Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Oct 15;9:e59281. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59281

Divergent projections of the prelimbic cortex bidirectionally regulate active avoidance

Maria M Diehl 1,†,, Jorge M Iravedra-Garcia 1, Jonathan Morán-Sierra 1, Gabriel Rojas-Bowe 1, Fabiola N Gonzalez-Diaz 1, Viviana P Valentín-Valentín 1, Gregory J Quirk 1
Editors: Geoffrey Schoenbaum2, Michael J Frank3
PMCID: PMC7588229  PMID: 33054975

Abstract

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) integrates incoming information to guide our actions. When motivation for food-seeking competes with avoidance of danger, the PFC likely plays a role in selecting the optimal choice. In platform-mediated active avoidance, rats avoid a tone-signaled footshock by stepping onto a nearby platform, delaying access to sucrose pellets. This avoidance requires prelimbic (PL) PFC, basolateral amygdala (BLA), and ventral striatum (VS). We previously showed that inhibitory tone responses of PL neurons correlate with avoidability of shock (Diehl et al., 2018). Here, we optogenetically modulated PL terminals in VS and BLA to identify PL outputs regulating avoidance. Photoactivating PL-VS projections reduced avoidance, whereas photoactivating PL-BLA projections increased avoidance. Moreover, photosilencing PL-BLA or BLA-VS projections reduced avoidance, suggesting that VS receives opposing inputs from PL and BLA. Bidirectional modulation of avoidance by PL projections to VS and BLA enables the animal to make appropriate decisions when faced with competing drives.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) controls goal-directed behaviors by integrating sensory, motor, and memory information to guide an individual’s actions (Fuster, 1997; Miller and Cohen, 2001; Kesner and Churchwell, 2011). A leading hypothesis is that the PFC evaluates possible outcomes in the presence of competing drives (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Rushworth and Behrens, 2008; Bissonette et al., 2013; Saunders et al., 2017), such as seeking food rewards in the presence of a potential threat (Aupperle et al., 2015; Friedman et al., 2015; Schumacher et al., 2016; Bublatzky et al., 2017). This situation has been modeled in rodents using the platform-mediated active avoidance (PMA) task, in which food-restricted rats learn to avoid a tone-signaled shock by moving to a nearby platform, at the cost of delaying access to sucrose pellets (Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014). Activity in the prelimbic (PL) PFC is necessary for the expression of PMA (Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014; Diehl et al., 2018), but little is known about the circuit-level mechanisms of the PFC that control this type of avoidance.

During PMA, neurons in PL show both excitatory and inhibitory responses to the tone (Diehl et al., 2018). Similar excitatory tone responses in PL are observed in fear-conditioned rats (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009), but inhibitory tone responses are limited to rats trained in PMA, suggesting the existence of a disinhibitory PL circuit for expression of avoidance (Diehl et al., 2018). PL projects heavily to the ventral striatum (VS) and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) (Sesack et al., 1989; Vertes, 2004), both of which are necessary for PMA (Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014) as well as other types of active avoidance (Darvas et al., 2011; Ramirez et al., 2015).

We therefore sought to determine the role of PL projections to VS and to BLA in PMA using optogenetic techniques to identify possible targets of avoidance-related PL activity. Following training in PMA, PL projections to VS or BLA were either photoactivated (with channelrhodopsin) or photosilenced (with archaerhodopsin). We also used this method to assess the role of BLA projections to VS. We suggest that PL controls active avoidance by bidirectionally modulating outputs to BLA and VS.

Results

PL projections to the VS: photoactivation impairs avoidance

Our previous study revealed that inhibitory tone responses in PL neurons were only observed in rats that received avoidance training (Diehl et al., 2018). We interpreted this as inhibitory responses in PL signaling the avoidability of the tone-predicted shock. Opposing these inhibitory responses by photoactivating PL at the baseline rate of 4 Hz impaired avoidance. Because PL projects densely to the VS (Sesack et al., 1989; Vertes, 2004), we hypothesized that inhibitory responses in PL neurons projecting to the VS would promote avoidance. If so, activating these projection neurons with channelrhodopsin (ChR2) would be expected to impair avoidance. Following viral infusion and surgical implantation of optic probes, rats were trained in PMA over 10 days as previously described (Figure 1A, Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014; Rodriguez-Romaguera et al., 2016; Diehl et al., 2018). Histological analysis showed that expression of ChR2 was largely confined to rostral PL with some spread to rostral anterior cingulate (Cg1) and caudal PL (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Following training, blue laser light (473 nm) was used to activate PL terminals in VS, at a frequency of either 4 Hz or 15 Hz (Figure 1B).

Figure 1. Photoactivation of PL projections to VS impairs avoidance.

(A) Rats were trained in platform-mediated avoidance (PMA) for 10 days, followed by a test with laser illumination during the tone. (B) Schematic of virus infusion and optic probe placement. The CaMKII-α promoter was used for all AAVs. (C) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and 4 Hz and 15 Hz Laser tests performed one or four days later. (D) Timecourse of avoidance during 4 Hz Laser revealed that PL-VS ChR2 (n = 8) rats showed delayed avoidance compared to eYFP (n = 12) controls (repeated-measures ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey). (E) Same as in panel D with 15 Hz Laser. (F) Schematic of ArchT virus infusion and optic probe placement. (G) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and Test day (Tone with Laser). (H) Timecourse of avoidance during Test revealed that ArchT (n = 8) rats showed similar percent time on platform compared to eYFP (n = 14) controls (NS; repeated-measures ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey). All data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p<0.05, **p<0.01.

Figure 1—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of PL-VS projections.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Spread of AAV expression and location of optic probes.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Min/max spread of AAV expression with example micrograph. (B) Location of optic fiber tips with example micrograph for each projection tested.

Figure 1C shows that ChR2 photoactivation at either frequency significantly reduced the time spent on the platform during the tone, compared to eYFP controls (4 Hz: eYFP 77% vs. ChR2 51%, t(18)=2.370, p>0.05, Bonferroni corrected; 15 Hz: eYFP 88% vs. ChR2 22%, t(18)=7.152, p<0.01, Bonferroni corrected). Analysis of avoidance across the tone in 3 s bins (Figure 1D) indicated that ChR2 rats were significantly delayed in their avoidance 3–6 s after tone onset during 4 Hz photoactivation (repeated-measures ANOVA, F(1,9) = 5.88, p=0.026; post-hoc Tukey test, 3–6 s p=0.029). Photoactivation at 15 Hz had a stronger effect, with ChR2 rats showing a significant reduction of avoidance throughout the tone (Figure 1E, repeated-measures ANOVA, F(1,9) = 48.92, p<0.001; post-hoc Tukey test, 6–30 s, all p’s < 0.01). Photoactivation of PL-VS projections had no effect on locomotion, as indicated by distance traveled in an open field (4 Hz; eYFP n = 15, 2.7 m vs. ChR2 n = 7, 2.2 m, t(20)=1.54 p=0.138), nor on anxiety levels, as both groups spent a similar amount of time in the center of the open field (4 Hz: eYFP n = 15, 4.1 s vs. ChR2 n = 7, 6.4 s, t(20)=1.17, p=0.255). Thus, photoactivation of PL terminals in VS during the tone impaired the expression of avoidance, similar to photoactivation of PL somata (Diehl et al., 2018).

If photoactivation impairs avoidance, we hypothesized that photosilencing PL-VS projections would enhance avoidance, similar to what we observed when photosilencing PL somata (Diehl et al., 2018). Archaerhodopsin (ArchT) was infused into PL and optic fibers were implanted to target PL terminals in VS (Figure 1F; Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). However, photosilencing PL-VS projections had no significant effect on average avoidance levels (Figure 1G, eYFP 76% vs. ArchT 69%, t(20)=0.8498, p=0.406) or on the timecourse of avoidance across the tone (Figure 1H, repeated-measures ANOVA, F(1,9)=0.7088, p=0.410). Photosilencing PL-VS projections also had no effect on spontaneous bar-pressing (ArchT, n = 9, seven average number of presses during laser OFF vs. six average number of presses during laser ON, t(8)=1.32, p=0.224). These negative findings of photosilencing PL-VS projections disagree with our prior somatic results (Diehl et al., 2018) and may be due to a ceiling effect or other factors (see Discussion).

PL projections to the BLA: photoactivation facilitates avoidance

Our previous study suggested that PL excitation was not necessary for avoidance, because photosilencing PL somata did not impair avoidance (Diehl et al., 2018). Nevertheless, excitatory responses of PL neurons were observed during both tone onset and platform entry, and the opposing effects on behavior of inhibitory and excitatory responses may have been masked by our non-specific targeting of PL somata. Anatomical studies demonstrate strong projections from PL to BLA (Sesack et al., 1989; Vertes, 2004). Therefore, we sought to determine if optogenetic manipulation of PL projections to BLA would modulate avoidance. ChR2 was infused into PL and optic fibers were implanted in BLA (Figure 2A; Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). In contrast to PL-VS photoactivation, PL-BLA photoactivation at 4 or 15 Hz did not impair avoidance; in fact, avoidance significantly increased with 15 Hz photoactivation (Figures 2B, 4 Hz: eYFP 80% vs. ChR2 90%, t(11)=1.652, p=0.127; 15 Hz: eYFP 75% vs. ChR2 88%, t(15)=2.700, p<0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Analysis across the tone (Figure 2C–D) revealed increased avoidance with 15 Hz photoactivation at both 3 s and 6 s time bins (Mann-Whitney U-test, 3–6 s p=0.006, 6–9 s p=0.027).

Figure 2. Avoidance requires activation of PL projections to BLA.

(A) Schematic of ChR2 virus infusion and optic probe placement. (B) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and 4 Hz and 15 Hz Tests (with Laser). (C) Timecourse of avoidance during the 4 Hz Test (ChR2 n = 5, eYFP n = 8). (D) Same as in panel B with 15 Hz Laser (ChR2 n = 9, eYFP n = 8). (E) Photoactivation of PL-BLA projections (15 Hz) reinstates avoidance following extinction (ChR2 n = 4, eYFP n = 4). (F) Schematic of ArchT virus infusion and optic probe placement. (G) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and Test (Tone with Laser). (H) Timecourse of avoidance during Test revealed that ArchT (n = 9) rats showed delayed avoidance compared to eYFP (n = 11) controls (repeated-measures ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey). All data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p<0.05, **p<0.01.

Figure 2—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of PL-BLA projections.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Spread of AAV expression and location of optic probes.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Min/max spread of AAV expression with example micrograph. (B) Location of optic fiber tips with example micrograph for each projection tested.

The small effect of photoactivation of PL-BLA projections is likely due to ceiling levels of avoidance, as control rats avoided at 82% (Figure 2B, No Laser Cond). To reduce avoidance levels, we extinguished a subset of rats by administering 15 trials of avoidance extinction (tone without shock, no laser, Figure 2E). On the last trial of extinction, photoactivation of PL terminals in BLA (at 15 Hz) significantly increased avoidance compared to eYFP controls (eYFP 2.4% vs. ChR2 34.5% time on platform, t(6)=3.269, p=0.017). Photoactivation of this pathway did not affect locomotion (15 Hz: n = 6 eYFP, 3.2 m vs. n = 5 ChR2, 3.1 m of distance traveled in an open field, t(9)=0.196, p=0.849), or anxiety levels (15 Hz: n = 6 eYFP, 5.1 s vs. n = 5 ChR2, 4.0 s of time spent in center of open field, t(9)=0.318, p=0.757;). Overall, these findings suggest that PL activation of BLA promotes avoidance.

PL projections to the BLA: photosilencing impairs avoidance

We next determined whether photosilencing PL-BLA projections would impair avoidance, demonstrating the necessity of this excitatory pathway in avoidance expression. Rats infused with ArchT in PL and implanted with optic fibers in BLA (Figure 2F; Figure 2—figure supplement 1B) underwent avoidance training followed by a test of avoidance expression. Photosilencing PL-BLA projections significantly impaired the expression of avoidance (Figure 2G, eYFP 77% vs. ArchT 38% time on platform, t(18)=2.985, p<0.05, Bonferroni corrected). Furthermore, ArchT rats showed an overall decrease in avoidance across the tone compared to eYFP controls (Figure 2H, repeated-measures ANOVA, F(1,9)=9.449, p=0.007, post-hoc Tukey tests, 3–9 and 12–24 s, all p’s < 0.05). Photosilencing PL-BLA projections had no effect on spontaneous bar-pressing (ArchT n = 11, 4.82 average number of presses during laser OFF vs. 5.77 average number of presses during laser ON, p=0.255, t(10)=1.21). These findings suggest that PL excitation of BLA is necessary for the expression of active avoidance.

BLA projections to the VS: photoactivation facilitates avoidance

Thus far, distinct PL projections appear to have opposite roles in avoidance: PL-VS projections inhibit avoidance whereas PL-BLA projections promote avoidance. How might signaling between VS and BLA regulate avoidance? Previous studies have shown that BLA sends strong projections to VS (Kelley et al., 1982; Mcdonald, 1991a; Mcdonald, 1991b), and pharmacological disconnection of BLA and VS impairs shuttle avoidance (Ramirez et al., 2015). Therefore, we reasoned that BLA-VS projections may also promote avoidance. Following ChR2 infusion into BLA and implantation of optic fibers targeting the VS (Figure 3A; Figure 3—figure supplement 1A), photoactivation of BLA-VS projections slightly increased avoidance, but this did not reach statistical significance (Figure 3B) at 4 Hz (eYFP 70% vs. ChR2 90% time on platform, t(12)=1.678, p=0.119) or 15 Hz (eYFP 70% vs ChR2 86% time on platform, t(12)=1.50, p=0.159). There was also no significant difference in the timecourse of avoidance across the tone (Figure 3C–D).

Figure 3. Avoidance requires activation of BLA projections to VS.

(A) Schematic of ChR2 virus infusion and optic probe placement. (B) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and 4 Hz and 15 Hz Laser tests. (C) Timecourse of avoidance during 4 Hz Laser revealed that BLA-VS ChR2 (n = 7) rats showed no significant difference in percent time on platform compared to eYFP (n = 7) controls (repeated-measures ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey). (D) Same conventions as in panel C during 15 Hz Laser. (E) 15 Hz photoactivation of VS-BLA projections reinstates avoidance following extinction (ChR2 n = 7, eYFP n = 7). (F) Schematic of ArchT virus infusion and optic probe placement. (G) Percent time on platform during the last day of avoidance conditioning (Cond; No Laser), and Test (Tone with Laser). (H) Timecourse of avoidance during Test revealed that ArchT (n = 7) rats showed impaired avoidance compared to eYFP (n = 14) controls (repeated-measures ANOVA, post-hoc Tukey). All data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p<0.05, **p<0.01.

Figure 3—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of BLA-VS projections.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Spread of AAV expression and location of optic probes.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Min/max spread of AAV expression with example micrograph. (B) Location of optic fiber tips with example micrograph for each projection tested.

Similar to photoactivation of PL-BLA projections, a ceiling effect was observed during photoactivation of BLA-VS projections. Therefore, a subset of rats underwent avoidance extinction to determine if photoactivating BLA-VS projections would reinstate avoidance expression (Figure 3E). In the final trial of extinction training, photoactivation of BLA-VS projections at 15 Hz significantly increased avoidance expression (eYFP 9% vs. ChR2 38% time on platform, t(11)=3.521, p=0.005). Photoactivation did not affect locomotion (15 Hz: n = 5 eYFP, 2.7 m vs. n = 7 ChR2, 3.2 m of distance traveled in an open field, t(10)=1.10, p=0.298), or anxiety levels (15 Hz: n = 5 eYFP, 4.0 s vs. n = 7 ChR2, 4.2 s of time spent in center of open field, t(10)=0.0085, p=0.934). Taken together, these findings show that BLA-VS projections facilitate the expression of avoidance.

BLA projections to the VS: photosilencing impairs avoidance

To establish the necessity of the BLA-VS excitatory pathway in avoidance expression, we assessed whether photosilencing BLA-VS projections would impair avoidance. Following infusions of ArchT into BLA and optic fiber implantation into VS (Figure 3F; Figure 3—figure supplement 1B), rats underwent PMA training followed by a test of avoidance expression. Photosilencing BLA-VS projections significantly impaired the expression of avoidance (Figure 3G, eYFP 78% vs. ArchT 28%, t(19)=3.849, p<0.01, Bonferroni corrected). Furthermore, ArchT rats showed a decrease in avoidance throughout the tone, compared to eYFP controls (Figure 3H, repeated-measures ANOVA, F(1,9)=5.23, p=0.034, post-hoc Tukey tests, 6–15 s, all p’s < 0.01, 15–18 s, p<0.05). Photosilencing BLA-VS projections did not affect spontaneous bar-pressing (ArchT n = 6, 5.75 average number of presses during laser OFF vs. 7.58 average number of presses during laser ON, t(5)=1.27, p=0.259). Overall, these findings suggest that BLA excitation of VS is necessary for the expression of active avoidance.

Discussion

In this study, we mapped PL outputs that control platform-mediated active avoidance. We found that photoactivation of PL-VS projections impaired avoidance, whereas photoactivation of PL-BLA projections promoted avoidance, suggesting a prefrontal circuit that bidirectionally modulates avoidance behavior. Optogenetic manipulations of BLA-VS projections also revealed that VS receives excitation from the BLA to promote avoidance. These findings add to the growing body of studies showing prefrontal control of active avoidance.

Our results show that photosilencing of the PL-VS circuit had no effect on avoidance, a result which is at odds with our hypothesis that such a manipulation would increase avoidance. One possibility is that a ceiling effect prevented us from observing an increase in avoidance, as we had observed in our initial study when PL somata were photosilenced (see Figure 2E in Diehl et al., 2018). A second possibility is that some of the optic probes for our ArchT PL-VS experiments ended up targeting the shell rather than the core of the VS (see Figure 1—figure supplement 1B), possibly suppressing any effect of our PL-VS manipulation. Anatomical studies show that the PL-VS projection targets the core region of the VS, whereas PL-BLA projection targets the shell region of the VS (Heimer et al., 1997; Gorelova and Yang, 1996; Zahm, 2000). Thus, photosilencing PL-VS fibers with the approach we used may have inadvertently photosilenced fibers of passage from PL to BLA, which could cancel out facilitating effects, as photosilencing PL-BLA decreases avoidance. To overcome these challenges, future studies using a recombinase-dependent viral vector to exclusively target PL-VS projections (Yizhar et al., 2011) are needed to confirm the effects of photosilencing this projection.

Despite the lack of effect of photosilencing PL-VS projections, we did observe that photoactivating PL-VS projections impaired avoidance. This is in agreement with our previous findings that photoactivation of PL somata impaired avoidance (Diehl et al., 2018), and provides indirect evidence that PL inhibition facilitates avoidance. This suggests that the inhibitory tone response in PL targets the VS to drive avoidance. In contrast, we found that excitatory projections from PL to BLA drive avoidance (see model in Figure 4). Future studies using PMA could determine if PL neurons showing inhibitory tone responses project to VS, and if PL neurons showing excitatory responses project to BLA by employing photoexcitation-assisted identification of neuronal populations (Jennings et al., 2013; Zhang et al., 2013; Ciocchi et al., 2015; Beyeler et al., 2016; Burgos-Robles et al., 2017).

Figure 4. Suggested circuit for bidirectional modulation of avoidance by PL.

Figure 4.

Activity in PL projections to VS decreases avoidance (top left projection). These projections may target fast-spiking interneurons within VS (orange neuron with question mark), which serve to inhibit VS output neurons. Activity in PL projections to BLA increases avoidance (top right projection), as do BLA projections to VS (bottom projection). In this way, PL inputs to VS could gate the impact of BLA inputs on VS output neurons.

Other studies have demonstrated bidirectional control of behavior by frontal areas. PL-VS projections were shown to promote reward-seeking, whereas PL-thalamic projections inhibited reward-seeking (Otis et al., 2017). A study of decision making demonstrated that PL dopamine (D2) neurons targeting the BLA signaled the probability of receiving a reward, whereas PL dopamine (D1) neurons targeting the VS signaled the probability of reward omission (Jenni et al., 2017). Additionally, a probabilistic reversal learning task investigating the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) found that OFC-VS projections were necessary for using negative outcomes to guide choices, whereas OFC-amygdala projections were necessary for stabilizing the value of actions (Groman et al., 2019). Collectively, these studies demonstrate that bidirectional control of behavior can be governed by specific circuits within PFC, allowing for the selection of the appropriate behavioral response.

A previous study using the shuttle-box avoidance task reported that pre-training lesions in VS enhance avoidance responses (Lichtenberg et al., 2014), which disagrees with the current study. However, successful avoidance was measured on the 7th day of training after lesions were made, introducing the possibility that other structures compensated for the loss of VS (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005), thereby forcing an alternative circuit to acquire the task. Moreover, the lesion approach cannot identify which inputs to VS may be responsible for enhancing avoidance. Another shuttle-box avoidance study showed that disconnecting BLA from VS impaired avoidance (Ramirez et al., 2015). Our findings extend this by showing that the activity of BLA terminals within VS is necessary for avoidance. A subset of BLA neurons projecting to VS have been shown to receive inputs from PL (McGarry and Carter, 2017), consistent with our model (Figure 4). Here, we propose that PL bidirectionally controls active avoidance by modulating the effect of BLA inputs to VS, through activation of BLA and/or feed-forward inhibition of VS, perhaps via fast-spiking interneurons (Berke, 2011). In PMA, PL driving of avoidance may be most pronounced during the early part of the tone, as pharmacological inactivation of PL delays but does not prevent avoidance (Diehl et al., 2019). This suggests that BLA projections to VS may override feed-forward inhibition from PL to VS, as the tone progresses and there is increased urgency to avoid.

The rodent PL is thought to be homologous to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) in humans (Bicks et al., 2015; Heilbronner et al., 2016). Neuroimaging studies have found that decreased activity in dACC correlates with the predictability of an aversive stimulus (Wood et al., 2015) and the avoidance of a threat (Aupperle et al., 2015). In the latter case, decreased activation of dACC was accompanied by increased activation of VS. Another study showed increased coupling between dACC and amygdala during active avoidance (Collins et al., 2014). Taken together, these findings demonstrate bidirectional control of active avoidance by dACC and are consistent with PL in rodents guiding appropriate action during the tone (lever pressing vs. moving to the platform).

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Genetic reagent (Chlamydomonas reinhardtii) AAV5:CaMKIIα::hChR2(H134R)-eYFP UNC vector core channelrhodopsin
(ChR2)
Serotype 5;
4 × 1012 particles/mL
Genetic reagent
(Aequorea victoria)
AAV5:CaMKIIα::eYFP UNC vector core Enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (eYFP) Serotype 5; 3 × 1012 particles/mL
Genetic reagent (Halorubrum sodomense) AAV5:CaMKIIα::eArchT3.0-eYFP UNC vector core Archaerhodopsin (ArchT) Serotype 5; 4 × 1012 particles/mL
Software, algorithm ANY-Maze behavioral software Stoelting ANY-Maze Woodale, IL

Subjects

118 adult male Sprague Dawley rats (Envigo Laboratories, Indianapolis, IN) aged 3–5 months and weighing 320–420 g were housed and handled as previously described (Diehl et al., 2018). Rats were kept on a restricted diet (18 g/day) of standard laboratory rat chow to facilitate pressing a bar for sucrose pellets (BioServ, Flemington, NJ) on a variable interval schedule of reinforcement (VI-30). Rats were trained until they reached a criterion of >15 presses/min. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Puerto Rico School of Medicine in compliance with the National Institutes of Health guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals.

Surgery

Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane inhalant gas (5%) first in an induction chamber, then positioned in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA). Isoflurane (1–3%) was delivered through a facemask for anesthesia maintenance. Prior to beginning the surgery, rats were administered an analgesic (Meloxicam, 1 mg/Kg) subcutaneously.

For optogenetic experiments, rats were bilaterally infused with viral vectors in the PL PFC(+3.15 mm AP;±0.40 mm ML; −3.60 mm DV to bregma, at a 0°angle) or the BLA ( −2.8 mm AP,±4.8 mm ML, −8.9 mm DV). The syringe was kept inside for an additional 10 min to reduce backflow. Optical fibers (length, 9 mm; 0.22 NA; 200 nm core; constructed with products from Thorlabs, Newton, NJ or purchased preassembled from Newdoon, Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China) were chronically implanted for PL or BLA terminal illumination. For PL terminals, fibers were placed in the VS (+1.2 mm AP,±3.0 mm ML, −3.6 mm DV, at a 15° angle) or the BLA (−2.8 mm AP,±4.8 mm ML, −7.7 mm DV, at a 0°angle). For BLA terminals, fibers were placed in the VS (+1.0 mm AP,±3.0 mm ML, −6.5 mm DV, at a 15°angle). Optical fibers were anchored to the skull with adhesive cement (C and B-Metabond, Parkell, Brentwood, NY; Ortho Acrylic, Bayamón, PR).

After surgery, triple antibiotic was applied topically around the surgery incision, and 24 hr following surgery, an analgesic (Meloxicam, 1 mg/kg) was injected subcutaneously once again. Rats were allowed a minimum of 7 days to recover from surgery prior to behavioral training.

Behavior

Rats were initially trained to press a bar to receive food pellets on a variable interval reinforcement schedule (VI-30) inside standard operant chambers (Coulbourn Instruments, Whitehall, PA) located in sound-attenuating cubicles (MED Associates, St. Albans, VT). Bar-pressing was used to maintain a constant level of activity against which avoidance could reliably be measured. For platform-mediated avoidance, rats were trained as previously described (Bravo-Rivera et al., 2014). Rats were conditioned with a pure tone (30 s, 4 kHz, 75 dB) co-terminating with a footshock delivered through the floor grids (2 s, 0.3–0.4 mA). The inter-trial interval (ITI) was variable, averaging 3 min. An acrylic square platform (14.0 cm each side, 0.33 cm tall) located in the opposite corner of the sucrose dish allowed rats to be protected from the shock. The platform was fixed to the floor and was present during all stages of training. Rats were conditioned for 10 days with nine tone-shock pairings per day and a VI-30 schedule maintained across all training and test sessions. The availability of food on the side opposite to the platform motivated rats to leave the platform during the ITI, facilitating trial-by-trial assessment of avoidance.

Once platform-mediated avoidance was learned, rats underwent an avoidance expression test, involving the presentation of tones without shock. Laser manipulation occurred during the presentation of tone 1. Following the avoidance expression test, a subset of rats underwent extinction training, which involved 15 tone presentations without shock. Laser manipulation took place on the 15th tone to assess for avoidance reinstatement after extinction.

Viruses

The adeno-associated viruses (AAVs; serotype 5) were obtained from the University of North Carolina Vector Core (Chapel Hill, NC). Viral titers were 4 × 1012 particles/mL for channelrhodopsin (AAV5:CaMKIIα::hChR2(H134R)-eYFP) and archaerhodopsin (AAV5:CaMKIIα::eArchT3.0-eYFP), and 3 × 1012 particles/mL control (AAV5:CaMKIIα::eYFP). Rats expressing eYFP in PL or BLA were used to control potential changes in neural activity due to laser-induced overheating of tissue (Stujenske et al., 2015). The CaMKIIα promoter was used to enable transgene expression favoring pyramidal neurons (Liu and Jones, 1996 ) in cortical regions (Jones et al., 1994; Van den Oever et al., 2013; Warthen et al., 2016). Viruses were housed in a −80°C freezer until the day of infusion.

Laser delivery

Rats expressing channelrhodopsin (ChR2) in PL or BLA were illuminated using a blue diode-pump solid state laser (DPSS, 473 nm, 4 or 15 Hz, 5 ms pulse width, 8–12 mW at the optical fiber tip; OptoEngine, Midvale, UT), similar to our previous study (Do-Monte et al., 2015). Rats expressing archaerhodopsin (ArchT) in PL were bilaterally illuminated using a DPSS green laser (532 nm, constant, 12–15 mW at the optical fiber tip; OptoEngine). For both ChR2 and ArchT experiments, the laser was activated at tone onset and persisted throughout the 30 s tone presentation. Laser light was passed through a shutter/coupler (200 nm, Oz Optics, Ontario, Canada), patch cord (200 nm core, ThorLabs, Newton NJ), rotary joint (200 nm core, 1 × 2, Doric Lenses, Quebec city, Canada), dual patch cord (0.22 NA, 200 nm core, ThorLabs), and bilateral optical fibers targeting the specific subregions in PL or BLA. Rats were familiarized with the patch cord during bar press training and the last 4 days of avoidance conditioning before the expression test.

Open field task

Locomotor activity in the open field arena (90 cm diameter) was automatically assessed (ANY-Maze) by measuring distance traveled, time spent, and speed in center or periphery of the arena during 30 s laser off and laser on time periods. A 3 min acclimation period preceded 30 s of 4 Hz photo-activation, which was followed by an additional 3 min ITI, and a second 30 s laser trial of 15 Hz photo-activation. The distance and speed traveled was used to assess locomotion and time in center was used to assess anxiety.

Pressing test

Rats pressed on a variable interval reinforcement schedule (VI-30). Changes in the average number of presses were measured during two trials of Laser On (30 s) and Laser Off periods (30 s preceding Laser On). The task began with a 60 s acclimation period, followed by 30 s of Laser On (532 nm), which was followed by a 60 s ITI and 30 s of Laser On. The number of lever activations was compared during Laser Off and Laser On periods within subjects using a paired-t-test.

Histology

After behavioral experiments, rats were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (450 mg/kg i.p.) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline followed by a 10% formalin solution. Brains were removed from the skull and stored in 30% sucrose for cryoprotection for at least 72 hr before sectioning and Nissl staining. Histology was analyzed for placement of viral expression and optic fibers.

Data collection and analysis

Behavior was recorded with digital video cameras (Micro Video Products, Peterborough, Ontario, Canada). ANY-Maze software (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL) was used to detect the animal’s location and movements. ANY-Maze was used to quantify the time spent on the platform during tone presentations as a measure of avoidance. Avoidance to the tone was expressed as a percentage of the 30 s tone presentation. Statistical significance was determined with Student’s two-tailed t tests, Fisher Exact tests, Mann–Whitney U tests, or repeated-measures ANOVA, followed by post- hoc Tukey analysis, where appropriate using STATISTICA (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK) and Prism (Graphpad, La Jolla, CA).

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by NIH grants F32-MH105185 to MMD and R37-MH058883 and P50-MH106435 to GJQ, and the University of Puerto Rico President’s Office. This project was also supported by undergraduate training grants R25-GM097635, T34-GM007821, and R25-GM061151. We thank Dr. Anthony Burgos-Robles for comments on the manuscript. We also thank Carlos Rodríguez and Zarkalys Quintero for technical assistance.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Maria M Diehl, Email: maria.m.diehl@gmail.com.

Geoffrey Schoenbaum, National Institute on Drug Abuse, National Institutes of Health, United States.

Michael J Frank, Brown University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Mental Health F32-MH105185 to Maria M Diehl.

  • National Institute of Mental Health R37-MH058883 to Gregory J Quirk.

  • National Institute of Mental Health P50-MH106435 to Gregory J Quirk.

  • University of Puerto Rico President's Office to Gregory J Quirk.

  • National Institute of General Medical Sciences R25-GM097635 to Jorge M Iravedra-Garcia, Viviana P Valentín-Valentín.

  • National Institute of General Medical Sciences R25-GM061151 to Fabiola N Gonzalez-Diaz, Jonathan Morán-Sierra.

  • National Institute of General Medical Sciences T34-GM007821 to Gabriel Rojas-Bowe.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation.

Data curation, Investigation, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation.

Data curation, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#A3340107) of the University of Puerto Rico. The protocol was approved by the Committee on the Ethics of Animal Experiments of the University of Puerto Rico. All surgery was performed under isofluorane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

  1. Anglada-Figueroa D, Quirk GJ. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9680–9685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aupperle RL, Melrose AJ, Francisco A, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Neural substrates of approach-avoidance conflict decision-making. Human Brain Mapping. 2015;36:449–462. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Berke JD. Functional properties of striatal fast-spiking interneurons. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011;5:45. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beyeler A, Namburi P, Glober GF, Simonnet C, Calhoon GG, Conyers GF, Luck R, Wildes CP, Tye KM. Divergent routing of positive and negative information from the amygdala during memory retrieval. Neuron. 2016;90:348–361. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bicks LK, Koike H, Akbarian S, Morishita H. Prefrontal cortex and social cognition in mouse and man. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:1805. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bissonette GB, Powell EM, Roesch MR. Neural structures underlying set-shifting: roles of medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. Behavioural Brain Research. 2013;250:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bravo-Rivera C, Roman-Ortiz C, Brignoni-Perez E, Sotres-Bayon F, Quirk GJ. Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:9736–9742. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0191-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bublatzky F, Alpers GW, Pittig A. From avoidance to approach: the influence of threat-of-shock on reward-based decision making. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2017;96:47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8474–8482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burgos-Robles A, Kimchi EY, Izadmehr EM, Porzenheim MJ, Ramos-Guasp WA, Nieh EH, Felix-Ortiz AC, Namburi P, Leppla CA, Presbrey KN, Anandalingam KK, Pagan-Rivera PA, Anahtar M, Beyeler A, Tye KM. Amygdala inputs to prefrontal cortex guide behavior amid conflicting cues of reward and punishment. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:824–835. doi: 10.1038/nn.4553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ciocchi S, Passecker J, Malagon-Vina H, Mikus N, Klausberger T. Brain computation selective information routing by ventral hippocampal CA1 projection neurons. Science. 2015;348:560–563. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa3245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Collins KA, Mendelsohn A, Cain CK, Schiller D. Taking action in the face of threat: neural synchronization predicts adaptive coping. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:14733–14738. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2152-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Darvas M, Fadok JP, Palmiter RD. Requirement of dopamine signaling in the amygdala and striatum for learning and maintenance of a conditioned avoidance response. Learning & Memory. 2011;18:136–143. doi: 10.1101/lm.2041211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Diehl MM, Bravo-Rivera C, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Pagan-Rivera PA, Burgos-Robles A, Roman-Ortiz C, Quirk GJ. Active avoidance requires inhibitory signaling in the rodent prelimbic prefrontal cortex. eLife. 2018;7:e34657. doi: 10.7554/eLife.34657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diehl MM, Bravo-Rivera C, Quirk GJ. The study of active avoidance: a platform for discussion. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2019;107:229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Do-Monte FH, Manzano-Nieves G, Quiñones-Laracuente K, Ramos-Medina L, Quirk GJ. Revisiting the role of infralimbic cortex in fear extinction with optogenetics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:3607–3615. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3137-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Friedman A, Homma D, Gibb LG, Amemori K, Rubin SJ, Hood AS, Riad MH, Graybiel AM. A corticostriatal path targeting striosomes controls Decision-Making under conflict. Cell. 2015;161:1320–1333. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.04.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fuster J. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe. Baltimore: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Raven Press; 1997. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gorelova N, Yang CR. The course of neural projection from the prefrontal cortex to the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience. 1996;76:689–706. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00380-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Groman SM, Keistler C, Keip AJ, Hammarlund E, DiLeone RJ, Pittenger C, Lee D, Taylor JR. Orbitofrontal Circuits Control Multiple Reinforcement-Learning Processes. Neuron. 2019;103:734–746. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Heilbronner SR, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Quirk GJ, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN. Circuit-Based Corticostriatal Homologies Between Rat and Primate. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;80:509–521. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Heimer L, Alheid GF, de Olmos JS, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN, Harlan RE, Zahm DS. 'The accumbens: beyond the core-shell dichotomy. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1997;9:354–381. doi: 10.1176/jnp.9.3.354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jenni NL, Larkin JD, Floresco SB. Prefrontal Dopamine D 1 and D 2 Receptors Regulate Dissociable Aspects of Decision Making via Distinct Ventral Striatal and Amygdalar Circuits. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:6200–6213. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0030-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Jennings JH, Sparta DR, Stamatakis AM, Ung RL, Pleil KE, Kash TL, Stuber GD. Distinct extended amygdala circuits for divergent motivational states. Nature. 2013;496:224–228. doi: 10.1038/nature12041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jones EG, Huntley GW, Benson DL. Alpha calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II selectively expressed in a subpopulation of excitatory neurons in monkey sensory-motor cortex: comparison with GAD-67 expression. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14:611–629. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-02-00611.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kelley AE, Domesick VB, Nauta WJH. The amygdalostriatal projection in the rat—an anatomical study by anterograde and retrograde tracing methods. Neuroscience. 1982;7:615–630. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(82)90067-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kesner RP, Churchwell JC. An analysis of rat prefrontal cortex in mediating executive function. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2011;96:417–431. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Lichtenberg NT, Kashtelyan V, Burton AC, Bissonette GB, Roesch MR. Nucleus accumbens core lesions enhance two-way active avoidance. Neuroscience. 2014;258:340–346. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Liu XB, Jones EG. Localization of alpha type II calcium calmodulin-dependent protein kinase at Glutamatergic but not gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAergic) synapses in thalamus and cerebral cortex. PNAS. 1996;93:7332–7336. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.14.7332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Mcdonald AJ. Organization of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex and associated striatum in the rat. Neuroscience. 1991a;44:1–14. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90247-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mcdonald AJ. Topographical organization of amygdaloid projections to the caudatoputamen, nucleus accumbens, and related striatal-like areas of the rat brain. Neuroscience. 1991b;44:15–33. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90248-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McGarry LM, Carter AG. Prefrontal Cortex Drives Distinct Projection Neurons in the Basolateral Amygdala. Cell Reports. 2017;21:1426–1433. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.10.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An Integrative Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Function. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Otis JM, Namboodiri VMK, Matan AM, Voets ES, Mohorn EP, Kosyk O, McHenry JA, Robinson JE, Resendez SL, Rossi MA, Stuber GD. Prefrontal cortex output circuits guide reward seeking through divergent cue encoding. Nature. 2017;543:103–107. doi: 10.1038/nature21376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ramirez F, Moscarello JM, LeDoux JE, Sears RM. Active Avoidance Requires a Serial Basal Amygdala to Nucleus Accumbens Shell Circuit. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:3470–3477. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1331-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control. Science. 2004;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Greenberg BD, Rasmussen SA, Quirk GJ. An Avoidance-Based Rodent Model of Exposure With Response Prevention Therapy for Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;80:534–540. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Rushworth MFS, Behrens TEJ. Choice, uncertainty and value in prefrontal and cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:389–397. doi: 10.1038/nn2066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Saunders B, Lin H, Milyavskaya M, Inzlicht M. The emotive nature of conflict monitoring in the medial prefrontal cortex. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2017;119:31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Schumacher A, Vlassov E, Ito R. The ventral hippocampus, but not the dorsal hippocampus is critical for learned approach-avoidance decision making. Hippocampus. 2016;26:530–542. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS. Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: an anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1989;290:213–242. doi: 10.1002/cne.902900205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Stujenske JM, Spellman T, Gordon JA. Modeling the spatiotemporal dynamics of light and heat propagation for in vivo optogenetics. Cell Reports. 2015;12:525–534. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2015.06.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Van den Oever MC, Rotaru DC, Heinsbroek JA, Gouwenberg Y, Deisseroth K, Stuber GD, Mansvelder HD, Smit AB. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex pyramidal cells have a temporal dynamic role in recall and extinction of cocaine-associated memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:18225–18233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2412-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Vertes RP. Differential projections of the infralimbic and prelimbic cortex in the rat. Synapse. 2004;51:32–58. doi: 10.1002/syn.10279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Warthen DM, Lambeth PS, Ottolini M, Shi Y, Barker BS, Gaykema RP, Newmyer BA, Joy-Gaba J, Ohmura Y, Perez-Reyes E, Güler AD, Patel MK, Scott MM. Activation of pyramidal neurons in mouse medial prefrontal cortex enhances Food-Seeking behavior while reducing impulsivity in the absence of an effect on food intake. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016;10:63–1006. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wood KH, Wheelock MD, Shumen JR, Bowen KH, Ver Hoef LW, Knight DC. Controllability modulates the neural response to predictable but not unpredictable threat in humans. NeuroImage. 2015;119:371–381. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Mogri M, Deisseroth K. Optogenetics in neural systems. Neuron. 2011;71:9–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Zahm DS. An integrative neuroanatomical perspective on some subcortical substrates of adaptive responding with emphasis on the nucleus accumbens. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2000;24:85–105. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00065-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Zhang SJ, Ye J, Miao C, Tsao A, Cerniauskas I, Ledergerber D, Moser MB, Moser EI. Optogenetic dissection of entorhinal-hippocampal functional connectivity. Science. 2013;340:1232627. doi: 10.1126/science.1232627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Geoffrey Schoenbaum1
Reviewed by: Geoffrey Schoenbaum2, Michael A McDannald3

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

In this study, the authors significantly extend a prior report showing that inhibitory activity in PL was necessary for PMA behavior, showing that this apparently simple relationship is more complex, involving projections to BLA and VS and also from BLA to VS. Specifically, they show that activating (inhibiting) projections from PL->BLA or from BLA->VS promotes (suppresses) PMA. Further they show that activating projections from PL->VS suppresses avoidance; interestingly inhibiting these projections did not cause increased avoidance.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Divergent projections of the prelimbic cortex bidirectionally regulate active avoidance." for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, including Geoffrey Schoenbaum as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Michael Frank as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Michael A McDannald (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, when editors judge that a submitted work as a whole belongs in eLife but that some conclusions require a modest amount of additional new data, as they do with your paper, we are asking that the manuscript be revised to either limit claims to those supported by data in hand, or to explicitly state that the relevant conclusions require additional supporting data.

Our expectation is that the authors will eventually carry out the additional experiments and report on how they affect the relevant conclusions either in a preprint on bioRxiv or medRxiv, or if appropriate, as a Research Advance in eLife, either of which would be linked to the original paper.

Summary:

In this study, the authors follow up a prior report showing that inhibitory activity in PL was necessary for PMA behavior. While the prior report provided evidence using optogenetic manipulations that the novel inhibitory activity was likely involved in the avoidance behavior, the current study extends that to show that this apparently simple relationship is more complex, involving projections to BLA and VS and also from BLA to VS. Specifically, they show that activating (inhibiting) projections from PL->BLA or from BLA->VS promotes (suppresses) PMA. Further they show that activating projections from PL->VS suppresses avoidance; interestingly inhibiting these projections did not cause increased avoidance.

Revisions for this paper:

Both reviewers thought the work was elegant in its design and execution and the results were for the most part very clear. The main issue is the discrepancy between the prior result, that inhibition of activity in some PL neurons is necessary to drive PMA, and the failure to clearly demonstrate this effect at terminals downstream in either BLA or VS. This seems to be a key finding that the authors were looking for but did not find, but the discussion and model at the end are presented without commenting on this missing result. This was noted by both reviewers, and on discussion was agreed to be important to address. We are happy to have this addressed with textual changes for now, and the reviews note a variety of reasons this might have occurred. If the authors could consider these and any others they think are worthwhile clearly in the Discussion, and implications for their functional model, that should do it.

Revisions expected in follow-up work:

Some clarification regarding the role of inhibitory activity in PL versus at the terminals in VS, BLA and maybe other areas is needed to resolve the discrepancy described above and in reviews.

Reviewer #1:

In this study, the authors follow up a prior report showing that inhibitory activity in PL was necessary for PMA behavior. While the prior report provided evidence using optogenetic manipulations that the novel inhibitory activity was likely involved in the avoidance behavior, the current study extends that to show that this apparently simple relationship is more complex, involving projections to BLA and VS and also from BLA to VS. Specifically, they show that activating (inhibiting) projections from PL->BLA or from BLA->VS promotes (suppresses) PMA. Further they show that activating projections from PL->VS suppresses avoidance; interestingly inhibiting these projections did not cause increased avoidance.

Generally, these results seem very straightforward and clear. The bidirectional effects are striking, and the work is nicely done and very clean. However I am bothered a bit by what seems to me to be a lack of consistency with the prior report, which is not addressed. Though it is also possible that I simply missed it. Depending on which it is, the authors can likely fix it in the text or straighten me out.

So the issue is this – the prior report claimed that the inhibitory correlates in PL were the unique and prime mover in supporting avoidance. Yet here they fail to find any such effect of inhibition of the downstream terminals. In BLA, inhibition of PL terminals disrupts avoidance and in VS inhibition has no effect. Further they find that activation of output from PL to both areas plays a role in modulating the PMA, which somewhat contradicts the assertation that such activity playing the same role in PMA as in other fear and extinction related procedures.

My own suspicion is that the lack of effects of inhibition in Figure 1F-H is due to a ceiling effect, and that if they had trained the rats more lightly or had extinguished as done in Figures 2 and 3, then they would have found that inhibition of the PL->VS pathway does support PMA. Such data would resolve the seeming inconsistency and, coupled with a more clear discussion of what the effects of activation mean for interpreting the excitatory responses, make more sense to me than the current interpretation, which seems to mostly ignore the issue.

The other possibility to me is that inhibiting across the entire tone is not the same as the neural suppression in some of the neurons at the time of the tone, in which case their approach is not well designed to mimic the correlates.

Of course as I said I may simply not understand how the data all fit together. If this is the case, the authors can clarify it for me. If this is not the case, then I think the failure to find the site where inhibitory PL activity facilitates PMA is a problem for the clean story of bidirectional control the authors want to paint. I am more than happy to have it addressed in the text if the authors want to do this, noting the above caveats and modifying how things are discussed. As I said, I think it is likely an unfortunate ceiling effect. Or the authors could do this experiment if they do not want to fix it textually, but I this is not something I'd require. This is really my only substantive issue with an otherwise very elegant set of results.

Reviewer #2:

This is an elegant series of studies that builds up on Diehl and colleagues prior publication. The rationale for the circuits investigated is clear. The experiments were well designed, executed and interpreted. The results are an important contribution to an emerging literature for the neural circuits supporting avoidance learning.

1) Like the authors, I was a little surprised to see that photoinhibition of PL terminals in the VS did not promote avoidance behavior. I think this null effect warrants more discussion. Looking at the histology in Figure 1—figure supplement 1, it appears that the optical placements for photo-inhibition were ventral to those for photoexcitation. So perhaps photoexcitation more effectively stimulated the accumbens core, whereas photoinhibition targeted the accumbens shell. Also clear is that the PL -> VS projections are extensive. Might it be the case that photoinhibition only targeted a small percentage of PL fibers, a number insufficient to facilitate avoidance? To this second point, the Sabitini lab has developed means of sharpening ferrules to achieve greater light diffusion: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5533215/.

2) The use of the term ventral striatum is warranted here, as both the PL and BLA target the shell and core subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Moving forward, it will likely be important to acknowledge and examine dissociable roles for the core and shell in avoidance. The authors rightly discuss the Ramirez study examining core. Some discussion of the finding by Lichtenberg et al., 2014 and colleagues is warranted and could be the basis of a more specific discussion of core vs. shell.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "Divergent projections of the prelimbic cortex bidirectionally regulate active avoidance." for further consideration by eLife. Your revised article has been evaluated by Michael Frank (Senior Editor) and a Reviewing Editor.

The manuscript has been improved but there are some remaining issues that need to be addressed before acceptance, as outlined below:

On discussion, the reviewer's felt that the paper presents enough new data and is enough at odds with the prior report that it would be better presented as a new article than as a straightforward Research Advance follow up. The category of the paper has been changed, but please confirm that this is acceptable in your response.

In addition, it was felt that some additional minor changes to the Discussion were needed to clarify the issue with regard to the specificity of the critical PFC->accumbent manipulation and the role of accumbent in avoidance. The first is that we would like you to state more explicitly that future studies using the recombinant approach would allow targeting of the core projections specifically. This is currently a bit unclear as understanding this requires reference to a phrase several sentences prior stating that the projections are primarily core. Please edit to make this point more easily understood.

The second is at the top of the next paragraph, where we think you could say something more direct regarding the significance of the activation data, assuming you agree. In particular, we feel like the fact that activation impairs avoidance provides indirect evidence c/w the prior findings (that inhibition facilitates). Noting this would help avoid the mis-impression that the data are completely contradictory to the prior study or that accumbent is not involved in avoidance.

Reviewer #1:

The authors have done an excellent job addressing my concerns. I have no further issues.

Reviewer #2:

The authors performed numerous optogenetic experiments, dissecting pathway-specific contributions to avoidance. The most critical experiment, and the reason for submitting as a Research Advance, was inhibition of PL fibers in the nucleus accumbens. Surprisingly, inhibiting PL inputs to the accumbens did not promote avoidance.

The authors reassessment of histology suggests that deferentially targeting the core vs shell may produce diverging effects on avoidance. In combination with the varied ferrule placement, it seems likely that a ceiling effect in avoidance behavior prevented the authors from seeing a facilitation in avoidance behavior.

The advance of the current results depends heavily on the interpretation of the PL to accumbens pathway inhibition. It now seems clear than a rigorous advance will require a new experiment in which the behavioral ceiling is lowered and accumbens core vs. shell are separately and specifically targeted.

eLife. 2020 Oct 15;9:e59281. doi: 10.7554/eLife.59281.sa2

Author response


Reviewer #1:

In this study, the authors follow up a prior report showing that inhibitory activity in PL was necessary for PMA behavior. While the prior report provided evidence using optogenetic manipulations that the novel inhibitory activity was likely involved in the avoidance behavior, the current study extends that to show that this apparently simple relationship is more complex, involving projections to BLA and VS and also from BLA to VS. Specifically, they show that activating (inhibiting) projections from PL->BLA or from BLA->VS promotes (suppresses) PMA. Further they show that activating projections from PL->VS suppresses avoidance; interestingly inhibiting these projections did not cause increased avoidance.

Generally, these results seem very straightforward and clear. The bidirectional effects are striking, and the work is nicely done and very clean. However I am bothered a bit by what seems to me to be a lack of consistency with the prior report, which is not addressed. Though it is also possible that I simply missed it. Depending on which it is, the authors can likely fix it in the text or straighten me out.

So the issue is this – the prior report claimed that the inhibitory correlates in PL were the unique and prime mover in supporting avoidance. Yet here they fail to find any such effect of inhibition of the downstream terminals. In BLA, inhibition of PL terminals disrupts avoidance and in VS inhibition has no effect. Further they find that activation of output from PL to both areas plays a role in modulating the PMA, which somewhat contradicts the assertation that such activity playing the same role in PMA as in other fear and extinction related procedures.

My own suspicion is that the lack of effects of inhibition in Figure 1F-H is due to a ceiling effect, and that if they had trained the rats more lightly or had extinguished as done in Figures 2 and 3, then they would have found that inhibition of the PL->VS pathway does support PMA. Such data would resolve the seeming inconsistency and, coupled with a more clear discussion of what the effects of activation mean for interpreting the excitatory responses, make more sense to me than the current interpretation, which seems to mostly ignore the issue.

The other possibility to me is that inhibiting across the entire tone is not the same as the neural suppression in some of the neurons at the time of the tone, in which case their approach is not well designed to mimic the correlates.

The reviewer is correct that photo-silencing PL projections for 30 sec is not equivalent to mimicking inhibitory tone responses in PL, since there is great variability in the duration of inhibition among these neurons. Rather, we are interested in how the overall inhibition of the PL-VS projection across the entire tone drives avoidance.

Of course as I said I may simply not understand how the data all fit together. If this is the case, the authors can clarify it for me. If this is not the case, then I think the failure to find the site where inhibitory PL activity facilitates PMA is a problem for the clean story of bidirectional control the authors want to paint. I am more than happy to have it addressed in the text if the authors want to do this, noting the above caveats and modifying how things are discussed. As I said, I think it is likely an unfortunate ceiling effect. Or the authors could do this experiment if they do not want to fix it textually, but I this is not something I'd require. This is really my only substantive issue with an otherwise very elegant set of results.

The reviewer is correct to point out that the lack of effect of silencing PL-VS appears to be inconsistent with our original report (Diehl, et al., 2018) and was not dealt with directly in the previous version of the manuscript. We have updated the text in the Discussion to address this point and mention the possibility of: 1) a ceiling effect, 2) fibers of passage from PL to BLA that may have been inadvertently silenced when silencing PL-VS projections, and 3) variation in our optic probe placement affecting our results.

Reviewer #2:

This is an elegant series of studies that builds up on Diehl and colleagues prior publication. The rationale for the circuits investigated is clear. The experiments were well designed, executed and interpreted. The results are an important contribution to an emerging literature for the neural circuits supporting avoidance learning.

1) Like the authors, I was a little surprised to see that photoinhibition of PL terminals in the VS did not promote avoidance behavior. I think this null effect warrants more discussion. Looking at the histology in Figure 1—figure supplement 1, it appears that the optical placements for photo-inhibition were ventral to those for photoexcitation. So perhaps photoexcitation more effectively stimulated the accumbens core, whereas photoinhibition targeted the accumbens shell. Also clear is that the PL -> VS projections are extensive. Might it be the case that photoinhibition only targeted a small percentage of PL fibers, a number insufficient to facilitate avoidance? To this second point, the Sabitini lab has developed means of sharpening ferrules to achieve greater light diffusion: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5533215/.

We re-examined the optical probe placements (see Author response image 1 with individual data points labeled, from Figure 1G) and some of these placements indeed targeted the shell, rather than the core for our PL-VS experiments. Rats with optical probes that targeted the shell region showed lower avoidance compared to most of the rats with optical probes targeting the core region. We mention this caveat in the Discussion. We also thank the reviewer for the reference on how to improve our ferrules to achieve greater light illumination for our future studies.

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

2) The use of the term ventral striatum is warranted here, as both the PL and BLA target the shell and core subregions of the nucleus accumbens. Moving forward, it will likely be important to acknowledge and examine dissociable roles for the core and shell in avoidance. The authors rightly discuss the Ramirez study examining core. Some discussion of the finding by Lichtenberg et al., 2014 is warranted and could be the basis of a more specific discussion of core vs. shell.

The study by Lichtenberg et al., 2014 reported that pre-training lesions in VS enhance avoidance responses in the shuttle task. However, successful avoidance was measured on the 7th day of training after lesions were made, introducing the possibility that other brain structures are likely compensating for the loss of VS (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk, 2005), thereby forcing an alternative circuit to acquire the task. Moreover, the lesion approach cannot tell us which inputs to VS may be responsible for enhancing avoidance. We now mention this study in the Discussion.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

On discussion, the reviewer's felt that the paper presents enough new data and is enough at odds with the prior report that it would be better presented as a new article than as a straightforward Research Advance follow up. The category of the paper has been changed, but please confirm that this is acceptable in your response.

We accept to change the article type from Research Advance to a new Research Article.

In addition, it was felt that some additional minor changes to the Discussion were needed to clarify the issue with regard to the specificity of the critical PFC->accumbent manipulation and the role of accumbent in avoidance. The first is that we would like you to state more explicitly that future studies using the recombinant approach would allow targeting of the core projections specifically. This is currently a bit unclear as understanding this requires reference to a phrase several sentences prior stating that the projections are primarily core. Please edit to make this point more easily understood.

We thank the reviewers for these helpful comments in further clarifying the interpretation of our results. We have modified the second paragraph of the Discussion to clarify the point that using a recombinant approach is needed to precisely target PL-VS projections to confirm the effects of photosilencing this projection.

The second is at the top of the next paragraph, where we think you could say something more direct regarding the significance of the activation data, assuming you agree. In particular, we feel like the fact that activation impairs avoidance provides indirect evidence c/w the prior findings (that inhibition facilitates). Noting this would help avoid the mis-impression that the data are completely contradictory to the prior study or that accumbent is not involved in avoidance.

The beginning of the third paragraph is now revised to tie together photoactivation of PL-VS impairing avoidance with our previous finding that photoactivation of PL somata also impaired avoidance.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of PL-VS projections.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of PL-BLA projections.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Percent time on platform during manipulation of BLA-VS projections.
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES