Abstract
Message framing plays an important role in advertising strategies and has been studied from various perspectives in different behavioral researches. In this study Event Rated Potentials technique helped to examine the neural mechanism of message framing effect on self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt regarding consumer purchase intention in the context of green marketing. Behavioral results showed that participants ranked higher on positive framing compared to negative framing. ERP results declared that N1 component was elicited by positive framed message with emotion of pride and guilt, reflecting use of attentional resources to acquire potential benefits at first stage of processing emotional information. At the second stage P2 was higher for negative framing containing pride and guilt slogan, showing more attention towards processing emotional information about potential loss. At the third stage LPP component portray that both positive and negative framing is supported by guilt emotion when processing emotional information in decision making. Our results delivered strong evidence that how purchase intention can be mediated by message framing under the pride and guilt emotions in the context of green marketing.
Subject terms: Attention, Decision
Introduction
As the uprising lift about environmental concerns turn out to be highly essential across business practices, companies have acknowledged the importance of environment safe marketing strategies and highly emphasized to adopt pro-environmental behavior and green advertising about their products and services1. In fact, green advertising conveys its message to the needs of pro-environmental consumers2. Environmental appeals have given promising attitudes and better purchase intentions3, whereas consumers emotional processing has an effective role in stimulating green purchase intentions4–6. In this regard an ideal convincing communication strategy often used to manipulate consumer behavior and attitude5 and to present objectively same information in terms of either the advantages gained or disadvantages suffered is called as message framing7–10.
The prospect theory declares that decision making can be influenced in response to objectively same information by whether it is presented as a potential gain or a potential loss11. An overview of the message framing portrays that positive and negative message framing has been practiced extensively to examine the potential gain and loss effects of different advertising strategies7,12. The companies concern in shaping a positively framed message is emphasizing the benefits to the environment after purchasing the green products ,while the companies concern in shaping a negatively framed message is to highlight potential destructive outcomes resulting from consumers’ willingness to purchase non green products8,13,14. Message framing holds a relevance to green marketing appeals which have selfless motivations and encouragement to acquire environment friendly behaviors9.
In the field of social marketing various motivational, individual and circumstantial factors are greatly considered for their influential role in environmental message framing10,15. When people assess their behavior in accordance with some personal and social standards it generates self-conscious emotions16,17. In this study we will focus on two self-conscious emotions, pride and guilt. Indeed, these two emotions seem like significant in the framework of pro-environmental behavior. Pride is a positive emotion that evokes pleasurable and self-confidant feelings worth18,19. Whereas guilt is a negative emotion that is experienced as apologetic, anxious and tense feeling20,21. Pride16,17,22 and guilt23,24 have some common characteristics. In fact, both of these emotions are evoked in result of responsible feelings for an act, and the evaluation of that act shows personal and social standards. Some limited studies report self-regulatory function of self-conscious emotions25,26, subsequently it helps in adapting people behavior to reflect their standards27.
Theory of Planned Behavior28, posit that intentions are the main predictors of behavior. Previous research reveals that pride and guilt are linked with pro-environmental behavior29,30. In their study31, found that pride and guilt reflect self-regulatory function and both of these emotions link the environment friendly intentions with personal norms, attitudes and social norms. A self-regulatory function was provided by both pride and guilt when studying consumer behavior in the context of environment friendly consumer choices32. Negative self-conscious emotions facilitate the relationship between attitudes and social norms on intentions, in both contexts of textbook piracy and condom use17,33. According to34, guilt join up with gain framed message is more influential to acquire environment friendly behavioral intention.
In the past few years research on emotions to examine framing effects has got an enhanced theoretical35,36 and empirical attention37,38. As the examination of framing effect with respect to self-conscious emotions appears in the shape of cognitive biasedness, but previous studies have examined framing effect from numerous behavioral perspectives. Various research studies have applied cognitive neuroscience methods to examine framing effects39–41. By using eye-tracking method39 found, larger number of active eye movements were induced by negative frames. While no moderating effect was found on purchase intention with eye-tracking method41. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technique revealed association of positive frames with reflexive brain regions, and negative frames with reflective brain regions40. By applying ERP method, it was found that positive and negative messages were processed differentially and provided dissimilar purchase intentions42.
In comparison with other neuroscientific methods, ERP technique provides high temporal resolution and excellent measure to track modulations of neural activity43. It’s valuable in a sense to differentiate early perceptual reactivity from more difficult and elaborate emotional processes44. The ERP components that occur up to 300 ms after the stimulus is presented to show a stage of attention, which possibly reflects early sensual encoding of stimulus that is emotionally significant45,46. In fact, this type of effect probably state more refined processing of emotional stimuli along with a rich emotional effect, that can be viewed in higher ERP components like P3 and Late Positive Potential47. In this study, the experimental stimuli would be positive and negative framed messages accompanied by emotional words of pride and guilt. The data would be analyzed based on the three ERP components, N1 (90–160 ms), P2 (200–300 ms) and LPP (500–700 ms) to elaborate the attentional and emotional mechanism. The N100 is a negative going wave that occurs approximately 100 ms post stimulus48. The N100 is modulated by attention49, emotion50, and congruence51,52.
Consumer decision making process in various marketing related spheres is illuminated by ERPs regarding emotions and consumer preferences53,54. ERP components that appear at the first 300 ms post stimulus are normally considered for sensory encoding of emotionally significant stimuli46,55. In this regard P2 is an early ERP component that is widely examined56. The P2 component is described as a positive going wave with a peak latency range of 150–250 ms, having sensitivity to emotional valence of stimuli57,58. Some of the studies have found that a higher P2 amplitude is recorded for negative than positive stimuli58–60.
LPP is a positive, long lasting wave with a time window of around 500–700 ms post stimulus61–63. In particular, LPP have a higher amplitude at centro-parietal sites for emotionally significant stimulus than for neutral stimulus64 such as images, faces47,65 and words33,66. This component may process task demands and reflects elaborate processing phase of attention, evaluation and emotional visual stimuli67,68. Some research studies have reported that LPP can differentiate emotional valence, resulting in different amplitudes for positive and negative words68–70.
A variety of the research studies have examined emotion’s role in framing effects from different perspectives39,40, but the literature lacks a clear synthesis of the current evidence addressing this issue. An neuroscientific method like ERP can thus help to provide more insights to construct a persuasive message design42. In fact, by using a neuroscientific method like ERP, this paper is first in its nature to uncover that effect of message framing on self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt regarding consumer purchase intentions in the context of green marketing. Explicitly, our main focus is to examine the brain regions where message framing affects self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt. We are looking that when the potential benefits and losses are portrayed by message framing, whether they are adopted by the feelings of pride or rejected by guilt? And how these positive and negative self-conscious emotions ultimately influence the consumer decision making and purchase intentions? Based on the above literature we have developed the hypothesis that the ERP components discussed above will explain the effect of different message framing on participants’ self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt regarding consumer environment safe purchase intentions.
Method
Participants
In this study twenty male Chinese post graduate students were taken to perform experiment. The participation in the experiment was requested to both the male and female students but the female students were unwilling to participate. All the students participated were from Zhejiang University, aged 20–27 years (M = 23.3 years, SD = 2.11 years). The participants were free from any neurological and psychiatric disease and had a normal or corrected to normal vision. As a result of abnormal EEG recordings, data of two participants was discarded. Eventually eighteen participants, aged 20–27 years (Mean = 23.23 years, SD = 2.19 years) were considered for this study. The protocol of this research was approved by the Ethics Committee of Neuromanagement Laboratory at Zhejiang University. All experiments were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. Before experiment written informed consent were obtained from all the participants.
Stimulus
Twenty pictures of the green products and four messages; positive and pride, positive and guilty, negative and pride and negative and guilty were included in the experiment. Each picture and message was presented on independent slide. The pictures used were of the daily life green products and all of them were taken from internet. The positive and pride message developed was “buying it makes you feeling pride”, positive and guilty message was “buying it makes you feeling not guilty”, negative and guilty message was “Ignoring it makes you feeling guilty” and negative and pride message was “Ignoring it makes you feeling not pride”. All the pictures and messages were developed by the authors and were evaluated and approved by five members of the Neuromanagement Lab. All the information was displayed in Chinese Language. In order to ensure a smooth flow and consistency in experiment background a uniform size of 840 by 640 pixels of product pictures was developed and a size of 1300 by 420 pixels was developed for the messages. This activity was done with the help of Photoshop software.
Procedure
All the participants received written instructions for conducting the experiment. The experiment was conducted in a dimly lit, electrically shielded room. The distance between each subject and computer screen was set as 100 cm. Participants used keyboard to record their choices. The experiment was designed in three unrepeated blocks. There were total 140 trials in the formal experiment and three practice trials were conducted before experiment to familiarize the participants with the experimental procedure. The experiment was run on E-prime 2.0 software package (Psychology Software tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). As shown in Fig. 1, the participants viewed in each trial a fixation point for 1000 ms then a product picture for 1500 ms followed by a message for 2000 ms and at the end a question about their choice of selection for 1500 ms. Based on the information provided in the message, the participants were asked are they willing to buy the product or not? The participants recorded their choices in the form of “Yes” or “No”, key 1 was used for Yes and key 3 was used for No.
EEG recordings
The EEG data were recorded (band-pass filter of 0.05–70 Hz, sampling rate 500 Hz) with a NeuroScan SynAmps2 Amplifier (Scan 4.3.1, Neurosoft Labs, Inc., Virginia, USA). Data recording was done on 64 scalp sites with Ag/AgCl electrodes, according to the standard international 10–20 system. The electro-oculograms were recorded from the electrodes located near the outer canthus of each eye (Horizontal), above and below the left eye (Vertical). During the experiment electrode impedance remained below 5 kΩ. The left mastoid was used as a reference electrode. Offline, EEG data was referenced to the average of left and right mastoid. Digital filtering of EOG artifacts was done through a zero-phase shift (low pass at 30 Hz, 24 dB/octave). The EEG level for epochs was set between − 200 to 800 ms. Baseline correction was made 200 ms interval pre-stimulus onset. Artifacts were rejected beyond ± 80 μV.
The mean amplitude of the ERP components N1 (90–160 ms) was scored at F3/FZ/F4/FC3/FCZ/FC4/C3/CZ/C4, and P200 (200–300 ms) and LPP (500–700 ms) both were scored at C3/CZ/C4/CP3/CPZ/CP4/P3/PZ/P4 electrode sites47,56,65,66,68. Repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to obtain statistical results of the ERP56,71.
Results
Behavioral results
To compare the mean values of the three framing variables one-way ANOVA was used. According to the results of this test we found a significant effect for framing (F (1, 17) = 4.387, p = 0.05), but an insignificant effect for emotions (F (1, 17) = 0.095, p = 0.762). Figure 2 indicates the purchase intentions of the participants. The mean scores for positive message was (M = 52.07, SD = 6.62), and negative framing was (M = 37.30, SD = 7.82). The mean scores for emotion pride was (M = 45.35, SD = 6.22) and emotion guilt was (M = 44.02, SD = 7.13).
ERP results
N1
Statistical analysis of repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect for framing effect (F (1, 17) = 6.995, p = 0.017), whereas no significant effect for emotions (F (1, 17) = 0.596, p = 0.451) and electrodes (F (8, 136) = 1.590, p = 0.133) were found. Also, no significant interaction effects were found for framing, emotions and electrodes (F (8, 136) = 0.935, p = 0.490). Multiple comparisons gave the amplitude estimates for positive framing (Mean = − 1.333, SE = 0.354), negative framing (Mean = − 0.363, SE = 0.286), for emotion pride (Mean = − 1.043, SE = 0.374) and guilt (Mean = − 0.653, SE = 0.357). The mean unit of voltage value of the nine electrodes (F3/FZ/F4/FC3/FCZ/FC4/C3/CZ/C4) was − 0.8481 μV.
P2
Statistical analysis of repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect for framing effect (F (1, 17) = 9.107, p = 0.008), and electrodes (F (8, 136) = 6.618, p = 0.001) were found, whereas no significant effect for emotions (F (1, 17) = 0.596, p = 0.188) was found. A significant interaction effects was found for framing and emotions (F (1, 17) = 9.791, p = 0.006). A simple effect analysis for this significant interaction effect was validated with the help of T-test. The results showed a significant effect for positive and pride message and negative and pride message (t = − 4.134, p = 0.001), while an insignificant effect was obtained for positive and guilty message and negative and guilty message (t = − 0.422, p = 0.678), this implies that positive and negative message under pride emotion has substantial effect which overcomes the insignificant effect of guilt emotion. Multiple comparisons gave the amplitude estimates for positive framing (Mean = 3.142, SE = 0.676), negative framing (Mean = 4.588, SE = 0.617), for emotion pride (Mean = 3.435, SE = 0.707) and guilt (Mean = 4.295, SE = 0.649). The mean unit of voltage value of the nine electrodes (C3/CZ/C4/CP3/CPZ/CP4/P3/PZ/P4) was 3.866 μV.
LPP
Statistical analysis of repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect for framing effect (F (1, 17) = 5.508, p = 0.031), and electrodes (F (8, 136) = 4.807, p = 0.001) were found, whereas no significant effect for emotions (F (1, 17) = 1.574, p = 0.227) was found. Also, no significant interaction effects were found for framing, emotions and electrodes. Multiple comparisons gave the amplitude estimates for positive framing (Mean = 1.116, SE = 0.676), negative framing (Mean = 2.141, SE = 0.615), for emotion pride (Mean = 1.138, SE = 0.735) and guilt (Mean = 2.119, SE = 0.711). The mean unit of voltage value of the nine electrodes (C3/CZ/C4/CP3/CPZ/CP4/P3/PZ/P4) was 1.629 μV.
Discussion
The present study examined that how an emotional stimulus can evoke ERP components by using message framing in the context of Green Marketing. The results confirm the findings and supported both the prospect theory and the theory of planned behavior. We got evidence (see Fig. 3) that positive message framing including the pride and guilt emotions has greater impact on N1 component, while negative message framing with emotions of pride and guilt has more influenced the P2 component. Moreover, message framing both in positive and negative form along with the emotion of guilt has more influenced the LPP component.
N1 at frontal region is found to have reflection of both selective attentional processes49,50 and emotional processes52,72. Consistent with these studies our results also found N1 amplitude at the frontal regions. In a study by73, fearful expression evoked N1 component than happy and neutral faces at frontal brain site. Whereas our results elicited N1 component when the emotions of pride and guilt were influenced by positively framed message. A study by50, found that smaller N1 amplitude was produced by low frequency positive and negative words than neutral words. Whereas in our case we found that the positive message containing positive and negative emotional words of pride and guilt evoked N1 amplitude.
In their study74 they found that a better word recognition is reflected by a longer latency P2 component. P2 has the ability to show at first glance the affective content shown in stimuli56,59. The results of this study provided significant effect of negative framing messages designed along with emotional words of pride and guilt as compared to positive framing message. Negative framing message holds more attentional resources with larger P2 amplitude than positive framing message42. According to73, emotions of participants were activated by attractive and unattractive faces during logos recommendation experiment. On the other hand, our study declares that participant’s emotions were triggered by emotionally designed message framing. Although the P2 amplitude was enhanced in a social context, irrespective of the emotional significance of provided visual stimuli75. While our results in the context of green marketing reflected higher P2 amplitude for emotionally significant visual stimuli.
Our results found a significant effect of message framing on self-conscious emotions at the later cognitive stage of LPP. Some researchers have reported that stimuli based on emotional words evoked increased LPP amplitude76–79. Interestingly in the current study, our stimuli containing the emotional word guilt also evoked higher LPP amplitude for positive and negative framing. According to some previous studies the processing of positive and negative emotional information of stimuli is amplified by the expectation of socially important events80–82. Also Larger LPP amplitude was evoked by providing stimuli based on emotional words83,84. Whereas the findings of our study reveal that stimuli containing emotional word guilt, have provided enhanced LPP amplitude in the context of environment safety. The research studies worked on interactions of task and emotion have provided fronto-central LPP effects46,85. In the current study, centro-pareital effects are found for guilt emotion under the positive and negative framed messages. And the LPP amplitude produced by positive and negative words as compared to neutral words shows that the contents of the message are deeply processed78. Likewise, the emotional content guilt in our messages is deeply processed by the participants under the positive and negative combinations of message framing.
The results of this study found significant effect for various messages framed with emotional words. The information of the messages was processed at different regions of the brain and reflected a non-similar purchase intention. Although there is prominent evidence on framing effect in several studies but our main focus was to investigate underlying neural mechanism for the message framing effect on positive and negative self-conscious emotions regarding consumer purchase intention in the context of green marketing. For this reason, we used ERP method and tried to explain why self-conscious emotions affected by message framing can work under the umbrella of green marketing. In our ERP study we found some components related to message framing in green marketing appeals. We found the components in three stages. In the first stage N1, in the second stage P2, while LPP component was found in the third stage. The findings revealed consistency of the ERP components with the hypothesis that there is a substantial effect of different message framing on participants’ self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt regarding consumer environment safe purchase intentions. This was due to the reason that these messages contain a slogan and direction to indulge in environment safe or unsafe action. The results declared that positive and negative messages framed with emotions of pride and guilt functions in the context of green marketing. It allows the insight that these emotions enabled the participants to acquire a purchase intention according to their social norms towards environment safety.
Talking about the first stage at a very early attentional level N1, we found that participants adopted the positive framing along with pride and guilt. It shows that just confronting the message inclined them to acquire the potential benefits stated in the positive framing. Also, at this level the emotional effect indicates the pursuit of positive emotion pride and avoidance of negative emotion guilt in the context of pro-environmental behavior. The underlying neural mechanism at a very early stage reflects that self-conscious emotions guide positive framing towards environment safe purchase intention. Whereas on the second stage at P2, participants ranked the negative framing influenced greater than positive framing. At this level even both the emotions pride and guilt played their significant role. The participants have processed the information given in the messages, and tend to show environment safe behavior with their purchase intention. Same like the intention at early stage, here they have promoted both the pursuit of pride and avoidance of guilt.
In the last stage at LPP, the participants adopted both the positive and negative framing along with self-conscious emotion of guilt. Interestingly this indicates that the participants have processed the stimuli information and they have preferred avoidance of guilt in both shapes of positive and negative framing. It shows in their decision-making process emotion of guilt play important role than pride.
There are some practical implications of this study. In this case, first marketers should try to construct their advertising claims with the specific emotional words that drive the customer purchase intentions. Positively framed messages induced with self-conscious emotional words will attract attention of the customers at a very first glance. Negatively framed messages would also help in the case when customers go deeper to understand the message slogan. Second, by using self-conscious emotional words in advertising would help marketers to secure early attention plus the encouragement to acquire pro-environmental behavior.
Third our research sheds light on the value and role of neuroscience method to study consumer immediate brain response. Prior research has done behavioral studies to examine message framing and did not consider framing effects in green marketing context. Even none of the studies have used the emotional words to identify the framing effect. However, our results indicate that emotionally framed messages work in Chinese culture. Eventually it is clearly reflected that ERP is a fertile method to study customer motivational engagement with emotional significant marketing stimuli.
The current findings support the method of laboratory experiments with neuroscientific method to examine marketing promotional activities. Investigations of environment friendly advertising messages through Neuromarketing could improve understanding of scholars about underlying neural mechanisms of brain. Furthermore, these insights could enable marketers with improved predictions about consumer behavior in Chinese markets.
Further research could examine, if the advertisement and customer promotional activities have more things like packaging, pricing then it will change the customer thinking and judgment. Based on the ERP results framing effect along with pride and guilt emotions can work at the attentional and emotional stage. Including some other self-conscious emotional words may change the customer preference to deeply involve in environment safe purchase intentions. The participants in this study were all the male students, in the future studies to examine the message framing effect could be investigated both from male and female subjects of a broader age group. We made stimuli containing positive and negative messages along with emotional words of pride and guilt in this study. Further work by examining the verbs, nouns and other types of information structuring used in green marketing context could be analyzed in the future studies.
Conclusion
In the current study we used message framing made-up with emotional words of pride and guilt in the green marketing context. In our real life it is very special pertaining to environmental safety and protection and is related to emotions. This study was performed to examine the attentional and emotional processing of message framing and its effect on self-conscious emotions resulting in ERP components modulation due to these messages. We found important contribution to ERP components like N1, P2, and LPP. Previous literature of behavioral studies showed these components were attracted by attention and related with early stage emotions. Whereas our findings from neural perspective indicate the message framing effect based on self-conscious emotions of pride and guilt.
Author contributions
M.Z. and X.W. designed the experiment. M.Z., S.M.U., and R.W. prepared the experiment and collected behavioral and ERP data. M.Z., M.A. and S.I. processed and analyzed data. M.Z. and X.W. wrote the main manuscript text and prepared all the figures. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Funding
This research study was supported by Grant No.71572176 from the National Natural Science Foundation of China https://www.nsfc.gov.cn/), and Grant No. 16ZJQN030YB of the Zhejiang Youth Social Science Fund (https://zjskw.gov.cn/).
Data availability
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Green Marketing: A Global Strategic Business Report—Asia Green Buildings. (2019).
- 2.Zinkhan GM, Carlson L. Green advertising and the reluctant consumer. J. Advert. 1995;24:1–6. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1995.10673471. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chan RYK, Leung TKP, Wong YH. The effectiveness of environmental claims for services advertising. J. Serv. Mark. 2006;20:233–250. doi: 10.1108/08876040610674580. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Carrus G, Passafaro P, Bonnes M. Emotions, habits and rational choices in ecological behaviours: The case of recycling and use of public transportation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008;28:51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Maheswaran D, Meyers-Levy J. The influence of message framing and issue involvement. J. Mark. Res. 1990;27:361–367. doi: 10.1177/002224379002700310. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Chang CT, Lee YK. Framing charity advertising: Influences of message framing, image valence, and temporal framing on a charitable appeal. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2009;39:2910–2935. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00555.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Rothman AJ, Bartels RD, Wlaschin J, Salovey P. The strategic use of gain- and loss-framed messages to promote healthy behavior: How theory can inform practice. J. Commun. 2006;56:202–220. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00290.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chandy RK, Tellis GJ, Macinnis DJ, Thaivanich P. What to say when: Advertising appeals in evolving markets. J. Mark. Res. 2001;38:399–414. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.38.4.399.18908. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Tu JC, Kao TF, Tu YC. Influences of framing effect and green message on advertising effect. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2013;41:1083–1098. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2013.41.7.1083. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Keller PA, Lipkus IM, Rimer BK. Affect, framing, and persuasion. J. Mark. Res. 2003;40:54–64. doi: 10.1509/jmkr.40.1.54.19133. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. (1981). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 12.Tsai S-P. Message framing strategy for brand communication. J. Advert. Res. 2007;47:364–380. doi: 10.2501/S0021849907070377. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Homer PM, Yoon SG. Message framing and the interrelationships among ad-based feelings, affect, and cognition. J. Advert. 1992;21:19–33. doi: 10.1080/00913367.1992.10673357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Levin IP, Schneider SL, Gaeth GJ. All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1998;76:149–188. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1998.2804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Yan C, Dillard JP, Shen F. The Effects of mood, message framing, and behavioral advocacy on persuasion. J. Commun. 2010;60:344–363. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2010.01485.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Lewis MA. Self-conscious emotions: Embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. In: Havil MLJM, editor. Handb. Emot. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tracy JL, Robins RW. Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: A theoretical model. Psychol. Inq. 2004;15:103–125. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1502_01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rodriguez Mosquera PM, Manstead ASR, Fischer AH. The role of honor-related values in the elicitation, experience, and communication of pride, shame, and anger: Spain and the Netherlands compared. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2000;26:833–844. doi: 10.1177/0146167200269008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Tracy JL, Robins RW. The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007;92:506–525. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baumeister RF, Stillwell AM, Heatherton TF. Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychol. Bull. 1994;115:243–267. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ferguson TJ, Stegge H, Damhuis I. Children’s understanding of guild and shame. Child Dev. 1991;62:827–839. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mascolo, M. F., & Fischer, K. W. Developmental transformations in appraisals for pride, shame, and guilt. In Self-Conscious Emotions: The Psychology of Shame, Guilt, Embarrassment, and Pride 64–113 (1995).
- 23.Kugler K, Jones WH. On conceptualizing and assessing guilt. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1992;62:318–327. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.62.2.318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tangney JP, Miller RS, Flicker L, Barlow DH. Are Shame, Guilt, and Embarrassment Distinct Emotions? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996;70:1256–1269. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hynie M, MacDonald TK, Marques S. Self-conscious emotions and self-regulation in the promotion of condom use. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2006;32:1072–1084. doi: 10.1177/0146167206288060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Su H-J, Lu L-C, Lin TA. The mediating role of anticipated guilt in consumers’ textbook piracy intention. Asia Pac. Manag. Rev. 2011;16:255–275. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Carver CS, Scheier MF. On the Self-Regulation of Behavior. Cambridge: (Cambridge University Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991;50:179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Harth NS, Leach CW, Kessler T. Guilt, anger, and pride about in-group environmental behaviour: Different emotions predict distinct intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013;34:18–26. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.12.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Arvola A, et al. Predicting intentions to purchase organic food: The role of affective and moral attitudes in the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Appetite. 2008;50:443–454. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Onwezen MC, Antonides G, Bartels J. The Norm Activation Model: An exploration of the functions of anticipated pride and guilt in pro-environmental behaviour. J. Econ. Psychol. 2013;39:141–153. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2013.07.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Onwezen MC, Bartels J, Antonides G. Environmentally friendly consumer choices: Cultural differences in the self-regulatory function of anticipated pride and guilt. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014;40:239–248. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.07.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fischler I, Bradley M. Event-related potential studies of language and emotion: Words, phrases, and task effects. Prog. Brain Res. 2006;156:185–203. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56009-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Baek TH, Yoon S. Guilt and shame: Environmental message framing effects. J. Advert. 2017;46:440–453. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2017.1321069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nabi RL. Exploring the framing effects of emotion do discrete emotions differentially influence information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Commun. Res. 2003;30:224–247. doi: 10.1177/0093650202250881. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Updegraff JA, Rothman AJ. Health message framing: Moderators, mediators, and mysteries. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 2013;7:668–679. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12056. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lerner JS, Keltner D. Fear, anger, and risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2001;81:146–159. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marti M, Stauffacher M, Matthes J, Wiemer S. Communicating earthquake preparedness: The influence of induced mood, perceived risk, and gain or loss frames on homeowners’ attitudes toward general precautionary measures for earthquakes. Risk Anal. 2018;38:710–723. doi: 10.1111/risa.12875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Lin HH, Yang SF. An eye movement study of attribute framing in online shopping. J. Mark. Anal. 2014;2:72–80. doi: 10.1057/jma.2014.8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Murch KB, Krawczyk DC. A neuroimaging investigation of attribute framing and individual differences. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2013;9:1464–1471. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Yang D, Lu Y, Zhu W, Su C. Going green: How different advertising appeals impact green consumption behavior. J. Bus. Res. 2015;68:2663–2675. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Jin J, Zhang W, Chen M. How consumers are affected by product descriptions in online shopping: Event-related potentials evidence of the attribute framing effect. Neurosci. Res. 2017;125:21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2017.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ma Q, Feng Y, Xu Q, Bian J, Tang H. Brain potentials associated with the outcome processing in framing effects. Neurosci. Lett. 2012;528:110–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Gardener EKT, Carr AR, Macgregor A, Felmingham KL. Sex differences and emotion regulation: An event-related potential study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:1–9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schupp HT, Junghöfer M, Weike AI, Hamm AO. Emotional facilitation of sensory processing in the visual cortex. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14:7–13. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schupp HT, et al. Selective visual attention to emotion. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:1082–1089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3223-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cuthbert BN, Schupp HT, Bradley MM, Birbaumer N, Lang PJ. Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biol. Psychol. 2000;52:95–111. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00044-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sur S, Sinha V. Event-related potential: An overview. Ind. Psychiatry J. 2009;18:70. doi: 10.4103/0972-6748.57865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Hillyard SA, Hink RF, Schwent VL, Picton TW. Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. New Ser. 1973;182:177–180. doi: 10.1126/science.182.4108.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Scott GG, O’Donnell PJ, Leuthold H, Sereno SC. Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biol. Psychol. 2009;80:95–104. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pourtois G, Thut G, Grave de Peralata R, Michel C, Vuilleumier P. Two electrophysiological stages of spatial orienting towards fearful faces: Early temporo-parietal activation preceding gain control in extrastriate visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;26:149–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Atkinson CM, Drysdale KA, Fulham WR. Event-related potentials to Stroop and reverse Stroop stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2003;47:1–21. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(02)00038-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Smidts A, et al. Advancing consumer neuroscience. Mark. Lett. 2014;25:257–267. doi: 10.1007/s11002-014-9306-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Camerer, C. & Yoon, C. Introduction to the Journal of Marketing Research Special Issue on Neuroscience and Marketing. (2015).
- 55.Junghöfer, M., Bradley, M. M., Elbert, T. R. & Lang, P. J. Fleeting Images: A New Look at Early Emotion Discrimination. (2001). [PubMed]
- 56.Wang X, Huang Y, Ma Q, Li N. Event-related potential P2 correlates of implicit aesthetic experience. NeuroReport. 2012;23:862–866. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283587161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Paulmann S, Kotz SA. Early emotional prosody perception based on different speaker voices. NeuroReport. 2008;19:209–213. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f454db. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lai VT, Huettig F. When prediction is fulfilled: Insight from emotion processing. Neuropsychologia. 2016;85:110–117. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Carretié L, Mercado F, Tapia M, Hinojosa JA. Emotion, attention, and the ‘negativity bias’, studied through event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2001;41:75–85. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00195-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Huang YX, Luo YJ. Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 2006;398:91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Schupp HT, et al. Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology. 2000;37:257–261. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3720257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hajcak G, MacNamara A, Olvet DM. Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: An integrative review. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010;35:129–155. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Münte TF, et al. Brain potentials reveal the timing of face identity and expression judgments. Neurosci. Res. 1998;30:25–34. doi: 10.1016/S0168-0102(97)00118-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Olofsson JK, Nordin S, Sequeira H, Polich J. Affective picture processing: An integrative review of ERP findings. Biol. Psychol. 2008;77:247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Schupp HT, et al. Brain processes in emotional perception: Motivated attention. Cogn. Emot. 2004;18:593–611. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000239. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Herbert C, Kissler J, Junghöfer M, Peyk P, Rockstroh B. Processing of emotional adjectives: Evidence from startle EMG and ERPs. Psychophysiology. 2006;43:197–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Herbert C, Junghofer M, Kissler J. Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:487–498. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00638.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Palazova M, Mantwill K, Sommer W, Schacht A. Are effects of emotion in single words non-lexical? Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2766–2775. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bernat E, Bunce S, Shevrin H. Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2001;42:11–34. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00133-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Bayer M, Sommer W, Schacht A. P1 and beyond: Functional separation of multiple emotion effects in word recognition. Psychophysiology. 2012;49:959–969. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01381.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Ma Q, Zhang L, Pei G, Abdeljelil H. Neural process of the preference cross-category transfer effect: Evidence from an event-related potential study. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:1. doi: 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Pourtois G, De Gelder B, Vroomen J, Rossion B, Crommelinck M. The time-course of intermodal binding between seeing and hearing affective information. NeuroReport. 2000;11:1329–1333. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004270-00036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Luo W, Feng W, He W, Wang NY, Luo YJ. Three stages of facial expression processing: ERP study with rapid serial visual presentation. Neuroimage. 2010;49:1857–1867. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Finnigan S, O’Connell RG, Cummins TDR, Broughton M, Robertson IH. ERP measures indicate both attention and working memory encoding decrements in aging. Psychophysiology. 2011;48:601–611. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01128.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Pozharliev R, Verbeke WJMI, Van Strien JW, Bagozzi RP. Merely being with you increases my attention to luxury products: Using EEG to understand consumers’ emotional experience with luxury branded products. J. Mark. Res. 2015;52:546–558. doi: 10.1509/jmr.13.0560. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hofmann MJ, Kuchinke L, Tamm S, Võ MLH, Jacobs AM. Affective processing within 1/10th of a second: High arousal is necessary for early facilitative processing of negative but not positive words. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;9:389–397. doi: 10.3758/9.4.389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Schacht A, Sommer W. Emotions in word and face processing: Early and late cortical responses. Brain Cogn. 2009;69:538–550. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hinojosa JA, Méndez-Bértolo C, Pozo MA. Looking at emotional words is not the same as reading emotional words: Behavioral and neural correlates. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:748–757. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.00982.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Kissler J, Herbert C, Winkler I, Junghofer M. Emotion and attention in visual word processing: An ERP study. Biol. Psychol. 2009;80:75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Cornwell BR, Johnson L, Berardi L, Grillon C. Anticipation of public speaking in virtual reality reveals a relationship between trait social anxiety and startle reactivity. Biol. Psychiatry. 2006;59:664–666. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Reicherts P, Mühlberger A. Don’t look at me in anger! Enhanced processing of angry faces in anticipation of public speaking. Psychophysiology. 2010;47:271–280. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00938.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Bublatzky F, Gerdes ABM, White AJ, Riemer M, Alpers GW. Social and emotional relevance in face processing: Happy faces of future interaction partners enhance the LPP. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Kayser J, et al. Event-related potential (ERP) asymmetries to emotional stimuli in a visual half-field paradigm. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:414–426. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02385.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Eimer M, Holmes A. An ERP study on the time course of emotional face processing. NeuroReport. 2002;13:427–431. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200203250-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Schindler S, Kissler J. Selective visual attention to emotional words: Early parallel frontal and visual activations followed by interactive effects in visual cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:3575–3587. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.