Skip to main content
UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts logoLink to UKPMC Funders Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Jun 10.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2018 Mar 4;49(15):2499–2504. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000454

Polygenic risk for schizophrenia and season of birth within the UK Biobank cohort

Valentina Escott-Price 1,, Daniel J Smith 2, Kimberley Kendall 1, Joey Ward 2, George Kirov 1, Michael J Owen 1, James Walters 1, Michael C O’Donovan 1
PMCID: PMC7610956  EMSID: EMS126976  PMID: 29501066

Abstract

Background

There is strong evidence that people born in winter and in spring have a small increased risk of schizophrenia. As this ‘season of birth’ effect underpins some of the most influential hypotheses concerning potentially modifiable risk exposures, it is important to exclude other possible explanations for the phenomenon.

Methods

Here we sought to determine whether the season of birth effect reflects gene–environment confounding rather than a pathogenic process indexing environmental exposure. We directly measured, in 136 538 participants from the UK Biobank (UKBB), the burdens of common schizophrenia risk alleles and of copy number variants known to increase the risk for the disorder, and tested whether these were correlated with a season of birth.

Results

Neither genetic measure was associated with season or month of birth within the UKBB sample.

Conclusions

As our study was highly powered to detect small effects, we conclude that the season of birth effect in schizophrenia reflects a true pathogenic effect of environmental exposure.

Keywords: Polygenic risk score, schizophrenia, season of birth

Introduction

People born in winter and early spring are at an elevated risk for schizophrenia (Bradbury & Miller, 1985; Boyd et al. 1986; Baron & Gruen, 1988; Mortensen et al. 1999). Although the effect is small, with an increase in the risk of about 10%, this ‘season of birth’ effect is one of the most robust findings in schizophrenia epidemiology (Davies et al. 2003). It has also been influential in developing hypotheses of schizophrenia pathogenesis, forming one of the central tenets of the viral infection hypothesis of the disorder (Torrey et al. 1977), as well as other less intensively investigated putative mechanisms such as vitamin D deficiency during foetal development (McGrath, 1999). However, at present, the mechanisms underpinning the season of birth effect are not known.

Schizophrenia is also known to be highly heritable (Cardno & Gottesman, 2000; Sullivan et al. 2003) and polygenic (International Schizophrenia et al. 2009; Purcell et al. 2014). Genomic studies have identified large numbers of risk alleles that contribute to the risk of the disorder, with risk being conferred by large numbers of variants spanning the full spectrum of population frequencies (Rees et al. 2014; Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2014; Singh et al. 2016). The relative contributions of alleles of various frequencies are not fully resolved, but recent studies estimate that common alleles, captured by genome-wide association study (GWAS) arrays, capture between a third and one half of the genetic variance in liability (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2014).

A key assumption underlying research into the causes of the season of birth effect is that season acts as a proxy for one or more environmental exposures (e.g. virus infection). While an environmental origin for the season of birth effect seems the most plausible explanation, it is also possible that the effect is the result of gene-environment correlation. There are already examples where correlation of genetic and apparent environmental risks have been observed in psychiatric research; one such example, the link between maternal smoking and attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), is at least partly driven by shared genetic liability to both ADHD and smoking rather than by exposure to smoking per se (Thapar et al. 2009). The link between obstetric complications and schizophrenia may be confounded by the correlation between genetic liability to both (Ellman et al. 2007). This is also likely to apply to cannabis and psychosis (Power et al. 2014; Vaucher et al. 2017) and indeed to the link between all substance use disorders and psychosis (Adan et al. 2017).

Gene-environment correlations are sometimes classified as passive, active and evocative; regardless of the nature of the correlation, each predicts people born in the winter should have elevated genetic liability to the disorder, even those who do not manifest the disorder. In contrast, if the association between winter birth and schizophrenia is not the result of gene-environment correlation, there should be no link between winter birth and genetic liability to the disorder. Evocative correlation is said to occur when an individual’s behaviour evokes an environmental response and is therefore not clearly applicable to the phenomenon under investigation. The passive gene-environment correlation could, in principle, operate if parents with elevated trait liability have seasonal patterns of sexual activity that favour winter birth. If this is the case, then their offspring are expected to have elevated liability to the disorder (on average the mean liability of the parents which will be higher than the population as a whole) and liability to winter birth. While this seems a priori unlikely, previous studies have shown that high schizophrenia trait liability is associated with a wide range of behaviours, amongst which of potential relevance are seasonal fluctuations in mood and activity levels (Byrne et al. 2015). Confounding through active gene–environment correlation is also possible if enhanced genetic risk for the disorder impacts upon seasonal patterns of foetal loss. Given the potential impacts on prevention and treatment of detecting modifiable environmental exposures, it is important to rigorously exclude alternative explanations for the season of birth effect on schizophrenia risk.

Population studies using family history have generally suggested that the season of birth effect is not a manifestation of genetic liability (Hettema et al. 1996; Suvisaari et al. 2004). However, the majority of people who develop schizophrenia do not have a history of the disorder in a close relative (Svensson et al. 2012). Advances in molecular genetics now allow genetic liability to be directly estimated by individuals regardless of their affected status or family history. Liability conferred by common risk alleles can be estimated through a process known as polygenic risk scoring; in a given individual, their polygenic risk score (PRS) represents the burden of common risk alleles carried by that individual. PRS studies have repeatedly been demonstrated to provide a useful index of genetic liability to the disorder (International Schizophrenia et al. 2009; Ripke et al. 2013; Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2014), and are increasingly being applied to investigate whether environmental risk factors operate independently of genetic liability (Power et al. 2014).

Here, we implement PRS analysis of the UK Biobank (UKBB) (Sudlow et al. 2015) to determine whether season and month of birth are associated with genetic risk for schizophrenia. The UKBB is a large prospective cohort of more than half a million residents of the UK for which genetic data and seasonality of birth data are available. We generated schizophrenia PRS for every participant in the UKBB cohort (June 2015 release, N = 136 538) and tested whether this score was associated with month or season of birth. As a secondary test of the plausibility of the season of birth being a genetically correlated confound, we also conducted a GWAS of the season of birth, aiming to estimate whether this phenotype is heritable.

Rare alleles in the form of pathogenic copy number variants (CNVs) are also known make a contribution to schizophrenia. Although the contribution to liability from CNVs at a population level (Purcell et al. 2014) is much smaller than that of common alleles, for completeness we also tested whether the frequencies of 93 pathogenic CNVs that have been linked with neurodevelopmental disorders (Dittwald et al. 2013; Coe et al. 2014; Kendall et al. 2017) were associated with season of birth.

UKBB obtained informed consent from all participants and this study was conducted under generic approval from the NHS National Research Ethics Service (approval letter dated 13 May 2016, Ref 16/NW/0274) and under UKBB approvals for applications #6553 (Smith) and # 14421 (Kirov).

Material and methods

To define risk alleles, we used the largest available schizophrenia GWAS comprising a meta-analysis of two large studies (Pardinas 2017, Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2014) which included 40 675 schizophrenia cases and 64 643 controls (Pardiñas et al. 2018). As PRS requires genome-wide data, we did not include the extension dataset provided to the PGC for selected SNPs by deCODE genetics. Both schizophrenia datasets were imputed using the SHAPEIT/IMPUTE2 software (Howie et al. 2012; Delaneau et al. 2013) with a combination of the 1000 Genomes phase 3 (1KGPp3) and UK10 K datasets as a reference panel.

For the UKBB study, given that the schizophrenia GWAS we used to define risk alleles was of primarily European Ancestry, we restricted the sample to those who self-reported as being of white UK ancestry (n = 136 538). For constructing PRS, we downloaded data that are publically available (https://www.med.unc.edu/pgc/results-and-downloads). We included autosomal SNPs that passed stringent quality control criteria [minor allele frequencies (MAF) ≥0.01] and imputation quality score greater than or equal to 0.9. This resulted in 5 471 613 SNPs. Using the UKBB genotypes, we pruned the SNPs keeping those which are the most significantly associated with schizophrenia in the region while excluding SNPs at which the genotypes are correlated with the selected SNPs with r 2 ≥ 0.2. A physical distance threshold for pruning SNPs was set to 1 Mb and p value threshold was 0.5. After pruning, 118 302 independent SNPs remained. We selected markers, based upon significance thresholds, to construct a polygenic score in the UKBB data. The PRS was calculated from the effect size-weighted sum of associated alleles within each subject. PRS was standardised by subtracting the population mean for PRS and dividing by the standard deviation.

As a test of robustness, we constructed PRS based on risk alleles passing a range of schizophrenia association thresholds in the PGC2 + CLOZUK data (e.g. significant at p ≤ 0.01, 0.05, 0.1, …, 0.5). The primary analysis was based on p <0.05 as this is the threshold that currently maximally captures polygenic risk (Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, 2014). The PRSs of people born in each month/season were compared to those born in January/winter (baseline) using linear regression analysis with season/month coded as a factor (glm() function in R). All analyses in the UKBB were adjusted for the array (the UKBB used two different arrays) and the first 8 principal components (PCs), reflecting underlying stratification in the sample due to population and/or genotyping differences. The first eight PCs, out of 15 available in the Biobank, were selected after visual inspection of each pair of PCs, taking forward only those that resulted in multiple clusters of individuals [see (Smith et al. 2016) for detail].

AVENGEME provides a set of R functions that allow power for PRS analyses to be calculated (Dudbridge, 2013). For this to be applicable, a priori, this method assumes that (1) there is a nonzero SNP-heritability for the season of birth and (2) there is a genetic correlation between SZ and the season of birth. As we show in the present study that both assumptions are violated, this widely used approach is not applicable. However, to illustrate that we had high power to detect the sought after effects, we calculated the power of our PRS analysis for the reported effect size OR 1.07 (Davies et al. 2003) comparing winter/spring v. summer/autumn births with pwr.norm.test() function in R.

Genome-wide association analysis (GWA) of the season of birth was performed using a binary phenotype defined as ‘0’ for individuals born in winter and spring (December–May) and ‘1’ for those born in summer and autumn (June–November). Association analysis was conducted using logistic regression with an array, and the first eight principal components as covariates (as described above). Genotype dosage data were initially converted to the most probable genotype format (with the probability >0.9), then filtered by removal of SNPs with Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium p <10–6, MAF <0.01, info <0.4, data on <95% of the sample after excluding genotype calls made with less than 90% posterior probability after which 8 989 945 variants were retained. LD-Score regression analysis (Bulik-Sullivan et al. 2015) was employed to estimate SNP-based heritability in this dataset.

The numbers of subjects with and without pathogenic CNVs born in winter/spring v. summer/autumn were compared with a χ2 test. The list of 93 pathogenic CNVs was compiled from two widely accepted sources (Dittwald et al. 2013; Coe et al. 2014) as we have previously reported (Kendall et al. 2017). The CNV calls for UKBB participants were made in-house as we have previously reported in a UKBB CNV study (Kendall et al. 2017).

Results

The frequencies of birth by month are presented in Fig. 1. The distribution of frequencies in our study is similar to the distribution in the whole UKBB population (N = 502 632; biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/field.cgi?id=52).

Fig. 1. Frequency of birth by month in the UK Biobank population.

Fig. 1

Correlations between the timing of birth and polygenic risk

We found no association between schizophrenia PRS and season of birth in the UKBB sample, with winter as the baseline. Figure 2 shows the distribution of the standardised PRS for each season. The figure clearly demonstrates no difference in mean or variance of PRS. Detailed results are presented in Table 1, with baseline winter as the season of birth. Similarly, we found no differences in the PRS by month of birth with January as the baseline (Fig. 3; online Supplementary Table S1). Note that the specification of the baseline season is arbitrary; the results remain the same regardless which season is used.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Distribution of schizophrenia polygenic risk scores (PRS) of 136 538 UK Biobank participants with respect to their season of birth. Schizophrenia polygenic risk scores were constructed using schizophrenia risk SNPs with association p value ≤0.05.

Table 1. Comparison of schizophrenia PRS of individuals in the UK Biobank sample split by season of births.

SNP selection threshold NSNPs Spring Summer Autumn
B p value B p value B p value
10–4 1749 –0.014 0.070 0.004 0.648 –0.001 0.899
0.001 4517 –0.009 0.222 0.008 0.303 0.004 0.607
0.01 13 700 –0.005 0.506 0.010 0.199 0.007 0.385
0.05 32 576 –0.005 0.538 0.003 0.737 0.003 0.746
0.1 48 188 –0.010 0.190 –0.002 0.766 0.000 0.951
0.2 72 075 –0.012 0.109 –0.004 0.559 0.001 0.893
0.3 90 443 –0.014 0.070 –0.008 0.313 –0.001 0.883
0.4 105 255 –0.014 0.059 –0.008 0.284 –0.003 0.731
0.5 117 618 –0.014 0.070 –0.008 0.271 –0.002 0.786

The schizophrenia PRS are generated using schizophrenia risk SNPs atdifferent thresholds for the association. The baseline category ofthe analysis is winter birth and therefore a negative B indicates a decrease in risk of schizophrenia in those born in the season, which is shown in the header row, compared to winter.

The first column shows the p value threshold for SNP selection from the GWAS discovery. The numbers of SNPs, which passed the selection criterion, are shown in the second column. The following three sections of the table present effect sizes (B) and p values comparing each season with winter, estimated simultaneously in a single nominal regression model for each SNP selection threshold. The row in bold represents the primary analysis (P threshold for risk SNPs p ≤0.05); other rows are exploratory

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Distribution of schizophrenia polygenic risk scores of 136 538 UK Biobank participants with respect to their month of birth. Schizophrenia polygenic risk scores were constructed using schizophrenia risk SNPs with association p value ≤0.05.

Given that previous studies have frequently identified both winter and spring as the season of elevated risk, we also collapsed those born in winter and spring into a single group and compared their PRS with those born in summer or autumn. Our findings were not consistent with a season of birth–PRS correlation; indeed those born in winter and spring actually had a slightly lower PRS than those born in summer or autumn, and this reached nominal significance for some of the secondary tests (Table 2).

Table 2. Comparison of schizophrenia PRS of individuals in the UK Biobank sample, born in winter-spring v. summer-autumn (‘Whole sample’ section) and bottom v. top deciles of schizophrenia PRS of individuals in the UK Biobank sample (‘Bottom v. Top deciles of schizophrenia PRS’ section).

SNP selection threshold Whole sample Bottom v. Top deciles of schizophrenia PRS
OR p value OR p value
1.00 × 10–04 1.009 0.114 1.034 0.415
0.001 1.011 0.041 1.013 0.759
0.01 1.011 0.040 1.051 0.229
0.05 1.005 0.325 1.075 0.082
0.1 1.004 0.433 1.101 0.020
0.2 1.005 0.350 1.047 0.266
0.3 1.003 0.546 1.035 0.401
0.4 1.003 0.622 1.019 0.646
0.5 1.003 0.638 1.011 0.787

The baseline of the analysis is winter-spring birth combined. The schizophrenia PRS are generated using schizophrenia risk SNPs at different thresholds for association (column 1). OR (column 2) is the exponentiation of the B-coefficient provided by logistic regression

To check whether this sample maybe underpowered to detect low-effect sizes, we calculated the power of the PRS analysis for the reported effect size OR 1.07 (Davies et al. 2003). The effect size for power calculation is usually estimated as (B 0B)/σ, where B 0 is the effect size under the null hypothesis [in our case B 0 = log(1) = 0], B is the effect size under the alternative hypothesis [in our case B = log(1.07) = 0.068] and σ is the standard deviation. Since the PRS were standardised in our analyses, σ = 1 under both null and alternative hypotheses. Thus, the effect size for the power calculation was d = 0.068/sqrt(2) = 0.048 and the significance level was set to α = 10–4, to account for multiple testing. The power to detect an association between PRS and season of birth was >99.9%. We also estimated d = 0.0128 as the smallest effect size which our sample is capable to detect with 80% power, which corresponds to OR 1.018 (B = 0.018) excess for winter/spring births compared to summer/autumn births. As the power to detect an association between PRS and season of birth is 80%, assuming association effect size as small as B = 0.018 at 10–4 significance level (accounting for all PRS tests), we exclude seasonal variation in PRS as a being correlated with a season of birth effect.

To evaluate effects that might only be observed at the extremes of liability, we also compared ‘winter and spring’ v. ‘summer and autumn’ births for the top and bottom deciles of the PRS distribution; again we found no evidence for association at the primary testing threshold for risk alleles from the PGC study. We did observe a nominally significant association (p = 0.02) when risk alleles were selected from the PGC GWAS at p <0.1, but again, the effect size was again inconsistent with that expected for a genotype-phenotype correlation, people in the top 10% for genetic loading to schizophrenia having a slightly higher chance of being born in summer or autumn compared with the bottom 10% of people (Table 2).

Genome-wide association study of the timing of birth

A Manhattan plot of the GWAS of the season of birth is given as online Supplementary Fig. S1. No genome-wide significant associations were identified, there was no evidence for inflation in the test statistics indicative of polygenic inheritance [genomic control (Devlin & Roeder, 1999) λ = 0.992] (see QQ-plot in Supplementary Figure 2), and no evidence that SNP heritability contributed to this phenotype (total liability scale h2 = –0.002 s.e. = 0.0052) as estimated by LD-score regression.

CNVs and timing of birth

There was no difference in the frequencies of pathogenic CNVs between groups of UKBB participants born in winter and spring compared with those born in summer or autumn [frequencies 0.017 (95% CI 0.016–0.018) and 0.016 (95%CI 0.015–0.017), respectively; χ2 test p = 0.137].

Conclusion

An excess of winter and spring births in people with schizophrenia is one of the most robustly supported and influential epidemiological findings in psychiatry (1–5). Here, we test and fail to support the hypothesis that the excess of winter and spring births in people with schizophrenia is an effect of gene-environment correlation. Not only do we fail to find evidence that schizophrenia liability in the form of common alleles or rare CNVs is associated with season of birth, our GWAS suggests that season of birth is not even a heritable trait, which alone makes such a correlation an untenable explanation for the season of birth effect (with respect to common variation). Rejecting a genetic–environmental correlation, we conclude that our study strongly supports the widely held view that the excess of winter births in schizophrenia is the result of an as yet unknown environmental risk exposure with a seasonal gradient.

Although not a direct aim of our study, we also note that the absence of heritability effects on season of birth implies that other traits that exhibit relative age effects, for example, personality traits, sporting ability and general academic performance (Jeronimus et al. 2015), are similarly unlikely to represent gene–environment correlation; rather, as widely interpreted, they most likely result from differential levels of maturity in school and other cohort intakes. However, some cognitive phenotypes exhibit season of birth fluctuations beyond effects attributable simply to the timing of school intake (Grootendorst-van Mil et al. 2017). It is therefore not possible to exclude the possibility that an as yet to be discovered seasonally fluctuating risk factor that increases the risk of schizophrenia also contributes to the season of birth effects which have been observed for these other cognitive phenotypes.

Our study has a number of major strengths: the use of the largest schizophrenia dataset to date to identify genetic risk loci; the largest available genotyped population cohorts to generate schizophrenia PRS; and the ability to measure liability to schizophrenia directly at a molecular level. Another strength conferred by the large population sample is the power to test the sample month by month. This is important given that many studies vary in their definition of winter–spring and in the month for which risk is maximal. Together, these strengths allow us to test the genetic confounding hypothesis with extremely high power; as a result, our failure to find evidence in support of that hypothesis allows us to refute it as an explanation. In doing so, our results are consistent with and complementary to, studies that have indirectly measured genetic liability based on family history (Hettema et al. 1996; Suvisaari et al. 2004; Svensson et al. 2012).

Our study has a number of limitations. One potential limitation of the study is that, like all environmental exposures, possible variance in the exposure rate to the pathogenic agent might mean our conclusion could be country or birth cohort specific. However, the season of birth effects (and therefore exposure to the putative pathogenic environmental exposures) have been widely documented in Northern European samples (5). They have also been shown to operate in the UK from at least 1921 up till the modern era, with the most recent study in the UK suggesting January births are associated with an OR for schizophrenia of 1.17 (Disanto et al. 2012). Another potential limitation is that our analyses did not account for the possibility that an individual’s circadian biology or chronotype might interact with a season of birth effects (Natale & Adan, 1999; Natale et al. 2009).

Finally, our PRS analysis, and heritability estimates were based upon common SNPs, and do not include a possible contribution from rare SNPs. However, the frequencies of rare CNVs, linked to neurodevelopmental disorders, also did not differ by season of birth, and it seems unlikely that rare mutations that increase liability to schizophrenia would have different effects on mating behaviours than the burden of common alleles. Nevertheless, when sufficient data become available, it may be useful to test for seasonal burdens of rare and de novo mutation events.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000454

Acknowledgements

This research was conducted using the UK Biobank resource. UK Biobank was established by the Wellcome Trust, Medical Research Council, Department of Health, Scottish Government and Northwest Regional Development Agency. UK Biobank has also funding from the Welsh Assembly Government and the British Heart Foundation. Data collection was funded by UK Biobank. The work at Cardiff University was supported by Medical Research Council (MRC) Centre (MR/L010305/1) and Program Grants (G0800509). The CLOZUK sample was genotyped with funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement n° 279227 (CRESTAR Consortium; http://www.crestar-project.eu/). JW is supported by a JMAS Sim Fellowship from the Royal College of Physicians of Edinburgh and DJS is supported by a Lister Institute Prize Fellowship. KK is supported by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Fellowship. The work at Cardiff University was supported by Medical Research Council (MRC) Centre (MR/L010305/1) and Program Grants (G0800509). The CLOZUK sample was genotyped with funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme for research, technological development and demonstration under grant agreement n° 279227 (CRESTAR Consortium; http://www.crestar-project.eu/). JW is supported by a JMAS Sim Fellowship from the Royal College of Physicians of Edinburgh and DJS is supported by a Lister Institute Prize Fellowship. KK is supported by a Wellcome Trust Clinical Research Fellowship.

References

  1. Adan A, Arredondo AY, Capella MD, Prat G, Forero DA, Navarro JF. Neurobiological underpinnings and modulating factors in schizophrenia spectrum disorders with a comorbid substance use disorder: a systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;75:361–377. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baron M, Gruen R. Risk factors in schizophrenia. Season of birth and family history. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1988;152:460–465. doi: 10.1192/bjp.152.4.460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boyd JH, Pulver AE, Stewart W. Season of birth: schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1986;12(2):173–186. doi: 10.1093/schbul/12.2.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bradbury TN, Miller GA. Season of birth in schizophrenia: a review of evidence, methodology, and etiology. Psychological Bulletin. 1985;98(3):569–594. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bulik-Sullivan BK, Loh PR, Finucane HK, Ripke S, Yang J, Schizophrenia Working Group of the PGC. Patterson N, et al. LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nature Genetics. 2015;47(3):291–295. doi: 10.1038/ng.3211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Psychiatric Genetics Consortium Major Depressive Disorder Working Group. Byrne EM, Raheja UK, Stephens SH, Heath AC, Madden PA, Vaswani D, et al. Seasonality shows evidence for polygenic architecture and genetic correlation with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2015;76(2):128–134. doi: 10.4088/JCP.14m08981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cardno AG, Gottesman II. Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics. American Journal of Medical Genetics. 2000;97(1):12–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Coe BP, Witherspoon K, Rosenfeld JA, van Bon BW, Vulto-van Silfhout AT, Bosco P, et al. Refining analyses of copy number variation identifies specific genes associated with developmental delay. Nature Genetics. 2014;46(10):1063–1071. doi: 10.1038/ng.3092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Davies G, Welham J, Chant D, Torrey EF, McGrath J. A systematic review and meta-analysis of northern hemisphere season of birth studies in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2003;29(3):587–593. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Delaneau O, Zagury JF, Marchini J. Improved whole-chromosome phasing for disease and population genetic studies. Nature Methods. 2013;10(1):5–6. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Devlin B, Roeder K. Genomic control for association studies. Biometrics. 1999;55(4):997–1004. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341x.1999.00997.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Disanto G, Morahan JM, Lacey MV, DeLuca GC, Giovannoni G, Ebers GC, et al. Seasonal distribution of psychiatric births in England. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(4):e34866. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dittwald P, Gambin T, Szafranski P, Li J, Amato S, Divon MY, et al. NAHR-mediated copy-number variants in a clinical population: mechanistic insights into both genomic disorders and Mendelizing traits. Genome Research. 2013;23(9):1395–1409. doi: 10.1101/gr.152454.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dudbridge F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genetics. 2013;9(3):e1003348. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1003348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ellman LM, Huttunen M, Lonnqvist J, Cannon TD. The effects of genetic liability for schizophrenia and maternal smoking during pregnancy on obstetric complications. Schizophrenia Research. 2007;93(1-3):229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Grootendorst-van Mil NH, Steegers-Theunissen RPM, Hofman A, Jaddoe VWV, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. Brighter children? The association between seasonality of birth and child IQ in a populationbased birth cohort. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e012406. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hettema JM, Walsh D, Kendler KS. Testing the effect of season of birth on familial risk for schizophrenia and related disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1996;168(2):205–209. doi: 10.1192/bjp.168.2.205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Howie B, Fuchsberger C, Stephens M, Marchini J, Abecasis GR. Fast and accurate genotype imputation in genome-wide association studies through pre-phasing. Nature Genetics. 2012;44(8):955–959. doi: 10.1038/ng.2354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. International Schizophrenia Consortium. Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O’Donovan MC, Sullivan PF, et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460(7256):748–752. doi: 10.1038/nature08185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jeronimus BF, Stavrakakis N, Veenstra R, Oldehinkel AJ. Relative Age effects in Dutch adolescents: concurrent and prospective analyses. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(6):e0128856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kendall KM, Rees E, Escott-Price V, Einon M, Thomas R, Hewitt J, et al. Cognitive performance Among carriers of pathogenic copy number variants: analysis of 152000 UK biobank subjects. Biological Psychiatry. 2017;82(2):103–110. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. McGrath J. Hypothesis: is low prenatal vitamin D a risk-modifying factor for schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Research. 1999;40(3):173–177. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(99)00052-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB, Westergaard T, Wohlfahrt J, Ewald H, Mors O, et al. Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine. 1999;340(8):603–608. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199902253400803. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Natale V, Adan A. Season of birth modulates morningness-eveningness preference in humans. Neuroscience Letter. 1999;274(2):139–141. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00672-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Natale V, Adan A, Fabbri M. Season of birth, gender, and social-cultural effects on sleep timing preferences in humans. Sleep. 2009;32(3):423–426. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Pardiñas AF, Holmans P, Pocklington AJ, Escott-Price V, Ripke S, Carrera N, et al. Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and maintained by background selection. Nature Genetics. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Power RA, Verweij KJ, Zuhair M, Montgomery GW, Henders AK, Heath AC, et al. Genetic predisposition to schizophrenia associated with increased use of cannabis. Molecular Psychiatry. 2014;19(11):1201–1204. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Purcell SM, Moran JL, Fromer M, Ruderfer D, Solovieff N, Roussos P, et al. A polygenic burden of rare disruptive mutations in schizophrenia. Nature. 2014;506(7487):185–190. doi: 10.1038/nature12975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Rees E, Walters JT, Georgieva L, Isles AR, Chambert KD, Richards AL, et al. Analysis of copy number variations at 15 schizophrenia-associated loci. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;204(2):108–114. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.131052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, Moran JL, Kahler AK, Akterin S, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nature Genetics. 2013;45(10):1150–1159. doi: 10.1038/ng.2742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511(7510):421–427. doi: 10.1038/nature13595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Singh T, Kurki MI, Curtis D, Purcell SM, Crooks L, McRae J, et al. Rare loss-of-function variants in SETD1A are associated with schizophrenia and developmental disorders. Nature Neuroscience. 2016;19(4):571–577. doi: 10.1038/nn.4267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Smith DJ, Escott-Price V, Davies G, Bailey ME, Colodro-Conde L, Ward J, et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106 000 individuals identifies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Molecular Psychiatry. 2016;21(6):749–757. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sudlow C, Gallacher J, Allen N, Beral V, Burton P, Danesh J, et al. UK Biobank: an open access resource for identifying the causes of a wide range of complex diseases of middle and old age. PLoS Medicine. 2015;12(3):e1001779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale MC. Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60(12):1187–1192. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Suvisaari JM, Haukka JK, Lonnqvist JK. No association between season of birth of patients with schizophrenia and risk of schizophrenia among their siblings. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;66(1):1–6. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00506-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Svensson AC, Lichtenstein P, Sandin S, Oberg S, Sullivan PF, Hultman CM. Familial aggregation of schizophrenia: the moderating effect of age at onset, parental immigration, paternal age and season of birth. Scandinavial Journal Public Health. 2012;40(1):43–50. doi: 10.1177/1403494811420485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Thapar A, Rice F, Hay D, Boivin J, Langley K, van den Bree M, et al. Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence from a novel design. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):722–727. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Torrey EF, Torrey BB, Peterson MR. Seasonality of schizophrenic births in the United States. Archives General Psychiatry. 1977;34(9):1065–1070. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770210079007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Vaucher J, Keating BJ, Lasserr AM, Gan W, Lyall DM, Ward J, et al. Cannabis use and risk of schizophrenia: a Mendelian randomization study. Molecular Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718000454

RESOURCES