Abstract
The right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) is critical to response inhibition. The right IFC referred in the human studies of response inhibition is located in the posterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus and the surrounding regions and consists of multiple areas that implement distinct functions. Recent studies using resting-state functional connectivity have parcellated the cerebral cortex and revealed across-subject variability of parcel-based cerebrocortical networks. However, how the right IFC of individual brains is functionally organized and what functional properties the IFC parcels possess regarding response inhibition remain elusive. In the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study, precision functional mapping of individual human brains was adopted to the parcels in the right IFC to evaluate their functional properties related to response inhibition. The right IFC consisted of six modules or subsets of subregions, and the spatial organization of the modules varied considerably across subjects. Each module revealed unique characteristics of brain activity and its correlation to behavior related to response inhibition. These results provide updated functional features of the IFC and demonstrate the importance of individual-focused approaches in studying response inhibition in the right IFC.
Keywords: areal parcellation, boundary mapping, functional connectivity, inferior frontal gyrus, stop-signal task
Introduction
Studies of neuropsychology, neuromodulation, and neuroimaging have highlighted the importance of the human right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) in response inhibition (Aron et al. 2004b). Damage to the right inferior frontal gyrus in the IFC (Aron et al. 2003), specifically the pars opercularis (Aron et al. 2004a), results in prolonged stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), which is a behavioral measure to evaluate response inhibition performance in the stop-signal task. Transcranial magnetic stimulation to the pars opercularis in the inferior frontal gyrus also prolonged the SSRT (Chambers et al. 2006). On the other hand, neuroimaging studies have reported brain activation during response inhibition in wider regions in the right IFC, including the pars opercularis, pars triangularis, inferior frontal junction (IFJ), and ventral part of the precentral sulcus (Hirose et al. 2009; Swick et al. 2011; Aron et al. 2014). A correlation between the brain activity and SSRT has also been reported in these regions in the right IFC (Aron et al. 2007; Congdon et al. 2010; Jimura et al. 2014). Moreover, subregions in the right IFC have been demonstrated to implement distinct functions related to response inhibition. For example, the ventral part of the IFC is more critical to updating action plans, whereas the dorsal part of the IFC is involved in the visual detection of changes in the environment (Chikazoe et al. 2009; Verbruggen et al. 2010). These prior studies highlight the need for a more comprehensive and precise understanding of the areal organization of the right IFC.
Functional connectivity can be used to parcellate the cerebral cortex into subregions (cortical parcels) that approximate functional areas (Mars et al. 2012; Wig et al. 2014b; Eickhoff et al. 2018; Van Essen et al. 2019). Moreover, precision functional mapping focusing on single subjects has been advocated as a model for neuroimaging studies examining the organization of healthy and diseased individual human brains (Finn et al. 2015; Laumann et al. 2015; Xu et al. 2016; Braga and Buckner 2017; Gordon et al. 2017b; Noble et al. 2017; Gratton et al. 2018; Kong et al. 2019; Greene et al. 2020; see Gratton et al. 2020 for review). In the present study, for a more comprehensive and precise understanding of the areal organization of the right IFC at the single-subject level, we parcellated the right IFC in individual brains and evaluated the functional properties related to brain activation and brain-behavior correlation during response inhibition. The IFC was defined in the present study as the pars opercularis and its surrounding regions in the frontal cortex (the pars triangularis, IFJ, and precentral sulcus). Based on group data, six parcels were identified in the right IFC (Fujimoto et al. 2020). Parcels in the right IFC of individual subjects were classified into the six “modules” or subsets of parcels (Bullmore and Sporns 2009; Nelson et al. 2010), which were defined by the spatial pattern of the group-level parcel-cortex functional connectivity. The functional properties of each module were then evaluated in terms of the brain activation and its correlation with SSRT during the performance of the stop-signal task at the single-subject level.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Twenty right-handed subjects (10 men and 10 women, age: 26.6 ± 9.2 years [mean ± SD]) participated in the experiments. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Juntendo University School of Medicine.
Magnetic resonance imaging Procedures
Image data were acquired using a 3-T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner and a 64-channel radio frequency head coil (Siemens Prisma, Germany). T1- and T2-weighted structural images were first obtained (resolution = 0.8 × 0.8 × 0.8 mm3). Functional images were then obtained using multiband gradient-echo echo-planar sequences (TR = 1.0 s, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 62°, FOV = 192 × 192 mm2, matrix size = 96 × 96, 78 contiguous slices, voxel size = 2.0 × 2.0 × 2.0 mm3, multiband factor = 6, phase-encoding direction = posterior to anterior). In the resting-state scan (Osada et al. 2017, 2019; Ogawa et al. 2018; Tamura et al. 2019; Fujimoto et al. 2020; Tanaka et al. 2020), subjects were asked to fixate on a cross displayed on a screen. One run consisted of 360 volumes, and 10 runs were administered. In the task scan, subjects performed the stop-signal task (see Behavioral Procedures). One run consisted of 330 volumes, and 10 runs were administered. Before each run, one functional image was acquired with the opposite phase-encoding direction for subsequent topup distortion correction in the image preprocessing.
Areal Parcellation Procedures
For the resting-state dataset, preprocessing was conducted mainly following Human Connectome Project (HCP) Pipelines (Glasser et al. 2013, 2016). Functional images were realigned, topup distortion corrected (Andersson et al. 2003), and spatially normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template. Time-series data were cleaned using the ICA-FIX method (Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2014). After volumetric preprocessing, the image data were projected to 32k fs_LR surface space using the full version of the multimodal surface matching method (MSMAll) (Robinson et al. 2014; Glasser et al. 2016). We evaluated the amount of head motion by employing frame-wise displacement (Power et al. 2012), a measurement of instantaneous head motion that can be calculated as a locational difference between two successive volumes. Frames with frame-wise displacement > 0.25 mm as well as uncensored segments of data lasting fewer than 5 contiguous volumes were censored; all such data were excluded from the subsequent parcellation analysis.
The parcellation analyses based on boundary mapping (Margulies et al. 2007; Cohen et al. 2008; Biswal et al. 2010; Zhang et al. 2012; Zhang and Li 2012; Hirose et al. 2012b, 2013, 2016; Wig et al. 2014a; Laumann et al. 2015; Poldrack et al. 2015; Wang et al. 2015; Glasser et al. 2016; Gordon et al. 2016, 2017b; Osada et al. 2017, 2019; Ogawa et al. 2018; Fujimoto et al. 2020) were applied to the cerebral cortical surface. The mean cortical grayordinate signal was regressed out, followed by spatial smoothing (FWHM = 6 mm). Each vertex in the cortical surface of each subject was used as a seed to calculate its correlations with all the vertices. The spatial pattern similarities of the correlation maps were evaluated using correlation coefficients (similarity maps). Spatial gradients of the similarity maps were computed for each seed vertex. After spatial smoothing (FWHM = 6 mm), a two-dimensional watershed algorithm was applied to the smoothed gradient maps, and the binary watershed maps were averaged across seed vertices to generate a boundary probability map. The watershed algorithm was again applied to the boundary probability map to delineate parcellated regions (parcels) for each subject. For a group analysis, the gradient maps were averaged across the subjects, and the same procedures (spatial smoothing and watershed application) were applied to the averaged gradient maps. There were 611.8 ± 18.2 (mean ± SD) parcels identified in the cortical surfaces in individual brains, and 377 parcels identified in the group analysis, consistent with the results of previous studies of cortical parcellation (Bellec et al. 2010; Craddock et al. 2012; Shen et al. 2013; Finn et al. 2015; Laumann et al. 2015; Glasser et al. 2016; Gordon et al. 2016, 2017b; Schaefer et al. 2018).
To avoid a nonuniform signal-to-noise ratio caused by the different number of vertices in the parcels, we defined a region of interest (ROI) for each parcel for subsequent analyses. The size of the ROI was 30 vertices for a single subject analysis and 40 vertices for a group analysis that were closest to the centroid of the parcel. When the parcel contained less than these numbers of vertices, the ROI included all of the vertices in the parcel.
Modules in the Right IFC of Individual Brains
The IFC was defined in the present study as the pars opercularis, which has been shown to be critical to response inhibition, and its surrounding regions in the frontal cortex (the pars triangularis, IFJ, and precentral sulcus). The right IFC consisted of six parcels at the group level: the ventral posterior IFC (vpIFC), dorsal posterior IFC (dpIFC), IFJ, middle IFC (mIFC), ventral precentral sulcus (vPCS), and dorsal precentral sulcus (dPCS) (Fig. 1; see Supplementary Table 1 for MNI coordinates of the centroid of each parcel). The coordinates indicate that the IFJ in the present study corresponds to the same IFJ in previous studies (Derrfuss et al. 2005). Note that the anterior bank of the precentral sulcus is located in the vpIFC and dpIFC, whereas the posterior bank of the precentral sulcus is located in the vPCS and dPCS. The vpIFC corresponds to FOP1 in Glasser et al. (2016) and CinguloOperc_38 in Gordon et al. (2016). The dpIFC corresponds to area 6r and the rostral part of DorsalAttn_32. The IFJ corresponds to IFJp and the dorsal part of DorsalAttn_32. The mIFC corresponds to area 44/45 and VentralAttn_18. The vPCS corresponds to area 6v and the caudal part of DorsalAttn_32. The dPCS corresponds to PEF and DorsalAttn_31 (see also Supplementary Table 2 for other classifications).
The functional connectivity between each of the six parcels in the right IFC, and the whole cerebral cortex was calculated to generate cortical correlation maps for the six parcels in the group data (Fig. 2). The parcel-cortex functional connectivity was also calculated for individual brains to generate cortical correlation maps for the IFC parcels. Parcels in the right IFC of individual subjects were classified into the six “modules” or subsets of parcels (Bullmore and Sporns 2009; Nelson et al. 2010; Miyamoto et al. 2013), which were defined by the spatial pattern of the group-level parcel-cortex functional connectivity. Each right IFC parcel of individual brains was assigned to one of the six modules that had the most similar connectivity pattern, that is, the spatial similarity between the cortical correlation map for each IFC parcel of individual subjects and the six cortical correlation maps in the group data (Fig. 2). When the similarity of the parcels did not reach the threshold of r (correlation coefficient) = 0.5 in any of the six connectivity patterns, the parcels were not assigned to any of the six modules.
Behavioral Procedures
In the task scan, the same subjects performed the stop-signal task (Aron et al. 2003; Osada et al. 2019). The task procedures conform to the guidelines described in Verbruggen et al. (2019). The stop-signal task comprised of Go trials and Stop trials. At the beginning of a trial, a circle was presented for 500 ms at the center of the screen as a warning. In Go trials, a left- or right-pointing arrow (Go signal) was presented inside the circle, and the subjects were instructed to press a button indicating the corresponding direction with their right thumb. In Stop trials, the Go signal was first presented inside the circle, similar to Go trials. After a stop-signal delay (SSD), however, the arrow was changed to an up-pointing arrow, and the subjects were required to withhold manual response. The SSD was updated with each Stop trial based on a tracking procedure with increments or decrements of 50 ms, enabling maintenance of the accuracy of Stop trials at approximately 50%. To evaluate the efficiency of the response inhibition, this study estimated the SSRT as a behavioral index for efficient response inhibition for each subject based on an integration method (Logan and Cowan 1984; Verbruggen et al. 2013). Prior to test runs, two practice runs were performed. Each run comprised of 96 Go trials and 32 Stop trials. To verify the independent race model in the stop-signal task, the following three assumptions were confirmed (Verbruggen et al. 2019): 1) reaction time in Stop failure trials is shorter than in Go success trials, 2) reaction time in Stop failure trials increases as a function of the SSD, and 3) the probability of Stop failure trials increases as a function of SSD.
Image Analysis for Task Dataset
For the task dataset, preprocessing was conducted similarly to the resting-state dataset except for ICA-FIX denoising. The data were also projected to 32k fs_LR surface space. Task activation was analyzed in each vertex or on the basis of the parcels by averaging time-series signals across vertices. A general linear model was then applied to each vertex or each parcel using FSL (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki). Two events of interest (Go success and Stop success), together with nuisance events (Go failure and Stop failure), were coded at the onset of the Go signal of each trial, and were modeled as transient events convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function and its temporal derivative. Six parameters of head motion derived from realignment were also included in the model as covariates of no interest. Time-series data were filtered with a high-pass filter (cutoff: 64 s). The magnitude images for individual subjects were contrasted between Stop success and Go success trials.
Results
The right IFC parcels of individual brains were assigned to one of the six modules based on the six group-level cortical correlation maps shown in Figure 3. The module organization of the right IFC in four representative subjects are shown in Figure 4A (see Supplementary Fig. 1 for all the subjects). Although the overall organization of the six modules was largely maintained (Fig. 4B), there was variability in the spatial extent of the modules. Table 1 summarizes the proportion of the six modules in the IFC parcels of individual brains. The vpIFC and dpIFC modules were observed robustly in the right IFC, but the IFJ, mIFC, vPCS, and dPCS modules were absent in some subjects. Figure 4C shows the matrix of the similarity of the cortical correlation maps among the six modules, averaged across subjects. The dendrogram of the matrix indicates that, relative to other combinations, the dpIFC and IFJ modules exhibited higher similarity (Fig. 4D).
Table 1.
vpIFC | dpIFC | IFJ | mIFC | vPCS | dPCS | N.A. | Total | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Subject 01 | 2 (18%) | 1 (9%) | 2 (18%) | 2 (18%) | 2 (18%) | 2 (18%) | 0 (0%) | 11 |
Subject 02 | 4 (36%) | 2 (18%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 2 (18%) | 0 (0%) | 11 |
Subject 03 | 2 (18%) | 3 (27%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 3 (27%) | 1 (9%) | 0 (0%) | 11 |
Subject 04 | 5 (45%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (18%) | 1 (9%) | 11 |
Subject 05 | 3 (25%) | 4 (33%) | 1 (8%) | 2 (17%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (17%) | 12 |
Subject 06 | 2 (18%) | 2 (18%) | 2 (18%) | 4 (36%) | 1 (9%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 11 |
Subject 07 | 2 (18%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 2 (18%) | 3 (27%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 11 |
Subject 08 | 4 (29%) | 1 (7%) | 4 (29%) | 3 (21%) | 1 (7%) | 1 (7%) | 0 (0%) | 14 |
Subject 09 | 4 (29%) | 3 (21%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (7%) | 2 (14%) | 2 (14%) | 2 (14%) | 14 |
Subject 10 | 3 (30%) | 2 (20%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (20%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (10%) | 2 (20%) | 10 |
Subject 11 | 2 (22%) | 3 (33%) | 1 (11%) | 1 (11%) | 1 (11%) | 1 (11%) | 0 (0%) | 9 |
Subject 12 | 2 (17%) | 2 (17%) | 1 (8%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (8%) | 0 (0%) | 6 (50%) | 12 |
Subject 13 | 5 (38%) | 2 (15%) | 2 (15%) | 1 (8%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (8%) | 2 (15%) | 13 |
Subject 14 | 2 (20%) | 3 (30%) | 1 (10%) | 1 (10%) | 1 (10%) | 2 (20%) | 0 (0%) | 10 |
Subject 15 | 1 (9%) | 2 (18%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 3 (27%) | 2 (18%) | 11 |
Subject 16 | 4 (40%) | 1 (10%) | 2 (20%) | 1 (10%) | 2 (20%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 10 |
Subject 17 | 4 (40%) | 5 (50%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (10%) | 10 |
Subject 18 | 3 (27%) | 2 (18%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (9%) | 3 (27%) | 2 (18%) | 0 (0%) | 11 |
Subject 19 | 1 (9%) | 3 (27%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 1 (9%) | 3 (27%) | 1 (9%) | 11 |
Subject 20 | 3 (23%) | 3 (23%) | 2 (15%) | 2 (15%) | 1 (8%) | 2 (15%) | 0 (0%) | 13 |
Proportion | 26% | 20% | 11% | 12% | 11% | 12% | 9% | |
Presence | 20/20 | 20/20 | 16/20 | 18/20 | 15/20 | 15/20 |
N.A.: not assigned.
The subjects performed the stop-signal task in the scanner (Fig. 5A). Table 2 summarizes the behavioral data. Reaction time for Go success trials was significantly longer than that for Stop failure trials (t(19) = 9.6, P < 0.001, paired t-test). The reaction time for Stop failure trials with longer SSDs was significantly longer than that for Stop failure trials with shorter SSDs (t(19) = 12.1, P < 0.001, paired t-test). Supplementary Figure 2 shows the normalized inhibition function that were used to plot the proportion of Stop failure trials as a function of the relative finishing times (RFT) of the response and stop processes. Z-transformed RFT (ZRFT) was calculated as ZRFT = (mean[RTgo] − SSD − SSRT)/SD[RTgo]. Positive/negative ZRFT values indicate shorter/longer SSD. These results confirmed that the assumptions of the independent race model were fulfilled.
Table 2.
Reaction time [Go success] (ms) | 457.7 ± 52.4 |
SSD (ms) | 237.1 ± 62.9 |
SSRT (ms) | 208.9 ± 31.7 |
Correct response rate [Go] (%) | 98.7 ± 1.9 |
Correct response rate [Stop] (%) | 49.9 ± 1.6 |
Reaction time [Stop failure] (ms) | 426.1 ± 50.9 |
Reaction time [Stop failure|shorter SSDs] (ms) | 395.5 ± 50.0 |
Reaction time [Stop failure|longer SSDs] (ms) | 457.1 ± 56.7 |
Behavioral data for the stop-signal task under fMRI scanning (mean ± SD). Shorter/longer SSDs indicate trials with shorter/longer SSDs than median SSD.
Figure 5B shows the vertex-wise brain activity during response inhibition (contrast: Stop success minus Go success) at the group level, overlaid with the boundaries of the IFC parcels assigned to the six modules. The brain activity and its correlation with behavior in the whole brain were shown in Supplementary Figure 3. The brain activity and its correlation with behavior were further calculated on the parcel basis using the parcels of Gordon et al. (2016) (Supplementary Fig. 4). Figure 5C shows the vertex-wise brain activity during response inhibition at the individual level (see Supplementary Fig. 5 for all the subjects). The boundaries of the mIFC parcels of individual subjects appeared to demonstrate moderate correspondence with the spatial extent of the low level of brain activity, as seen in the group results.
The brain activity was quantified in the six modules of individual subjects (Fig. 6A). The brain activation was prominent in the parcels near the anterior bank of the precentral sulcus, the vpIFC module (t(19) = 4.0, P < 0.001, one sample t-test), dpIFC module (t(19) = 5.6, P < 0.001, one sample t-test), IFJ module (t(19) = 7.2, P < 0.001, one sample t-test), and dPCS module (t(19) = 7.8, P < 0.001, one sample t-test). On the other hand, the correlation between the brain activity and SSRT was significant in the vpIFC module (r = −0.53, P = 0.02), mIFC module (r = −0.49, P = 0.04), and vPCS module (r = −0.69, P = 0.004) (Fig. 6B,C). As a control, the brain activity and its correlation with SSRT were calculated by applying common group-level parcels to all the subjects. As shown in Supplementary Figure 6, the significance levels of the correlation appear less differentiated when the group-level parcels were used. For example, the correlations with SSRT in the dpIFC, mIFC, and vPCS, which were statistically mediocre in the group-level parcels, were almost absent in the dpIFC and statistically significant in the mIFC and vPCS in the individual-focused approach (Fig. 6). Since the significance level of the correlations in the individual-focused approach was marginal, an independent dataset from our previous study of the stop-signal task using larger samples (N = 46) (Jimura et al. 2014) was used to replicate the results. Each vertex in the six group-level parcels in the IFC was mapped to a three-dimensional voxel in MNI space in the dataset, and correlation between the brain activity and SSRT was calculated in each corresponding parcel. The correlations were significant in the vpIFC, mIFC, and vPCS (vpIFC: r = −0.37, P = 0.01; mIFC: r = −0.38, P = 0.01; and vPCS: r = −0.30, P = 0.04) but not significant in the dpIFC, IFJ, and dPCS (dpIFC: r = −0.19, P = 0.2; IFJ: r = −0.23, P = 0.1; and dPCS: r = −0.26, P = 0.08).
We also performed network analyses, assigning the 17 known networks in the whole cerebral cortex of individual brains using Infomap (Rosvall and Bergstrom 2008) following Power et al. (2011) and Gordon et al. (2017b) (Supplementary Fig. 7A). We then compared the six modules with the 17 networks in the right IFC of individual subjects. Supplementary Figure 7B shows the percentage of the 17 networks located in each of the six modules in the right IFC. Most of the parcels assigned as the vpIFC module belonged to the cingulo-opercular network. Most of the parcels assigned as the dpIFC or IFJ module belonged to the frontoparietal network, consistent with the high similarity of connectivity between the dpIFC and IFJ modules. Most of the parcels in the mIFC, vPCS, and dPCS modules belonged to the default mode, dorsal attention, and cingulo-opercular network, respectively. The brain activity and its correlation with behavior were further quantified for each module on the basis of the parcels belonging to the single network. The brain activity in each module that belonged to the dominant network is shown in Supplementary Figure 7C, and its correlation with behavior is shown in Supplementary Figure 7D,E. The results appeared basically similar to those presented in Figure 6.
Discussion
In the present functional MRI study, precision functional mapping of individual human brains was adopted to the parcels in the right IFC for a more comprehensive and precise understanding of the areal organization of the right IFC. The functional properties of six modules during response inhibition were evaluated. The areal organization of the right IFC varied considerably across subjects, and the six modules in the right IFC showed distinct patterns of characteristics in the brain activity and its correlation to behavior. These results provide finer details of the functional features of the IFC parcels and highlight the utility of the precision brain mapping approaches in neuroimaging and neuromodulation studies of response inhibition.
The number of parcels in the whole cerebral cortex at the group level in the present study was 377, consistent with those in previous studies of cortical parcellation: 268 in Finn et al. (2015), 360 in Glasser et al. (2016), 333 in Gordon et al. (2016), and 400 in Schaefer et al. (2018). The average number of cortical parcels of individual brains, on the other hand, was 612 in the present study, whereas those in previous studies were 616 in Laumann et al. (2015) and 621 in Gordon et al. (2017b). Thus, the number of cortical parcels of individual brains was approximately double of that at the group level, which allowed us to evaluate the variability of the location and extent of the six modules in a parcel-wise fashion. Whereas prior studies employing the precision mapping approach have investigated the individual variability of cerebrocortical networks (Mueller et al. 2013; Harrison et al. 2015; Laumann et al. 2015; Wang et al. 2015; Braga and Buckner 2017; Gordon et al. 2017a, 2017b; Gratton et al. 2018; Kong et al. 2019; Seitzman et al. 2019), the present study focused on the variability of the right IFC at the parcel level to investigate more local functional properties of the IFC parcels.
The advantage of the individual-focused approach may be seen in the results of the present study. For example, the correlations in the dpIFC, mIFC, and vPCS were not significant in the group-level parcels (Supplementary Fig. 6). In the individual-focused approach, the correlations in the mIFC and vPCS were significant, and the correlation in the dpIFC was nearly absent, as presented in Figure 6. The comparison between the group-level and individual-focused approaches suggests that the latter approach sharpens the significance level of the correlation between the brain activity and SSRT. It is to be noted that parcels in the same module sometimes appear to be “split” in a noncontiguous manner. Such phenomena can be seen in highly sampled subjects (Gordon et al. 2017b), suggesting that the individual-focused approach can detect small, spatially variable module pieces of individual brains.
The IFC parcels of individual brains were assigned to the six modules in the present study, and the same parcels were also assigned to the 17 networks using Infomap-based community detection analysis (Supplementary Fig. 7). Most of the IFC parcels in each module were assigned to a common particular network. It should be noted that most of the parcels assigned to the dpIFC and IFJ modules commonly belonged to the frontoparietal network, although the second top networks were different. The results denied a simple view that the six modules corresponded to six (out of 17) different particular networks. One likely explanation for the discrepancy would be that the six modules were not located in the centers of the 17 network clusters (Gordon et al. 2017b) in the spring-embedded plots showing the functional connectivity profiles of the whole cortical parcels (Power et al. 2011). The missing modules in some subjects (the IFJ, mIFC, vPCS, and dPCS modules) may also be explained by interindividual variability in the network configuration (Gordon et al. 2017b), which may have led to atypical configuration of the IFC modules.
Brain activation during response inhibition has been reported in cortical and subcortical regions including the IFC, presupplementary motor area (preSMA), intraparietal sulcus (IPS), and temporoparietal junction (TPJ) (Aron and Poldrack 2006; Chikazoe et al. 2009; Neubert et al. 2010; Sharp et al. 2010; Zandbelt and Vink 2010; Whelan et al. 2012; Cai et al. 2014; Erika-Florence et al. 2014; Rae et al. 2015; Watanabe et al. 2015; Yamasaki et al. 2018; Osada et al. 2019). In the IFC, the use of the individual-focused approach in the present study showed that significant brain activity during response inhibition was observed in rather limited regions: the vpIFC, dpIFC, IFJ, and dPCS modules, centered around the anterior bank of the precentral sulcus. Along with other regions outside the IFC showing the correlation between the brain activity and SSRT (Li et al. 2006, 2008; Aron et al. 2007; Forstmann et al. 2008; Hirose et al. 2012a), significant correlations were also observed in the IFC regions in the present study: the vpIFC, mIFC, and vPCS modules. The dpIFC, IFJ, and dPCS were active but were not associated with performance, and the mIFC and vPCS were not active but were associated with performance. These two types of regions were both thought to be related to response inhibition, but the functional difference was not clearly understood. A significant correlation has been reported in a region significantly activated during response inhibition (Aron et al. 2007), which is likely located in the vpIFC module that showed both significant brain activity and its correlation with behavior. Regions that showed significant brain activity, the dpIFC and IFJ modules, have also been highlighted in response inhibition (Derrfuss et al. 2005; Chikazoe et al. 2009). A significant correlation was also reported in a region that is not activated during response inhibition (Jimura et al. 2014), which is likely located in the mIFC module. Although there have been few studies, to our knowledge, that highlighted the posterior bank of the precentral sulcus, the significant brain activity in dPCS module and the significant correlation in the vPCS module may raise the possibility that they play important roles for response inhibition. More broadly, the precision functional mapping at the single-subject level may raise important hypotheses to be tested for other cognitive functions in local regions in the cerebral cortex (Miyashita 2016).
Funding
JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Number 18K07348 to T.O., 19K07807 to A.O.); Takeda Science Foundation (to S.K.).
Notes
We thank Mr T. Kamiya and Mr H. Goto for technical assistance. Conflict of Interest: None declared.
Supplementary Material
References
- Andersson JL, Skare S, Ashburner J. 2003. How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage. 20:870–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Behrens TE, Smith S, Frank MJ, Poldrack RA. 2007. Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. J Neurosci. 27:3743–3752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. 2003. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. 6:115–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Monsell S, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. 2004a. A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain. 127:1561–1573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Poldrack RA. 2006. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 26:2424–2433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. 2004b. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 8:170–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. 2014. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci. 18:177–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellec P, Rosa-Neto P, Lyttelton OC, Benali H, Evans AC. 2010. Multi-level bootstrap analysis of stable clusters in resting-state fMRI. NeuroImage. 51:1126–1139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswal BB, Mennes M, Zuo XN, Gohel S, Kelly C, Smith SM, Beckmann CF, Adelstein JS, Buckner RL, Colcombe S et al. 2010. Toward discovery science of human brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 107:4734–4739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braga RM, Buckner RL. 2017. Parallel interdigitated distributed networks within the individual estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Neuron. 95:457–471.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullmore E, Sporns O. 2009. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 10:186–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cai W, Ryali S, Chen T, Li CS, Menon V. 2014. Dissociable roles of right inferior frontal cortex and anterior insula in inhibitory control: evidence from intrinsic and task-related functional parcellation, connectivity, and response profile analyses across multiple datasets. J Neurosci. 34:14652–14667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chambers CD, Bellgrove MA, Stokes MG, Henderson TR, Garavan H, Robertson IH, Morris AP, Mattingley JB. 2006. Executive "brake failure" following deactivation of human frontal lobe. J Cogn Neurosci. 18:444–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Hirose S, Yamashita K, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. 2009. Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task. J Neurosci. 29:15870–15877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen AL, Fair DA, Dosenbach NU, Miezin FM, Dierker D, Van Essen DC, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2008. Defining functional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage. 41:45–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Congdon E, Munford JA, Cohen JR, Galvan A, Aron AR, Xue G, Miller E, Poldrack RA. 2010. Engagement of large-scale network is related to individual difference in inhibitory control. NeuroImage. 53:653–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craddock RC, James GA, Holtzheimer PE, Hu XP, Mayberg HS. 2012. A whole brain fMRI atlas generated via spatially constrained spectral clustering. Hum Brain Mapp. 33:1914–1928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derrfuss J, Brass M, Neumann J, von Cramon DY. 2005. Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and stroop studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 25:22–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT, Genon S. 2018. Imaging-based parcellations of the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 19:672–686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erika-Florence M, Leech R, Hampshire A. 2014. A functional network perspective on response inhibition and attentional control. Nat Commun. 5:4073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finn ES, Shen X, Scheinost D, Rosenberg MD, Huang J, Chun MM, Papademetris X, Constable RT. 2015. Functional connectome fingerprinting: identifying individuals using patterns of brain connectivity. Nat Neurosci. 18:1664–1671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forstmann BU, Jahfari S, Scholte HS, Wolfensteller U, van den Wildenberg WPM, Ridderinkhof KR. 2008. Function and structure of right inferior frontal cortex predict individual differences in response inhibition: a model-based approach. J Neurosci. 28:9790–9796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujimoto U, Ogawa A, Osada T, Tanaka M, Suda A, Hattori N, Kamagata K, Aoki S, Konishi S. 2020. Network centrality reveals dissociable brain activity during response inhibition in human right ventral part of inferior frontal cortex. Neuroscience. 433:163–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, Xu J, Jbabdi S, Webster M, Polimeni JR et al. 2013. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage. 80:105–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Andersson J, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M et al. 2016. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 536:171–178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Huckins JF, Kelley WM, Petersen SE. 2016. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb Cortex. 26:288–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Petersen SE. 2017a. Individual variability of the system-level organization of the human brain. Cereb Cortex. 27:386–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Gilmore AW, Newbold DJ, Greene DJ, Berg JJ, Ortega M, Hoyt-Drazen C, Gratton C, Sun H et al. 2017b. Precision functional mapping of individual human brains. Neuron. 95:791–807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton C, Kraus BT, Greene DJ, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NUF, Petersen SE. 2020. Defining individual-specific functional neuroanatomy for precision psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry. 88:28–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratton C, Laumann TO, Nielsen AN, Greene DJ, Gordon EM, Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, Coalson RS, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL et al. 2018. Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron. 98:439–452.e5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene DJ, Marek S, Gordon EM, Siegel JS, Gratton C, Laumann TO, Gilmore AW, Berg JJ, Nguyen AL, Dierker D et al. 2020. Integrative and network-specific connectivity of the basal ganglia and thalamus defined in individuals. Neuron. 105:742–758.e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison SJ, Woolrich MW, Robinson EC, Glasser MF, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. 2015. Large-scale probabilistic functional modes from resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 109:217–231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirose S, Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Yamashita K, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. 2009. Sub-centimeter scale functional organization in human inferior frontal gyrus. NeuroImage. 47:442–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirose S, Chikazoe J, Watanabe T, Jimura K, Kunimatsu A, Abe O, Ohtomo K, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. 2012a. Efficiency of go/no-gotask performance implemented in the left hemisphere. J Neurosci. 32:9059–9065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirose S, Osada T, Ogawa A, Tanaka M, Wada H, Yoshizawa Y, Imai Y, Machida T, Akahane M, Shirouzu I et al. 2016. Lateral-medial dissociation in rrbitofrontal cortex-hypothalamus connectivity. Front Hum Neurosci. 10:244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirose S, Watanabe T, Jimura K, Katsura M, Kunimatsu A, Abe O, Ohtomo K, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. 2012b. Local signal time-series during rest used for areal boundary mapping in individual human brains. PLoS One. 7:e36496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirose S, Watanabe T, Wada H, Imai Y, Machida T, Shirouzu I, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. 2013. Functional relevance of micromodules in the human association cortex delineated with high-resolution FMRI. Cereb Cortex. 23:2863–2871. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jimura K, Hirose S, Kunimatsu A, Ohtomo K, Koike Y, Konishi S. 2014. Late enhancement of brain-behavior correlations during response inhibition. Neuroscience. 274:383–392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong R, Li J, Orban C, Sabuncu MR, Liu H, Schaefer A, Sun N, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB et al. 2019. Spatial topography of individual-specific cortical networks predicts human cognition, personality, and emotion. Cereb Cortex. 29:2533–2551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laumann TO, Gordon EM, Adeyemo B, Snyder AZ, Joo SJ, Chen MY, Gilmore AW, McDermott KB, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NU et al. 2015. Functional system and areal organization of a highly sampled individual human brain. Neuron. 87:657–670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li CS, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R. 2006. Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci. 26:186–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li CS, Yan P, Shinha R, Lee TW. 2008. Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task. NeuroImage. 41:1352–1363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logan GD, Cowan WB. 1984. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 91:295–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margulies DS, Kelly AM, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. 2007. Mapping the functional connectivity of anterior cingulate cortex. NeuroImage. 37:579–588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars RB, Sallet J, Schüffelgen U, Jbabdi S, Toni I, Rushworth MF. 2012. Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right "temporoparietal junction area": evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cereb Cortex. 22:1894–1903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyamoto K, Osada T, Adachi Y, Matsui T, Kimura HM, Miyashita Y. 2013. Functional differentiation of memory retrieval network in macaque posterior parietal cortex. Neuron. 77:787–799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyashita Y. 2016. The cutting edge in brain science and sportology. Juntendo Med J. 62(Suppl 1):6–11. [Google Scholar]
- Mueller S, Wang D, Fox MD, Yeo BT, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Shafee R, Lu J, Liu H. 2013. Individual variability in functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Neuron. 77:586–595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson SM, Cohen AL, Power JD, Wig GS, Miezin FM, Wheeler ME, Velanova K, Donaldson DI, Phillips JS, Schlaggar BL et al. 2010. A parcellation scheme for human left lateral parietal cortex. Neuron. 67:156–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neubert FX, Mars RB, Buch ER, Olivier E, Rushworth MF. 2010. Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 107:13240–13245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noble S, Spann MN, Tokoglu F, Shen X, Constable RT, Scheinost D. 2017. Influences on the test-retest reliability of functional connectivity MRI and its relationship with behavioral utility. Cereb Cortex. 27:5415–5429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa A, Osada T, Tanaka M, Hori M, Aoki S, Nikolaidis A, Milham MP, Konishi S. 2018. Striatal subdivisions that coherently interact with multiple cerebrocortical networks. Hum Brain Mapp. 39:4349–4359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osada T, Ohta S, Ogawa A, Tanaka M, Suda A, Kamagata K, Hori M, Aoki S, Shimo Y, Hattori N et al. 2019. An essential role of the intraparietal sulcus in response inhibition predicted by parcellation-based network. J Neurosci. 39:2509–2521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osada T, Suzuki R, Ogawa A, Tanaka M, Hori M, Aoki S, Tamura Y, Watada H, Kawamori R, Konishi S. 2017. Functional subdivisions of the hypothalamus using areal parcellation and their signal changes related to glucose metabolism. NeuroImage. 162:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack RA, Laumann TO, Koyejo O, Gregory B, Hover A, Chen MY, Gorgolewski KJ, Luci J, Joo SJ, Boyd RL et al. 2015. Long-term neural and physiological phenotyping of a single human. Nat Commun. 6:8885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2012. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage. 59:2142–2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL et al. 2011. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 72:665–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rae CL, Hughes LE, Anderson MC, Rowe JB. 2015. The prefrontal cortex achieves inhibitory control by facilitating subcortical motor pathway connectivity. J Neurosci. 35:786–794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson EC, Jbabdi S, Glasser MF, Andersson J, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Smith SM, Van Essen DC, Jenkinson M. 2014. MSM: a new flexible framework for multimodal surface matching. NeuroImage. 100:414–426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosvall M, Bergstrom CT. 2008. Maps of random walks on complex networks reveal community structure. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 105:1118–1123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salimi-Khorshidi G, Douaud G, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Griffanti L, Smith SM. 2014. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. 90:449–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT. 2018. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex. 28:3095–3114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seitzman BA, Gratton C, Laumann TO, Gordon EM, Adeyemo B, Dworetsky A, Kraus BT, Gilmore AW, Berg JJ, Ortega M et al. 2019. Trait-like variants in human functional brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 116:22851–22861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp DJ, Bonnelle V, De Boissezon X, Beckmann CF, James SG, Patel MC, Mehta MA. 2010. Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 107:6106–6111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen X, Tokoglu F, Papademetris X, Constable RT. 2013. Groupwise whole-brain parcellation from resting-state fMRI data for network node identification. NeuroImage. 82:403–415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U. 2011. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. NeuroImage. 56:1655–1665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamura K, Osada T, Ogawa A, Tanaka M, Suda A, Shimo Y, Hattori N, Kamagata K, Hori M, Aoki S et al. 2019. MRI-based visualization of rTMS-induced cortical plasticity in the primary motor cortex. PLoS One. 14:e0224175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka M, Osada T, Ogawa A, Kamagata K, Aoki S, Konishi S. 2020. Dissociable networks of the lateral/medial mammillary body in the human brain. Front Hum Neurosci. 14:228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Essen DC, Donahue CJ, Coalson TS, Kennedy H, Hayashi T, Glasser MF. 2019. Cerebral cortical folding, parcellation, and connectivity in humans, nonhuman primates, and mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 116:26173–26180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Stevens MA, Chambers CD. 2010. Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 107:13966–13971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Logan GD. 2013. Fictitious inhibitory differences: how skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychol Sci. 24:352–362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Band GP, Beste C, Bissett PG, Brockett AT, Brown JW, Chamberlain SR, Chambers CD, Colonius H et al. 2019. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. elife. 8:e46323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang D, Buckner RL, Fox MD, Holt DJ, Holmes AJ, Stoecklein S, Langs G, Pan R, Qian T, Li K et al. 2015. Parcellating cortical functional networks in individuals. Nat Neurosci. 18:1853–1860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe T, Hanajima R, Shirota Y, Tsutsumi R, Shimizu T, Hayashi T, Terao Y, Ugawa Y, Katsura M, Kunimatsu A et al. 2015. Effects of rTMS of pre-supplementary motor area on fronto basal ganglia network activity during stop-signal task. J Neurosci. 35:4813–4823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whelan R, Conrod PJ, Poline JB, Lourdusamy A, Banaschewski T, Barker GJ, Bellgrove MA, Büchel C, Byrne M, Cummins TD et al. 2012. Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks. Nat Neurosci. 15:920–925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wig GS, Laumann TO, Cohen AL, Power JD, Nelson SM, Glasser MF, Miezin FM, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2014a. Parcellating an individual subject's cortical and subcortical brain structures using snowball sampling of resting-state correlations. Cereb Cortex. 24:2036–2054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wig GS, Laumann TO, Petersen SE. 2014b. An approach for parcellating human cortical areas using resting-state correlations. NeuroImage. 93:276–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu T, Opitz A, Craddock RC, Wright MJ, Zuo XN, Milham MP. 2016. Assessing variations in areal organization for the intrinsic brain: from fingerprints to reliability. Cereb Cortex. 26:4192–4211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamasaki T, Ogawa A, Osada T, Jimura K, Konishi S. 2018. Within-subject correlation analysis to detect functional areas associated with response inhibition. Front Hum Neurosci. 12:208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zandbelt BB, Vink M. 2010. On the role of the striatum in response inhibition. PLoS One. 5:e13848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Ide JS, Li CS. 2012. Resting-state functional connectivity of the medial superior frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 22:99–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang S, Li CS. 2012. Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. NeuroImage. 59:3548–3562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.