Abstract
Objectives
The number of people testing positive for Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-COV-2) in the UK, particularly among young adults, is increasing. We report here on the mental health of young adults and related psychological and behavioural responses to the pandemic and consider the role of these factors in fuelling the increase in coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) in this group.
Methods
An online survey was completed during the first six weeks of the first UK-wide lockdown by 3097 respondents, including data for 364 respondents aged 18–24 years. The survey included measures of mental health and indices capturing related psychological and behavioural responses to the pandemic.
Results
The mental health of 18- to 24-years-olds in the first 6 weeks of lockdown was significantly poorer than that of older respondents and previously published norms: with 84% reporting symptoms of depression and 72% reporting symptoms of anxiety. Young adults also reported significantly greater loneliness and reduced positive mood, both of which were also associated with greater mental health difficulties.
Conclusions
We contend that the combination of mental health, social and economic considerations may have contributed to the rise of COVID-19 infections in young adults, and ascribing blame to this group will not aid our efforts to regain control of the disease.
Keywords: COVID-19, Behaviour, Young people, Mental health
Highlights
-
•
The UK has witnessed an exponential rise in coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) infections, particularly among young adults. It has been assumed that this is occurring due to poorer engagement in risk reduction behaviours by young people.
-
•
We demonstrate that the mental health impact of the COVID-19 pandemic has been significantly greater in young adults and is strongly associated with increased loneliness and lower positive mood. We suggest that these factors, combined with the social and economic circumstances of young adults, have increased their risk of infection. We propose that containing the spread of COVID-19 among the young will require a recognition of these factors and is ill-served by a culture of blame.
Introduction
In autumn 2020, there was growing alarm at the increase in the number of people testing positive for Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-COV-2) in the UK. Initially, this increase was attributed to younger people who were being vilified by politicians and the media1 and being implored to ‘stick to the rules’. However, this admonishment was being offered in a vacuum, without any consideration given to how the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic has affected young people or some of the legitimate and understandable reasons why they may be being infected with COVID-19 in greater numbers. We consider here some of those reasons and, in particular, provide evidence from the COVID-19 Stress and Health Study2 on the mental health consequences of the pandemic on young people which, we suggest, may also have played a role.
First, it is relevant to note that the context in which lockdown was eased, particularly in England, was such that the risk of ongoing transmission was high. Furthermore, the public health messaging then, and subsequently, has been criticised for being increasingly unclear and, therefore, ineffective. While neither of these factors impinged on young people alone, it is possible that the social, employment and mental health circumstances of this group led them to be among the first to resume participation in this disease context.
In terms of employment and social circumstances, we know that young adults are much more likely to have precarious contracts of employment such as zero-hour contracts,3 to be employed in the hospitality sector4 and potentially more likely to use public transport to get to their place of work.5 Thus, it is likely they were among the first to return to work when lockdown eased; the first to resume participation in society per se but also the first to find themselves in contexts harbouring elevated risks of infection.
In terms of mental health, we established the COVID-19 Stress and Health Study2 to prospectively examine the mental health impact of the pandemic on adults living in the UK. We have previously reported high levels of psychological morbidity in the cohort as a whole.6 Here, we present additional analysis examining the mental health impact of the pandemic and related psychological and behavioural responses in 18- to 24-year-olds. We consider the differences between this group and older participants and hypothesise how these differences may have further increased their risk of infection.
Methods
Ethics, recruitment, eligibility
Ethical approval was granted from the University of Nottingham Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences (ref: 506–2003) and the NHS Health Research Authority (ref: 20/HRA/1858). The study was launched on 3/4/20 with participants recruited in the community through a social and mainstream media campaign. Recruitment continued until 30/4/20.
Eligibility criteria specified that participants should be aged 18 years and older, able to give informed consent, able to read English, residing in the UK at the time of completing the survey and able to provide a sample of hair at least 1 cm long. The latter was collected for the determination of the stress biomarker cortisol.
Procedures
The procedures are described in detail elsewhere.6 In brief, participants were recruited in the community through a social and mainstream media campaign involving, but not limited to, Facebook and Twitter. In addition, Health Research Authority (HRA) regulatory approval enabled us to approach National Health Service organisations and request they advertise the research through their routine communications. Participants completed an online survey which included validated measures capturing anxiety (Generalised Anxiety Disorder Scale; α = 0.88), depression (Patient Health Questionnaire; α = 0.92) and stress (Perceived Stress Scale; α = 0.76),7, 8, 9 as well as indices capturing a range of psychological and behavioural responses to the pandemic.
Results
Data were available from 364 respondents aged 18–24 years and 2733 respondents aged older than 24 years. Comparisons with available UK data reported previously6 indicate that women were proportionally over-represented and participants older than 75 years, and from Northern Ireland, were under-represented in the current cohort. Otherwise, the sample was reasonably representative of the wider UK population. Demographic comparisons between participants aged 18–24 years and those >24 years appear in supplementary appendix Table S1.
In relation to mental health, we observed that 18- to 24-year-olds reported significantly increased levels of stress, anxiety and depression, compared with older participants and also previously published population norms (Table 1 ). Further analysis according to clinical thresholds on the measures of anxiety and depression revealed that 84% of 18- to 24-year-olds reported symptoms of depression and 72% reported symptoms of anxiety (with 56% meeting the threshold for high intensity psychology support for depression and 44% for anxiety: supplementary appendix Table S2). We also observed that young adults reported significantly greater loneliness (despite only 5.5% reporting living alone) and reduced positive mood (Table 2 ), both of which were consistently associated with greater stress, anxiety and depression after controlling for demographic covariates (supplementary appendix Tables S3–S5).
Table 1.
Scales | PHQ-9 score |
GAD-7 score |
PSS-4 score |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
18–24 years |
>24 years |
Norms |
18–24 years |
>24 years |
Norms |
18–24 years |
>24 years |
Norms |
|
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |
Total score | 11.23 (6.4)b | 7.2 (5.8) | 2.91 (3.5) | 9.02 (6.0)b | 6.3 (5.4) | 2.95 (3.4) | 8.13 (3.3)b | 6.3 (3.2) | 6.11 (3.1) |
Gender | |||||||||
Male | 9.68 (7.1)b | 5.9 (5.7) | 2.7 (3.5) | 7.16 (6.5)b | 4.9 (5.1) | 2.66 (3.2) | 6.83 (3.7)b | 5.7 (3.2) | 5.56 (3.0) |
Female | 11.66 (6.1)b | 7.4 (5.8) | 3.1 (3.5) | 9.52 (5.7)b | 6.5 (5.4) | 3.20 (3.5) | 8.47 (3.1)b | 6.4 (3.2) | 6.38 (3.2) |
PHQ-9, the 9-item Patient Health Questionnaire; GAD-7, the 7-item Generalised Anxiety Disorder Scale; PSS-4, the 4-item Perceived Stress Scale. Published population normative data for PHQ-9; SD, standard deviation (Kocalevent et al., 2013)12, GAD-7 (Löwe et al., 2008)14, PSS-4 (Warttig et al., 2013)13.
Mean scores were significantly higher among young respondents aged between 18 and 24 years compared with older respondents (age >24 years) and published population normative data (age ≥18 years), all P < 0.0001.
Table 2.
Scales | 18–25 years | >24 years |
---|---|---|
Engaged in social distancing | ||
Yes | 345 (94.8%) | 2523 (92.3%) |
No | 19 (15.2%) | 210 (7.7%) |
Positive mood (scale 1–30) | 17.7 (4.9)∗ | 19.2 (5.1) |
Perceived risk of getting COVID-19 (scale 1–10) | 4.1 (2.0)∗ | 4.8 (2.2) |
Perceived loneliness (scale 1–10) | 5.3 (2.7)∗ | 3.7 (2.7) |
COVID-19 worry about self | ||
‘I do not worry about getting COVID-19’ | 105 (28.9%) | 407 (14.9%) |
‘I occasionally worry about getting COVID-19’ | 209 (57.4%) | 1841 (67.4%) |
‘I spend much of the time worrying about getting COVID-19’ | 39 (10.7%) | 374 (13.7%) |
‘I spend most of the time worrying about getting COVID-19’ | 11 (3.0%) | 111 (4.1%) |
COVID-19 worry about others | ||
‘I do not worry about my close relative(s)/friend(s) getting COVID-19’ | 19 (5.2%) | 89 (3.3%) |
‘I occasionally worry about close relative(s)/friend(s) getting COVID-19’ | 214 (58.8%) | 1654 (60.5%) |
‘I spend much of the time worrying about close relative(s)/friend(s) getting COVID-19’ | 92 (25.3%) | 769 (28.1%) |
‘I spend most of the time worrying about close relative(s)/friend(s) getting COVID-19’ | 39 (10.7%) | 221 (8.1%) |
Data are n (%) or mean (SD). COVID-19, coronavirus disease 2019; SD, standard deviation.
∗Statistically significantly different between the two age groups at P < 0.0001.
An examination of other psychological and behavioural responses to the pandemic revealed that young adults were less likely to worry about contracting COVID-19 than older adults (X2 = 45.6, P < 0.001) but that they were as likely to worry about their close relative(s) or friend(s) getting COVID-19 (X2 = 7.30, P = 0.06) and as likely to engage in social distancing (Table 2), when compared with older respondents.
Discussion
Our analyses reveal that the mental health impact of the pandemic has been greater in 18- to 24-year-olds, compared with older adults. This age group also reported significantly greater loneliness and reduced positive mood, both of which were also associated with greater mental health difficulties. We suggest that, in combination with the social and employment considerations described earlier, this unprecedented increase in psychological morbidity and loneliness may also have contributed to the increased risk of infection in young adults. Two mechanisms can be considered. First, the easing of lockdown provided a much needed opportunity for increased social interaction and with it a means of restoring emotional well-being, assuaging loneliness and rediscovering positive emotional experiences. In the absence of any other strategies to restore their well-being, concurrent economic messages encouraging greater social interaction (‘eat out to help out') and public health messaging which, from the outset, minimised the risk of the disease to this group; it is perhaps not remarkable that young adults seized this opportunity. As such, the very social interaction which became necessary to restore their mental health may have become the vector through which the risk of infection was increased in this group.
Second, the constellation of psychological risk factors identified in young people in this cohort (i.e. poorer mental health and increased loneliness) have been shown time and again to dysregulate the immune system and increase the risk of viral infections, including coronavirus infections.10 Thus, the psychological repercussions of lockdown may also have directly affected their immunological competence and ability to resist COVID-19 infection.
The results also illustrated that during this first lockdown, 18- to 24-year-olds were as likely to report adhering to social distancing rules, as likely to be worried about the risk of COVID-19 to others, although less worried about the risk of COVID-19 to themselves, when compared with the rest of the cohort. These indicators do not support the caricature that is being presented by some of young people being reckless. Indeed, one could argue that the evidence of elevated infections, at a time when obtaining a test is increasingly difficult, is testament to the fact that they are being responsible.
It is perhaps timely to consider the possibility that the political and public health decisions that have been taken throughout the course of the pandemic, combined with the economic, social and emotional circumstances of young adults, has put them on a course whereby they have been exposed to COVID-19 sooner, and for longer, since lockdown was eased. While this may not wholly explain the increase in new infections in young adults, it is the case that a culture of blame will not provide the key to unlocking this issue,11 and we should be mindful of this as we plan to welcome back students to universities across the UK.
Author statements
Ethical approval
Ethical approval was granted from the University of Nottingham Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences (ref: 506-2003) and the NHS Health Research Authority (ref: 20/HRA/1858).
Funding
K.A. is supported by funding from the National Institute for Health Research School for Primary Care Research, United Kingdom (NIHR SPCR). T.C. acknowledges the financial support of the Department of Health via the National Institute for Health Research, United Kingdom (NIHR) Specialist Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health award to the South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust (SLaM) and the Institute of Psychiatry at King's College London. J.R.M. is funded by a Medical Research Council Clinician Scientist Fellowship [grant number MR/P008348/1]. J.R.M. is an editor of the Public Health and has been in no way involved in the editorial decision-making in the consideration of this manuscript. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. No other funding supported the work described in this manuscript.
Competing interests
J.R.M. is an editor of Public Health and has been in no way involved in the editorial decision-making in the consideration of this manuscript. No other competing interests declared.
Author contributions
K.V. had the idea for this opinion piece and wrote the first draft, with R.J. performing analyses and all other authors contributing to content and reviewing and approving the final version. As the corresponding author, K.V. attests that all listed authors meet authorship criteria and that no others meeting the criteria have been omitted.
Footnotes
The Corresponding Author has the right to grant on behalf of all authors and does grant on behalf of all authors, a worldwide licence to the Publishers and its licensees in perpetuity, in all forms, formats and media (whether known now or created in the future), to i) publish, reproduce, distribute, display and store the Contribution, ii) translate the Contribution into other languages, create adaptations, reprints, include within collections and create summaries, extracts and/or, abstracts of the Contribution, iii) create any other derivative work(s) based on the Contribution, iv) to exploit all subsidiary rights in the Contribution, v) the inclusion of electronic links from the Contribution to third party material where-ever it may be located; and, vi) licence any third party to do any or all of the above.
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhe.2020.10.018.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following is/are the Supplementary data to this article:
References
- 1.The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/sep/07/coronavirus-young-people-urged-to-follow-rules-as-uk-cases-rise [Accessed 10/09/2020].
- 2.COVID-19 Stress and Health Study. https://www.covidstressstudy.com/[Accessed 10/09/2020].
- 3.Office for National Statistics. https://www.ons.gov.uk/employmentandlabourmarket/peopleinwork/employmentandemployeetypes/datasets/emp17peopleinemploymentonzerohourscontracts [Accessed 10/09/2020].
- 4.People 1st. skills and workforce profile, hospitality and tourism. https://www.people1st.co.uk/getattachment/Insight-opinion/Latest-insights/Industry-profiles/Hospitality-tourism-skills-and-workforce-profile-2016.pdf/?lang=en-GB.
- 5.International Longevity Centre UK. https://ilcuk.org.uk/age-and-the-national-travel-survey/[Accessed 14/09/2020].
- 6.Jia R., Ayling K., Chalder T. Mental health in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic: early observations. BMJ Open. 2020 doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-040620. 0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kroenke K., Spitzer R.L., Williams J.B. The patient health Questionnaire somatic, anxiety, and depressive symptom scales: a systematic review. Gen Hosp Psychiatr. 2010;32(4):345–359. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2010.03.006. [published Online First: 2010/07/17] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Spitzer R.L., Kroenke K., Williams J.B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(10):1092–1097. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092. [published Online First: 2006/05/24] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Diener E., Wirtz D., Tov W. New well-being measures: short scales to assess flourishing and positive and negative feelings. Soc Indicat Res. 2010;97:143–156. [Google Scholar]
- 10.O'Connor D.B., Thayer J.F., Vedhara K. Stress and health: a review of psychobiological processes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2021;72(1) doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-062520-122331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Abraham T. Risk and outbreak communication: lessons from alternative paradigms. Bull World Health Organ. 2009;87(8):604–607. doi: 10.2471/BLT.08.058149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kocalevent R.D., Hinz A., Brähler E. Standardization of the depression screener patient health questionnaire (PHQ-9) in the general population. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2013;35(5):551–555. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.04.006. [published Online First: 2013/05/15] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Warttig S.L., Forshaw M.J., South J. New, normative, English-sample data for the Short Form Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4) J Health Psychol. 2013;18(12):1617–1628. doi: 10.1177/1359105313508346. [published Online First: 2013/10/25] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Löwe B., Decker O., Müller S. Validation and standardization of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder Screener (GAD-7) in the general population. Med Care. 2008;46(3):266–274. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e318160d093. [published Online First: 2008/04/05] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.