Abstract
The visual word form area (VWFA) in the left ventral occipito-temporal (vOT) cortex is key to fluent reading in children and adults. Diminished VWFA activation during print processing tasks is a common finding in subjects with severe reading problems. Here, we report fMRI data from a multicentre study with 140 children in primary school (7.9–12.2 years; 55 children with dyslexia, 73 typical readers, 12 intermediate readers). All performed a semantic task on visually presented words and a matched control task on symbol strings. With this large group of children, including the entire spectrum from severely impaired to highly fluent readers, we aimed to clarify the association of reading fluency and left vOT activation during visual word processing. The results of this study confirm reduced word-sensitive activation within the left vOT in children with dyslexia. Interestingly, the association of reading skills and left vOT activation was especially strong and spatially extended in children with dyslexia. Thus, deficits in basic visual word form processing increase with the severity of reading disability but seem only weakly associated with fluency within the typical reading range suggesting a linear dependence of reading scores with VFWA activation only in the poorest readers.
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Reading
Introduction
Reading words correctly and fluently is a prerequisite for understanding written text. Like other scholastic skills, reading shows a normal distribution within the population1. The poorest 5–10% of the children with insufficient mastery of reading are considered as presenting with developmental dyslexia2. While phonological deficits have been suggested to play a central role for the impairments seen in dyslexia3, processing visual words for meaning remains the central task in reading. Thus, understanding how poor readers and especially children with dyslexia process visual words in the context of a semantic task is essential, and functional neuroimaging studies offer insight into the neural mechanisms involved.
It is undisputed that the left ventral occipito-temporal cortex (vOT) has a prominent role in processing print4–15 and consequently in reading for meaning. Especially an area within the left mid-fusiform gyrus, often referred to as the visual word form area (VWFA) typically shows a preference in responding to words or orthographic stimuli as compared to visual control stimuli such as checkerboard patterns7 even though this region is also involved in other processes16,17. Subsequent studies extended the notion of a VWFA to an entire visual word form system (VWFS) in the inferior occipito-temporal cortex that is progressively tuned to orthographic regularities with more anterior locations14,18–22 and a slightly more posterior region to encode letters (letter form area: LFA23). Even more fine grained functional divisions of the complex left vOT system along the posterior-anterior and lateral-medial dimensions were detected in more recent studies: Functional and microarchitectonic properties divide the larger visual word form system into a posterior perceptual word form area responsible for feature extraction and a more anterior lexical integration site corresponding approximately to the location of the classic VWFA24. Lately, Bouhali and colleagues suggested an additional lateral to medial functional segregation of the vOT, whereby multiletter sublexical graphemes are processed in medial regions in comparison to the lexico-semantic encoding in more lateral sites25.
Developmental studies indicate that the print-sensitive response of the VWFA shows a rapid development in the first few months and years of learning to read26–33 and that it is initiated by grapheme-phoneme correspondence training in children26,29,34 and adults35,36. The location of the emerging print-sensitive area within the vOT seems constrained by its established connectivity to language regions long before children learn to read at school13,37 and develops in cortical patches with only weak specialization29. The exact location of the VWFA within the left vOT is subject to individual variations10,29,33,38. Importantly, the strength of the print sensitive response in the VWFA is modulated by the expertise level of reading in children and adults9,27,28,30,31,33. It is for this reason that the VWFA has also been referred to as reading skill zone39,40. Meta-analyses on children and adults with reading impairments revealed some of the most robust and consistent functional alterations in left vOT areas when tasks involve reading. Poor readers such as those with dyslexia activate this region less than typical readers41–43. This deficient activation in poor readers seems convergent for shallow and deep alphabetic languages44,45 and even generalizes to different writings systems46. Disruption of the activation in the VWFS through stimulation or through acquired brain lesions47 results in impaired reading48, again emphasizing its critical role for fluent reading.
Taken together, recent evidence points to a pivotal role of the VWFA for reading and reading acquisition49. The visual word processing system in the left vOT is sensitive to the perceptual and lexical properties of words23–25,31 and sublexical units23,25. This system is, however at the same time strongly influenced by top-down input from higher level phonological and semantic language areas19,50 and the dorsal attention network51. Therefore, also task demands modulate the differences seen between typical readers and subjects with dyslexia in the left vOT. Several studies reporting reduced left vOT activation in dyslexia for example employed tasks that put strong demands on phonological processing, such as reading pseudowords45,52–55, reading pseudohomophones22,54, or reading words having orthographic-phonological conflicts56. Studies employing explicit or implicit reading tasks with regular words typically reported less robust underactivation in left vOT in dyslexia (57–60; but see61), although deviant left vOT activation could still be detected in more focal ROI analyses22,58–60 or when comparing basic word processing and not print-sensitivity contrasts among groups30. While these studies emphasize that reading impairments are strongly associated with hypoactivation in inferior occipito-temporal regions under certain task conditions, it is less clear to what extent such deviations occur in more natural reading situations when readers focus on the meaning of words.
Some ambiguity in the dyslexia results might also stem from the typically small samples and the often-used categorical approaches in neuroimaging studies. Large-scale imaging studies on reading are still rare, and so far often examined tasks challenging the phonological system62–64. Thus, the goal of the current study was to investigate word reading in a large sample of children at varying reading skills in a primarily dimensional approach using a task that emphasizes semantic processing, i.e. reading for meaning. The use of our large sample allowed us to use reading fluency as a continuous regressor in the analyses instead of examining just categorical differences.
In order to obtain a large sample of subjects we developed a short controlled reading task in a multi-center study (NEURODYS). This task not only included visual words in the context of a semantic task, but also a low-level visual control task (cross-hatches and stars), where subjects had to decide whether all characters were the same or not [similar to52,58,63,65]. Data analyses focussed on the modulation of print-sensitivity through reading skills across the entire multi-center sample while controlling for differences between sites.
Methods
Subjects
From an initial group of 182 children with functional (fMRI) and structural (sMRI) data collected in three sites across Europe, 42 children were excluded, because of poor fMRI data quality or excessive movement (n = 18), because of prominent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms (n = 10), low IQ (n = 6) or absence of behavioural log files (n = 8). The analyses for the remaining 140 children (age 7.9–12.2 years) who participated either in Zurich (ZRH, n = 80), Maastricht (MAS, n = 35), or Salzburg (SBG, n = 25) are reported in this paper.
Children whose reading fluency score (correct words per minute) was more than 1.25 SD below the normative mean were considered dyslexic (< 10.5 percentile, n = 55). Children with reading fluency scores ≥ − 0.85 SD below the normative mean were considered typical readers (≥ 19.7 percentile, n = 73). Children (n = 12) having a z-score between − 1.25 and − 0.85 formed the gap group of “intermediate readers”. These intermediate readers were included for correlational analyses but excluded for categorical group comparisons. All children had an estimated nonverbal and verbal IQ of at least 85 based on the block design and similarities subtests of the WISC66. No child was excluded due to poor task performance if behavioural data were available.
Reading skills were assessed with the “Salzburger Lese- und Rechtschreibtest II” (SLRT-II67) in Zurich and Salzburg, and with the Dyslexia Differential Diagnostics68 in Maastricht.
Prominent ADHD symptoms were either a clinical diagnosis (reported by the parents), or T-scores ≥ 66.5 in the German version of the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) attention scale (69 only available for Zurich and Salzburg).
For covariance analyses that used reading as a covariate instead of categorical groups the z-score (relative to the norms) of the word fluency measure was used.
As indicated in Table 1, the group of children with dyslexia differed from the group of typical readers in reading fluency, but not in IQ, age, sex, or handedness.
Table 1.
Variables | Typical (TYP) | Dyslexia (DYS) | Intermediate | t-test TYP-DYS |
---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | p | |
Behavioural and demographic measures | ||||
Site ZRH:SBG:MAS (n) | 45:13:15 | 27:11:17 | 8:1:3 | |
Sex (male:female) (n) | 37:36 | 30:25 | 8:4 | 0.722b |
Handedness (r:l) (n) | 59:14 | 48:7 | 9:3 | 0.470b |
Age (years) | 10.05 (1.14) | 9.87 (1.33) | 10.2(1.6) | 0.372 |
School grade | 4.1 (1.4) | 4.2 (1.3) | 4.4 (1.4) | 0.444 |
Word reading (SLRT-II) fluencya | 0.4 (0.93) | − 1.8 (0.44) | − 0.99 (0.13) | < 0.001 |
Nonverbal IQ (block design WISC) | 109.4 (12.3) | 106.9 (12.64) | 115.4 (10.8) | 0.264 |
Verbal IQ (similarities WISC) | 112.9 (14.9) | 111.0 (11.5) | 107.5 (13.7) | 0.423 |
Task performance fMRI | ||||
Words accuracy (% correct) | 92.2 (8.7) | 84.5 (17.7) | 91.2 (8.8) | 0.004 |
Symbols accuracy (% correct) | 92.4 (12.6) | 89.8 (12.5) | 93.9 (9.3) | 0.249 |
Words RT (ms) | 1104 (252) | 1413 (334) | 1233 (294) | < 0.001 |
Symbols RT (ms) | 941 (214) | 1002 (205) | 918 (107) | 0.111 |
Bold values are statistically significant
az-score.
bChi square text, two-sided.
Ethical approval and guidelines
The study procedures (methods and experimental procedures) were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations as approved by the local ethical committees of the three sites (Zurich: “Kantonale Ethikkomission Zürich”; Salzburg: ethical committee of the University of Salzburg; Maastricht: research ethics committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University) and in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Children and their legal guardians gave informed consent before participating in the study.
Procedure
Either a word or a symbol string was presented every three seconds in the middle of the screen for 1200 ms followed by a fixation cross (1800 ms). The children were asked to press the left button, if the word was an animal or the symbol string was composed of identical symbols, and to press the right button, if the word was a thing, i.e., an inanimate object, or if one of the symbols in the string differed from the other symbols. There were 34 words (all nouns; half animals, half inanimate objects) and 34 symbol strings (half of the strings consisting of hatch marks, half containing a star), which were presented in a pseudo-randomized order. In order to remind the children on the task, the words “animal” and “same” were presented on the left of the screen, and the words “thing” and “different” on the right of the screen constantly throughout the presentation of stimuli and fixation cross (see Fig. 1). The assignment of left and right buttons to the target properties were counterbalanced across subjects. In order to allow a better modelling of the fMRI data 42 null events (fixation cross instead of stimulus) were added.
fMRI recording and analysis
Across the three sites, data acquisition was kept as similar as possible. The parameters of the T2*-sensitive echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequences, repetition time (TR), number of scans, and voxel size slightly differed between the three sites, to optimize recordings on the particular scanners: In Zurich, fMRI data were acquired on a 3 T GE Healthcare scanner using a T2* sensitive multi-slice echo planar imaging sequence (25 axial slices of 4.6 mm thickness and 0.4 mm gap, TR = 1499 ms, TE = 31 ms, slice resolution = 3.75 mm × 3.75 mm, 64 × 64-pixel matrix, flip angle 50°). In Salzburg, fMRI data were acquired on a 1.5 T Philips Gyroscan NT Scanner (25 axial slices of 5 mm with 0.7 mm gap, TR = 2200 ms, TE = 45 ms, slice resolution = 3.44 mm × 3.44 mm, 64 × 64-pixel matrix, flip angle 90°). In Maastricht, fMRI data were acquired on a 3 T Siemens Allegra scanner (25 axial slices of 5 mm (no gap), TR = 1500 ms, TE = 28 ms, slice resolution = 3.5 mm × 3.5 mm, 64 × 64-pixel matrix, flip angle 90°).
Image processing and statistical analyses were performed jointly for all three sites using SPM12 (v6225) (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) in Zurich. The first few scans were discarded to allow for magnetization equilibration (ZRH/MAS: 4, SBG: 3). Standard preprocessing steps were applied in the following order: Realignment and unwarping, slice time correction, coregistration and segmentation, normalization, resampling (3 × 3 × 3 mm) and smoothing (6 mm FWHM). Normalization to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space was done based on the deformations derived by segmentation and a paediatric anatomical template (mean age 9.88 years) created using the Template-OMatic toolbox70,71. Movement artefact correction was performed using the ArtRepair toolbox72. Based on the scan-to-scan (i.e. framewise) motion threshold of 1.5 mm/TR, volumes exceeding this threshold were repaired using linear interpolation between the nearest unrepaired scans. Out of the 140 analyzed data sets, 32 data sets had at least one volume exceeding the predefined motion threshold (number of repaired volumes for included datasets: mean + SD: 0.8 + 1.9 volumes/data set). Datasets for which more than 5% of the volumes had to be repaired by interpolation (18 datasets excluded out of the initial sample of n = 182) were excluded from all analyses.
A random-effect generalized linear model (GLM) was calculated for each individual, including vectors for both conditions of interest (correctly responded words and symbol strings) as well as incorrectly responded or missed words or symbol stimuli and six movement parameters as vectors of no interest.
The second level analysis focused on print sensitive processing (contrast: words – symbol strings) but the simple contrasts of words or symbols strings vs baseline are illustrated in supplementary Fig. 1 for both groups.
In order to investigate effects of reading fluency on activation patterns, two different strategies were used for our whole brain voxel-wise and the subsequent region of interest (ROI) analyses: First, word reading fluency (z-score) was used as a continuous regressor across all 140 children including dyslexic, typical and intermediate readers. Importantly, this regression approach considers the severity of the reading difficulty. To relate our results to previous group studies we also performed a secondary classical group comparison between the 55 children with dyslexia and the 73 typical readers (as defined in the methods section) using unpaired t-tests. Further we also computed regression analyses with word reading fluency (z-scores) within the group of typical and within the group of dyslexic readers.
Voxel-wise results for regression and group analyses are all illustrated on p(unc) < 0.001 (Fig. 2) but only areas surviving voxel wise p(FWEp) correction (and k ≥ 5) or cluster-extent correction (p(FWEc) < 0.05) at a cluster-defining threshold (CDT) of p(unc) < 0.005 are listed in Tables 2, 3 and Table S1 and subsequently discussed. Anatomical labels for maximal voxels in a cluster and subpeaks have been determined using XJView (v8.11) (https://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/) based on the Automated anatomical labelling (AAL) toolbox73 and are listed in the corresponding activation Tables 2, 3 and S1.
Table 2.
Typical readers (n = 73) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hemisphere | Brain area | (MNI) x, y, z | k | Peak p(FWEc) | Cluster p(FWEp) | T | Z |
Words > symbols | |||||||
L | Inferior frontal gyrus | − 53, 30, 9 | 17 | 0 | 0 | 6.93 | 6.03 |
L | Fusiform gyrus | − 35, − 42, − 21 | 45 | 0 | 0 | 6.8 | 5.94 |
R | Precentral gyrus | 46, − 15, 63 | 10 | 0.001 | 0.001 | 6.29 | 5.58 |
L | Inferior frontal gyrus | − 38, 21, 6 | 21 | 0 | 0.003 | 5.92 | 5.31 |
L | Middle frontal gyrus | − 35, 33, − 12 | 8 | 0.002 | 0.003 | 5.9 | 5.3 |
Undef | 1, − 12, 9 | 6 | 0.004 | 0.004 | 5.78 | 5.21 | |
L | Medial frontal gyrus | − 11, − 3, 60 | 5 | 0.005 | 0.006 | 5.72 | 5.16 |
R | Caudate | 22, 9, 15 | 5 | 0.005 | 0.018 | 5.38 | 4.9 |
L | Superior temporal gyrus | − 53, − 42, 6 | 5 | 0.005 | 0.022 | 5.33 | 4.86 |
Symbols > words | |||||||
None | |||||||
Dyslexia (n = 55) | |||||||
Words > symbols | |||||||
L/R | Superior/medial frontal gyrus | − 2, 3, 60 | 147 | 0 | 0 | 9.86 | 7.37 |
L | Inferior frontal gyrus | − 35, 30, 9 | 245 | 0 | 0 | 9.34 | 7.12 |
L | Undef | − 11, − 6, 24 | 5 | 0.003 | 0 | 7.44 | 6.11 |
R | Inferior frontal gyrus | 34, 24, 6 | 44 | 0 | 0 | 7.19 | 5.97 |
L | Precentral gyrus | − 53, − 9, 45 | 31 | 0 | 0.001 | 6.6 | 5.6 |
L | Cerebellum | − 11, − 45, − 33 | 15 | 0 | 0.001 | 6.53 | 5.55 |
R | Cerebellum | 34, − 54, − 30 | 12 | 0 | 0.001 | 6.39 | 5.47 |
R | Precentral gyrus | 49, − 6, 48 | 12 | 0 | 0.002 | 6.26 | 5.38 |
L | Putamen/insula | − 26, 9, 12 | 5 | 0.003 | 0.004 | 6.1 | 5.28 |
R | Caudate | 25, 24, 9 | 6 | 0.002 | 0.01 | 5.84 | 5.1 |
Symbols > words | |||||||
R | Parahippocampal gyrus | 28, − 48, − 6 | 7 | 0.001 | 0.008 | 5.89 | 5.13 |
For all contrasts the significance level at whole-brain peak-level threshold p(FWEp) < 0.05, k ≥ 5. Labels of brain regions determined using the XJView (AAL atlas); k: cluster size; R: right; L: left.
Table 3.
Regressions with reading fluency z- scores | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hemisphere | Brain area | (MNI) x, y, z | k | Peak p(FWEc) | Cluster p(FWEp) | T | Z | |
All (n = 140) | ||||||||
Words > symbols, positive regression with reading fluency z-score# | ||||||||
L | Fusiform gyrus# | − 44, − 36, − 21 | 186 | 0.021 | 0.692 | 3.89 | 3.78 | |
Words > symbols, negative regression with reading fluency z-score# | ||||||||
None# | ||||||||
Typical readers (n = 73) | ||||||||
Words > symbols, positive regression with reading fluency z-score# | ||||||||
None# | ||||||||
Words > symbols, negative regression with reading fluency z-score# | ||||||||
None# | ||||||||
Dyslexia (n = 55) | ||||||||
Words > symbols, positive regression with reading fluency z-score | ||||||||
L | Fusiform gyrus | − 38, − 57, − 15 | 8 | 0.001 | 0.001 | 6.42 | 5.47 | |
R | Fusiform gyrus/middle occipital gyrus | 40, − 69, − 12 | 29 | 0 | 0.001 | 6.4 | 5.46 | |
Words > symbols, negative regression with reading fluency z-score# | ||||||||
None# | ||||||||
Group contrasts | ||||||||
Dyslexia > typical readers | ||||||||
Words > symbols# | ||||||||
L | Precentral gyrus# | − 53, − 21, 42 | 264 | 0.001 | 0.239 | 4.42 | 4.25 | |
R | Precentral gyrus# | 61, − 6, 24 | 172 | 0.015 | 0.711 | 3.95 | 3.82 |
For all contrasts the significance level at whole-brain peak-level threshold p(FWEp) < 0.05, k ≥ 5 except for contrasts designated with# are reported on cluster-level threshold p(FWEc) < 0.05 at cluster-defining threshold at p(CDT) < 0.005. Labels of brain regions were determined using the XJView (AAL atlas); k: cluster size; R: right; L: left.
In addition, literature-based ROI analyses (spheres, radius = 4 mm) were computed for the left visual WFA (MNI: − 46, − 52, − 2) and the left LFA (MNI: − 40, − 78, − 18)23. The mean beta values of these ROIs were extracted with SPM12 and entered into linear mixed model analyses (LMM) using SAS® 9.4 (procedure PROC MIXED) including the fixed factors ROI (WFA, LFW), condition (words, symbols) and reading fluency (continuous measure or groups: dyslexia, typical readers) and random factors site (SBG, ZRH, MAS) and subject as well as the covariate age. To account for possible differences between sites, we corrected for site effects either by using effects coding (whole brain analyses)74,75 or by including site as a random factor in our LMM. For LMM analyses, studentized conditional residuals were computed to identify and exclude potential outliers and in order to correct for variance inhomogeneity. An outlier cutoff of three standard deviations from the mean was used for all analyses76. In addition, QQ-plots were inspected to ensure the assumption of normality and homoscedasticity of predicted versus conditional residual plots. All reported p-values of post hoc analyses are Tukey–Kramer corrected.
Behavioral analysis
Accuracy (Acc) and reaction time (RT) of the in-scanner task were analysed using SAS® 9.4 (Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.; https://www.sas.com/en_us/software/sas9.html) using two types of models: (1) with a within subject factor condition (words vs. symbols) and reading fluency as a covariate of interest for the whole sample (including the intermediate readers) and, (2) with a within subject factor condition (words vs. symbols) and a between subject factor group (dyslexic vs. typical readers; intermediate readers excluded). For accuracy, a beta regression model (proc glimmix) has been used to account for the distribution of the values in the standard unit interval (0, 1). For reaction time the function Proc Mixed was applied. Both models included the random intercepts for subject and site.
Results
Behavioral results
On average, accuracy in the fMRI task was very high (mean > 84% correct in all groups and both conditions, see Table 1). The more fluently a child was reading, the higher the accuracy was in the fMRI task (reading fluency, F(1,124) = 9.73, p < 0.0022), irrespective of condition (reading fluency x condition, F(1,124) = 0.27, p = 0.6075). Symbol strings were responded to more accurately (Accuracy: condition, F(1,124) = 18.31, p < 0.0001) and faster (RT: condition, F(1,129) = 230.33; p < 0.0001) than words. This condition-sensitive slowing was more pronounced, the less fluent a child was reading (reading fluency x condition, F(1,129) = 44.35, p < 0.0001). This interaction modulated the main effect of reading fluency on RT (reading fluency, F(1,129) = 17.24, p < 0.0001).
These behavioural effects were also reflected in the groupwise analysis: At the generally high performance level, children with typical reading skills were more accurate than children with dyslexia (group, F(1,112) = 4.75, p = 0.0314), but irrespective of condition (group x condition, F(1,112) = 0.27, p = 0.6033). Accuracy was higher for symbol strings than words (condition, F(1,112) = 22.14, p < 0.0001) and the children, responded more slowly to words than to symbol strings (condition, F(1,118) = 379.99; p < 0.001). This reaction time difference was more pronounced in the children with dyslexia than in the typical reading children resulting in an interaction (group × condition, F(1,118) = 46.54, p < 0.0001) that also modulated the main effect of group (F(1,118) = 14.34, p < 0.0002). Post-hoc t-tests showed that children with dyslexia had slower reaction times (p < 0.001) and lower accuracies (p = 0.004) for words than typical reading children but the groups did not differ in their performance on symbol strings.
fMRI whole brain results
Print-sensitive activation
Both children with typical reading skills and children with dyslexia showed word-sensitive activation (words > symbol strings) in left inferior and medial frontal regions and parts of the basal ganglia (p(FWEp) < 0.05, Fig. 2, Table 2). Word-sensitive activation in the left fusiform and superior temporal gyri was only found for the typical readers and not the children with dyslexia.
The contrasts words vs baseline or symbols vs baseline showed activation in both groups in bilateral ventral occipito-temporal regions and in left precentral regions. Words additionally showed bilateral middle and inferior frontal activation. For a detailed description of activated cluster per group and condition see supplementary Fig. 1.
Regression of print-sensitive processing with reading fluency
Only the left fusiform gyrus showed a significant relation of word-sensitive activation with reading ability, in the form of increased functional activation with increased reading performance (Table 3, Fig. 2; p(FWEc) < 0.05, cluster size-corrected at CDT p(unc) < 0.005). Interestingly, separate regression analyses by group for the a) typical readers and b) the children with dyslexia showed an increase in the functional activation of bilateral fusiform gyri (FFG) with reading fluency (p(FWEc) < 0.05) in the children with dyslexia only.
No significant (p(FWEc) < 0.05) negative correlation with reading fluency and print-sensitive activation was found neither for the whole sample, nor for any of the two reading groups (Table 3).
Group effects on print sensitive activation
The group of typical readers did not show any region with increased word-sensitive activation compared to the children with dyslexia surviving a cluster extent (or voxel-wise) correction. On the contrary, bilateral precentral gyri showed enhanced activation for print-sensitive activation in children with dyslexia compared to typical readers (Table 3, Fig. 3; p(FWEc) < 0.05, cluster size-corrected at CDT p(unc) < 0.005).
Left vOT region of interest (ROI) analyses
Impact of reading fluency on print-sensitive processing in the left vOT
When using word reading fluency score as a covariate of interest for the whole sample of 140 children, a trend for the triple interaction of reading fluency, ROI (WFA, LFA) and condition (Words, Symbols) (F(1,411) = 3.53, p = 0.0610), a significant interaction of reading fluency and condition (F(1,411) = 6.66, p = 0.0102) and a trend for ROI x condition (F(1,411) = 3.1, p = 0.0790) was found. In addition, main effects of ROI (F(1,411) = 40.42, p = 0.0001) and condition (F(1,411) = 12.22, p = 0.0005) were also significant. The significant regression of print-sensitive activation with reading fluency in the left WFA and the nonsignificant regression with left LFA ROIs are illustrated in Fig. 4, for supplementary site-wise analyses, please refer to the supplementary analysis section.
Because of the significant association of reading fluency and print sensitive processing in left VOT in children with dyslexia but not in typical reading children as revealed in the whole brain analyses we repeated the same model as described above separately for the group of typical readers and for children with dyslexia. Both groups showed a significant (typical) or trend (dyslexia) main effect of ROI (typical readers: (F(1,211) = 21.85, p = 0.0001; dyslexia (F(1,157) = 3.78, p = 0.0535)) and an interaction of condition and reading fluency (typical readers: (F(1,211) = 6.70, p = 0.0103; dyslexia (F(1,157) = 18.11, p < 0.0001)). The condition main effect was only significant in children with dyslexia (typical readers: (F(1,211) = 0.99, p = 0.3197; dyslexia (F(1,157) = 19.97, p < 0.0001)), while only typical readers showed an interaction of ROI and condition (typical readers: (F(1,211) = 4.23, p = 0.0408; dyslexia (F(1,157) = 2.10, p = 0.1495)), indicating a more focal print sensitivity effect in the classic WFA in typical readers as compared to children with dyslexia.
Group differences in print-sensitive vOT activation
Activation in the vOT ROIs corrected for age and site was stronger for words than for symbol strings (condition, F(1,374) = 42.6, p < 0.001) and overall activation in the LFA was stronger than activation in the WFA (ROI, F(1,374) = 8.2, p = 0.0044). The word-sensitive activation was stronger in typical readers than in children with dyslexia in the WFA but not in the LFA (group x condition x ROI, F(1,374) = 4.66, p = 0.0316, c.f. Fig. 5). The group main effect was not significant (group, F(1,374) = 0.52, p = 0.472). Posthoc t-tests showed that only the typical readers had a more pronounced BOLD signal to words than symbol strings (print sensitive processing) in the WFA (t = 3.37, p = 0.0189) and more activation to symbol strings in the LFA than the WFA (t = − 5.63, p = < 0.001). Children with dyslexia showed more pronounced activation in the LFA than WFA to words (t = − 3.20, p = 0.0323) c.f. Fig. 5. Two additional categorical analyses included also the small group of intermediate reading children (n = 12) or an extended intermediate reader group (n = 29) in the models. These LMMs largely confirmed the results of the main model with the two core groups and are added in the supplementary material.
Discussion
Neuroimaging studies and a growing number of meta-analyses in the past years consistently reported alterations in the microstructure and in activation of the left ventral occipito-temporal cortex during reading-related tasks in poorly reading children and adults as compared with typical readers41–43,64,77,78. With this multicentre study we aimed to confirm the findings of a hypoactive vOT system in poor readers in an exceptionally large sample (n = 140) of children spanning the whole spectrum of reading skills from very poor (dyslexic) to highly fluent readers. Specifically, we examined functional activation differences with a reading for meaning and a symbol string comparison task to clarify how print-sensitive processing in the left vOT is related to reading fluency. We took advantage of our large sample to conduct both continuous and categorical (group comparison) analyses. The results of the whole brain and the ROI analyses revealed two key findings: First, a nonlinear association of print-sensitive activation in left vOT with reading scores, which is especially pronounced in the poor reading range but less in the typical reading range. And second, a more focal print-sensitive activation in the WFA in children performing within the typical reading range as compared with poorly reading children.
Continuous whole-brain analyses in the full sample revealed a positive association of print-sensitive activation with reading fluency in only one distinct cluster over the whole brain located in the centre of the visual word form system of the left midfusiform gyrus: the poorer a child was reading, the lower was his/her print-sensitive activation in this region. This is in agreement with many previous studies showing the critical role of this region for reading and dyslexia (see for example meta-analyses41–43).
The current findings, however, extend those from previous studies by showing that the relation between left vOT activation and reading skills is neither linear across the entire range of reading nor dependent on classical thresholds for impaired and unimpaired reading. Despite the large sample of typical readers and their large variation in reading scores, no relation of reading fluency to activation in the left vOT survived activation thresholds in the voxel-wise analysis in this group. Children with dyslexia, however, showed a strong positive association between their reading fluency and print-sensitive processing in extended and bilateral regions including the fusiform gyri. This suggests that word-sensitive activation decreases mainly among the children with dyslexia with increasing severity of their reading difficulties, but not among the typical readers. Importantly the absence of this brain-behaviour association cannot be explained by reduced variation in the reading fluency of typical readers.
Our group wise analyses provided more details to explain this finding. First, no print-sensitive activation in the fusiform gyri was detected in the group of children with dyslexia. Together with the findings of a very strong positive association with reading skills, this result thus suggests a very high variability in the print-sensitive response of the vOT in the group of poor readers that is partly explained by severity. Second, children with typical reading skills showed strong print-sensitive activation in the left fusiform gyrus. Together with the absent modulation of print-sensitive activation through reading skills, our data indicate a minor impact of reading fluency on print-sensitive activation in the typical reading range. Moreover, the direct group contrast of print-sensitive processing, showed only a stronger involvement of bilateral precentral gyri in children with dyslexia but did not yield any differences in the activation of the vOT. These results indicate that a certain basic level of print-sensitive processing in the left vOT is a prerequisite for reading skills in the average performance range. And while the individual variability of reading fluency in the typical range in school children seems to be only weakly related to the level of print-sensitive vOT activity, the variability in reading performance in the very poor performance range shows a strong and linear association with the vOT sensitivity to print.
Our findings extend the results of two recent studies comparing the print-sensitivity effect in younger children30 or print, symbol, falsefont and digit string processing in somewhat older (~ 11.5 years) children79. While no group difference was detected for the print-sensitivity effects in these studies, clear and unspecific hypoactivation in the left vOT was reported for processing words, symbol and digit strings30,79 suggesting a more general visual object processing failure in children with dyslexia. In contrast to these studies the activation to symbol strings did not differ between groups in our study as shown in the ROI analyses of the WFA and LFA. Instead, a distinct reduction of the print sensitivity effect in the WFA in children with dyslexia thus suggested a more focal modulation of print-sensitive processing in the WFA of typical readers. While a failure to detect group effects in the left vOT in many previous studies could have been due to the rather small group sizes, or the often rather lenient criteria to categorize children with dyslexia this is unlikely the case for the current study which included more than 50 children per group and only severely impaired children formed the dyslexia group. More likely, based on our results, is the assumption that children with more severe forms of reading disability show a larger failure in print-sensitive processing in the left vOT which shows up also in simple tasks, while children close to average reading scores hardly differ anymore in the functional activation of this system during simple implicit reading and semantic judgment tasks. This is also corroborated by the performance data in our fMRI tasks. Even though typical readers performed more accurately and faster, the overall performance in this task was very good also in the group of children with dyslexia. While it was our aim, to implement a task that can be done by all children, more challenging tasks, may have resulted in more pronounced group differences also regarding the activation of the VWFA.
Previous studies showed that, deficient VWFA function may be explained by a basic deficit in learning grapheme-phoneme correspondences in children with dyslexia80,81. Deficient activation may become more accentuated also in poor and intermediate readers when specifically challenging the system with tasks that emphasize phonological processing22,45,52–56,62,63,82. How much the left vOT is engaged in visual word processing may thus not only depend on the reading skill level of a child, but could also be influenced by differential deployment of top-down recruitment with task requirements9. Like in other studies, applying region of interest analyses to specifically examine the activation pattern of the visual word form area yielded focal print-sensitive activation differences between groups. Connectivity studies show, that the VWFA development13,29, and its function are largely constrained and predicted by its privileged connectivity to various areas within the (temporal) language network but also its tight coupling to the dorsal fronto-parietal attention network51,83,84. Such connectivity findings suggest that the VWFA activation is tuned by input from higher order language areas and attentional processes, depending also on the task requirements, the reading strategy and experience of the children. The extended connectivity of the left vOT thus seems a prerequisite for guiding the emerging specialization in the VWFA13 which is initiated within the first months of formal reading acquisition through the learning of letter and speech sound associations in alphabetic languages26,27,29,33,85,86. In addition the strong structural and functional link of the VWFA to the attentional system51,87,88 may support the guidance of visual attention by amplifying the representations of words and fostering subsequent phonological and lexical processing51. Impairments in attentional mechanisms in dyslexia may cause poor orthographic representations89 and phonological decoding deficits90. Importantly targeting these core systems through the application of phonics training enhancing grapheme-phoneme correspondences91,92 or also attentional training with action video games93,94 and reading acceleration95 may support poor readers.
The strong group difference of increased (rather than decreased) print-sensitive activation in children with dyslexia in left and right precentral regions is well in line with previous findings. Such overactivation has been reported consistently across studies [e.g.30,96] and meta-analyses41,42. A recent meta-analysis specifically addressing word processing in shallow and deep orthographies indicated that this overactivation is especially prominent in shallow orthographies44. Accordingly, the hyperactivation observed in our sample of children with reading difficulties may have been especially pronounced because of using simple and short words in our task, which can be read easily and automatically by typical but not by children with reading difficulties. The precentral regions are known to be involved in the articulatory network97, speech production98 and silent articulation99. An overlap of frontal regions usually showing hyperactivation in individuals with reading disorder with regions supporting articulation has been demonstrated in a recent quantitative meta-analysis and thereby supports the notion that such hyperactivation reflects compensatory processing to access the meaning of print96. Thus, poor reading children may use these regions to support reading by employing letter-by-letter decoding or covert articulation processes30. As an alternative explanation the hyperactivation in the precentral gyri could also reflect greater difficulties with and increased resources necessary for covert articulation or general processing of words in children with dyslexia. In either case these explanations converge with less efficient processing as indicated by slower reaction times to semantic decisions on words (and not symbols strings) in our task.
In order to achieve a large sample size, we collected data across three different sites, including two different languages with similar semi-transparent100,101 orthographic complexity (Dutch and German). This merging of three different groups may be seen as a limitation of this study. To address this potential limitation, our linear mixed models included site and age as covariates to regress out potential confounds. This suggests that the critical effects in this study were not affected by the different conditions across the three sites or by the differences in age among the children, and thus generalize across involved languages and educational systems. Further one needs to keep in mind, that the present study design does not allow to finally conclude on whether the hypoactivation of the VWFA and the hyperactivation in the precentral gyri to print reflect specific dyslexia effects or whether these differences may rather be explained by differences in print exposure or reading level between children with dyslexia and typical reading skills. However, a study comparing adolescents with dyslexia to age- and reading-level matched control groups, suggested that the hypoactivation in the left parietal and fusiform gyri indeed reflect atypical brain function in dyslexic individuals, while the frontal hyperactivation is more likely related to the current reading skills independent of dyslexia102.
Taken together, this large-scale neuroimaging study on the influence of word reading fluency on brain activation emphasizes the important role of the left vOT for reading skills and highlights its functional impairment in poor reading children. It extends the insight of previous large-scale studies specifically challenging phonological processing and using more liberal criteria for defining dyslexia by focussing on reading for meaning. Thus, the present study represents one of the largest datasets (n = 140) on functional activation in children with severe dyslexia compared to typical readers and clearly corroborates previous notions that the print-sensitive activation in the left vOT represents a critical limiting factor of reading for meaning especially at the lower end of the reading fluency spectrum. Given that the most robust underactivation in children with dyslexia occurred in this region, the left vOT function appears to be a prime target for supportive interventions in those needing it most.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
Supported by the EU FP6 project NeuroDys, the Swiss National Science Foundation (32-108130), and the Austrian Science Fund (18832-B02 and 18351-B02).
Author contributions
S.B. and U.M. performed statistical analyses and visualizations; S.B. preprocessed fMRI data; S.B., U.M. and D.B. wrote the manuscript; U.M., M.K., M.S., F.R., V.B.,. J.R., S.v.M., E.S., K.B., K.M. collected the data; K.L., E.M., R.G., G.S., H.W., D.B., L.B. were responsible for study design, provided project infrastructure and critical feedback. All authors (except for L.B., who unfortunately passed away) critically read and commented on the manuscript.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, due to the restricted and site-specific consent of research participants. Data are however available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of the principal investigator of each site.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: S. Brem and U. Maurer.
L. Blomert is deceased.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-020-75111-8.
References
- 1.Peters L, Ansari D. Are specific learning disorders truly specific, and are they disorders? Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2019;17:100115. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2019.100115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Escobar MD. Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Results of the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. JAMA. 1990;264:998–1002. doi: 10.1001/jama.1990.03450080084036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bradley L, Bryant PE. Difficulties in auditory organisation as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature. 1978;271:746–747. doi: 10.1038/271746a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Baker CI, et al. Visual word processing and experiential origins of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:9087–9092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703300104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Centanni TM, et al. Early development of letter specialization in left fusiform is associated with better word reading and smaller fusiform face area. Dev. Sci. 2018;21:e12658–e12658. doi: 10.1111/desc.12658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Coch D, Meade G. N1 and P2 to words and wordlike stimuli in late elementary school children and adults. Psychophysiology. 2016;53:115–128. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cohen L, et al. The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain. 2000;123(Pt 2):291–307. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cohen L, et al. Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain. 2002;125:1054–1069. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Dehaene S, Cohen L. The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Glezer LS, Jiang X, Riesenhuber M. Evidence for highly selective neuronal tuning to whole words in the "visual word form area". Neuron. 2009;62:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:293–299. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Price CJ, Moore CJ, Frackowiak RS. The effect of varying stimulus rate and duration on brain activity during reading. Neuroimage. 1996;3:40–52. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Saygin ZM, et al. Connectivity precedes function in the development of the visual word form area. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:1250–1255. doi: 10.1038/nn.4354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Vinckier F, et al. Hierarchical coding of letter strings in the ventral stream: Dissecting the inner organization of the visual word-form system. Neuron. 2007;55:143–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Turkeltaub PE, Gareau L, Flowers DL, Zeffiro TA, Eden GF. Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:767–773. doi: 10.1038/nn1065nn1065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Price CJ, Devlin JT. The myth of the visual word form area. Neuroimage. 2003;19:473–481. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00084-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Vogel AC, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The VWFA: It's not just for words anymore. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:88–88. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Brem S, et al. Evidence for developmental changes in the visual word processing network beyond adolescence. Neuroimage. 2006;29:822–837. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mano QR, et al. The role of left occipitotemporal cortex in reading: Reconciling stimulus, task, and lexicality effects. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:988–1001. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kronschnabel J, Schmid R, Maurer U, Brandeis D. Visual print tuning deficits in dyslexic adolescents under minimized phonological demands. Neuroimage. 2013;74:58–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Vogel AC, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The left occipitotemporal cortex does not show preferential activity for words. Cereb. Cortex. 2012;22:2715–2732. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.van der Mark S, et al. Children with dyslexia lack multiple specializations along the visual word-form (VWF) system. Neuroimage. 2009;47:1940–1949. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Thesen T, et al. Sequential then interactive processing of letters and words in the left fusiform gyrus. Nat. Commun. 2012;3:1284. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lerma-Usabiaga G, Carreiras M, Paz-Alonso PM. Converging evidence for functional and structural segregation within the left ventral occipitotemporal cortex in reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2018;115:E9981–E9990. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1803003115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bouhali F, Bézagu Z, Dehaene S, Cohen L. A mesial-to-lateral dissociation for orthographic processing in the visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2019;116:21936–21946. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1904184116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brem S, et al. Brain sensitivity to print emerges when children learn letter-speech sound correspondences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:7939–7944. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904402107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Maurer U, et al. Coarse neural tuning for print peaks when children learn to read. Neuroimage. 2006;33:749–758. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Ben-Shachar M, Dougherty RF, Deutsch GK, Wandell BA. The development of cortical sensitivity to visual word forms. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23:2387–2399. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21615. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dehaene-Lambertz G, Monzalvo K, Dehaene S. The emergence of the visual word form: Longitudinal evolution of category-specific ventral visual areas during reading acquisition. PLoS Biol. 2018;16:e2004103. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chyl K, et al. Prereader to beginning reader: Changes induced by reading acquisition in print and speech brain networks. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2018;59:76–87. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dehaene S, Cohen L, Sigman M, Vinckier F. The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005;9:335–341. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Zhao J, et al. Fine neural tuning for orthographic properties of words emerges early in children reading alphabetic script. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;26:2431–2442. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pleisch G, et al. Simultaneous EEG and fMRI reveals stronger sensitivity to orthographic strings in the left occipito-temporal cortex of typical versus poor beginning readers. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;40:100717. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pleisch G, et al. Emerging neural specialization of the ventral occipitotemporal cortex to characters through phonological association learning in preschool children. Neuroimage. 2019;189:813–831. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Xue G, Chen C, Jin Z, Dong Q. Language experience shapes fusiform activation when processing a logographic artificial language: An fMRI training study. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1315–1326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Martin L, et al. The VWFA is the home of orthographic learning when houses are used as letters. eneuro. 2019 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0425-17.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Li J, Osher DE, Hansen HA, Saygin ZM. Cortical selectivity driven by connectivity: Innate connectivity patterns of the visual word form area. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/712455. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Glezer LS, Riesenhuber M. Individual variability in location impacts orthographic selectivity in the "visual word form area". J. Neurosci. 2013;33:11221–11226. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5002-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bruno JL, Zumberge A, Manis FR, Lu ZL, Goldman JG. Sensitivity to orthographic familiarity in the occipito-temporal region. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1988–2001. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sandak R, Mencl WE, Frost SJ, Pugh KR. The neurobiological basis of skilled and impaired reading: Recent findings and new directions. Sci. Stud. Read. 2004;8:273–292. doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0803_6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:3299–3308. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1735–1742. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Maisog JM, Einbinder ER, Flowers DL, Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF. A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1145:237–259. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Martin A, Kronbichler M, Richlan F. Dyslexic brain activation abnormalities in deep and shallow orthographies: A meta-analysis of 28 functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2016;37:2676–2699. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Paulesu E, et al. Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science. 2001;291:2165–2167. doi: 10.1126/science.1057179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Siok WT, Perfetti CA, Jin Z, Tan LH. Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature. 2004;431:71–76. doi: 10.1038/nature02865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pflugshaupt T, et al. About the role of visual field defects in pure alexia. Brain. 2009;132:1907–1917. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hirshorn EA, et al. Decoding and disrupting left midfusiform gyrus activity during word reading. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2016;113:8162–8167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1604126113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Dehaene S, Cohen L, Morais J, Kolinsky R. Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16:234–244. doi: 10.1038/nrn3924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Price CJ, Devlin JT. The interactive account of ventral occipitotemporal contributions to reading. Trends Cogn. Sci.. 2011;15:246–253. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Chen L, et al. The visual word form area (VWFA) is part of both language and attention circuitry. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:5601. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13634-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Brambati SM, et al. Neuropsychological deficits and neural dysfunction in familial dyslexia. Brain Res. 2006;1113:174–185. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Richlan F, et al. A common left occipito-temporal dysfunction in developmental dyslexia and acquired letter-by-letter reading? PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e12073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Wimmer H, et al. A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: An fMRI study. Cortex. 2010;46:1284–1298. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Shaywitz BA, et al. Disruption of posterior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2002;52:101–110. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01365-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cao F, Bitan T, Chou TL, Burman DD, Booth JR. Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2006;47:1041–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Meyler A, et al. Brain activation during sentence comprehension among good and poor readers. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:2780–2787. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kronbichler M, et al. Evidence for a dysfunction of left posterior reading areas in German dyslexic readers. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:1822–1832. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Schulz E, et al. Impaired semantic processing during sentence reading in children with dyslexia: Combined fMRI and ERP evidence. Neuroimage. 2008;41:153–168. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Maurer U, et al. The development of print tuning in children with dyslexia: Evidence from longitudinal ERP data supported by fMRI. Neuroimage. 2010 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.McCrory EJ, Mechelli A, Frith U, Price CJ. More than words: A common neural basis for reading and naming deficits in developmental dyslexia? Brain. 2005;128:261–267. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Tanaka H, et al. The brain basis of the phonological deficit in dyslexia is independent of IQ. Psychol. Sci. 2011;22:1442–1451. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Shaywitz BA, et al. Age-related changes in reading systems of dyslexic children. Ann. Neurol. 2007;61:363–370. doi: 10.1002/ana.21093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Jednorog K, et al. How reliable are gray matter disruptions in specific reading disability across multiple countries and languages? insights from a large-scale voxel-based morphometry study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015 doi: 10.1002/hbm.22734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Bach S, Richardson U, Brandeis D, Martin E, Brem S. Print-specific multimodal brain activation in kindergarten improves prediction of reading skills in second grade. Neuroimage. 2013;82:605–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Tewes U, Rossmann P, Schallberger UH. HAWIK-III Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Kinder [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III)-German Version] Edison: Huber; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Moll K, Landerl K. SLRT-II: Lese-und Rechtschreibtest. Bern: Verlag Hans Huber; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 68.68Blomert, L. & Vaessen, A. Differentiaal Diagnostiek van Dyslexie: Cognitieve analyse van lezen en spellen [Dyslexia Differential Diagnosis: Cognitive analysis of reading and spelling]. (Boom Test Publishers, 2009).
- 69.Achenbach, T. M. & Edelbrock, C. S. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. (Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, 1983)
- 70.Abdullaev YG, Posner MI. Event-related brain potential imaging of semantic encoding during processing single words. Neuroimage. 1998;7:1–13. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Wilke M, Holland SK, Altaye M, Gaser C. Template-O-Matic: A toolbox for creating customized pediatric templates. Neuroimage. 2008;41:903–913. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Mazaika, P., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Reiss, A. & Glover, G. In Organization of Human Brain Mapping International Conference.
- 73.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Pedhazur, E. J. Multiple regression in behavioral research: explanation and prediction (2nd edition). (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, 1982).
- 75.Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. S. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. (Lawrence Erlbaum Ass., 2003).
- 76.Roth RM, et al. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007;62:901–909. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Linkersdorfer J, Lonnemann J, Lindberg S, Hasselhorn M, Fiebach CJ. Grey matter alterations co-localize with functional abnormalities in developmental dyslexia: An ALE meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e43122. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Structural abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Hum. Brain. Mapp. 2013;34:3055–3065. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Boros M, et al. Orthographic processing deficits in developmental dyslexia: Beyond the ventral visual stream. Neuroimage. 2016;128:316–327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Blomert L. The neural signature of orthographic-phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. Neuroimage. 2011;57:695–703. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fraga Gonzalez G, et al. A randomized controlled trial on the beneficial effects of training letter-speech sound integration on reading fluency in children with dyslexia. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0143914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0143914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hoeft F, et al. Neural basis of dyslexia: A comparison between dyslexic and nondyslexic children equated for reading ability. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:10700–10708. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4931-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Stevens WD, Kravitz DJ, Peng CS, Henry Tessler M, Martin A. Privileged functional connectivity between the visual word form area and the language system. J. Neurosci. 2017 doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0138-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Vogel AC, Miezin F, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The putative visual word form area is functionally connected to the dorsal attention network. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;22:537–549. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Karipidis II, et al. Neural initialization of audiovisual integration in prereaders at varying risk for developmental dyslexia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38:1038–1055. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.van de Walle de Ghelcke A, Rossion B, Schiltz C, Lochy A. Developmental changes in neural letter-selectivity: A 1-year follow-up of beginning readers. Dev. Sci. 2020 doi: 10.1111/desc.12999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Vogel AC, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL. The putative visual word form area is functionally connected to the dorsal attention network. Cereb. Cortex. 2012;22:537–549. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Chen C, et al. Sex determines the neurofunctional predictors of visual word learning. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:741–747. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Vidyasagar TR, Pammer K. Dyslexia: A deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010;14:57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Grainger J, Dufau S, Ziegler JC. A vision of reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Galuschka K, Ise E, Krick K, Schulte-Korne G. Effectiveness of treatment approaches for children and adolescents with reading disabilities: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e89900. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Mehringer H, et al. (Swiss) GraphoLearn: An app-based tool to support beginning readers. Res. Pract. Technol. Enhanc. Learn. 2020;15:5. doi: 10.1186/s41039-020-0125-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Franceschini S, et al. Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Franceschini S, et al. Action video games improve reading abilities and visual-to-auditory attentional shifting in English-speaking children with dyslexia. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:5863. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05826-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Breznitz Z, et al. Enhanced reading by training with imposed time constraint in typical and dyslexic adults. Nat. Commun. 2013 doi: 10.1038/ncomms2488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Hancock R, Richlan F, Hoeft F. Possible roles for fronto-striatal circuits in reading disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;72:243–260. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Hickok G, Poeppel D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:393–402. doi: 10.1038/nrn2113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Brown S, Ngan E, Liotti M. A larynx area in the human motor cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:837–845. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Richlan F. Functional neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: The role of orthographic depth. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:347. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Landerl K, et al. Predictors of developmental dyslexia in European orthographies with varying complexity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2013;54:686–694. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Seymour PH, Aro M, Erskine JM. Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 2003;94:143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Hoeft F, et al. Functional and morphometric brain dissociation between dyslexia and reading ability. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:4234–4239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609399104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rorden C, Karnath HO, Bonilha L. Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007;19:1081–1088. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, due to the restricted and site-specific consent of research participants. Data are however available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of the principal investigator of each site.