Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2020 Nov 3;15(11):e0241465. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241465

What triggers online help-seeking retransmission during the COVID-19 period? Empirical evidence from Chinese social media

Chen Luo 1,2, Yuru Li 3,*, Anfan Chen 4,*, Yulong Tang 5
Editor: Valerio Capraro6
PMCID: PMC7608884  PMID: 33141860

Abstract

The past nine months witnessed COVID-19's fast-spreading at the global level. Limited by medical resources shortage and uneven facilities distribution, online help-seeking becomes an essential approach to cope with public health emergencies for many ordinaries. This study explores the driving forces behind the retransmission of online help-seeking posts. We built an analytical framework that emphasized content characteristics, including information completeness, proximity, support seeking type, disease severity, and emotion of help-seeking messages. A quantitative content analysis was conducted with a probability sample consisting of 727 posts. The results illustrate the importance of individual information completeness, high proximity, instrumental support seeking. This study also demonstrates slight inconformity with the severity principle but stresses the power of anger in help-seeking messages dissemination. As one of the first online help-seeking diffusion analyses in the COVID-19 period, our research provides a reference for constructing compelling and effective help-seeking posts during a particular period. It also reveals further possibilities for harnessing social media’s power to promote reciprocal and cooperative actions as a response to this deepening global concern.

Introduction

According to the World Health Organization (WHO), COVID-19 has resulted in more than 14 million confirmed cases and nearly 0.6 million deaths worldwide by July 20th, 2020 [1]. Due to the destructive power and highly infectious feature of this new coronavirus, many countries have taken various kinds of measures to prevent virus transmission. Meanwhile, people use social media to acquire and exchange multiple types of information at a historic and unprecedented scale [2]. So far, some researches have concentrated on the effects of social media in this particular period. For example, harness the social media posts to predict infected case counts and inform timely responses under the infoveillance or infodemiology framework [3,4]; analyze the help-seeking posts to identify the characteristics of COVID-19 patients [5]; discuss the online censorship on social media from the risk communication perspective [6]. Among all the research, online help-seeking, as a practical approach to handle difficulties during public health emergencies, has a unique research significance. Firstly, when facing scarce medical resources during the initial outbreak stage, many suspected and confirmed COVID-19 patients were failing to be admitted to a hospital, many of them turned to social media fork help (e.g., sickbeds, testing kits, emotional comforts) and share information instantly [5]. Secondly, online help-seeking helps relevant actors to ease anxiety, build mutual aid networks, and make the most of the collective power to cope with risks [7,8]. Social support or so-called supportive communication in computer-mediated communication contexts has always been a hotly discussed issue [9]. COVID-19, as an epidemic affecting the whole world, provides a valuable chance to explore the mechanisms and effects of online help-seeking. Its practical and theoretical implications not only offer an insight into understanding help-seeking and promote problem-solving during a public health crisis but also enrich the strategies on how to develop compelling online help-seeking posts.

As Van Bavel et al. [10] suggested, social and behavioral science can be a beneficial framework to support the COVID-19 pandemic response. Online help-seeking and help-providing are typical reactions toward pandemic, encourage social connection to buffer the negative feelings of social isolation in the shelter-in-place period [10]. This study aims to explore help-seeking information diffusion on Chinese social media. As Wang et al. suggest, probing into information diffusion is the basis for understanding and managing the dynamics of content on social media [11]. Generally speaking, retweeting help-seeking posts can be interpreted as a kind of prosocial behavior, which means retweeters intended to help others by joining the retransmission process [12,13]. Meanwhile, online retweeting behavior is driven by a dual-process [14]. It is not only impelled by logical cues (e.g., deliberation based on facts) but also affected by intuitive cues (e.g., emotions) [10,14]. Previous studies provided limited insights on evaluating help-seeking information diffusion during health emergencies. Many studies followed the "sender-content" framework [15] but failed to develop a comprehensive content measurement for health crisis communication. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first online help-seeking diffusion analysis during the COVID-19 period. We intended to combine logical cues and intuitive cues to replenish content measurement, makes it more plausible and suitable to the current situation. Our findings will provide a reference for social media help-seeking in a similar period in the future.

Literature review and research hypotheses

Retransmission of help-seeking posts and content characteristics

With the rapid development of social media, new possibilities of seeking help online keep emerging. More and more people are turning to cyberspace to seek specific social support in various domains like coping with daily hassles and battling a life-threatening illness [16]. Social network sites like Twitter [17], Facebook [18], Instagram [19] or online forums [2022] increase the opportunities of receiving and providing social support from all sides. Retransmission, or the so-called retweeting, has always been seen as a crucial indicator of information diffusion in social media platforms [23]. Some studies treated retransmission as a judgment of the effectiveness of communication [11,24]. The retweeting behavior has been demonstrated as a conversational practice on social media platform [25]; a way to seek personal benefits from the social network [26]; a kind of prosocial behavior aims to offer help or provide advice to others driven by altruistic and reciprocal motivations [13]. Retransmission is critical in online help-seeking, especially in the epidemic context for two reasons. Firstly, retweeting is a typical one-to-many communication describing the degree of viral reach on social media [24]. More retransmission means more users receiving the message, thus increasing the chance of getting help. Secondly, from a practical point of view, facing the medical resources shortage and uneven distribution of medical facilities, widely disseminated help-seeking posts played the role of social warning, assisted official institutions in understanding urgent affairs as well as allocating supplies more effectively.

What are the driving factors behind online help-seeking messages retransmission? Most existing studies categorized driving forces into two dimensions: the sender factors and the content factors [11,23,27]. However, significant driving forces are varied by context. In this study, ordinary users are actively adopting the Weibo platform to express their needs and compete for public attention. Other types of users, including celebrities and organizations, are rarely asking for help online. In short, the main component of online help seekers is ordinaries, and their online identities are nearly the same. Thus, it would be more reasonable and significant to explore further in the content factor. There is plenty of research providing conceptual references, such as the depth of self-disclosure [16,28], different types of support messages [16,29], physical and emotional proximity to the target [30], the social capital stock of the help seeker [22]. Enlightened by existing experience, we will summarize the impelling factors of help-seeking information diffusion into five dimensions: completeness, proximity, support typology, disease severity, emotion, and elaborate them in the following sections. Under the dual-process perspective, the former four determinants stay close with the slow and deliberative process while the emotion is akin to the fast and intuitive cognitive process. In other words, retweeting behavior is propelled by "relying on reason" and "relying on emotion" simultaneously [14,31].

Completeness

Completeness is bound up with credibility. Credibility has always been seen as perceived quality and as a result of intertwined dimensions [32,33]. Previous studies usually concentrated on source credibility and stressed the significant influence of source credibility perception on retweeting behavior in sports news [34] and health information diffusion [35], the importance of content credibility was overlooked to some extent. In brief, content with high credibility should be internally consistent and clearly presented. Many scholars have elucidated the connotation of content credibility, including complete, in-depth, precise, reliable, accurate, unbiased, objective, factual, and fair [3638]. In a systematic literature review, Sbaffi and Rowley [39] listed dozens of content features influencing trust judgments and credibility perception, which means credibility is a typical compound concept made up of various subdimensions [40]. As a result, it is impossible to investigate all elements of credibility in one study. The proper approach is to pick out the critical factor based on the specific research context.

Completeness is a pivotal part of content credibility, especially in online help-seeking during COVID-19. As the name suggests, it means how much information does an individual disclose and the clarity of the provided information [41]. Completeness contributes to the perception of health information quality on the Internet [42]. Firstly, more complete information means more additional information, which increases the "real world feel" [43]. More detailed information implies more communication cues, improves the social presence of communicators in nonverbally environments [44]. Secondly, completeness is directly bound up with the amount and depth of self-disclosure. Altman and Taylor [28] argued that breadth is the amount of disclosed information, while depth is the intimacy of disclosed information. A complete self-disclosure can effectively convey individuals' needs and reveal who they are, also benefits the likelihood of receiving social support [45]. In this study, we extrapolate that online help seekers' willingness to divulge their personal information helps the audience comprehend their identities. Besides, a complete expression of disease development or health condition usually demonstrates the seriousness of the current problem, improves credibility perception, and eventually leads to successful support provision. We propose the following:

  • H1: Completeness of individual information in online help-seeking posts is positively associated with retransmission.

  • H2: Completeness of disease status in online help-seeking posts is positively associated with retransmission.

Proximity

Proximity can be interpreted as a form of perceived psychological distance bound up with the construal level theory (CLT) proposed by Trope and Liberman [46]. The psychological distance is defined on spatial, temporal, social, and hypothetical dimensions—those distances further influence how people think about an object or event [46,47]. Substantial studies have scrutinized the impacts of proximity on an individual's responses to others' misfortune, and the main mechanisms can be elaborated in two aspects. Firstly, high proximity implies low perceived distance, which increases empathy and compassion toward the victim, while low proximity weakens it [48,49]. Secondly, proximity links to the perceived trustworthiness of specific information or source. Generally speaking, people often conceive a proximal source as more credible than a distant one [4,35]. Driven by those two threads, when engaged in online activities, high proximity promotes information sharing [50] and information adoption [51]. The work of Huang et al. [30] also manifests the evident influences of physical and emotional proximity on online information seeking under the crisis context.

Based on the above rationale, in the supportive communication context, high proximity induces compassion and sympathy more efficiently, which has a potential impact on information diffusion. Likewise, some studies noted that reciprocal social support behaviors frequently occur in close social proximity circumstances [50] and a higher social presence environment arousing psychological proximity [44]. Thus, we propose the following hypothesis:

  • H3: High proximity triggers more retransmission than low proximity.

Support typology

People give support to others in multiple ways, while how different types of social support are associated with the seeking-provision remains underexplored. A considerable number of studies summarized distinct social support types and put forward some conceptual frameworks [52,53]. Some scholars conceptually divided social support into four categories, including emotional support (e.g., expression of empathy, love, trust, and caring), informational support (e.g., advice-giving, providing suggestions, sharing information), appraisal support (i.e., offering information for self-evaluation), and instrumental support (i.e., providing tangible support) [5254]. Others extended the categories of social support by incorporating emotion, esteem, information, network, and tangible assistance [5557]. Moreover, the social support inventory of UCLA (the University of California at Los Angeles) proposes a classification framework consists of three types: information or advice, tangible assistance or aid, and emotional support [58].

For concision and clarity, also following the previous operation [5961], we merged the existing frameworks into two major types: emotional support and instrumental support. The former one typically refers to needs regarding caring, empathy, love, and trust [5962], while the latter one denotes the requirements of instrumental resources and practical help [53,59]. The interplay between social support types and support provision is still under-investigated, particularly the comparison of different types' outcomes has not been thoroughly examined in the online context. This study intends to remedy the defects by discussing how distinct types of social support induce different feedbacks. Therefore, we raise a research question:

  • RQ1: What is the relationship between support seeking types and retransmission?

Disease severity

A patient's demands vary by the disease phase because each phase presents a new set of challenges and concomitant opportunities [63,64]. COVID-19 is a relatively novel virus with severe clinical manifestation, and it even incurs death [65]. However, bounded by the shortage of testing capacity in COVID-19's early stage, not all infected patients can get diagnosed formally on time [4]. As a result, suspected cases and confirmed cases were treated differently, receiving different medical services. Before the establishment of makeshift hospitals, patients who have mild symptoms but not get laboratory-confirmed often isolated themselves at home, while those who got confirmed were admitted to hospitals in a relatively short time. Thus, it is reasonable to infer that help seekers in different disease phases would get dissimilar attention. And normally, the more serious the illness, the more urgent the help-seeking and the more likely for the patient to obtain support provision. Accordingly, we posit:

  • H4: Disease severity expressed in online help-seeking posts is positively associated with retransmission.

Emotion

Previous studies have proven different emotions affect social decisions differently [66,67]. The logical cues and emotional cues always work in parallel, and emotions sometimes surpass factual information and deliberation [10]. Recent studies regarding emotions and preventive practices (e.g., wear a face covering) during the COVID-19 period indicated that negative emotions mediate the relationship between gender difference and face covering usage [68]. Since rely on reasoning and rely on emotions often occur simultaneously [69], we decide to dive deep into negative emotions in our study for positive feelings are rarely exist in help-seeking posts. Among a plethora of emotional types concerning illness [70], fear is the most prominent emotion during pandemic times, which is highly contagious and makes people feel imminent threats easily [10,71]. Lazarus [72] and Dillard et al. [73] summarized primary discrete emotional kinds in health communication. Apart from fear, anger and sadness are the other two dominating emotions. Anger always connected with removing obstacles, while sadness denotes a manifestation of a sense of failure [7274]. However, it is still not clear how distinct emotional types induce different retweeting effects. Therefore, we formulate the following research question:

  • RQ2: What is the relationship between emotional types and retransmission?

Materials and methods

Data collection

We choose China as our research field for the following reasons. Firstly, Wuhan, the capital of Hubei province, is the earliest epicenter of the COVID-19 outbreak [75]. However, the Chinese government adopted a series of prevention and control measures to bring the virus under control, almost tamed the spread of COVID-19 in mainland China [76]. China's experience can be an essential reference to other countries, especially in current global pandemic time. Secondly, with the rapid development of Internet facilities in China, Internet penetration has reached 61.2% in China until June 2019, along with a continuous growing scale of social media users [77]. Some social media platforms play the role of organizing campaigns, promoting the formation of civil society and the public sphere in China [78]. In today's crisis period, Chinese social media exerts the potential to mobilize collective intelligence to overcome difficulties, inspiring other countries to utilize social media to solve health-related problems and cross tough barriers.

Sina Weibo, one of the most popular social media platforms in China, was selected as the sample pool. According to Weibo's user report in 2018, this social media service has accumulated more than 0.4 billion monthly active users and nearly 0.2 billion daily active users until Q4 of 2018 [79]. Weibo has been proved as a vibrant discussion platform and help-seeking space during major social events, especially in the COVID-19 period [4,5]. All authors went through relevant help-seeking posts on Weibo and picked up pertinent keywords. After screening and merging, three pairs of keyword combinations were determined as search terms, including "pneumonia + ask for help" (“肺炎 + 求救” in Chinese), "pneumonia + seek help" (“肺炎 + 求助” in Chinese), and "pneumonia + help me" (“肺炎 + 救救” in Chinese). Date range starts from Jan. 20th, 2020, which is the date that Nanshan Zhong confirmed human-to-human transmission [80], and ends at Mar. 1st, 2020, as the closing date of the first Fang Cang makeshift hospital in the epicenter Wuhan City, indicating the epidemic had been roughly under control in China [81]. We retrieved and collected the data on June 3, 2020.

For now, Sina Weibo doesn't prohibit automated data access explicitly. Previous studies have adopted web crawling scripts to collect Sina Weibo posts related to COVID-19 [4,82]. Besides, our study has been reviewed and approved by the Ethics Review Committee of Tsinghua University, China (No. THU202023). Thus, we took a similar strategy to write a web crawler in Python programming language to retrieve all qualified posts in the designated date range. 34,088 posts were collected in the first round, and 9,826 posted remained after removing duplicated and unqualified posts. We conducted a random sampling to extract a representative sample from the overall corpus because of the large amount of data. Following the American Association for Public Opinion Research's (AAPOR) suggestion [83], we set a 95% confidence level and a 3.5% sampling error, as typical thresholds in social science research. It turns out that the minimum sample size should be 726. Thus, we randomly sampled 727 posts for further analysis. Every single post acts as the analysis unit in our study, contains the user ID, user name, user’s registration time, post time, number of followers, number of retweets, content, images' links, authentication type, post's link.

Measurement

The measurement scheme was built on the literature review. Emotion type is composed of fear, anger, sadness, and others. Support typology contains emotional support seeking, instrumental support seeking, and no specific kind of support seeking. Proximity was operationalized into reporting self-illness, reporting others' illness, and both. Disease severity consists of three kinds: suspected case, confirmed case, and others. Completeness is further decomposed into the completeness of individual information and disease status. Detailed meanings of those categories are listed in Table 1. Number of followers, posting frequency and some other indicators are incorporated into analysis as control variables according to existing research [15,84].

Table 1. Concepts and corresponding variables.

Concept Variable Category Connotation Examplea
Completeness Completeness Completeness of individual information Disclosure of demographic and biographic information, including name, age, ID number, phone number, geographical information, photo [28,85]. Name: Real name of the patient, such as Aixia Li
Age: Such as 65
ID number: ID card number (18 digits in China)
Phone number: Phone number of the patient or related person
Geographical information: Such as Qibiao Community, Hongshan Ave.
Photo: Photo of the patient
Completeness of disease status Disclosure of disease status, including disease development, diagnostic report, chest X-ray photo, medical record, underlying disease, personal experience [45,86]. Disease development: "My father couldn't get out of bed four days ago. In the next two days, he became unconscious!"
Diagnostic report: Such as blood test report
Chest X-ray photo: Photo of chest X-ray
Medical record: Such as treatment record
Underlying disease: Such as history of hypertension
Personal experience: "I have asked the @Wuhan Hotline account and Guanshan Community for help, but none of them can arrange hospitalization."
Proximity Reporting type Reporting self-illness Seeking help by disclosing one's own and immediate family members' COVID-19 sickness [4]. "I was infected with coronavirus, need help."
Reporting others' illness Seeking help by mentioning or reporting others' COVID-19 sickness [4]. "I forward this post from a confirmed patient in Wuhan City, he was infected with COVID-19 but is in a severe shortage of medical care."
Both Reporting self-illness and others' illness simultaneously. "I got infected when taking care of my sick mother. My friend, who lives outside Hubei Province, also infected with this pneumonia."
Support typology Support seeking type Emotional support seeking Emotional support involves acting as a confidant for someone, providing empathy or other positive affection toward people who suffer from misfortune [5961]. "I have a low fever these nights, so afraid. Can someone give me some comfort?"
Instrumental support seeking Instrumental support means offering assistance tangibly or physically, such as donating money, providing medical supplies to someone in sickness [5961]. "My mother is in urgent need of a sickbed, can someone help me contact an available hospital?"
No specific kind of support Not mention any specific kind of support seeking. “Who can help them?”
Severity Stage of illness Suspected Patients with some COVID-19 infection symptoms but not get laboratory-confirmed [87]. "I took a chest X-ray, and the doctor said I am a suspected case. But I haven't taken a nucleic acid test yet."
Confirmed Patients who get laboratory-confirmed [87]. "The nucleic test is positive. It turns out I was infected with the coronavirus."
Others Unknown status. Illness status is not mentioned.
Emotion Emotional type Fear An emotion experienced in anticipation of some specific pain or danger. Fear is predicated on the belief that the individual faces impending danger over which he or she may have little or no control [73]. "At present, Mr. Zhao is in critical condition. He has been sent to the intensive care unit of Central South Hospital. He is in urgent need of blood plasma of type O from recovered patients."
Anger The health system and government may evoke anger due to their incapability to provide the necessary protection. Particular individuals can also stir up anger for transmitting the disease [88]. "I'm furious at the work efficiency of local hospitals!!!"
Sadness Sorrow and sadness are commonly experienced for loss [88] and irrevocable failure to meet the goal [73]. "I feel too sad and helpless in front of this pneumonia, will you help me?"
Others Other emotions except fear, anger, and sadness. "I hope everything will get better soon."

a All examples come from posts in our research corpus (translated from Chinese).

Coding

A pilot study with 100 posts was conducted to test the coding scheme and train the coders. The pilot study validated the effectiveness of the proposed coding scheme and the accuracy of the corresponding classifications. Three coders major in communication studies were recruited to code the posts for inter-coder reliability evaluation. The average Krippendorff's alpha coefficient was 0.753 in the first round, which is slightly below the acceptable level. We then retrained the coders, resolved all discrepancies, and sampled another 114 posts from the corpus randomly for a new round trial coding. The average reliability coefficient improved to 0.873 in the second round, which means highly consistent among the coders. All coders then performed coding work on the remaining posts independently.

Analysis

Since the dependent variable is the number of retweets, traditional linear regression models are inadequate for modeling this kind of highly skewed count variable. Four count models: Poisson regression, negative binomial regression, zero-inflated Poisson regression, and zero-inflated negative binomial regression were compared to fit the data. Firstly, the conditional variance of the outcome variable far exceeds the conditional mean on most categorical explanatory variables, which violates the underlying assumption of Poisson regression. Secondly, we compared the negative binomial regression with the zero-inflated negative binomial regression. Fig 1 displays the residuals from the two tested models, and small residual distribution indicates the good-fitting of the corresponding model.

Fig 1. Model comparison based on residuals.

Fig 1

The line with hollow circles represents the zero-inflated negative binomial regression model, and the line with solid triangles represents the negative binomial regression model. Small residuals are indicative of good-fitting model, the model with the line closest to zero should be considered for our research data.

Besides, the BIC indicator and the sum of Pearson also approved the conclusion that the negative binomial regression model (BIC = 243.908, Sum of Pearson = 7.887) is more preferred to the other one (BIC = 265.635, Sum of Pearson = 27.138). Table 2 shows the descriptive statistical results of all variables.

Table 2. Descriptive statistical results of all variables.

Variable N Mean (SD) Min, Max Description
Retransmission 727 1029.779 (9506.688) 0, 160129 Number of retransmissions
Authentication 727 Binary 0, 1 1 being authenticated as influencer (17.33%)
With image 727 Binary 0, 1 1 being post with image (34.25%)
With link 727 Binary 0, 1 1 being post with extra link (5.91%)
With hashtag 727 Binary 0, 1 1 being post with hashtag (74.69%)
Mention other user 727 Binary 0, 1 1 being post mentions other user (14.31%)
Followers (logarithmized) 727 2.498 (1.261) 0, 7.098 Number of followers
Posting frequency 727 1.839 (7.223) 0.001, 123.282 Average number of posts sent by the user per day
Text length (logarithmized) 727 2.137 (0.442) 1.041, 3.217 Length of post
Growth rate of confirmed cases 727 0.147 (0.576) -0.664, 6.520 Daily growth rate of national confirmed cases
Emotion type 727 Four categories 1, 4 1 being fear (42.78%); 2 being anger (11.00%); 3 being sadness (33.70%); 4 being others (12.52%)
Support seeking type 727 Three categories 1, 3 1 being instrumental support seeking (53.65%); 2 being emotional support seeking (13.76%); 3 being no specific kind of support seeking (32.60%)
Proximity 727 Three categories 1, 3 1 being reporting self-illness (7.43%); 2 being reporting others' illness (89.13%); 3 being both (3.44%)
Disease severity 727 Three categories 1, 3 1 being suspected (28.47%); 2 being confirmed (32.46%); 3 being others (39.06%)
Completeness of individual information 727 1.915 (1.771) 0, 6 Level of detail of demographic and biographic information. 0 = disclose none of the corresponding components, 6 = disclose all corresponding components.
Completeness of disease status 727 1.505 (1.640) 0, 6 Level of detail of disease status. 0 = disclose none of the corresponding components, 6 = disclose all corresponding components.

Results

Table 3 summarized the results of negative binomial regression models. Model A contains all control variables, while Model B contains both control variables and principal independent variables. All coefficients are evaluated based on robust standard errors.

Table 3. Results of negative binomial regression models.

Model A: Retransmission (n = 727) Model B: Retransmission (n = 727)
Factors B (95% CI) P-value B (95% CI) P-value
Constant -7.980 (-10.280–5.680) <0.001 -6.810 (-9.165–4.455) <0.001
Control variables
Authentication -0.628 (-1.652–0.395) 0.229 -1.352 (-2.100–0.603) <0.001
Followers (logarithmized) 1.946 (1.474–2.419) <0.001 1.809 (1.515–2.103) <0.001
Posting frequency -0.159 (-0.190–0.128) <0.001 -0.116 (-0.146–0.086) <0.001
Image vs. no image 1.839 (1.174–2.505) <0.001 0.977 (0.244–1.710) 0.009
Link vs. no link 2.219 (0.467–3.971) 0.013 2.692 (1.103–4.282) 0.001
Hashtag vs. no hashtag 0.492 (-0.288–1.273) 0.216 0.651 (-0.046–1.348) 0.067
Mention other users vs. not mention -0.421 (-1.422–0.581) 0.410 0.842 (-0.120–1.805) 0.086
Text length (logarithmized) 3.077 (2.202–3.951) <0.001 1.465 (0.519–2.412) 0.002
Growth rate of confirmed cases -0.065 (-0.284–0.154) 0.562 0.018 (-0.189–0.224) 0.867
Independent variables
Completeness of individual information 0.565 (0.343–0.787) <0.001
Completeness of disease status 0.046 (-0.274–0.366) 0.779
Reporting self-illness vs. reporting others’ illness 1.853 (0.397–3.309) 0.013
Reporting both vs. reporting others’ illness 1.634 (0.099–3.169) 0.037
Instrumental support vs. emotional support 2.330 (1.415–3.245) <0.001
No specific support vs. emotional support 1.400 (0.449–2.350) 0.004
Suspected vs. other stage of illness 1.686 (0.749–2.623) <0.001
Confirmed vs. other stage of illness 1.583 (0.606–2.559) 0.001
Fear vs. anger -2.725 (-3.824–1.626) <0.001
Sadness vs. anger -2.749 (-3.863–1.634) <0.001
Other emotion vs. anger -2.184 (-3.574–0.794) 0.002
Model fit
Log pseudolikelihood -2525.923 -2444.551
Wald χ2 298.530 666.600
Pseudo R2 0.065 0.095
AIC 5073.847 4933.101
BIC 5124.325 5034.058

With regard to the completeness, for each one-unit increase in disclosing individual information, the expected log count of the number of retweets increased by 0.565 (p < 0.001, IRR = 1.759). However, the correlation between completeness of disease status and the number of retweets is insignificant. Thus, H1 was supported but H2 was rejected.

When it comes to proximity, reporting self-illness (B = 1.853, p < 0.05, IRR = 6.377) and both self and others' illnesses (B = 1.634, p < 0.05, IRR = 5.126) are expected to receive more retweets than only reporting others' illnesses. H3 was supported.

For support seeking type, the expected log retweet count of instrumental support seeking is significantly more than emotional support seeking (B = 2.330, p < 0.001, IRR = 10.279), no specific support seeking comes afterwards (B = 1.400, p < 0.01, IRR = 4.054). RQ1 was answered.

H4 discusses the relationship between disease severity and retransmission. Compared with other illness stages, posts about suspected cases receive more attention (B = 1.686, p < 0.001, IRR = 5.398), confirmed cases afterwards (B = 1.583, p < 0.01, IRR = 4.868). H4 was rejected.

RQ2 asks how emotional types affect help-seeking diffusion. The expected log retweet count for fear is lower than the expected log count for anger (B = -2.725, p < 0.001, IRR = 0.066), followed by sadness (B = -2.749, p < 0.001, IRR = 0.064).

Borrowing former scholars' experience [11], we further conducted a sensitivity test using the MANOVA analysis for cross-validation. The robustness check results (both coefficient size and significance) are close to the negative binomial regression, proving the accuracy of our estimation.

Discussion

First of all, the findings of the completeness part are in concert with one recent research. Pan et al. [85] found that support-seeking posts with peripheral self-disclosure elicit lower perceived anonymity in the support-provider side, thus increasing the trustworthiness of the support-seeking messages and improving the quality of advice. Peripheral self-disclosure denotes biographical data, including name, age, gender, geographical information [85], which is precisely the "completeness of individual information" defined in our study. From the perspective of uncertainty reduction theory, message without clear source identity is more likely to be perceived as low credibility, further impeding support-providers' engagement in supportive communication [89,90]. In other words, detailed personal information disclosure can effectively diminish perceived anonymity and demonstrate the vulnerability of help-seekers, which is pivotal in text-based online anonymous settings.

On the contrary, the completeness of disease status fails to trigger retweet behavior. One possible explanation could be the high threshold of medical knowledge posed a high demand for laypersons, resulting in an invisible "communication gap" [91]. The diagnostic report, chest X-ray photo, or medical record requires expertise to understand, which seems impossible to most Weibo users. However, this does not mean that the description of the disease is not important. To improve the communication effect, symptoms and development of the illness should be described in detail through simple words to make it easily understood by ordinary people. This rule could be verified by the positive correlation between the text length and the retransmission [92].

The impact of proximity on information transmission is consistent with the previous studies [30,35], which demonstrates that a high level of proximity triggers more retransmission. Proximity was operationalized into reporting self-illness, reporting others' illness, and reporting both in our study. On the one hand, proximity can be interpreted as a type of constructed imaginary relationship with "others," or a perceived connection between individuals [48]. Imaginary relationship plays a vital role in eliciting emotional or informational responses to suffering. Reporting self-illness, reporting others' illness, and reporting both represent different levels of perceived psychological distance, lead to varying amounts of information retransmission. On the other hand, the rhetoric school emphasizes the effect of "narrative distance" on audiences' emotional involvement [93]. Specifically, narrative perspectives (first-person versus third-person) can influence victim blame and supporting intention by affecting the perceived psychological distance [94]. Follow this thread, reporting self-illness and reporting others' illness can be separately associated with first-person and third-person narrative perspectives, stimulating disparate psychological distances and leads to different responses toward the patients eventually. In other words, reporting self-illness usually creates a sense of telling firsthand experience, which fosters a perception of reliability and authenticity on the support-provider side. It is especially true in the health communication context because a direct expression always outperforms interpreting others' stories. Thus, it is critical to add information about "myself" in a help-seeking message if one intends to gain extensive attention.

Although social network services generally offer substantial opportunities for social support transactions, their potential to provide emotional and instrumental support may differ [95]. In this study, more than half of posts contain instrumental support seeking intention, far exceed emotional support seeking and aimless support seeking. Instrumental support seeking posts receive more retransmission. This result suggests that in the health communication field, especially during a severe pandemic, it is inevitable to face the explosive growth of information. Under this circumstance, attention becomes a scarce but valuable resource [96]. Compared with nihilistic emotional support seeking, instrumental support seeking focused on improving one's health condition, demonstrating direct material needs, and showing the willingness to receive immediate help. Besides, based on Vitak and Ellison's work [97], many people are reluctant to express emotional needs online because they do not want to appear "needy." Emotional support also articulates with intimacy, a prerequisite for emotional communication between interactive partners [98]. The anonymity feature of online communication hinders help seekers' desire for emotional support, impedes the occurrence of in-depth emotional connection [99]. Additionally, the dissemination effect of aimless support seeking also surpasses the emotional one. One possible reason is that there exist many similar posts with intense emotions (e.g., fear toward COVID-19). The audience may immune to those contents after long-lasting exposure, thus mitigate the desire to forward such posts.

Regarding severity, existing studies revealed that giving priority to the worst-case when allocating health resources in pandemics is a fundamental principle [100102]. However, our findings go against this principle. One significant reason is that demands in posts about suspected cases are easier to meet than posts about confirmed cases. Suspected patients are eager for testing kits and opportunities to visit doctors, while confirmed patients are craving for sickbeds, plasma, and even transfer to another hospital. The former type is more achievable, along with less probability of failure. Impelled by difficulty comparison, retweeters incline to disseminate help-seeking messages which are easy to implement.

When it comes to emotion, posts expressing anger received more retransmission than fear, sadness, and other kinds of emotions. Based on our observation, anger mostly stems from hospitals' unfair treatment or official institutions' delayed responses. It is reasonable for social media users to stand on the "just side" to support the unfortunate patients. By retweeting the angry posts, retweeters vented their feelings, intended to get more attention, and urged relevant departments to take effective measures. In our research corpus, sadness is the second common emotion. However, it received the least retransmission. This can be attributed to sadness's intrinsic characteristics: posts with sadness mainly express disappointment toward reality but resist to advocate practical attempts to change reality. This finding implies that when seeking help online, one should stress the principal problem and avoid simple catharsis. Similarly, ingeniously using prevailing emotion contributes to receiving sympathy and pragmatic feedback from others.

Conclusion

COVID-19 poses enormous threats to the whole world. Due to the unbalanced distribution of medical resources and inadequate response at the early breakout stage, many patients (both suspected and confirmed), along with their relatives or friends, turned to social media to seek support. This study explores the driving forces behind online help-seeking post transmission in a global pandemic period by emphasizing the content characteristics. Completeness, proximity, support typology, disease severity, and emotion composed the analytical framework. By employing content analysis, 727 randomly sampled help-seeking posts were analyzed based on a coding scheme we construct. Negative binomial regression reveals that posts release anger, express instrumental support seeking intention, report self-illness, expound suspected cases' conditions, and have detailed individual information disclosure are likely to have more retransmission.

Coronavirus is still spreading fast around the world. As one of the first countries to restrain the spread of the epidemic, China provides a valuable experience to other countries stuck in a dilemma. As the first online help-seeking analysis research in the COVID-19 period, this study offers some insights about health communication via social media, especially how to develop a potent help-seeking post in public health emergencies. However, a couple of limitations need to be mentioned. First, retransmission of post, as the core dependent variable in our study, is a multidimensional concept. For example, Wang et al. [11] decomposed retransmission into "scale" and "structural virality." Limited by research resources and time, we failed to elucidate information diffusion comprehensively. Second, although China's experience is representative, whether this pattern can be generalized into other contexts worth carefully examining. Future studies should conduct more explorations on other social media platforms (e.g., Twitter) to verify the reliability and validity of our results and summarize effective online help-seeking strategies in diversified environments.

Acknowledgments

The authors want to thank Prof. Jing Qian (Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University) for her tremendous help in the ethical review process.

Data Availability

Data are available via figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12743726.v1 Statistical commands are also available via figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12743750.v1

Funding Statement

This study is supported by the Science Popularization and Risk Communication of Transgenic Biotechnologies Project (Grant code: 2016ZX08015002) to CL and AC, China Scholarship (Grant code: 201906210115) to CL, the 25th department funding of USTC (Grant code: DA2110251001) to AC, 2019 New Humanities Funding of USTC (Grant code: YD2110002015) to AC. The funders had no role in the study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or manuscript preparation.

References

  • 1.World Health Organization [Internet]. Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) pandemic [cited 2020 Jul 21]. Available from: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Li L, Zhang Q, Wang X, Zhang J, Wang T, Gao T, et al. Characterizing the propagation of situational information in social media during COVID-19 epidemic: A case study on Weibo. IEEE Trans Comput Soc Syst. 2020;7(2): 556–562. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Li J, Xu Q, Cuomo R, Purushothaman V, Mackey T. Data mining and content analysis of the Chinese social media platform Weibo during the early COVID-19 outbreak: Retrospective observational infoveillance study. JMIR Public Health Surveill [Internet]. 2020. April 21 [cited 2020 Jun 10]; 6(2): e18700 Available from: https://publichealth.jmir.org/2020/2/e18700/. 10.2196/18700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Shen C, Chen A, Luo C, Zhang J, Feng B, Liao W. Using reports of symptoms and diagnoses on social media to predict COVID-19 case counts in mainland China: Observational infoveillance study. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2020. May 28 [cited 2020 Jun 10]; 22(5): e19421 Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2020/5/e19421/. 10.2196/19421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Huang C, Xu X, Cai Y, Ge Q, Zeng G, Li X, et al. Mining the characteristics of COVID-19 patients in China: Analysis of social media posts. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2020. May 17 [cited 2020 Jun 10]; 22(5): e19087 Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2020/5/e19087/. 10.2196/19087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Fu K, Zhu Y. Did the world overlook the media's early warning of COVID-19? Journal of Risk Research. J Risk Res [Internet]. 2020. April 24 [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: 10.1080/13669877.2020.1756380 Subscription required. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mukkamala A, Beck R. The role of social media for collective behavior development in response to natural disasters [Internet]. Twenty-Sixth European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS2018); 2018. Available from: https://aisel.aisnet.org/ecis2018_rp/109/. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barros JM, Duggan J, Rebholz-Schuhmann D. The application of Internet-based sources for public health surveillance (Infoveillance): Systematic review. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2020. March 13 [cited 2020 Jun 10]; 22(3): e13680 Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2020/3/e13680/. 10.2196/13680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rains SA, Peterson EB, Wright KB. Communicating social support in computer-mediated contexts: A meta-analytic review of content analyses examining support messages shared online among individuals coping with illness. Commun Monogr. 2015;82(4): 403–430. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Van Bavel JJ, Baicker K, Boggio PS, Capraro V, Cichocka A, Cikara M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response.Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4: 460–471. 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wang X, Chen L, Shi J, Peng T. What makes cancer information viral on social media? Comput Human Behav. 2019;93: 149–156. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dovidio JF. Helping behavior and altruism: An empirical and conceptual overview In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1984. p. 361–427. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lee M, Kim H, Kim O. Why do people retweet a tweet? Altruistic, egoistic, and reciprocity motivations for retweeting. Psychologia. 2015;58: 189–201. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Capraro V. The dual-process approach to human sociality: A review. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. 2019. [cited 2020 Jul 20]: [153 p.]. Available from: 10.31234/osf.io/432yw. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Liu Z, Liu L, Li H. Determinants of information retweeting in microblogging. Internet Research. 2012;22(4): 443–466. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pan W, Feng B, Wingate VS. What you say is what you get: How self-disclosure in support seeking affects language use in support provision in online support forums. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2018;37(1): 3–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Rui JR, Chen Y, Damiano A. Health organizations providing and seeking social support: A Twitter-based content analysis. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2013;16(9): 669–673. 10.1089/cyber.2012.0350 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Indian M, Grieve R. When Facebook is easier than face-to-face: Social support derived from Facebook in socially anxious individuals. Pers Individ Dif. 2014;59: 102–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Andalibi N, Ozturk P, Forte A. Sensitive self-disclosures, responses, and social support on Instagram: The case of #Depression In: Lee CP, Poltrock S, editors. Proceedings of the 2017 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Social Computing; 2017 Feb; Oregon, USA. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2017. p. 1485–1500. 10.1007/s12223-017-0491-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Braithwaite DO, Waldron VR, Finn J. Communication of social support in computer-mediated groups for people with disabilities. Health Commun. 1999;11(2): 123–151. 10.1207/s15327027hc1102_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Coursaris CK, Liu M. An analysis of social support exchanges in online HIV/AIDS self-help groups. Comput Human Behav. 2009;25(4): 911–918. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Pan W, Shen C, Feng B. You get what you give: Understanding reply reciprocity and social capital in online health support forums. J Health Commun. 2017;22(1): 45–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Yang Q, Tufts C, Ungar L, Guntuku S, Merchant R. To retweet or not to retweet: Understanding what features of cardiovascular tweets influence their retransmission. J Health Commun. 2018;23(12): 1026–1035. 10.1080/10810730.2018.1540671 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Liu X, Lu J, Wang H. When health information meets social media: Exploring virality on Sina Weibo. Health Commun. 2017;32(10): 1252–1260. 10.1080/10410236.2016.1217454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Boyd D, Golder S, Lotan G. Tweet, tweet, retweet: Conversational aspects of retweeting on Twitter 43rd Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences; 2010. January 5–8; Honolulu, USA: IEEE; 2010. p. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Recuero R, Araujo R, Zago G. How does social capital affect retweets? In: Nicolov N, Shanahan JG, editors. Proceedings of the Fifth International AAAI Conference on Weblog and Social Media; 2011. July 17–21; Catalonia, Spain. California: AAAI Press; 2011. p. 305–312. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Briones R, Nan X, Madden K, Waks L. When vaccines go viral: An analysis of HPV vaccine coverage on YouTube. Health Commun. 2012;27(5): 478–485. 10.1080/10410236.2011.610258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Altman I, Taylor DA. Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships. 1st ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston; 1973. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wingate VS, Feng B, Kim C, Pan W, Jang JY. The influence of self-disclosure in online support seeking on quality of received advice. J Lang Soc Psychol. 2020;39(3): 397–413. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Huang YL, Starbird K, Orand M, Stanek SA, Pedersen HT. Connected through crisis: Emotional proximity and the spread of misinformation online In: Cosley D, Forte A, editors. Proceedings of the 18th ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing; 2015. March; Vancouver, Canada. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2015. p. 969–980. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Capraro V, Everett JAC, Earp BD. Priming intuition disfavors instrumental harm but not impartial beneficence. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2019;83: 142–149. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fogg BJ, Tseng H. The elements of computer credibility In: Williams MG, Altom MW, editors. CHI ‘99: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems; 1999. May; Pennsylvania, USA. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 1999. p. 80–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fogg BJ, Marshall J, Osipovich A, Varma C, Laraki O, Fang N, et al. Elements that affect web credibility: Early results from a self-report study In: Tremaine M, editor. CHI EA ‘00: CHI ‘00 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems; 2000. April; Hague, Netherlands. New York: Association for Computing Machinery; 2000. p. 287–288. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Boehmer J, Tandoc EC. Why we retweet: Factors influencing intentions to share sport news on Twitter. International Journal of Sport Communication. 2015;8(2): 212–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lee JY, Sundar SS. To tweet or to retweet? That is the question for health professionals on Twitter. Health Commun. 2013;28(5): 509–524. 10.1080/10410236.2012.700391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Sundar SS. Exploring receivers' criteria for perception of print and online news. Journal Mass Commun Q. 1999;76(2): 373–386. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bucy EP. Media credibility reconsidered: Synergy effects between on-air and online news. Journal Mass Commun Q. 2003;80(2): 247–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Hilligoss B, Rieh SY. Developing a unifying framework of credibility assessment: Construct, heuristics, and interaction in context. Inf Process Manag. 2008;44(4): 1467–1484. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Sbaffi L, Rowley J. Trust and credibility in web-based health information: A review and agenda for future research. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2017. June 19 [cited 2020 Jun 10];19(6): e218 Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2017/6/e218/. 10.2196/jmir.7579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Self CC. Credibility In: Salwen MB, Stacks DW, editors. An integrated approach to communication theory and research. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. p. 421–441. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Stvilia B, Mon L, Yi YJ. A model for online consumer health information quality. J Am Soc Inf Sci Techol. 2009;60(9): 1781–1791. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bates BR, Romina S, Ahmed R, Hopson D. The effect of source credibility on consumers' perceptions of the quality of health information on the Internet. Med Inform Internet Med. 2006;31(1): 45–52. 10.1080/14639230600552601 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Freeman KS, Spyridakis JH. An examination of factors that affect the credibility of online health information. Tech Commun. 2004;51: 239–263. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Li S, Feng B, Li N, Tan X. How social context cues in online support-seeking influence self-disclosure in support provision. Commun Q. 2015;63(5): 586–602. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Huang HY. Examining the beneficial effects of individual's self-disclosure on the social network site. Comput Human Behav. 2016;57: 122–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Trope Y, Liberman N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol Rev. 2010;117(2): 440–463. 10.1037/a0018963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kwon KH, Chadha M, Pellizzaro K. Proximity and terrorism news in social media: A construal-level theoretical approach to networked framing of terrorism in Twitter. Mass Commun Soc. 2017;20(6): 869–894. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Loewenstein G, Small DA. The scarecrow and the tin man: The vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Rev Gen Psychol. 2007;11: 112–126. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Batson CD, Lishner DA, Cook J, Sawyer S. Similarity and nurturance: Two possible sources of empathy for strangers. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2005;27(1): 15–25. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ha S, Ahn J. Why are you sharing others’ tweets?: The impact of argument quality and source credibility on information sharing behavior. ICIS 2011 Proceedings [Internet]. 2011. October [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://aisel.aisnet.org/icis2011/proceedings/humanbehavior/4/. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rabjohn N, Cheung CMK, Lee MKO. Examining the perceived credibility of online opinions: information adoption in the online environment. Proceedings of the 41st Annual Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences (HICSS 2008) [Internet]. 2008. January [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/4438991. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.House JS. Work stress and social support. 1st ed. Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Malecki CK, Demaray MK. What type of support do they need? Investigating student adjustment as related to emotional, informational, appraisal, and instrumental support. Sch Psychol Q. 2003;18(3): 231–252. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Muñoz-Laboy M, Severson N, Perry A, Guilamo-Ramos V. Differential impact of types of social support in the mental health of formerly incarcerated Latino men. Am J Mens Health. 2014;8(3): 226–239. 10.1177/1557988313508303 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cutrona CE, Suhr JA. Controllability of stressful events and satisfaction with spouse support behaviors. Communic Res. 1992;19: 154–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Coulson NS. Receiving social support online: An analysis of a computer-mediated support group for individuals living with irritable bowel syndrome. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2005;8(6): 580–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Coulson NS, Buchanan H, Aubeeluck A. Social support in cyberspace: A content analysis of communication within a Huntington's disease online support group. Patient Eudc Couns. 2007;68(2): 173–178. 10.1016/j.pec.2007.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rini C, Schetter CD, Hobel CJ, Glynn LM, Sandman CA. Effective social support: Antecedents and consequences of partner support during pregnancy. Pers Relatsh. 2006;13: 207–229. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wilson DK, Kliewer W, Bayer L, Jones D, Welleford A, Heiney M, et al. (1999). The influence of gender and emotional versus instrumental support on cardiovascular reactivity in African-American adolescents. Ann Behav Med. 1999;21(3): 235–243. 10.1007/BF02884840 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Shakespeare-Finch J, Obst PL. The development of the 2-way social support scale: A measure of giving and receiving emotional and instrumental support. J Pers Assess. 2011;93(5): 483–490. 10.1080/00223891.2011.594124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Federici RA, Skaalvik EM. Students' perceptions of emotional and instrumental teacher support: Relations with motivational and emotional responses. International Education Studies. 2014;7(1): 21–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Ko HC, Wang LL, Xu YT. Understanding the different types of social support offered by audience to A-list diary-like and informative bloggers. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. 2013;16(3): 194–199. 10.1089/cyber.2012.0297 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Luker KA, Beaver K, Leinster SJ, Owens RG. Information needs and sources of information for women with breast cancer: A follow-up study. J Adv Nurs. 1996;23(3): 487–495. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.1996.tb00010.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.van der Molen B. Relating information needs to the cancer experience: Themes from six cancer narratives. Eur J Cancer Care. 2000;9(1): 48–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Emanuel EJ, Persad G, Upshur R, Thome B, Parker M, Glickman A, et al. Fair allocation of scarce medical resources in the time of Covid-19. N Engl J Med. 2020;382(21): 2049–2055. 10.1056/NEJMsb2005114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Strohminger N, Lewis RL, Meyer DE. Divergent effects of different positive emotions on moral judgment. Cognition. 2011;119: 295–300. 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Motro D, Ordonez LD, Pittarello A, Welsh DT. Investigating the effects of anger and guilt on unethical behavior: A dual-process approach. J Bus Ethics. 2018;152: 133–148. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Capraro V, Barcelo H. The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. 2020. [cited 2020 Jul 20]: [19 p.]. Available from: 10.31234/osf.io/tg7vz. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Capraro V, Barcelo H. Priming reasoning increases intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. arXiv 2006.11273 [Preprint]. 2020. [cited 2020 Jul 20]: [14 p.]. Available from: https://arxiv.org/abs/2006.11273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Mukherjee S. The emperor of all maladies: A biography of cancer. 1st ed. New York: Scribner; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Cole S, Balcetis E, Dunning D. Affective signals of threat increase perceived proximity. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(1): 34–40. 10.1177/0956797612446953 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Lazarus RS. Emotion and adaptation 1st ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Dillard JP, Nabi RL. The persuasive influence of emotion in cancer prevention and detection messages. J Commun. 2006;56: S123–S139. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Myrick JG, Holton AE, Himelboim I, Love B. #Stupidcancer: Exploring a typology of social support and the role of emotional expression in a social media community. Health Commun. 2016;31(5): 596–605. 10.1080/10410236.2014.981664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wu JT, Leung K, Leung GM. Nowcasting and forecasting the potential domestic and international spread of the 2019-nCoV outbreak originating in Wuhan, China: A modelling study. Lancet. 2020;395(10225): 689–697. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30260-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.China Daily. Report: China’s fight against COVID-19 (full text). Beijing: China Daily; 2020. April 21 [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://covid-19.chinadaily.com.cn/a/202004/21/WS5e9e2c62a3105d50a3d17880.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.China Internet Network Information Center. Statistical Report on Internet Development in China. China Internet Network Information Center. 2019. August [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://cnnic.com.cn/IDR/ReportDownloads/201911/P020191112539794960687.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Lu J, Qiu Y. Microblogging and social change in China. Asian Perspect. 2013;37(3): 305–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Sina Weibo Data Center. Weibo User Report in 2018 Sina Weibo. 2019. March [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://data.weibo.com/report/reportDetail?id=433. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wang B, Qu T. The development of novel coronavirus infection: Human-to-human transmission confirmed and medical staff infected. Beijing: Xinhua News Agency; 2020. January 20 [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: http://www.xinhuanet.com/politics/2020-01/20/c_1125487200.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Xu K. Closure of Wuhan's 11 makeshift hospitals reflects epidemic under control. Beijing: Global Times; 2020. March 8 [cited 2020 Jun 10]. Available from: https://www.globaltimes.cn/content/1181971.shtml. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Hu Y, Huang H, Chen A, Mao XL. Weibo-COV: A large-scale COVID-19 social media dataset from Weibo. arXiv: 2005.09174 [Preprint]. 2020. [cited 2020 Jul 21]: [9 p.]. Available from: https://arxiv.org/abs/2005.09174. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.American Association for Public Opinion Research. Margin of sampling error/credibility interval [Internet]. American Association for Public Opinion Research [cited 2020 Jul 17]. Available from: https://www.aapor.org/Education-Resources/Election-Polling-Resources/Margin-of-Sampling-Error-Credibility-Interval.aspx. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Zhang L, Peng TQ, Zhang YP, Wang XH, Zhu JJH. Content or context: Which matters more in information processing on microblogging sites. Comput Human Behav. 2014;31: 242–249. [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Pan W, Feng B, Wingate VS, Li S. What to say when seeking support online: A comparison among different levels of self-disclosure. Front Psychol. 2020;11: 978 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Zhang J, Le G, Larochelle D, Pasick R, Sawaya GF, Sarkar U, et al. Facts or stories? How to use social media for cervical cancer prevention: A multi-method study of the effects of sender type and content type on increased message sharing. Prev Med. 2019. September;126. Epub 2019 Jun 19. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.105751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jin Y, Cai L, Cheng Z, Cheng H, Deng T, Fan Y, et al. A rapid advice guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of 2019 novel coronavirus (2019-nCoV) infected pneumonia (Standard Version). Medical Journal of Chinese People’s Liberation Army. 2020;45(1): 1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zhuang J, Peng T, Tang J, Wu Y. Mixed and blended emotional reactions to 2014 Ebola outbreak. J Glob Health. 2020;10(1): 010304 10.7189/jogh.10.010304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Berger CR. Communicating under uncertainty In: Roloff ME, Miller GR, editors. Interpersonal processes: New directions in communication research. California: Sage; 1987. p. 39–62. [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Rains SA. The impact of anonymity on perceptions of source credibility and influence in computer-mediated group communication: A test of two competing hypotheses. Commun Res. 2007;34(1): 100–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Filho PCL, de Macedo TS, Ferreira AKA, de Melo MCF, de Araujo MMS, de Melo Freitas JL, et al. Assessing the quality, readability and reliability of online information on COVID-19: An infoveillance observational study. medRxiv 2020.05.30.20117614 [Preprint]. 2020. [cited 2020 Jun 10]: [15 p.]. Available from: 10.1101/2020.05.30.20117614. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Liu G, Shi C, Chen Q, Wu B, Qi J. A two-phase model for retweet number prediction In: Li F, Li G, Hwang S, Yao B, Zhang Z, editors. Web-age information management. Springer; 2014. p. 781–792. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Andringa E. Effects of ‘narrative distance’ on readers' emotional involvement and response. Poetics (Amst). 1996;23(6): 431–452. [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Cao X, Decker D. Psychological distancing: The effects of narrative perspectives and levels of access to a victim's inner world on victim blame and helping intention. International Journal of Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Marketing. 2015;20(1): 12–24. [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Trepte S, Dienlin T, Reinecke L. Influence of social support received in online and offline contexts on satisfaction with social support and satisfaction with life: A longitudinal study. Media Psychol. 2015;18(1): 74–105. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.World Health Organization. Managing epidemics: Key facts about major deadly diseases [internet]. WHO;2018. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/managing-epidemics-key-facts-about-major-deadly-diseases. [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Vitak J, Ellison NB. ‘‘There’s a network out there you might as well tap”: Exploring the benefits of and barriers to exchanging information and support-based resources on Facebook. New Media Soc. 2013;15(2): 243–259. [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Stokes JP. Predicting satisfaction with social support from social network structure. Am J Community Psychol. 1983;11(2): 141–152. [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Newman MW, Lauterbach D, Munson SA, Resnick P, Morris ME. It’s not that I don’t have problems, I’m just not putting them on Facebook: Challenges and opportunities in using online social networks for health In: Hinds P, Tang JC, Wang J, editors. Proceedings of the ACM 2011 Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work. New York, NY: ACM; 2011. p. 341–350. [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Emanuel EJ, Wertheimer A. Who should get influenza vaccine when not all can? Science. 2006;312(5775): 854–855. 10.1126/science.1125347 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Persad G, Wertheimer A, Emanuel EJ. Principles for allocation of scarce medical interventions. Lancet. 2009;373(9661): 423–431. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60137-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Biddison LD, Berkowitz KA, Courtney B, De Jong CMJ, Devereaux AV, Kissoon N, et al. Ethical considerations: Care of the critically ill and injured during pandemics and disasters: CHEST consensus statement. Chest. 2014;146(4 Suppl): e145S–e155S. 10.1378/chest.14-0742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Valerio Capraro

16 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-18830

What triggers online help-seeking retransmission during the COVID-19 period? Empirical evidence from Chinese social media

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Li,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please find below the reviewer's and mine's comments.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 30 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In your Methods section, please include additional information about your dataset and ensure that you have included a statement specifying whether the collection method complied with the terms and conditions for the website.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

I have now collected one review from one expert in the field. The reviewer is positive but suggests several revisions. Therefore, I would like to invite you to revise your work along the reviewer's comments. Moreover, I would like to add a couple more comments that I have had while reading the manuscript. (i) in the general introduction, the "perspective article" about what social and behavioural science can do to support pandemic response, published by Van Bavel et al. in Nature Human Behaviour, can be a useful general reference. (ii) you correctly say that retweeting can be seen as a form of prosocial behaviour; at the same time, many of these posts are very emotional. Therefore, it seems that your research question is related to the work on how emotions affect prosocial behaviour. There is a huge literature about this (see Capraro 2019) for a review. And recently emotions have been also linked to mask wearing (Capraro & Barcelo 2020a, 2020b). I think this potential link should be discussed. Of course it is not a requirement to cite these works, but I am mentioning them because they look very related.

I am looking forward for the revision.

References

Capraro, V. (2019). The dual-process approach to human sociality: A review. Available at SSRN 3409146.

Capraro, V., & Barcelo, H. (2020a). The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. arXiv preprint arXiv:2005.05467.

Capraro, V., & Barcelo, H. (2020b). Priming reasoning increases intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. arXiv preprint arXiv:2006.11273.

Van Bavel, J. J., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 1-12.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This work presents results pertaining the factors behind the retransmission/retweeting of online help-seeking posts that emphasized on content characteristics which are information completeness, proximity, support seeking type, disease severity, and emotion related to COVID-19.

This is an in principle interesting topic, however, the current manuscript has numerous concerns including some crucial conceptual and methodological issues that can affect the effective strength of findings.

Major Points

1. How do you conclude to get the 727 posts? Please state sample size calculation.

2. How did you make sure that pneumonia as keywords are related to covid-19?

3. Could the author give one example how variables are extracted from the post.

Minor Points

1. Please elaborate "sender identities are very close" page 6 line 22.

2. The impacts of proximity are reflected in two aspects. Page 9 line 11. Which aspects are you referring to?

3. Please state the necessary attributes in Page 15 line 2

4. What is level of detail (0-6) represent in completeness attribute.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2020 Nov 3;15(11):e0241465. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241465.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


25 Sep 2020

Dear editor and reviewer,

Thank you very much for providing us an opportunity to revise and respond to reviewers’ valuable comments. Following all these comments, we have thoroughly revised our manuscript. The main changes are summarized as follows:

1. We carefully adjusted the format of this article to meet the style requirements. Also, we added approval from the Ethics Review Committee to justify our research.

2. According to the comments, we significantly expanded our literature review on the relationship between pandemic response and behavioral science (as reflected on Page 3), as well as how emotions affect prosocial behavior (as reflected on Page 10).

3. We followed reviewers' suggestions to provide more information on conceptual and methodological issues (as reflected on Page 11-12). Moreover, we rechecked our dataset to make everything accurate. Thus, we replaced a few duplicated cases with newly added posts and reevaluated all coefficients in our paper. The final dataset (which has been uploaded to the figshare platform) and statistical results in the current version are entirely accurate and reliable.

4. We refined our statistical reporting with more details to meet the standard of PLOS ONE, including replaced the regression coefficient plot with a more common regression table (see Table 3 on Page 20-21).

We believe the manuscript is much stronger and technically sound. In the following pages, please find a detailed point-to-point response to the editorial and reviewer comments.

Along with my coauthors, I thank you sincerely for the consideration of publishing our paper on PLOS ONE.

Kind regards,

Yuru Li, Ph.D. (E-mail: yuru.li@foxmail.com)

● Response to Editor

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

Thanks for your comment, we have revised our format in strict accordance with PLOS ONE's style requirements.

2. In your Methods section, please include additional information about your dataset and ensure that you have included a statement specifying whether the collection method complied with the terms and conditions for the website.

We appreciate this comment, as this issue of terms of service violation is a formidable hindrance to researchers of infoveillance and the Internet in general. First of all, we have included additional information about our data gathering in the Methods section, which incorporates the approval from the Ethics Review Committee (as shown on Page 12, Line 1-4). Then, we would like to further enumerate several reasons to verify our data collection method's reasonability and morality.

1) We collected our research corpus with our self-developed web crawling program from the Sina Weibo platform. The design principle of Sina Weibo is to make user-generated content as visible as possible, which means anyone can read content posted by Weibo users. From this point, we didn't infringe on Weibo users' privacy. Besides, we choose not to disclose the original help-seeking posts, but anyone can see the same content on the Weibo platform using the designated searching criteria in our paper.

2) Two conventional approaches to collecting online data are calling the application program interface (API) and applying self-developed web crawling scripts. As a matter of fact, Sina Weibo has issued a policy announcing that API services are paid services and merely opened to commercial entities [1]. For researchers who are interested in social media, only limited access is provided. If we adopt the official APIs, we can only obtain incompleted data and bear the loss of missing attributes.

3) Many websites (including Weibo) now prohibit, or at least not explicitly permit the use of automated crawling software to access its content. Fortunately, a precedent has been set recently in Sandvig v. Sessions [2], in which Judge John Bates ruled in favor of all researchers to get publicly accessible information on websites under First Amendment protection, despite the possible violation of terms of service. Since Weibo is a publicly-traded company in the U.S., this rule applies to Weibo. We firmly believe this ruling is an important step forward for Internet-related research. It protects researchers like us from unwarranted legal liabilities while all we do is contribute to public knowledge and the public good.

4) Previous social science research on COVID-19 took similar collecting strategies. For instance, Shen et al. [3] crawled all posts sent by users in their Weibo user pool and applied keywords searching to obtain the research corpus. Hu et al. [4] released a large-scale COVID-19 social media dataset based on Weibo, in which 40 million posts from Weibo users were crawled and recorded (with anonymous identities). We believe the data collecting operation in our research is entirely legal, and this strategy benefits us to cope with the COVID-19 epidemic more timely and efficiently.

3. (i) In the general introduction, the "perspective article" about what social and behavioral science can do to support pandemic response, published by Van Bavel et al. in Nature Human Behaviour, can be a useful general reference.

(ii) You correctly say that retweeting can be seen as a form of prosocial behavior; at the same time, many of these posts are very emotional. Therefore, it seems that your research question is related to the work on how emotions affect prosocial behavior. There is a huge literature about this (see Capraro 2019) for a review. And recently emotions have been also linked to mask-wearing (Capraro & Barcelo 2020a, 2020b).

Thanks for your recommendation. We appreciate your advice because the literature is thought-provoking and helpful, and we have absorbed them in our research paper. The details are summarized as follow:

1) The "perspective article" published on Nature Human Behavior provides us with a comprehensive framework to understand retweeting behavior under social and behavioral science. We incorporated this literature in our introduction section, as reflected on Page 3, Line 19-22. Besides, the dual-process approach summarized by Capraro (2019) also helps us to organize the following content more organically. Thus, we mentioned this literature in the introduction section, as reflected on Page 4, Line 4-6.

2) Emotion is a vital factor in affecting retweeting behavior. Capraro and Barcelo's works on the relationship between emotion and preventive practices during the COVID-19 pandemic time is quite pertinent to our research. We referred to those works in the "emotion" section on Page 10, Line 4-7.

● Response to Reviewer #1

1. Major point 1: How do you conclude to get the 727 posts? Please state the sample size calculation.

Thank you for the comment. According to the American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR), sampling error and confidence level jointly determined the sample size in a random sampling operation [5]. Generally, researchers set the confidence level as 95% and the sampling error between 3% and 6% in social science research. In our study, we adopted a 95% confidence level and 3.5% sampling error to calculate the proper sample size. Combined with the population size (n=9,826), it turns out that we need at least 726 posts to achieve the specified error level (calculation website: https://www.surveysystem.com/sscalc.htm). We also explain this briefly in the paper, as reflected on Page 12, Line 5-10.

2. Major point 2: How did you make sure that pneumonia as keywords are related to COVID-19?

Thanks for your comment. Firstly, in the Chinese context, "COVID-19" (“新型冠状肺炎”), "new coronavirus pneumonia" (“新冠病毒肺炎”), and "new pneumonia" (“新型肺炎”) are common appellations for this epidemic. No matter what the name is, it contains the keyword "pneumonia" (“肺炎”). Thus, adopting "pneumonia" as the search term ensures us get the most comprehensive corpus.

Secondly, based on our observation (three coders read almost all the crawled posts), most Weibo users tend to use the word "pneumonia" to describe the COVID-19 and its symptoms within the date range we set.

Thirdly, during the pandemic period, the Weibo platform opened several specific hashtags and super topics, which contained highly relevant help-seeking posts sent by infected people and their friends or relatives. Those hashtags are like "help-seeking from pneumonia patients" (“肺炎患者求助”), they did not include the words "new coronavirus" (“新冠病毒”), "new type" (“新型”), or "nCoV-19" (“新冠”). Therefore, the "pneumonia" search term also guarantees us to obtain the most accurate corpus in the specific date range.

Besides, we applied keyword combinations as search terms instead of using the "pneumonia" word only. This operation helped us to target the help-seeking posts related to the COVID-19 epidemic. For clarity, we add corresponding Chinese translation of keyword combinations in our paper, as reflected on Page 11, Line 17-18.

3. Major point 3: Could the author give one example of how variables are extracted from the post?

Thank you for the comment. We have added a column (“Example”) in Table 1 to elucidate how those variables are extracted from the post. Contents in the newly added column are translated from posts in our corpus. Besides, our answer to minor point 4 also provides some clues on the operationalization process.

4. Minor point 1: Please elaborate "sender identities are very close" page 6 line 22.

Thank you for the comment. Weibo has evolved into a critical virtual platform for Chinese grassroots to express their opinions [6]. During the COVID-19 pandemic period, the public, or the so-called ordinaries, disclosing their symptoms and asking for help actively on Weibo [3,7]. However, there are three kinds of users on the Weibo platform, including ordinary users, organizational users, and celebrities. The latter two are featured with specific visual symbols, and they are more influential than the ordinary user in the virtual world. By mentioning "sender identities are very close," we intended to indicate that help seekers are mainly composed of ordinary users. Organizational users and celebrities usually occupy more resources and seldom asking for help online. To make our meaning more transparent, we slightly adjust the words in the main text, as reflected on Page 5, Line 15-18.

5. Minor point 2: The impacts of proximity are reflected in two aspects. Page 9 line 11. Which aspects are you referring to?

Thanks for the comment. We have to say your comment comes right to the point because the two aspects here seem confusing. Actually, we intend to explain the underlying mechanisms of how proximity works in the mental process by listing the two aspects. For legibility, we reframe this part by using the "Firstly..., secondly..." sentence pattern and removing the weakly related content (as reflected on Page 7-8). By this operation, key concepts (including proximity, psychological distance, and information processing) are more tightly integrated.

6. Minor point 3: Please state the necessary attributes on Page 15 line 2.

Thanks for this comment. We have supplemented the attributes in our paper, as reflected on Page 12, Line 11-12. Besides, for ease of understanding, we provided two screenshots with annotations in this response letter to illustrate those attributes.

(Two illustrations are inserted here, please refer to the "Response to Reviewers.docx" for details)

7. Minor point 4: What is the level of detail (0-6) represent in the completeness attribute.

Thank you for the comment. As shown in Table 1, the completeness attribute is composed of two dimensions: the completeness of individual information (stresses the demographic and biographic information), and the completeness of disease status (emphasizes the disease status). For operationalization, we further listed several components under each dimension. If one post contains more components, then the corresponding dimension score increases, which means the sender discloses more relevant information. A score of 0 means the sender disclosed none of the components, while a score of 6 means the sender disclosed all the components.

We have supplemented the meanings in Table 2 for clarity. In the meantime, we provide an example here to illustrate this further. The following screenshot is the sample post, and we translated it into English.

(One illustration is inserted here, please refer to the "Response to Reviewers.docx" for details)

Translation:

My parents both infected with COVID-19 and got confirmed. My father has difficulty breathing and feels sick, but he has not been hospitalized up to now!!!

[Name] Liyong Liu

[Age] 60

[City] Wuhan

[Address] Qibiao Community, Guanshan St.

[Time of illness] Jan. 22nd, 2020

[Symptoms] Persistent high fever, cough, chest tightness, dyspnea, diarrhea, and poor spirit

My family lives in the Qibiao Community, Guanshan Ave., Hongshan District, Wuhan City. My father was hospitalized twice in January this year because of stomach diseases. Since then, he has been frail, and my mother was infected with COVID-19 while taking care of him. I have asked the @Wuhan Hotline Weibo account and the Guanshan Community for help, but none of them can arrange hospitalization. So I can only make registration and stay on the waiting list. Yesterday, my father went through further consultation. The doctor pointed out that his lung infection is very severe and has to be hospitalized immediately. However, the Guanshan Hospital is short of sickbeds, and we have no choice but to wait for the community arrangement. Please help me. Please save my parents. I beg the government for a sickbed to get my father treated as soon as possible! My parents lived a hard life before. They suffered from natural disasters, got polio at a young age. They also experienced layoffs in their prime period. It is hard for them to come across the COVID-19 epidemic in their retirement. Please help us! [Location: Guanggu District, Wuhan City]

(Below are the pictures.)

This post is coded as below:

(One illustration is inserted here, please refer to the "Response to Reviewers.docx" for details)

References

1. weibo.com [Internet]. Application program interfaces for commercial data [cited 2020 Jul 21]. Available from: https://open.weibo.com/wiki/%E5%95%86%E4%B8%9A%E6%95%B0%E6%8D%AEAPI.

2. Williams J. D.C. Court: Accessing public information is not a computer crime [Internet]. Electronic Frontier Foundation; 2018 [cited 2020 Jul 21]. Available from: https://www.eff.org/deeplinks/2018/04/dc-court-accessing-public-information-not-computer-crime.

3. Shen C, Chen A, Luo C, Zhang J, Feng B, Liao W. Using reports of symptoms and diagnoses on social media to predict COVID-19 case counts in mainland China: Observational infoveillance study. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2020 May 28 [cited 2020 Jul 17]; 22(5): e19421. Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2020/5/e19421/

4. Hu Y, Huang H, Chen A, Mao X. Weibo-COV: A large-scale COVID-19 social media dataset from Weibo. arXiv: 2005.09174 [Preprint]. 2020 [cited 2020 Jul 21]: [9 p.]. Available from: https://arxiv.org/abs/2005.09174

5. American Association for Public Opinion Research. Margin of sampling error/credibility interval [Internet]. American Association for Public Opinion Research [cited 2020 Jul 17]. Available from: https://www.aapor.org/Education-Resources/Election-Polling-Resources/Margin-of-Sampling-Error-Credibility-Interval.aspx

6. Lu J, Qiu Y. Microblogging and social change in China. Asian Perspect. 2013;37(3): 305-331.

7. Huang C, Xu X, Cai Y, Ge Q, Zeng G, Li X, et al. Mining the characteristics of COVID-19 patients in China: Analysis of social media posts. J Med Internet Res [Internet]. 2020 May 17 [cited 2020 Jul 17]; 22(5): e19087. Available from: https://www.jmir.org/2020/5/e19087/

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Valerio Capraro

16 Oct 2020

What triggers online help-seeking retransmission during the COVID-19 period? Empirical evidence from Chinese social media

PONE-D-20-18830R1

Dear Dr. Li,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Valerio Capraro

22 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-18830R1

What triggers online help-seeking retransmission during the COVID-19 period? Empirical evidence from Chinese social media

Dear Dr. Li:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES