serotonergic tone [20], which has been previously shown to improve discrimination learning [3,4]. A recent ex vivo electrophysiology study found that application of a 5-HT2B receptor agonist increased serotonin neuronal firing in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN). Based on this and other observations, the authors suggest that somatodendritic serotonin release in the DRN stimulates Gq-coupled 5-HT2B receptors resulting in a positive feedback loop that increases serotonergic tone [20]. This mechanism is thought to contribute to the role of 5-HT2B receptors in the antidepressant actions of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors [21] and other drugs targeting SERT [34]. If 5-HT2B activation potentiates serotonergic signaling, then lesser Htr2b gene expression in Htr2b +/− mice could have decreased basal extracellular serotonin levels and reduced serotonin release events. Because the current study is, to our knowledge, the first examination of heterozygous Htr2b null mutant mice, this mechanism remains speculative as an explanation of the improved discrimination learning in these mice. It is, however, supported by earlier work showing that reductions in serotonin signaling facilitate this form of learning [3–5]. Further work is needed to determine the precise nature of serotoninergic abnormalities in Htr2b +/− mice.
Serotonin levels, particularly in the orbitofrontal and prefrontal cortices, positively correlate with reversal learning performance [35–37] and experimentally increasing serotonin signaling promotes reversal learning [2,8,10,11]. Given that partial deletion of the 5-HT2B receptor may indirectly decrease serotonin release, these prior findings predict reversal learning might be impaired in Htr2b +/− mice. However, these mice did not display abnormalities in reversal performance in the touchscreen-based task. These negative data suggest that, although 5-HT2B receptors may be involved in initial rule learning, they may be less critical for intra-dimensional shifting within the same rule, possibly because partial Htr2b deletion does not reduce serotonin levels sufficiently to alter reversal learning. In this context, the current data show that Htr2b +/− mice exhibit normal conditioned fear, whereas a previous study found fear conditioning deficits in full Htr2b −/− mice [27]. It will therefore be interesting to test whether Htr2b −/− mice display deficits in reversal learning.
We have previously shown that dopamine and striatal regions in which dopamine signals contribute to performance in the touchscreen-based discrimination [28,38,39] and reversal [29] tasks employed in the current study. For example, we found impaired discrimination performance and slower response latencies in mutant mice with reduced phasic dopamine neuron firing due to deletion of the NMDA NR1 subunit on dopaminergic neurons [39]. These findings are pertinent to the current study in that the 5-HT2B receptor has been shown to alter dopaminergic signaling and other neurotransmitter systems relevant to behavioral flexibility [40,41]. 5-HT2B receptors regulate dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens [40,41] and targeted deletion of the receptor on mesolimbic dopamine neurons alters the firing of those cells [42]. Tissue levels of dopamine and dopamine type-2 receptor mRNA expression are decreased in the dorsal striatum of Htr2b −/−mice [27]. If present, similar alterations in dopaminergic signaling in Htr2b +/− mice could account for the slower response and reward retrieval latencies observed here. Of interest, this latter finding was unexpected as baseline locomotion is preserved in mice with full Htr2b deletion while the locomotor response to novelty and psychostimulants in enhanced [27,34]. Further studies will be needed to determine whether longer latencies represent motivational or attentional deficits.
It is important to recognize that the results reported here may be influenced by factors such as the age or sex of the mice. Due in part to the extended nature of the discrimination and reversal learning tasks, some mice were approaching 10 months of age by the end of this study. Further, these experiments were only conducted in male mice. It is, unfortunately, difficult to speculate whether these results would also translate to female mice, as there is very little data on reversal learning in females. It should also be noted that the sample size used here might also not have been large enough to detect some effects, such as the trend for increased correct response latencies during reversal.
In summary, partial gene deletion of the serotonin Htr2b receptor subtype in male mice results in improved touchscreen discrimination learning, without concomitant alterations in reversal learning or Pavlovian fear conditioning. These findings provide further evidence for a role for this receptor subtype in cognition, with implications for understanding how 5-HT2B receptor abnormalities might contribute to the pathophysiology of disordered cognition in psychiatric disease.
Highlights.
5-HT2B receptors are implicated in cognitive function and regulation of serotonin signaling
Male mice with partial Htr2b deletion were tested for discrimination, reversal, and fear learing
Htr2b +/− mice were faster to reach discrimination criterion than +/+ controls and made fewer errors
Reversal and fear learning were normal in Htr2b +/−mice
Acknowledgments
Research supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and the National Institute on Drug Abuse Intramural Research Programs.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- [1].Dalley JW, Roiser JP, Dopamine, serotonin and impulsivity, Neuroscience. 215 (2012) 42–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Izquierdo A, Brigman JL, Radke AK, Rudebeck PH, Holmes A, The neural basis of reversal learning: An updated perspective, Neuroscience. 345 (2017) 12–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Stevens DA, Resnick O, Krus DM, The effects of p-chlorophenylalanine, a depletor of brain serotonin, on behavior. I. Facilitation of discrimination learning, Life Sci. 6 (1967) 2215–2220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Ward BO, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ, Forebrain serotonin depletion facilitates the acquisition and performance of a conditional visual discrimination task in rats, Behav. Brain Res 100 (1999) 51–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Harrison AA, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, Central serotonin depletion impairs both the acquisition and performance of a symmetrically reinforced go/no-go conditional visual discrimination, Behav. Brain Res 100 (1999) 99–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Woolley DW, van der Hoeven T, Alteration in learning ability caused by changes in cerebral serotonin and catecholamines, Science. 139 (1963) 610–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Woolley DW, Van der Hoeven T, Serotonin deficiency in infancy as a cause of a mental defect in experimental phenylketonuria, Int. J. Neuropsychiatry 1 (1965) 529–544. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [8].Brigman JL, Mathur P, Harvey-White J, Izquierdo A, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ, Fox S, Deneris E, Murphy DL, Holmes A, Pharmacological or genetic inactivation of the serotonin transporter improves reversal learning in mice, Cereb. Cortex 20 (2010) 1955–1963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts AC, Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion, Science. 304 (2004) 878–880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Clarke HF, Walker SC, Crofts HS, Dalley JW, Robbins TW, Roberts AC, Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting, J. Neurosci 25 (2005) 532–538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [11].Clarke HF, Hill GJ, Robbins TW, Roberts AC, Dopamine, but not serotonin, regulates reversal learning in the marmoset caudate nucleus, J. Neurosci 31 (2011) 4290–4297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Hatcher PD, Brown VJ, Tait DS, Bate S, Overend P, Hagan JJ, Jones DNC, 5-HT6 receptor antagonists improve performance in an attentional set shifting task in rats, Psychopharmacology. 181 (2005) 253–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Holmes A, Genetic variation in cortico-amygdala serotonin function and risk for stress-related disease, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 32 (2008) 1293–1314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Boulougouris V, Glennon JC, Robbins TW, Dissociable effects of selective 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptor antagonists on serial spatial reversal learning in rats, Neuropsychopharmacology. 33 (2008) 2007–2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Baker PM, Thompson JL, Sweeney JA, Ragozzino ME, Differential effects of 5-HT(2A) and 5-HT(2C) receptor blockade on strategy-switching, Behav. Brain Res 219 (2011) 123–131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Nikiforuk A, Selective blockade of 5-HT7 receptors facilitates attentional set-shifting in stressed and control rats, Behav. Brain Res 226 (2012) 118–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Choi DS, Maroteaux L, Immunohistochemical localisation of the serotonin 5-HT2B receptor in mouse gut, cardiovascular system, and brain, FEBS Lett. 391 (1996) 45–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Duxon MS, Flanigan TP, Reavley AC, Baxter GS, Blackburn TP, Fone KC, Evidence for expression of the 5-hydroxytryptamine-2B receptor protein in the rat central nervous system, Neuroscience. 76 (1997) 323–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Launay JM, Schneider B, Loric S, Da Prada M, Kellermann O, Serotonin transport and serotonin transporter-mediated antidepressant recognition are controlled by 5-HT2B receptor signaling in serotonergic neuronal cells, FASEB J. 20 (2006) 1843–1854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Belmer A, Quentin E, Diaz SL, Guiard BP, Fernandez SP, Doly S, Banas SM, Pitychoutis PM, Moutkine I, Muzerelle A, Tchenio A, Roumier A, Mameli M, Maroteaux L, Positive regulation of raphe serotonin neurons by serotonin 2B receptors, Neuropsychopharmacology. 43 (2018) 1623–1632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Diaz SL, Doly S, Narboux-Nême N, Fernández S, Mazot P, Banas SM, Boutourlinsky K, Moutkine I, Belmer A, Roumier A, Maroteaux L, 5-HT(2B) receptors are required for serotonin-selective antidepressant actions, Mol. Psychiatry 17 (2012) 154–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Bevilacqua L, Doly S, Kaprio J, Yuan Q, Tikkanen R, Paunio T, Zhou Z, Wedenoja J, Maroteaux L, Diaz S, Belmer A, Hodgkinson CA, Dell’osso L, Suvisaari J, Coccaro E, Rose RJ, Peltonen L, Virkkunen M, Goldman D, A population-specific HTR2B stop codon predisposes to severe impulsivity, Nature. 468 (2010) 1061–1066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Tikkanen R, Tiihonen J, Rautiainen MR, Paunio T, Bevilacqua L, Panarsky R, Goldman D, Virkkunen M, Impulsive alcohol-related risk-behavior and emotional dysregulation among individuals with a serotonin 2B receptor stop codon, Transl. Psychiatry 5 (2015) e681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Lacoste J, Lamy S, Ramoz N, Ballon N, Jehel L, Maroteaux L, Thibaut F, A positive association between a polymorphism in the HTR2B gene and cocaine-crack in a French Afro-Caribbean population, World J. Biol. Psychiatry (2019) 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Montalvo-Ortiz JL, Zhou H, D’Andrea I, Maroteaux L, Lori A, Smith A, Ressler KJ, Nuñez YZ, Farrer LA, Zhao H, Kranzler HR, Gelernter J, Translational studies support a role for serotonin 2B receptor (HTR2B) gene in aggression-related cannabis response, Mol. Psychiatry 23 (2018) 2277–2286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Zhu B, Chen C, Moyzis RK, Dong Q, Chen C, He Q, Li J, Lei X, Lin C, Association between the HTR2B gene and the personality trait of fun seeking, Pers. Individ. Dif 53 (2012) 1029–1033. [Google Scholar]
- [27].Pitychoutis PM, Belmer A, Moutkine I, Adrien J, Maroteaux L, Mice lacking the serotonin Htr2B receptor gene present an antipsychotic-sensitive schizophrenic-like phenotype, Neuropsychopharmacology. 40 (2015) 2764–2773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Bergstrom HC, Lipkin AM, Lieberman AG, Pinard CR, Gunduz-Cinar O, Brockway ET, Taylor WW, Nonaka M, Bukalo O, Wills TA, Rubio FJ, Li X, Pickens CL, Winder DG, Holmes A, Dorsolateral striatum engagement interferes with early discrimination learning, Cell Rep. 23 (2018) 2264–2272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Radke AK, Kocharian A, Covey DP, Lovinger DM, Cheer JF, Mateo Y, Holmes A, Contributions of nucleus accumbens dopamine to cognitive flexibility, Eur. J. Neurosci 50 (2019) 2023–2035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].National Research Council, Division on Earth and Life Studies, Institute for Laboratory Animal Research, Committee for the Update of the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals: Eighth Edition, National Academies Press, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- [31].Brigman JL, Ihne J, Saksida LM, Bussey TJ, Holmes A, Effects of subchronic Phencyclidine (PCP) treatment on social Behaviors, and operant discrimination and reversal learning in C57BL/6J mice, Front. Behav. Neurosci 3 (2009) 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Gunduz-Cinar O, Brockway E, Lederle L, Wilcox T, Halladay LR, Ding Y, Oh H, Busch EF, Kaugars K, Flynn S, Limoges A, Bukalo O, MacPherson KP, Masneuf S, Pinard C, Sibille E, Chesler EJ, Holmes A, Identification of a novel gene regulating amygdala-mediated fear extinction, Mol. Psychiatry 24 (2019) 601–612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Sengupta A, Holmes A, A Discrete Dorsal Raphe to Basal Amygdala 5-HT circuit calibrates aversive memory, Neuron. 103 (2019) 489–505.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Doly S, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Callebert J, Bruneau A, Banas SM, Belmer A, Boutourlinsky K, Hervé D, Launay J-M, Maroteaux L, Role of serotonin via 5-HT2B receptors in the reinforcing effects of MDMA in mice, PLoS One. 4 (2009) e7952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Groman SM, James AS, Seu E, Crawford MA, Harpster SN, Jentsch JD, Monoamine levels within the orbitofrontal cortex and putamen interact to predict reversal learning performance, Biol. Psychiatry 73 (2013) 756–762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Stolyarova A, O’Dell SJ, Marshall JF, Izquierdo A, Positive and negative feedback learning and associated dopamine and serotonin transporter binding after methamphetamine, Behav. Brain Res 271 (2014) 195–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Barlow RL, Alsiö J, Jupp B, Rabinovich R, Shrestha S, Roberts AC, Robbins TW, Dalley JW, Markers of serotonergic function in the orbitofrontal cortex and dorsal raphé nucleus predict individual variation in spatial-discrimination serial reversal learning, Neuropsychopharmacology. 40 (2015) 1619–1630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Brigman JL, Wright T, Talani G, Prasad-Mulcare S, Jinde S, Seabold GK, Mathur P, Davis MI, Bock R, Gustin RM, Colbran RJ, Alvarez VA, Nakazawa K, Delpire E, Lovinger DM, Holmes A, Loss of GluN2B-containing NMDA receptors in CA1 hippocampus and cortex impairs long-term depression, reduces dendritic spine density, and disrupts learning, J. Neurosci 30 (2010) 4590–4600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Radke AK, Zweifel LS, Holmes A, NMDA receptor deletion on dopamine neurons disrupts visual discrimination and reversal learning, Neurosci. Lett 699 (2019) 109–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Auclair AL, Cathala A, Sarrazin F, Depoortère R, Piazza PV, Newman-Tancredi A, Spampinato U, The central serotonin2B receptor: a new pharmacological target to modulate the mesoaccumbens dopaminergic pathway activity, J. Neurochem 114 (2010) 1323–1332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Devroye C, Cathala A, Di Marco B, Caraci F, Drago F, Piazza PV, Spampinato U, Central serotonin2B receptor blockade inhibits cocaine-induced hyperlocomotion independently of changes of subcortical dopamine outflow, Neuropharmacology. 97 (2015) 329–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Doly S, Quentin E, Eddine R, Tolu S, Fernandez SP, Bertran-Gonzalez J, Valjent E, Belmer A, Viñals X, Callebert J, Faure P, Meye FJ, Hervé D, Robledo P, Mameli M, Launay J-M, Maldonado R, Maroteaux L, Serotonin 2B receptors in mesoaccumbens dopamine pathway regulate cocaine responses, J. Neurosci 37 (2017) 10372–10388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]